Skip to main content
Scientific Reports logoLink to Scientific Reports
. 2021 Jan 12;11:542. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-79451-3

Newborns’ sensitivity to speed changes as a building block for animacy perception

Elisa Di Giorgio 1,, Marco Lunghi 1, Giorgio Vallortigara 2, Francesca Simion 1
PMCID: PMC7803759  PMID: 33436701

Abstract

The human visual system can discriminate between animate beings vs. inanimate objects on the basis of some kinematic cues, such as starting from rest and speed changes by self-propulsion. The ontogenetic origin of such capability is still under debate. Here we investigate for the first time whether newborns manifest an attentional bias toward objects that abruptly change their speed along a trajectory as contrasted with objects that move at a constant speed. To this end, we systematically manipulated the motion speed of two objects. An object that moves with a constant speed was contrasted with an object that suddenly increases (Experiment 1) or with one that suddenly decreases its speed (Experiment 2). When presented with a single speed change, newborns did not show any visual preference. However, newborns preferred an object that abruptly increases and then decreases its speed (Experiment 3), but they did not show any visual preference for the reverse sequence pattern (Experiment 4). Overall, results are discussed in line with the hypothesis of the existence of attentional biases in newborns that trigger their attention towards some visual cues of motion that characterized animate perception in adults.

Subject terms: Psychology, Human behaviour, Neuroscience, Social behaviour

Introduction

Motion of living things—i.e., animate motion—is a rich source of cues about their behaviors, goals and intentions, and humans, as well as other animals, attend to these cues14.

Animacy perception, a perceptual phenomenon which reflects visual processing that is specialized for the extraction of animacy from visual cues of motion5,6, can be studied with simple stimuli showing the motion of just one or two objects714. Heider and Simmel’s15 classic study is the most compelling evidence of such a phenomenon, demonstrating that a “fairly fast, automatic, irresistible and highly stimulus-driven” sense of animacy could arise from basic dynamic perceptual cues. Among the visual cues of motion that trigger animacy perception in adults, the most powerful are self-propulsion (e.g., an entity’s capacity to initiate motion without an applied external force), motion contingency, discontinuity in motion trajectory, the violation of Newtonian laws, body-axis alignment, and sudden direction and speed changes without any contact with other objects8,9,12,13,16.

From a developmental perspective, the irresistible and automatic nature of animacy perception in adults based on these simple low-level perceptual visual cues of motion raises questions on its ontogenetic origins. Whether the animate-inanimate distinction is present early in life, what perceptual cues signal to the infant that an entity is animate and what is the role of visual experience, are relevant and intriguing open questions. The animate-inanimate distinction is well in place in the first year of life. Broadly speaking, infants understand very early that animate entities behave in ways in which inanimate objects do not1726. Very young infants not only categorize animate entities on the basis of start from rest and speed changes by self-propulsion2628, but also attribute goals and intentions to objects that move on their own2933. Further, chasing perception in 4- and 10-month-old infants is mainly triggered by the presence of sudden accelerations and by the specific relations between the movements of the two objects34.

However, controversy still surrounds the question about the developmental origin of animacy perception. Some authors credit the infant with innately based, domain-specific systems that are sensitive to certain perceptual visual cues of motion (i.e., self-propulsion) in identifying agents and animate beings20,22,35,36. For instance, Mandler has hypothesized that motion provides infants with conceptual knowledge about the “kinds of things” objects are36. In the same way, other authors have hypothesized that at birth, infants possess separate core knowledge systems for animate and inanimate objects, and that they understand that constraints on motion that apply to physical objects may not hold true for animate beings22,37,38. As an alternative to this domain-specific view, other authors presented a theoretical framework that has at its core the idea that general rather than specific mechanisms are sufficient to account for how infants perceive and process the motion properties that characterized an object as animate or inanimate (i.e., the constrained attentional associative learning framework3941). Specifically, according to this more perceptually oriented view, infants use learning mechanisms that operate across all domains of knowledge to encode statistical regularities in their environment: they would thus learn about properties of the objects that appear in a self-propelled event through their experiences with specific kinds of entities that engage in these motions. A third theoretical framework, the cue-based bootstrapping model, proposed that animacy perception develops by bootstrapping from an inborn sensitivity to general low-level visual cues of motion, into a learning mode central to infants’ later concept of animacy that appears later during development. In this view, the presence of inborn predispositions to some visual cues of motion that characterized animate beings might be a sort of jumpstart to the development of animacy perception through learning mechanisms42.

The aim of the present paper was to investigate whether a sensitivity to some rudimentary visual cues of motion that in older infants and adults elicited animacy perception is present at birth or is the result of learning and experience-dependent processes.

Differently from adult studies where kinematic cues are manipulated together to elicit the highest level of animacy perception13, here the main aim was to investigate the role of each of those individual visual cues of motion in attracting the newborns’ attention, put them under separate experimental control. Using such method, recent comparative studies in newborns and chicks demonstrated that sensitivity to some cues of motion that trigger animacy perception in adults are present very early in life. For example, visually self-propelled objects whose motion reveals an internal energy source of energy attract naïve chicks43. Chicks also are attracted by moving objects that can accelerate and decelerate by their own44. They were presented with two animated-event stimuli, in each one a red ball appeared on the screen already in motion (i.e., no cues about the onset motion were available), moved linearly along the horizontal trajectory and then disappeared at the opposite side of the screen. In the “speed-change stimulus”, the object changes its speed two times: it suddenly accelerated at one third of the way and then suddenly decelerated to the original speed at two thirds of the way. This stimulus was compared to the “constant-speed stimulus”, where the object moved at a constant speed. Results showed that naïve chicks show a preference to an object that changes its speed over an object that moves at a constant speed. Intriguingly, the preference disappeared when the exact moment of the sudden speed change is occluded.

As for newborns, it has been demonstrated that they are sensitive to certain well-defined perceptual spatio-temporal cues present in a physical causal event (i.e., launching event)45. Recently, Di Giorgio and colleagues showed for the first time that newborns are sensitive to a start from rest by self-propulsion46. Newborns were presented with two-computer generated events in which the sudden onset motion of an object without an applied external force, and therefore perceived as self-propelled by adults, was manipulated. In that study, an event in which an object suddenly started from rest without the application of external forces was clearly visible to the baby, was contrasted with an ambiguous event, in which the object appeared already on the screen in motion so as no cues about the onset motion were available. In that scenario, newborns showed a spontaneous visual preference for the object that abruptly started from rest by self-propulsion, demonstrating an early sensitivity to this visual cue of motion that triggers animacy perception in adults46.

At present, it is not known whether human newborns are sensitive to other elementary motion cues that elicit animacy perception in adults, besides start from rest without any external force. Indeed, objects that change their velocity without an apparent external cause, tend to be perceived as animate and intentional. On the other hand, objects that do not change velocity or change their motion on the base of external causes are judged as inanimate. Moreover, objects perceived as moving relatively faster are perceived as animate more often than relatively slower objects12, and a sudden deceleration tends to favour the perception of animacy less than a corresponding sudden acceleration13.

Due to speed change relevance as a visual cue of motion in eliciting animacy perception in adults7,8,13, here we sought to investigate whether a sensitivity to an object that changes its speed on its own, without any interaction with an external object, is present at birth.

To test this hypothesis, newborns were presented simultaneously with two computer-generated events in which we systematically manipulated only one visual cue of motion that is known to elicit animacy perception in adults, that is the speed of an object, in four different visual preference experiments. In Experiment 1 and 2, a single speed change was tested. Experiment 1 comprised an object that after a third of its own trajectory suddenly increases its velocity and continues its trajectory with such motion pattern speed. In Experiment 2, an object that after a third of its own trajectory suddenly diminished its velocity and continued its trajectory with such motion pattern speed was presented. Then, we tested newborns’ sensitivity to two consecutive speed changes, Experiment 3 comprised an object that at one third of its trajectory suddenly increases and after another third diminished its velocity, returning to its original speed. Experiment 4, comprised an object that at one third of its trajectory suddenly diminished and after another third increased its velocity, returning to its original speed. The control stimulus was always an object that moves at a constant speed (that we called “speed-constant stimulus”) (Fig. 1).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Schematic representation of stimuli employed in the four Experiments. In each graph, the velocity of the object in the speed-changes event vs. the velocity of the object in the constant speed event as a function of time was plotted. The black line represents the speed change stimuli, whereas the gray line represents the constant speed stimuli.

The dependent variables recorded were the mean fixation duration towards the stimuli and the visual preference percentage towards the speed-change stimulus. In addition, as in a previous study from our lab46, we analysed the location where newborns were looking at the exact time when the object changes its speed (for details see “Methods” and Supplementary Information).

Results

Results showed that when stimuli comprised a single speed-change (Experiment 1 and 2), they did not trigger newborns’ attention. Specifically, newborns did not look longer at the stimulus that abruptly increases (Mmean duration fixation = 3.73 s, SD = 1.36 vs. “speed-constant stimulus”, Mmean duration fixation = 4.95 s, SD = 3.62, t11 = 1.43, p = 0.18, n.s.) or decreases its speed (Mmean duration fixation = 3.94 s, SD = 1.50 vs. “speed-constant stimulus”, Mmean duration fixation = 4.13 s, SD = 1.83, t10 = 0.54, p = 0.67, n.s.). In line with this, newborns did not show any visual preference for the speed-change stimulus neither in Experiment 1 (MPercentage score = 47.80%, SD = 10.82, t11 = 0.72, p = 0.49, n.s.) nor in Experiment 2 (MPercentage score = 49.4%, SD = 11.30, t10 = 0.19, p = 0.86, n.s.). Seven out of 12 newborns preferred to look at the increased speed stimulus (binomial test, p = 0.77, n.s.), and only 4 out of 11 newborns preferred to look at the decreased speed stimulus compared to the speed-constant event (binomial test, p = 0.55, n.s.). Further, the analyses on the location where newborns were looking at the exact time when the object changes its speed suggested that they were not able to detect a single and abrupt speed change, since the percentage was not above the chance level (Experiment 1, M = 46.10%, SD = 9.10, one sample t-test, t11 = 1.33, p = 0.21) (Experiment 2, M = 47.31%, SD = 7.42, one sample t-test, t10 = 1.26, p = 0.23).

Results of Experiment 1 and 2 seem to suggest that a single speed change in terms of increase and decrease speed respectively is not sufficient for eliciting a spontaneous newborns’ visual preference. At a first glance, this suggested that speed changes as perceptual visual cues are not relevant for triggering newborns’ attention such as self-propulsion did46. However, even if the null results should be interpreted with caution, they are in line with those studies that demonstrated that only one single speed-change was not a very strong perceptual visual cue in triggering animacy perception in adults8,13. In contrast, it has been demonstrated that two consecutive speed changes (a sudden speed increase followed by an abrupt speed decreased), if they are allocated in coherent and coordinated sequence, were particularly effective in eliciting perception of animacy in adults8 as well as in chicks44. Therefore, the specific aim of Experiments 2 and 3 was to test whether this is the case also for human newborns.

Results suggested that newborns are sensitive to two consecutive speed changes, but only in the presence of an abrupt velocity increase followed by a sudden velocity decrease (Mmean duration fixation = 3.47 s, SD = 1.53 vs. speed-constant stimulus Mmean duration fixation = 2.43 s, SD = 0.57, t12 = 2.38, p = 0.035, Cohen’s d = 0.66) (Experiment 3). Eleven out of the 13 newborns preferred to look at the two consecutive increase–decrease speed change stimulus (binomial test, p = 0.022), and their visual preference for that stimulus was significantly above the chance level, (M = 55.10%, SD = 7), t12 = 2.40, p = 0.033, Cohen’s d = 0.66). In addition, the analyses on the location where newborns were looking at the exact time when the object changes its speed revealed that the percentage was above the chance level (Condition 3, M = 62.20%, SD = 15.10, one sample t-test, t12 = 2.84, p = 0.015, Cohen’s d = 0.79). On the contrary, the stimulus with the reverse speed change sequence (abrupt decrease followed by a sudden increase in velocity) did not grab newborns’ attention (Experiment 4). They did not look longer at that stimulus (Mmean duration fixation = 3.64 s, SD = 1.07 vs. speed-constant stimulus Mmean duration fixation = 3.53 s, SD = 1.10, t11 = 0.38, p = 0.71, Cohen’s d = 0.11). Only 4 out of the 12 newborns preferred to look at the two consecutive decrease-increase speed change stimulus (binomial test, p = 0.038), and their visual preference for that stimulus was not significantly above the chance level, (M = 49.32%, SD = 12.20, one sample t-test, t11 = 0.32, p = 0.89). Finally, the analyses on the location where newborns were looking at the exact time when the object changes its speed revealed that the percentage was not above the chance level (M = 51.30%, SD = 16.0, one sample t-test, t11 = 0.16, p = 0.875) (Figs. 2, 3 and 4).

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Average percentage of visual preference for the speed-change event in the four Experiments. The error bars represent the 95% Confidence Interval for the mean value.

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Average percentage of number of orienting responses for the speed-change event in the four Experiments. The error bars represent the 95% Confidence Interval for the mean value.

Figure 4.

Figure 4

Mean duration fixation toward speed-change and constant speed stimuli in the four Experiments. The error bars represent the 95% Confidence Interval for the mean value.

Overall, results seemed to suggest that newborns have some rudimentary attentional bias toward speed-changes, but they need to have sufficient information, since they preferred the object that changed its speed twice.

Discussion

Inferring animacy is the developmental precursor to complex social understanding. While there is mounting evidence for the precocious acquisition of the animacy concept, much less is known about perceptual visual cues infants use to acquire such an “abstract” concept from early on. Among dynamic cues, start from rest by self-propulsion is critical to trigger animacy perception in adults13, and recent comparative studies demonstrated that sensitivity to this visual motion cue is present from birth in chicks43 and human newborns46. In addition to such visual cues of motion, in the present work we demonstrated that human newborns’ attention is triggered also by stimuli that abruptly change their speed without any contact with external forces or objects.

In line with those studies that demonstrated that the presence of one change in speed does not favor much the perception of animacy in adults8,13, we found that a single change in speed is not sufficient to elicit a visual preference in newborns (Experiment 1 and 2). On the contrary, newborns looked longer at the object that suddenly increased and then decreased its speed compared to the same object that moves at a constant speed (Experiment 3). Further, when the object abruptly decreased and then increased its speed (Experiment 4), newborns did not show any visual preference. Overall, results suggest that newborns have some rudimentary attentional bias toward speed-changes, but they need to have sufficient information (e.g. two consecutive abrupt speed changes), and that the ordering of the speed change has a role in eliciting their visual preference. Apparently, an abrupt speed increase, followed by a sudden speed decrease, is a powerful combination of speed changes for eliciting newborns’ preference.

An interesting point concerns the fact that, in order to investigate newborns’ sensitivity to speed-changes, the stimuli employed here resemble a step acceleration or deceleration change. Indeed, our “speed-change stimulus” abruptly increased or decreased their speed, without any pause. Therefore, to test whether the visual perceptual system at birth is sensitive to acceleration, future studies should implement stimuli, such those employed in the present study, with a gradual speed change, that is the more natural speed profile expected for animate creatures. Indeed, we already know that such stimulus manipulation elicited a spontaneous preference for the natural speed changing stimulus in visually naïve chicks44, but not in human newborns47,48. However, it must be pointed out that in the study on newborns, the stimuli employed were complex Point-Light Display (PLD) of goal-directed grasping hand actions and, therefore, no direct comparison between the results of the previous and present study can be done.

Overall, the present work showed that newborns respond also to changes in speed of already moving stimuli, in addition to start from rest by self-propelled movement found in a previous study46, supporting the hypothesis that since birth our species has some attentional biases toward visual cues of motion that trigger animacy perception in adults.

These biases might be considered as the building blocks on which, during the development, infants and adults enhance their ability to discriminate animate from inanimate beings. The present result, if considered together with similar data found in visually naïve chicks44, demonstrated the presence of attentional predispositions tuned toward one of the general properties of animate creatures that older infants and adults perceived as animate.

Although speed changes and start from rest by self-propulsion are motion cues that elicit newborns’ visual preferences, how these cues interact to promote such preferences is still largely unknown. Therefore, future studies should investigate whether one of these visual cues is more powerful compared to the other, or whether they have additive effects in triggering newborns’ attention.

To conclude, our data are compatible with the idea that an initial sensitivity to low-level visual cues leads infants from birth to attend to visual cues of motion that characterized animate beings49, and to learn rich correlations among these cues as they parse animates from inanimates (i.e., cue-based bootstrapping model, 42). This learning appears to be central to infants’ later concept of animacy, guiding their socio-emotional understanding, the theory of mind, and predictions of action17,26,40. The extent to which experience is needed for the expression of animacy distinction should be tested in future.

Understanding how our visual system perceives animacy is a mandatory step in understanding how we perceive the social world. The perception of animacy is an automatic process that begins in early infancy, but in some cases this perception develops atypically, leading to difficulties in perceiving animacy, such as in the case of autism spectrum disorders50,51 (see some recent studies on an animal model of autism52). Therefore, knowing more about the quality of motions that triggers such a fundamental perceptual ability is vital to understanding how our social cognition works in both typical and atypical individuals.

Methods

Participants

A total of 61 healthy and full-term newborns were tested (31 males). Thirteen babies were excluded from analyses because they showed a position bias (n = 5) (i.e., he looked in one direction more than 80% of the total fixation time)46,53 or because they became tired and started crying during testing (n = 8).

Therefore, the final sample comprised 48 (28 males) healthy and full-term newborns. They were randomly assigned to one of the four Experiments: n = 12 in Experiment 1, n = 11 in Experiment 2, n = 13 in Experiment 3, and n = 12 in Experiment 4. Their postnatal age ranged from 12 to 139 h (Mage = 43.4 h, SD = 27.8). All subjects met the normal delivery screening criteria, had a birth weight between 2440 and 4105 g (M = 3325 g, SD = 440.5), and had an Apgar score > 9 at 5 min. Newborns were tested only if awake and in an alert state, and after the parents had provided informed consent. All the experiments described have been carried out in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. Further, the Azienda Ospedale—Università degli Studi di Padova has licensed all experimental procedures (Protocol number: 91644645).

Stimuli

Stimuli consisted of two video-animation events displayed on an Apple LED Cinema Display (Flat Panel 30″) computer monitor (refresh rate = 60 Hz, 2560 × 1600), representing the movement of an object (i.e., a gray ball of 3 cm of diameter already used in previous studies with human newborns45,46. The baby sat on an experimenter’s lap at a distance of approximately 30 cm from the computer screen, therefore at such distance 1 degree of visual angle is equivalent to 0.53 cm. In each of the four Experiments, two stimuli were presented simultaneously on the screen, on the right and one on the left. In both events, the object always entered the newborn’s view already in motion, appearing from the external limit of a white background, therefore no cues about “start from rest” were available. Similarly, once completed its motion, the object disappeared from view while still moving. After the entrance, the object always moved along a straight trajectory towards the center of the screen. One stimulus was always characterized by the presence of visible speed-changes (i.e., speed increment or decrement, and both) during his trajectory, whereas the other moved at a constant speed (see Fig. 1). Videos were produced by looping the animations.

Experiment 1

An object that increases its speed without the contact of external forces and a speed-constant stimulus were presented. In the speed-change stimulus, the object suddenly increased its speed at one-third (5.3 cm, 10.02°) of the distance (16 cm, 28.07°). The speed changed from 3.36 deg/s to 6.08 deg/s. In contrast, in the speed-constant stimulus the object covered the same distance (16 cm) at a constant speed of 4.95 deg/s. Both events described lasted 6 s (150 frames, 25 frames/s).

Experiment 2

The speed-change stimulus comprised an object that suddenly decreases its speed without any contact with external forces at one-third (5.3 cm, 10.02°) of the distance (16 cm, 28.07°). The speed changed from 6.08 deg/s to 3.36 deg/s. In contrast, in the speed-constant stimulus, the object covered the same distance (16 cm) at a constant speed of 3.81 deg/s. Both events described lasted 7.5 s (188 frames, 25 frames/s) (see Figs. 1 and 5).

Figure 5.

Figure 5

Schematic representation of the events used in the four Experiments. Panel (a) Experiment 1 and 2, left sequence represents the Speed-Change Event and right sequence the Constant Speed Event. Panel (b) Experiments 2 and 3, left sequence represents the Speed-Change Event and right sequence the Constant Speed Event.

Experiment 3

The speed change stimulus was an object that suddenly increased its speed at one third (5.3 cm, 10.02°) of the total distance (16 cm, 28.07°), and then decreased it back to the original speed at two third of the way (10.6 cm, 19.46°). The speed changed from 3.36 deg/s to 6.08 deg/s and then returned at 3.36 deg/s. In contrast, in the constant-speed motion event, the object covered the same distance (16 cm) at a constant speed of 3.81 deg/s. Both events described lasted 8 s (200 frames, 25 frames/s).

Experiment 4

An object that abruptly decreased its speed at one third (5.3 cm, 10.02°) of the total distance (16 cm, 28.07°) and then increased it back to the original speed at two third of the way (10.6 cm, 19.46°), characterized the speed-change stimulus. In this case, the speed changed from 6.08 deg/s to 3.36 deg/s and then returned at 6.08 deg/s. In contrast, in the constant-speed motion event, the object covered the same distance (16 cm) at a constant speed of 4.57 deg/s. Both events described lasted 6.5 s (163 frames, 25 frames/s) (see Figs. 1 and 5).

To note, the low speed values adopted here derive from previous studies. For instance, it has been shown54, using a visual preference procedure, estimated thresholds at about 1.4 deg/s at 4 weeks of age, and 6.7 deg/s at 10 weeks55,56.

Apparatus and procedure

We employed the infant-control preferential looking technique57. Stimuli presentation and data collection were performed using E-Prime 2.0. The baby sat on an experimenter’s lap in front of the screen and white curtains were drawn on both sides of the newborn to prevent interference from irrelevant distractors. Above the monitor, the video camera recorded the eye movements of newborns to control their looking behavior on-line and to allow off-line coding of their fixations46 (see Fig. 6).

Figure 6.

Figure 6

Schematic representation of the Lab Set up during data collection. On the right, the black line circle represents the baby sitting on the experimenter’s lap (the dot line circle) at a distance of 30 cm from the computer screen (the thin grey rectangle). At such a distance 1 degree of visual angle is equivalent to 0.53 cm. Above the monitor, the video camera (the black square connected to a monitor behind the newborns’ computer screen) recorded the eye movements of newborns. On the left, the second experimenter (the dashed line circle) manages the stimuli presentation with E-prime 2.0 installed on a second computer (black line rectangle).

At the beginning of each experimental condition, a red disc on a black background appeared to attract the newborn’s gaze to the centre of the monitor (i.e., fixation point). In a continuous fashion, the disc changed in size from small (diameter of 1.8 cm) to large (diameter of 2.5 cm), and vice-versa until the newborn’s gaze was properly aligned with the red disc. The red disk blinked at a rate of 300 ms on, and 300 ms off. The sequence of trials was then started by a second experimenter who watched the newborn’s eyes through the monitor. When the newborn’ s gaze was aligned with the red disk, the second experimenter pressed a keyboard key that automatically turned off the central disc activated the onset of the stimuli and a timer on the video, thereby initiating the sequence of trials. Because stimuli were presented bilaterally on the left and the right side of the monitor with a convergent motion pattern (from peripheral to the central visual field), each newborn was given two paired presentations (trials 1 and 2) of the test stimuli. In each trial, the position of the stimuli was reversed (the initial left–right order of presentation was counterbalanced across subjects). A trial ended when the newborn did not fixate on the display for at least 10 s. When the newborn shifted his/her gaze away from the stimuli for more than 10 s, the stimuli were removed and the disc was turned on to start Trial 2. The experiment finished when the newborn shifted his/her gaze away from the stimuli for more than 10 s during Trial 2.

The four Experiments were planned and conducted as completely independent experiments, and thus, no direct comparison among the experiments was applicable and no correction for the alpha level in any of the statistical tests was conducted for each of the experiments.

We recorded both the baby’s eye-movement by a camera in front of the stimuli presented and the trial sequence in order to permit the coding off-line frame by frame. Further, a timer on the video that starts exactly at the same time when the first trial starts allows us to synchronize the video of the baby’s eyes and the stimuli’s sequence to connect the exact moment on the frame to the presented stimuli.

Two coders, unaware of the stimuli presented, analyzed off-line the videos by coding newborns’ eye movements frame-by-frame. The average level of inter-observer agreement was Pearson’s r = 0.93, p < 0.001.

According to Cohen's model of infants’ attention58, the dependent variables that we recorded are i) the total number of orienting responses towards the stimuli as index of the attention getting mechanisms. This index is calculated as the sum of how many times newborns orient their gaze towards either the stimulus on the left or the stimulus on the right in both trials, and ii) the mean duration of fixation towards the stimuli as an index of the attention holding mechanisms. Moreover, we calculated the percentage of visual preference in each experimental condition, defined as the total fixation time (TFT) toward the speed-change stimulus divided by the total fixation time spent in looking at both stimuli (TFT speed-change/TFT speed-change + TFT constant-speed) and compared this percentage to the chance level (50%). Scores significantly above 50% indicate a visual preference for the speed-change stimulus. These analyses are traditionally performed with data from newborns50,59 To note, results on the total number of orienting responses missed statistical significance and therefore we did not report them (but see Supplementary Information).

In addition, we decided to conduct further analyses in order to investigate whether the sudden speed changes motion per se attracted newborns’ attention in each experimental condition. We analysed the location where newborns were looking at the exact time when the object changes its speed. To do so, we calculated the total number of orienting responses towards the object that changes its speed and divided this number by the total number of orienting responses towards both stimuli at the same exact time, X 10046. Since in Experiment 3 and 4 there were two consecutive speed changes each and newborns who looked at the first change remained on that stimulus and looked also at the second one (Experiment 3, t12 = 0.26, p = 0.80 and Experiment 4, t11 = 0.90, p = 0.39), we collapsed together the two speed changes.

Supplementary Information

Acknowledgements

The authors are deeply indebted to Professor Eugenio Baraldi, Director of the Intensive Care Unit and Professor Giorgio Perilongo Director of the Department of Women and Child Health for their collaboration. Special thanks to the nursing staff of the Pediatric Clinic (Azienda Ospedaliera) to the babies who took part in the study and to their parents, to Elena Berto, Daniela Carà, and Anna Bianchi for assistance with newborn testing. This study was carried out within the framework of the agreement between the University and the Azienda Ospedaliera of Padova prot.: 91644645. The research was supported from the European Research Council under the European Union's Seventh Framework Programme (FP7/2007‐2013) Grant ERC‐2011‐ADG_20110406, Project No: 461 295517, PREMESOR to G.V. and supported by Fondazione Caritro Grant Biomarker DSA [40102839] to G.V., PRIN 2018, Project No: 20152M5A5J, to G.V.

Author contributions

E.D.G., M.L., G.V. and F.S. designed the study; M.L. designed the stimuli; E.D.G. and M.L. collected and analyzed the data, and did the statistical analyses; E.D.G interpreted the data and wrote the manuscript; M.L., G.V. and F.S. discussed the results and critically revised the manuscript.

Data availability

The dataset of the present study is available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Footnotes

Publisher's note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1038/s41598-020-79451-3.

References

  • 1.Blakemore SJ, Boyer P, Pachot-Clouard M, Meltzoff A, Segebarth C, Decety J. The detection of contingency and animacy from simple animations in the human brain. Cereb. Cortex. 2003;13:837–844. doi: 10.1093/cercor/13.8.837. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Craighero L, Leo I, Umiltà C, Simion F. Newborns’ preference for goal-directed actions. Cognition. 2011;120:26–32. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2011.02.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Di Giorgio E, et al. Filial responses as predisposed and learned preferences: Early attachment in chicks and babies. Behav. Brain Res. 2017;325:90–104. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2016.09.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Rosa-Salva O, Mayer U, Vallortigara G. Roots of a social brain: Developmental models of emerging animacy-detection mechanisms. Neurosci. Biobehav. R. 2015;50:150–168. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.12.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Rutherford MD. Evidence for specialized perception of animate motion. In: Rutherford MD, Kuhlmeier VA, editors. Social Perception: Detection and Interpretation of Animacy, Agency, and Intention. Cambridge: MIT Press; 2013. pp. 115–138. [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Scholl BJ, Gao T. Perceiving animacy and intentionality: visual processing or higher-level judgment? In: Rutherford MD, Kuhlmeier VA, editors. Social Perception: Detection and Interpretation of Animacy, Agency, and Intention. Cambridge: MIT Press; 2013. pp. 197–230. [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Gao T, Newman GE, Scholl BJ. The psychophysics of chasing: A case study in the perception of animacy. Cognit. Psychol. 2009;59:154–179. doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2009.03.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Gyulai E. Considerations on perception of animacy in the motion of a single object. Percept. Motor Skill. 2004;99:1014–1026. doi: 10.2466/pms.99.3.1014-1026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Santos NS, David N, Bente G, Vogeley K. Parametric induction of animacy experience. Conscious. Cogn. 2008;17:425–437. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2008.03.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Scholl BJ, Tremoulet PD. Perceptual causality and animacy. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2000;4:299–309. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01506-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Schultz J, Bülthoff HH. Parametric animacy percept evoked by a single moving dot mimicking natural stimuli. J. Vis. 2013;13:15. doi: 10.1167/13.4.15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Szego PA, Rutherford MD. Life is not just in the fast lane: Dissociating the perceptions of speed and animacy. J. Vis. 2007;7:480–480. doi: 10.1167/7.9.480. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Tremoulet PD, Feldman J. Perception of animacy from the motion of a single object. Perception. 2000;29:943–951. doi: 10.1068/p3101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Tremoulet PD, Feldman J. The influence of spatial context and the role of intentionality in the interpretation of animacy from motion. Percept. Psychophys. 2006;68:1047–1058. doi: 10.3758/BF03193364. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Heider F, Simmel M. An experimental study of apparent behavior. Am. J. Psychol. 1944;57:243–249. doi: 10.2307/1416950. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Abrams RA, Christ SE. Motion onset captures attention. Psychol. Sci. 2003;14:427–432. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.01458. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Arterberry ME, Bornstein MH. Infant perceptual and conceptual categorization: The roles of static and dynamic stimulus attributes. Cognition. 2002;86:1–24. doi: 10.1016/S0010-0277(02)00108-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Baker RK, Pettigrew TL, Poulin-Dubois D. Infants’ ability to associate motion paths with object kinds. Infant Behav. Dev. 2014;37:119–129. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2013.12.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Gelman R. First principles organize attention to and learning about relevant data: Number and the animate-inanimate distinction as examples. Cognit. Sci. 1990;14:79–106. doi: 10.1207/s15516709cog1401_5. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Premack D. The infant’s theory of self-propelled objects. Cognition. 1990;36:1–16. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(90)90051-K. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Saxe R, Tzelnic T, Carey S. Knowing who dunnit: Infants identify the causal agent in an unseen causal interaction. Dev. Psychol. 2007;43:149. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.43.1.149. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Spelke ES, Kinzler KD. Core knowledge. Dev. Sci. 2007;10:89–96. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00569.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Spelke ES, Phillips A, Woodward AL. Infant’s knowledge of object motion and human action. In: Sperber D, Premack D, Premack AJ, editors. Causal Cognition. Oxford: Clarendon; 1995. pp. 44–78. [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Shimizu YA, Johnson SP. Infants’ attribution of a goal to a morphologically unfamiliar agent. Dev. Sci. 2004;7:425–430. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2004.00362.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Surian L, Caldi S. Infants' individuation of agents and inert objects. Dev. Sci. 2010;13:143–150. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00873.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Träuble B, Pauen S, Poulin-Dubois D. Speed and direction changes induce the perception of animacy in 7-month-old infants. Front. Psychol. 2014;5:1141. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01141. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Golinkoff R, Harding C, Carlson V, Sexton ME. The infant’s perception of causal events: the distinction between animate and inanimate objects. In: Lipsitt LP, Rovee-Collier C, editors. Advances in Infancy Research. New York: Ablex; 1984. pp. 145–151. [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Poulin-Dubois D, Lepage A, Ferland D. Infants' concept of animacy. Cogn. Dev. 1996;11:19–36. doi: 10.1016/S0885-2014(96)90026-X. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Csibra G. Goal attribution to inanimate agents by 6.5-month-old infants. Cognition. 2008;10:705–717. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2007.08.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Csibra G, Bíró S, Koós O, Gergely G. One-year-old infants use teleological representations of actions productively. Cogn. Sci. 2003;27:111–133. doi: 10.1207/s15516709cog2701_4. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Luo Y. Three-month-old infants attribute goals to a non-human agent. Dev. Sci. 2011;14:453–460. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.00995.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Luo Y, Baillargeon R. Can a self-propelled box have a goal? Psychological reasoning in 5-month-old infants. Psychol. Sci. 2005;16:601–608. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01582.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Luo Y, Kaufman L, Baillargeon R. Young infants’ reasoning about physical events involving inert and self-propelled objects. Cogn. Psychol. 2009;58:441–486. doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2008.11.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Frankenhuis WE, House B, Barrett HC, Johnson SP. Infants’ perception of chasing. Cognition. 2013;126:224–233. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2012.10.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Leslie AM. ToMM, ToBY, and agency: core architecture and domain specificity. In: Hirschfeld L, Gelman S, editors. Mapping the Mind: Domain Specificity in Cognition and Culture. New York: Cambridge University Press; 1994. pp. 119–148. [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Mandler JM. Conceptual categorization. In: Rakison D, Oakes LM, editors. Early Category and Concept Development. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2003. pp. 103–131. [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Vallortigara G. Core knowledge of object, number, and geometry: A comparative and neural approach. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 2012;29:213–236. doi: 10.1080/02643294.2012.654772. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Vallortigara G. Aristotle and the chicken: Animacy and the origins of beliefs. In: Fasolo A, editor. The Theory of Evolution and its Impact. New York: Springer; 2012. pp. 189–200. [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Rakison DH. Developing knowledge of objects' motion properties in infancy. Cognition. 2005;96:183–214. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2004.07.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Rakison, D. H., Lupyan, G., Oakes, L. M. & Walker-Andrews, A. S. Developing object concepts in infancy: An associative learning perspective. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. i-127, https://www.jstor.org/stable/30162860 (2008). [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 41.Rakison DH, Poulin-Dubois D. Developmental origin of the animate–inanimate distinction. Psychol. Bull. 2001;127:209–228. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.127.2.209. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Biro S, Leslie AM. Infants’ perception of goal directed actions: Development through cue-based bootstrapping. Dev. Sci. 2007;10:379–398. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2006.00544.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Mascalzoni E, Regolin L, Vallortigara G. Innate sensitivity for self-propelled causal agency in newly hatched chicks. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. USA. 2010;107:4483–4485. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0908792107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Rosa-Salva O, Grassi M, Lorenzi E, Regolin L, Vallortigara G. Spontaneous preference for visual cues of animacy in naïve domestic chicks: The case of speed changes. Cognition. 2016;157:49–60. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2016.08.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Mascalzoni E, Regolin L, Vallortigara G, Simion F. The cradle of causal reasoning: Newborns’ preference for physical causality. Dev. Sci. 2013;16:327–335. doi: 10.1111/desc.12018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Di Giorgio E, Lunghi M, Simion F, Vallortigara G. Visual cues of motion that trigger animacy perception at birth: The case of self-propulsion. Dev. Sci. 2017;20:e12394. doi: 10.1111/desc.12394. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Fox R, McDaniel C. The perception of biological motion by human infants. Science. 1982;218:486–487. doi: 10.1126/science.7123249. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Craighero L, Ghirardi V, Lunghi M, Panin F, Simion F. Two-day-old newborns learn to discriminate accelerated-decelerated biological kinematics from constant velocity motion. Cognition. 2020;195:104126. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2019.104126. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Simion F, Bardi L, Mascalzoni E, Regolin L. From motion cues to social perception: innate predispositions. In: Rutherford MD, Kuhlmeier VA, editors. Social Perception: Detection and Interpretation of Animacy, Agency, and Intention. Cambridge: MIT Press; 2013. pp. 37–72. [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Di Giorgio E, et al. Difference in visual social predispositions between newborns at low- and high-risk for autism. Sci. Rep. 2016;6:26395. doi: 10.1038/srep26395. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Rutherford MD, Pennington BF, Rogers SJ. The perception of animacy in young children with autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2006;36:983–992. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0136-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Lorenzi E, et al. Embryonic exposure to valproic acid impairs social predispositions for dynamic cues of animate motion in newly-hatched chicks. Front. Psychol. 2019;10:501. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2019.00501. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Bulf H, Johnson SP, Valenza E. Visual statistical learning in the newborn infant. Cognition. 2011;121:127–132. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2011.06.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Kaufmann F, Stucki M, Kaufmann-Hayoz R. Development of infants' sensitivity for slow and rapid motions. Infant Behav. Dev. 1985;8(1):89–98. doi: 10.1016/S0163-6383(85)80019-9. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Wattam-Bell J. Visual motion processing in one-month-old infants: Preferential looking experiments. Vis. Res. 1996;36(11):1671–1677. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(95)00236-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Kellman PJ. Ontogenesis of space and motion perception. In: Epstein W, Rogers S, editors. Perception of Space and Motion. New York: Academic Press; 1995. pp. 327–364. [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Fantz RL. The origin of form perception. Sci. Am. 1961;204:66–72. doi: 10.1038/scientificamerican0561-66. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Cohen LB. Attention-getting and attention-holding processes of infant visual preferences. Child Dev. 1972;43:869–879. doi: 10.2307/1127638. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Di Giorgio E, Lunghi M, Rugani R, Regolin L, Dalla Barba B, Vallortigara G, Simion F. A mental number line in human newborns. Dev. Sci. 2019;22(6):e12801. doi: 10.1111/desc.12801. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Data Availability Statement

The dataset of the present study is available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.


Articles from Scientific Reports are provided here courtesy of Nature Publishing Group

RESOURCES