Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2021 Jul 1.
Published in final edited form as: Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2020 Jul 1;49(2):202–209. doi: 10.1159/000508095

Pronoun Use during Patient-Caregiver Interactions: Associations with Caregiver Well-being

Dyan E Connelly 1, Alice Verstaen 2, Casey L Brown 1, Sandy J Lwi 3, Robert W Levenson 1
PMCID: PMC7805608  NIHMSID: NIHMS1600600  PMID: 32610328

Abstract

Introduction:

Caring for a spouse with dementia can be extremely challenging. Many caregivers experience profound declines in well-being; however, others remain healthy.

Objective:

This study determined whether the personal pronouns used in interactions between persons with dementia (PWDs) and their spousal caregivers were associated with caregiver well-being.

Methods:

Fifty-eight PWDs and their spousal caregivers engaged in a 10-minute conversation about an area of disagreement in a laboratory setting. Verbatim transcripts of the conversation were coded using text analysis software and caregivers and PWDs each received scores for (a) I-pronouns, (b) you-pronouns, and (c) we-pronouns. Caregivers’ well-being was assessed using a composite measure of depression, anxiety, burden and strain.

Results:

Results revealed that less use of we-pronouns by caregivers and PWDs and greater use of I-pronouns by PWDs and you-pronouns by caregivers were associated with lower caregiver well-being.

Conclusions:

These findings indicate that less use of pronouns that refer to the couple (we-pronouns used by either partner) and greater use of pronouns that refer to the PWD (patient I-pronouns and caregiver you-pronouns used by the PWD) are indicative of caregivers at heightened risk for lower well-being.

Keywords: dementia, caregiving, language, pronouns, well-being, social interaction, caregiver burden


Caring for a loved one with a neurodegenerative disease can be a highly meaningful and fulfilling part of family life [1, 2]. However, the demands of caregiving causes many to experience profound declines in well-being, including increased depression, anxiety, burden, and strain [3]. Research on the causes of declines in caregiver well-being [312] has largely focused on factors related to the external environment (e.g., social support) [4], the person with dementia (PWD; e.g., particular symptoms) [39] or the caregiver (e.g., age, gender) [3, 4, 10] that contribute to caregiver vulnerability. While research has linked relationship quality with declines in caregiver well-being [13, 14], less attention has been given to the specific characteristics of the PWD-caregiver relationship, such as the ways that PWDs and caregivers communicate and interact. Examining declines in caregiver well-being in the context of the PWD-caregiver relationship could be highly informative given prior research that indicates that caregiver outcomes are highly impacted by the interpersonal nature of caregiving [1318].

Dementia can alter the nature of the PWD-caregiver relationship [14, 15, 17, 18] in ways that create additional stress and strain for caregivers. For example, PWDs may progressively withdraw from the social world, which can increase the sense of isolation and loneliness in caregivers and contribute to the development of depression [19]. This can be particularly true for spousal caregivers, for whom the loss of a life partner significantly reduces the size and richness of their social world.

Using Language to Understand the Nature of Intimate Relationships

Healthy couples.

The words that people use in their interactions with relationship partners can provide important clues about their own thoughts and feelings as well as the nature of the relationship [20, 21]. One particularly revealing characteristic of the language used in interpersonal interactions is pronoun usage. Pronouns can provide important clues about how partners think about and relate to each other [20, 21]. For example, researchers have distinguished between the use of we-ness pronouns (e.g., we, us, ours) and separateness pronouns (e.g., I, me, yours) as indicators of how partners view themselves in relation to each other [20]. According to this viewpoint, greater relative use of we-ness versus separateness pronouns indicates that the partners identify themselves as part of a team rather than as autonomous individuals. Given that research suggests that happy couples have higher inclusion of other in self and greater cognitive interdependence [22, 23], assessing we-ness has become an important method for objectively measuring mutuality between relationship partners [24].

Prior research with healthy couples has shown that greater use of we-pronouns is related to a number of different positive relationship outcomes [25] including greater relationship commitment, intimacy, feelings of togetherness, and marital quality [22, 126]. Greater we-pronoun use has also been associated with lower cardiovascular arousal, more positive emotional behavior, and more adaptive problem-solving behaviors when conflict arises [20, 27].

In contrast, greater use of separateness pronouns has been related to a number of negative relationship outcomes including lower marital satisfaction and expressing more negative emotion during an interaction [20, 28]. Comparing you and I variants of separateness pronouns, greater use of you-pronouns has been linked with negative interaction qualities including criticism, blame, and less shared identity. In contrast, greater use of I-pronouns has been linked with greater self-focus [27]. In a study of distressed and non-distressed couples [29], greater use of I-pronouns was associated with lower marital satisfaction in non-distressed couples but not in distressed couples. To summarize, there is a large body of scientific evidence supporting the associations between pronoun use and a range of important relationship qualities in healthy couples.

Vulnerable couples.

There have also been a few studies that examined pronoun use in more vulnerable couples such as those in which one partner is afflicted with a serious health problem. In a study of patients with heart disease, greater use of we-pronouns by partners during an interview predicted fewer symptoms of heart failure for patients over the following six months [30]. A similar result was found in an intervention study with smokers where greater use of we-pronouns during a conflict discussion predicted greater success in smoking cessation [31]. Investigators have also examined pronoun use in patient-caregiver dyads during a discussion about how they cope with breast cancer, finding that greater use of we-pronouns was associated with better dyadic adjustment and lower depressive symptoms in patients [22]. Finally, Skerrett posited that nourishing and building the “we” in a relationship can help promote healing in illness by helping couples overcome the feelings of isolation that often accompany declines in health [33].

The Present Study

Although we have studied the use of emotionally positive (e.g., “happy”) and negative (e.g., “angry”) words in the interactions of PWDs and caregivers [34], we are aware of no studies that have examined pronoun use in these kinds of couples. Based on the notion that greater use of we-ness pronouns reflects greater connection and less isolation between partners and that greater use of separateness pronouns reflects the opposite, we hypothesized that less use of we-ness pronouns in the interactions of dementia PWDs and spousal caregivers would be associated with lower levels of caregiver well-being. Similarly, we hypothesized that greater use of separateness pronouns (I-pronouns and you-pronouns) would be associated with lower caregiver well-being. Moreover, if these hypotheses were supported, we planned to examine the role that caregiver marital satisfaction played in explaining the relationship between pronoun usage and caregiver well-being.

Method

Participants

Fifty-eight PWDs (Age: M = 62.78, SD= 8.85; 54.2% male) and their spousal caregivers (age: M= 63.98, SD= 10.33; 44.8% male) were recruited through the Memory and Aging Center at the University of California, San Francisco. PWDs received a diagnosis based on a neurological exam, neuropsychological assessment, and neuroimaging using current research criteria. Thirty-seven PWDs were diagnosed with frontotemporal dementia [3537], 13 were diagnosed with Alzheimer’s Disease [38], and the remaining 8 had a variety of other diagnoses (e.g., Parkinson’s Disease, progressive supranuclear palsy). All couples were paid $90 for their participation in addition to being reimbursed for any transportation costs.

Procedure

A week before their laboratory visit, caregivers completed a questionnaire packet (described below). PWDs and caregivers came to the Berkeley Psychophysiology Laboratory at the University of California, Berkeley to participate in a day-long comprehensive assessment of emotional functioning that included tests of emotional reactivity, regulation, and recognition [39]. Couples were told that their participation was completely voluntary, their responses were confidential, and that either partner could withdraw at any time. All participants provided consent at each site for research projects approved by their respective institutional review boards.

For the purpose of the present study, we focused on one part of the assessment, a well-established task for studying marital interaction that has been used extensively with healthy couples of all ages [40, 41] as well as with couples with a partner with a neurodegenerative disease [14, 15]. Participants sat facing each other in comfortable chairs in a laboratory room set up for video recording of emotional behavior and monitoring of physiological responding (physiological data were not used for the present study). Participants were interviewed briefly by a trained experimenter to identify an area of unresolved conflict in their relationship. Conflict topics varied among couples (e.g., communication, home improvements). After the experimenter left the room, the couple sat quietly for five minutes and then engaged in a 10-minute unrehearsed conversation about the chosen topic.

Measures

Text analysis.

Verbatim transcripts of the conflict conversations were prepared by trained transcribers working with the video recordings. These transcripts were processed using text analysis software, Oedipus Text (OT) [42] that compared each word against a dictionary of personal pronouns to provide frequency counts of PWDs’ and caregivers’ use of pronouns in three categories: (a) I-pronouns (I, I’d, I’ll, I’m, I’ve, me, mine, my, myself); (b) you-pronouns (you, you’d, you’ll, you’re, you’ve, your, yours, yourself); and (c) we-pronouns (our, ours, ourselves, us, we, we’d, we’ll, we’re, we’ve). In most studies of pronoun use using text analysis software, the program detects pronouns and assigns them to categories with no additional processing [24, 27, 29]. In the present study, OT also allowed coders to consider the context in which a pronoun was spoken and to use that information to change its dictionary-based categorization if needed. This additional layer of context-based analysis is potentially important in studies of couples’ interactions because the pronouns used may not always refer to the couple and/or either spouse. For example, a spouse might say, “In the United States, our unemployment levels are currently quite low.” In this instance, the pronoun “our” does not refer to the couple but rather to “Americans”.

OT presented trained coders with each pronoun that had been detected in the transcript in the context of the sentence it occurred in along with the previous and following sentences. Coders then assigned each pronoun to one of the following eight categories: (a) I-words; (b) You-words; (c) We-words; (d) We-phrases; (e) dysfluencies, which occur when there is a repetition and/or the truncation of a proposition.(e.g., “I, I…I need to stop at the store on the way home.”); (f) fillers, which are used by speakers to fill gaps in their narrative but serve no communicative function (e.g., you know, I mean); (g) generics, which occurs when the speaker uses pronouns in a general way, rather than referring to a specific person (e.g., “We have to take better care of the planet.”); (h) references to others, which occurs when the speaker is referring to or speaking for another person (e.g., “After work, we went to happy hour to celebrate her last day.”); and (i) elder speak, which is a form of baby talk often directed at older adults in care (e.g., “Did we forget to take our medicine today?”). Inter-rater reliability for context coding was high (interclass correlation coefficient was .92). Only pronouns that referred to the couple or individual spouses were used in all subsequent analyses. For each participant, a ratio score for words in each category (we, you, I) was computed by dividing the total number of pronouns in that category by the total number of pronouns spoken.

Caregiver well-being.

We characterized caregiver well-being using a composite measure of depression, anxiety, burden, and strain. This measure was calculated by averaging normalized values of four caregiver outcome measures inversed as needed so that lower values always indicated lower well-being. Reliability of this composite well-being score was high (alpha=.841):

Caregiver depression.

Caregivers completed a 20-item version of the Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale (CESD) [43]. This measure assesses depressive symptoms (e.g., depressed mood, feelings of worthlessness, loss of appetite) on a scale ranging from 0 (Rarely or None of the Time) to 3 (Most or Almost All the Time) during the previous week. Caregivers’ average depression score was 12.05 (SD=9.42, range 0–44).

Caregiver anxiety.

Caregivers completed the Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI), a 21-item measure that assesses cognitive and somatic symptoms of anxiety (e.g. heart pounding, difficulty breathing, fear of the worst) on a scale ranging from 0 (Not at All) to 3 (Severely) [44]. Caregivers’ average anxiety score was 6.53 (SD=7.58, range 0–37).

Caregiver strain.

Caregivers completed the Caregiver Strain Index (CSI), a 13-item measure that assesses subjective and objective elements of caregiver strain (e.g., inconvenience, disturbed sleep, financial strain) which was computed by summing the no (0) and yes (1) responses for the 13 items [45]. Caregivers’ average strain score was 5.36 (SD=3.46, range 0–13).

Caregiver burden.

Caregivers completed the Zarit Burden Interview-Short Form (ZBI-SF), a 12-item measure that assesses caregivers’ subjective appraisal of burden associated with functional and behavioral impairments of the care recipient (e.g., “Do you feel you have lost control of our life since your relative’s illness?”) on a scale of 0 (Never) to 4 (Nearly Always) [46]. Caregivers’ average burden score was 17.60 (SD=9.35, range 1–37).

Caregiver marital satisfaction.

Caregivers completed the Locke-Wallace Marital Adjustment scale [47], a 15-item measure that assesses agreement on a number of domains including family finances, demonstrations of affection, sexual relations, and philosophy of life along with a global question, “Do you ever wish you had not married?” Scores range from 2 to 158, with higher scores indicative of greater marital satisfaction. Scores of 100 or above indicate suggest satisfaction with marriage; scores below 100 indicate dissatisfaction with marriage. Caregivers’ average marital satisfaction score was 101.48 (SD=27.57, range 39–148).

PWD cognitive functioning.

PWDs cognitive functioning was measured using the Mini-Mental Status Exam (MMSE), an 11-item measure that assesses cognitive abilities in multiple domains (e.g., memory, attention, ability to follow verbal commands) [48]. PWDs’ average MMSE score was 25.22 (SD= 4.57, range 3–30), which falls in the mild impairment range.

Results

Data Analysis

Because pronoun scores in each category were computed as the proportion of total scores, they were not statistically independent. Thus, to avoid problems with collinearity, the data were analyzed initially using six (three pronoun categories X two participants) linear regressions with PWD or caregiver pronoun type as the predictor and caregiver well-being as the dependent measure. To isolate the effects of pronoun use on caregiver well-being, we considered three potential covariates (i.e., caregiver sex, caregiver age, PWD cognitive functioning), factors that have consistently been found to predict negative caregiver outcomes [4,10). There were significant differences between female and male caregivers in caregiver well-being, with female caregivers reporting lower well-being than their male counterparts (r(54)= −.289, p=.034; females M=.266, SD=1.09; males M= −.309, SD=.79). Further, PWD cognitive functioning (r(54)= .307, p=.024) was related to caregiver well-being, with lower PWD cognitive functioning associated with lower caregiver well-being. However, caregiver age (r(54)= .243, p=.085) was not significantly related to caregiver well-being. Thus, we used caregiver sex (coded as 1 for males and 2 for females) and PWD cognitive functioning as covariates in all analyses.

To determine whether found associations between pronoun use and caregiver well-being could be attributed to caregivers’ marital satisfaction, a path analysis was conducted using bootstrapping [49]. Unstandardized indirect effects were computed for each of 5,000 bootstrapped samples, and the 95% confidence intervals were computed for the effects of caregiver’s marital satisfaction.

Preliminary analyses

Total words and proportion of pronouns in each category. To assess whether PWDs and their spousal caregivers differed in total words spoken and proportion of pronouns in each category spoken, two-way (caregiver versus PWD X male versus female) analyses of variance (ANOVA) were conducted for total words spoken and each pronoun category. Results indicated that PWDs used significantly fewer total words than caregivers (F(1,108)=38.66, p<.001; PWDs: M= 545.65, SD= 258.66; caregivers: M= 878.98, SD= 260.62). There were no sex differences, as indicated by nonsignificant main effects for sex and nonsignificant interactions between the two factors.

For proportion of pronouns, results revealed that PWDs used a significantly greater proportion of I-pronouns than caregivers (F(1,108)=21.03, p<.001; PWDs: M= .57, SD= .20; caregivers: M= .41, SD= .16), and a significantly smaller proportion of you-pronouns (F(1,108)=25.34, p<.001; PWDs: M= .26, SD= .13; caregivers: M= .42, SD= .19). No differences were found between PWDs and caregivers for proportion of we-pronouns (F(1,108)=.008, p=.930). There were no sex differences, as indicated by nonsignificant main effects for sex and nonsignificant interactions between the two factors.

Pronoun Use and Caregiver Well-being

Pronoun use and caregiver well-being. As shown in Table 2, analyses revealed that after controlling for caregiver sex and PWD cognitive functioning, lower proportions of both PWD we-pronouns (B = 2.84, SE(B) = .71, β= .45, p = .000) and caregiver we-pronouns (B = 2.23, SE(B) = .99, β= .28, p = .036) were associated with lower levels of caregiver well-being. Additionally, higher proportions of PWD I-pronouns (B = −1.78, SE(B) = .62, β= −.35, p = .006) and caregiver you-pronouns (B = −1.50, SE(B) = .70, β= −.28, p = .038) were associated with lower levels of caregiver well-being. There was no relationship between caregiver proportions of I-pronouns and PWD proportions of you-pronouns and caregiver well-being.

Table 2.

Pronoun Type Usage by Patient and Caregiver as Predictors of Caregiver Well-being

Caregiver Well-being

Patient I Patient You Patient We Caregiver I Caregiver You Caregiver We
B (SE[B]) β B (SE[B]) β B (SE[B]) β B (SE[B]) β B (SE[B]) β B (SE[B]) β
Pronoun Type −1.78 (.62) −.35** −.26 (.99) −.03 2.84 (.71) .45*** .08 (.72) .09 −1.50 (.70) − .28* 2.23 (.99) .28*
Caregiver Sex −.52 (.23) −.26* −.52 (.26) −.26 −.58 (.23) −.29* −.48 (.26) −.24 −.43 (.25) −.22 −.48 (.25) −.26*
PWD Cognitive Functioning .23 (.12) .23 .28 (.13) .28* .26 (.11) .26* .24 (.14) .25 .21 (.13) .21 .30 (.12) .30*
*

p <.05,

**

p <.01,

***

p <.001

Thus, our hypothesis that less use of we-ness pronouns would be associated with lower caregiver well-being was fully supported (for both PWD and caregiver we-pronouns). However, our hypothesis that greater use of separateness pronouns would be associated with lower caregiver well-being was only partially supported (supported for PWD I-pronouns and caregiver you-pronouns, but not for caregiver and PWD you-pronouns or caregiver I-pronouns).

Role of Caregiver Marital Satisfaction

For each of the four types of pronouns that were associated with caregiver well-being (i.e., PWD I-pronouns, caregiver you-pronouns, and PWD and caregiver we-pronouns) we conducted a path analysis to determine whether caregiver marital satisfaction explained the association. Results indicated that caregiver marital satisfaction did not explain the associations between pronoun use and caregiver well-being for any of these pronoun categories.

Discussion

The use of pronouns by caregivers and PWDs during discussions about areas of conflict can provide valuable insights into aspects of their relationship that are important for understanding caregiver well-being. The present study found that less use of we-pronouns by both PWDs and caregivers was associated with lower levels of caregiver well-being. These findings supported our hypothesis that less use of we-ness pronouns would be associated with lower caregiver well-being. Partial support was also obtained for our hypothesis that greater use of separateness pronouns would be associated with lower caregiver well-being; this relationship was found for greater use of PWD I-pronouns and caregiver you-pronouns only. Use of you-pronouns by the PWD as well as I-pronouns by the caregiver were not associated with caregiver well-being. Examining the pattern of these latter findings, it appears that greater use of pronouns by the caregiver and the PWD that refer to the PWD (i.e., when PWDs use I-pronouns or caregivers use you-pronouns, they are referencing the PWD) are associated with lower caregiver well-being, but pronouns that refer to the caregiver are not associated with caregiver well-being.

Our finding that less use of we-ness pronouns is associated with lower caregiver well-being is consistent with prior findings. Lower use of we-ness pronouns has been associated with a range of negative outcomes in both healthy couples and in couples coping with addiction and illnesses other than dementia [20, 22, 26, 27, 30, 32]. The use of we-ness pronouns reflects a sense of partnership and togetherness that highlights the salience and importance of the relationship to the partners. In these couples, partners often view themselves as members of a team that can face life’s struggles together and thus reduce the stress and strain on each individual [25]. Low levels of we-ness pronouns suggest low levels of partnership, togetherness, and working together, which is a formula for worse caregiver well-being. This relationship between we-ness and well-being may be strongest in late life when social networks shrink and close familial and spousal relationships become more important [50]. For caregiver-PWD couples, reduced social contacts associated with older age, the demands of caregiving, and the disabilities of PWDs can further increase the caregiver vulnerability to loss of a sense of partnership and togetherness in the caregiver-PWD relationship [51].

Our other finding that use of pronouns that refer to the PWD was associated with lower caregiver well-being may reflect the insidious and progressive nature of dementia and other neurodegenerative diseases. As these diseases progress and the symptoms worsen, the PWD increasingly withdraws from their social world, causing the relationship with the caregiver to recede into the background. This can increase the stress and strain on caregivers, who may feel that their partner in life is disappearing and is being replaced by a person who is increasingly dependent and needy. Greater use of I-pronouns by PWDs may also reflect the increasing self-focus seen in PWDs with some forms of dementia [52]. This self-focus can lead to reduced interactions with and declining interest in other people, including close family members [53]. When PWDs lose interest in others, caregivers may feel an increasing loss of connection and greater isolation [54], which in turn can lead to lower levels of well-being and to significant mental health issues.

Strength, Limitations, and Future Directions

Strengths of this study include assessing interaction quality via an objective measure (i.e., pronoun use by caregivers and PWDs); focusing on the caregiver-PWD relationship; using a semi-naturalistic unrehearsed conversation between spouses about an important relationship issue; including PWDs with a range of neurodegenerative diseases; and using a composite measure of caregiver well-being that encompassed a number of aspects of caregiver functioning. Limitations include a moderate sample size; measuring pronoun use in a single conversation; and measuring pronoun use and caregiver outcomes at a single point in time (which is not optimal for determining temporal relationships among variables or how associations change with disease progression).

Future studies with larger sample sizes, multiple conversations or other samples of pronoun use, additional aspects of language use (e.g., emotion words), additional potential mediators (e.g., pre-disease pronoun use, pre-disease closeness), and longitudinal designs would be extremely useful in exploring how pronouns and other features of language are linked to individual differences in vulnerabilities to the adverse effects of caregiving. Future research should also examine pronoun use during couple interactions in conjunction with other known risk factors for declines in well-being, including PWD behaviors (e.g., disinhibition, aggression) and caregiver characteristics (e.g., ethnicity, income) [3, 4, 10].

Conclusion

We examined pronoun use during semi-naturalistic conversations between PWDs with neurodegenerative diseases and their spousal caregivers. Results indicated that less use of we-pronouns by both PWDs and caregivers and greater use of pronouns that referred to the PWD (PWD I-pronouns and caregiver you-pronouns) were associated with lower levels of caregiver well-being. These results suggest that assessing pronoun use can be a useful way to monitor the nature and quality of the connection between relationship partners, which can contribute significantly to our understanding of the interpersonal aspects of dementia caregiving.

Table 1.

Correlations between Proportion of Pronoun Type Usage, Caregiver Loneliness, Caregiver Well-being, and Covariates

Variable 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
1. Patient I-pronouns - −.590** −.738*** −.233 .575*** −.547*** −.379** −.225 .003 −.268 −.131
2. Patient you-pronouns - - −.109 .212 −.080 −.156 .036 .106 −.088 .151 .168
3. Patient we-pronouns - - - .109 −.641*** .804*** .436** .263 .070 .180 .020
4. Caregiver I-pronouns - - - - −.746*** −.190 .232 .151 −.226 −.051 .358**
5. Caregiver you-pronouns - - - - - −.512** −.370** −.227 .180 −.107 −.253*
6. Caregiver we-pronouns - - - - - - .246* .262 .026 .214 −.089
7. Caregiver Well-being - - - - - - - .243 −.289* .522** .307**
8. Caregiver age - - - - - - - - .053 .080 .466**
9. Caregiver sex - - - - - - - - - −.157 −.116
10. Caregiver Marital Satisfaction −.130
11. Patient cognitive functioning - - - - - - - - - - -

Note:

*

p<.05,

**

p<.01,

**

p<.001

Acknowledgements

The research was supported by National Institute on Aging grants 1R01AG041762-01A1 and 2P01AG019724-11 to Robert W. Levenson, and Bruce L. Miller.

Footnotes

Statement of Ethics

The participants (or their legal guardians) have given their written informed consent. The study protocols have been approved by the Institutional Review Boards of the UCB and the UCSF.

Disclosure Statement

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  • 1.Cohen CA, Colantonio A, Vernich L. Positive aspects of caregiving: rounding out the caregiver experience. International journal of geriatric psychiatry. 2002. February;17(2):184–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Kramer BJ. Gain in the caregiving experience: Where are we? What next?. The Gerontologist. 1997. April 1;37(2):218–32. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Covinsky KE, Newcomer R, Fox P, Wood J, Sands L, Dane K, Yaffe K. Patient and caregiver characteristics associated with depression in caregivers of patients with dementia. Journal of general internal medicine. 2003. December;18(12):1006–14. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Pinquart M, Sörensen S. Differences between caregivers and noncaregivers in psychological health and physical health: A meta-analysis. Psychology and Aging. 2003. June; 18(2): 250. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Brown CL, Lwi SJ, Goodkind MS, Rankin KP, Merrilees J, Miller BL, et al. Empathic accuracy deficits in patients with neurodegenerative disease: association with caregiver depression. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2018. April; 26(4): 484–93 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Chen KH, Wells JL, Otero MC, Lwi SJ, Haase CM, Levenson RW. Greater experience of negative non-target emotions by patients with neurodegenerative diseases is related to lower emotional well-being in caregivers. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2017; 44(5–6): 245–55 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.de Vugt ME, Riedijk SR, Aalten P, Tibben A, van Swieten JC, Verhey FR. Impact of behavioural problems on spousal caregivers: a comparison between Alzheimer’s disease and frontotemporal dementia. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2006; 22(1): 35–41 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Matsumoto N, Ikeda M, Fukuhara R, Shinagawa S, Ishikawa T, Mori T, et al. Caregiver burden associated with behavioral and psychological symptoms of dementia in elderly people in the local community. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2007; 23(4): 219–24 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Otero MC, Levenson RW. Lower visual avoidance in dementia patients is associated with greater psychological distress in caregivers. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2017; 43(5–6): 247–58. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Schulz R, O’Brien AT, Bookwala J, Fleissner K. Psychiatric and physical morbidity effects of dementia caregiving: prevalence, correlates, and causes. The Gerontologist. 1995. December 1;35(6):771–91. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Clipp EC, George LK. Dementia and cancer: a comparison of spouse caregivers. The Gerontologist. 1993. August 1;33(4):534–41. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Ornstein K, Gaugler JE. The problem with “problem behaviors”: a systematic review of the association between individual patient behavioral and psychological symptoms and caregiver depression and burden within the dementia patient–caregiver dyad. International Psychogeriatrics. 2012. October;24(10):1536–52. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Monin JK, Schulz R. Interpersonal effects of suffering in older adult caregiving relationships. Psychology and aging. 2009. September;24(3):681. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Sturm VE, McCarthy ME, Yun I, Madan A, Yuan JW, Holley SR, Ascher EA, Boxer AL, Miller BL, Levenson RW. Mutual gaze in Alzheimer’s disease, frontotemporal and semantic dementia couples. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 2010. June 29;6(3):359–67. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Ascher EA, Smith VE, Seider BH, Nelson KL, Leavitt JD, Levenson RW, Rosen HJ, Miller BL. Emotional language in frontotemporal lobar degeneration In PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGY 2004. January 1 (Vol. 41, pp. S38–S38). 9600 GARSINGTON RD, OXFORD OX4 2DG, OXON, ENGLAND: BLACKWELL PUBLISHING LTD. [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Fauth E, Hess K, Piercy K, Norton M, Corcoran C, Rabins P, Lyketsos C, Tschanz J. Caregivers’ relationship closeness with the person with dementia predicts both positive and negative outcomes for caregivers’ physical health and psychological well-being. Aging & mental health. 2012. August 1;16(6):699–711. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Fitzpatrick KE, Vacha-Haase T. Marital satisfaction and resilience in caregivers of spouses with dementia. Clinical gerontologist. 2010. May 28;33(3):165–80. [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Simonelli C, Tripodi F, Rossi R, Fabrizi A, Lembo D, Cosmi V, Pierleoni L. The influence of caregiver burden on sexual intimacy and marital satisfaction in couples with an Alzheimer spouse. International Journal of Clinical Practice. 2008. January;62(1):47–52. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Beeson RA. Loneliness and depression in spousal caregivers of those with Alzheimer’s disease versus non-caregiving spouses. Archives of psychiatric nursing. 2003. June 1;17(3):135–43. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Seider BH, Hirschberger G, Nelson KL, Levenson RW. We can work it out: age differences in relational pronouns, physiology, and behavior in marital conflict. Psychology and aging. 2009. September;24(3):604. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Pennebaker JW, Mehl MR, Niederhoffer KG. Psychological aspects of natural language use: Our words, our selves. Annual review of psychology. 2003. February;54(1):547–77. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Agnew CR, Van Lange PA, Rusbult CE, Langston CA. Cognitive interdependence: Commitment and the mental representation of close relationships. Journal of personality and social psychology. 1998. April;74(4):939. [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Aron A, Aron EN. Self-expansion motivation and including other in the self.
  • 24.Alea N, Singer JA, Labunko B. “We-ness” in relationship-defining memories and marital satisfaction InCouple Resilience 2015. (pp. 163–177). Springer, Dordrecht. [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Buehlman KT, Gottman JM, Katz LF. How a couple views their past predicts their future: Predicting divorce from an oral history interview. Journal of Family Psychology. 1992. March;5(3–4):295. [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Fitzsimons GM, Kay AC. Language and interpersonal cognition: Causal effects of variations in pronoun usage on perceptions of closeness. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 2004. May;30(5):547–57. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Simmons RA, Gordon PC, Chambless DL. Pronouns in marital interaction: What do “you” and “I” say about marital health?. Psychological science. 2005. December;16(12):932–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Sillars A, Shellen W, McIntosh A, Pomegranate M. Relational characteristics of language: Elaboration and differentiation in marital conversations. Western Journal of Communication (includes Communication Reports). 1997. December 1;61(4):403–22. [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Williams-Baucom KJ, Atkins DC, Sevier MI, Eldridge KA, Christensen A. “You” and “I” need to talk about “us”: Linguistic patterns in marital interactions. Personal Relationships. 2010. March;17(1):41–56. [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Rohrbaugh MJ, Mehl MR, Shoham V, Reilly ES, Ewy GA. Prognostic significance of spouse we talk in couples coping with heart failure. Journal of consulting and clinical psychology. 2008. October;76(5):781. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Rohrbaugh MJ, Shoham V, Skoyen JA, Jensen M, Mehl MR. We-talk, communal coping, and cessation success in a couple-focused intervention for health-compromised smokers. Family process. 2012. March;51(1):107–21. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Robbins ML, Mehl MR, Smith HL, Weihs KL. Linguistic indicators of patient, couple, and family adjustment following breast cancer. Psycho-Oncology. 2013. July;22(7):1501–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Skerrett K Couple dialogues with illness: Expanding the” we”. Families, Systems, & Health. 2003;21(1):69. [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Ascher EA, Sturm VE, Seider BH, Holley SR, Miller BL, Levenson RW. Relationship satisfaction and emotional language in frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer’s disease patients and spousal caregivers. Alzheimer disease and associated disorders. 2010. January;24(1):49. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Piguet O, Hornberger M, Mioshi E, Hodges JR. Behavioural-variant frontotemporal dementia: diagnosis, clinical staging, and management. The Lancet Neurology. 2011. February 1;10(2):162–72. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Rascovsky K, Hodges JR, Kipps CM, Johnson JK, Seeley WW, Mendez MF, Knopman D, Kertesz A, Mesulam M, Salmon DP, Galasko D. Diagnostic criteria for the behavioral variant of frontotemporal dementia (bvFTD): current limitations and future directions. Alzheimer Disease & Associated Disorders. 2007. October 1;21(4):S14–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Rascovsky K, Hodges JR, Knopman D, Mendez MF, Kramer JH, Neuhaus J, Van Swieten JC, Seelaar H, Dopper EG, Onyike CU, Hillis AE. Sensitivity of revised diagnostic criteria for the behavioural variant of frontotemporal dementia. Brain. 2011. August 2;134(9):2456–77. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Dubois B, Feldman HH, Jacova C, DeKosky ST, Barberger-Gateau P, Cummings J, Delacourte A, Galasko D, Gauthier S, Jicha G, Meguro K. Research criteria for the diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease: revising the NINCDS–ADRDA criteria. The Lancet Neurology. 2007. August 1;6(8):734–46. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Levenson RW. Emotion elicitation with neurological patients. Handbook of emotion elicitation and assessment. 2007. April 19:158–68. [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Levenson RW, Carstensen LL, Gottman JM. Influence of age and gender on affect, physiology, and their interrelations: A study of long-term marriages. Journal of personality and social psychology. 1994. July;67(1):56. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Levenson RW, Gottman JM. Marital interaction: physiological linkage and affective exchange. Journal of personality and social psychology. 1983. September;45(3):587. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Levenson RW. Oedipus Text (Computer Software) Berkeley. CA: University of California, Berkeley; 1992. [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Irwin M, Artin KH, Oxman MN. Screening for depression in the older adult: criterion validity of the 10-item Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale (CES-D). Archives of internal medicine. 1999. August 9;159(15):1701–4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Beck AT, Epstein N, Brown G, Steer RA. An inventory for measuring clinical anxiety: psychometric properties. Journal of consulting and clinical psychology. 1988. December;56(6):893. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Robinson BC. Validation of a caregiver strain index. Journal of gerontology. 1983. May 1;38(3):344–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Bédard M, Molloy DW, Squire L, Dubois S, Lever JA, O’Donnell M. The Zarit Burden Interview: a new short version and screening version. The gerontologist. 2001. October 1;41(5):652–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.de Folstein MM. Mini-Mental State Examination.
  • 48.Hayes AF. PROCESS: A versatile computational tool for observed variable mediation, moderation, and conditional process modeling.
  • 49.Carstensen LL, Gottman JM, Levenson RW. Emotional behavior in long-term marriage. Psychology and aging. 1995. March;10(1):140. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Fauth E, Hess K, Piercy K, Norton M, Corcoran C, Rabins P, Lyketsos C, Tschanz J. Caregivers’ relationship closeness with the person with dementia predicts both positive and negative outcomes for caregivers’ physical health and psychological well-being. Aging & mental health. 2012. August 1;16(6):699–711. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Levenson RW, Miller BL. Loss of cells—loss of self: frontotemporal lobar degeneration and human emotion. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2007. December;16(6):289–94. [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Bozeat S, Gregory CA, Ralph MA, Hodges JR. Which neuropsychiatric and behavioural features distinguish frontal and temporal variants of frontotemporal dementia from Alzheimer’s disease?. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry. 2000. August 1;69(2):178–86. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Beeson R, Horton-Deutsch S, Farran C, Neundorfer M. Loneliness and depression in caregivers of persons with Alzheimer’s disease or related disorders. Issues in Mental Health Nursing. 2000. January 1;21(8):779–806. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Beeson RA. Loneliness and depression in caregivers of persons with Alzheimer’s disease versus non-caregiving spouses. Archives of psychiatric nursing. 2003. June 1;17(3):135–43. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES