Skip to main content
Scientific Reports logoLink to Scientific Reports
. 2021 Jan 13;11:860. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-79557-8

Dogs’ insensitivity to scaffolding behaviour in an A-not-B task provides support for the theory of natural pedagogy

Patrick Neilands 1,, Olivia Kingsley-Smith 2, Alex H Taylor 1
PMCID: PMC7807054  PMID: 33441674

Abstract

Executive function plays a critical role in regulating behaviour. Behaviour which directs attention towards the correct solution leads to increased executive function performance in children, but it is unknown how other animals respond to such scaffolding behaviour. Dogs were presented with an A-not-B detour task. After learning to go through gap A to obtain the reward, the barrier was reversed, and the dogs had to inhibit their learned response and enter through gap B on the opposite side. Failure to do so is known as the perseveration error. In test trials, dogs taking part in one of two scaffolding conditions, a pointing condition, where the experimenter pointed to the new gap, and a demonstration condition, where the experimenter demonstrated the new route, were no less likely to commit the perseveration error than dogs in a control condition with no scaffolding behaviour. Dogs’ lack of responsiveness to scaffolding behaviour provides little support for suggestions that simple social learning mechanisms explains scaffolding behaviour in humans. Instead, our results suggest that the theory of natural pedagogy extends to the development of executive function in humans. This suggests that human children’s predisposition to interpret ostensive-communicative cues as informative may be an innate, species-specific adaptation.

Subject terms: Evolution, Animal behaviour, Psychology

Introduction

Patiently waiting your turn, flexibly switching between two different tasks, and being able to learn a novel approach to solving an old problem; all these behaviours rely heavily on executive function, otherwise known as executive control. Executive functions are an important suite of cognitive mechanisms that are central to regulating behaviour and are crucial for problem-solving and task-completion1. Executive function takes many years to develop2 and there can be substantial individual differences in children’s performance in executive function tasks3. In humans, executive functions appear to be closely associated with the prefrontal cortex (PFC) 4. Similarly, the ability to flexibly regulate and control behaviour in other mammals appears to be mediated by the PFC57 and homologous regions of the brain in birds8, suggesting that executive function has deep evolutionary roots.

In humans there has been much interest in what factors may predict better executive control, as strong executive function ability correlates with stronger performance in school9,10, better health outcomes1113, and reductions in anti-social behaviour14 amongst many other benefits1. While early research emphasized the importance of genetic influences on individual differences in executive function15, more recent research has recognized the impact that the socio-cultural environment can have on executive function development, particularly the social interactions between young children and adults such as their parents and teachers16. When interacting with children while they are completing a task or solving a problem, parents and teachers often exhibit a range of scaffolding behaviours. These behaviours, such as directing the child’s attention to salient parts of the task, encouraging them if they are on the right track, and redirecting their attention if they are on the wrong track, are a form of “autonomy support”17,18. Such behaviour enable children to better solve problems by themselves, and include both verbal and non-verbal actions16. For example, when young children have had experience of retrieving an object from location A several times and this object is subsequently removed from location A and placed at location B, children struggle to inhibit their learned response to search at location A, a mistake termed the ‘perseveration error’19. However, in A-not-B tasks where an experimenter points at location B before allowing the participant to search for the object, children are less likely to commit this error20. Such scaffolding appears to play a key role in the development of executive function: the extent to which parents engage in both verbal and non-verbal scaffolding behaviour predicts children’s degree of future executive control and their competency in skills such as maths and language17,18,2126.

While, there is robust evidence that scaffolding improves both children’s executive function and learning outcomes, there is considerable debate regarding the mechanisms underpinning scaffolding behaviour27. Attentional accounts suggest that the main role of scaffolding behaviour is to help children overcome attentional inertia28 and focus on relevant parts of the task. In these accounts, successfully responding to scaffolding only requires a redirection of attention20 and thus even simple social learning mechanisms such as social facilitation or stimulus enhancement29 may be sufficient to respond successfully to scaffolding behaviour. In contrast, natural pedagogy theory argues that human infants are uniquely adapted to interpret ostensive-communication cues as informative from a young age3033. That is, rather than ostensive-communication cues merely eliciting the infants’ attention, infants have an expectation that these cues will provide relevant and generalizable information34,35 that refers to a specific object or location3638.

Dogs are an ideal species to test between the attentional accounts and the natural pedagogy accounts of scaffolding behaviour. Dogs are capable of learning socially via social facilitation39 and stimulus enhancement40, as well as more complex mechanisms such as imitation41,42 and, similarly to humans, appear to be sensitive to ostensive-communication cues43,44. However, despite both dogs and infants being sensitive to such cues, there appears to be stark contrasts in how they interpret these gestures. For infants, such gestures are declarative: a means with which to convey information about the world45,46. Correspondingly, infants use pointing gestures to both share47 and obtain48 information from an early age and prioritize ostensive-communication cues49 to the extent that they will reorganize their own knowledge on the assumption that such cues signal meaningful information50. In contrast, dogs appear to interpret ostensive-communicative cues as a means to either request51 or command52,53 but show little evidence of interpreting such cues as truly informative5255. Instead, dogs appear to view such cues as being lightly imperative52: following these cues in novel contexts but tending to prioritize their own prior experience56,57. Therefore, if scaffolding functions by simply re-directing infant attention, we would predict that scaffolding behaviour should lead to a similar improvement in dogs’ performance in an inhibitory control task due to their sensitivity to human ostensive-communication cues. However, if infants’ response to scaffolding behaviour is built upon a uniquely human expectation that ostensive-communication cues are consistently and meaningfully informative, we would predict that scaffolding should not improve dogs’ performance in an inhibitory control task.

We tested how dogs respond to scaffolding behaviour by presenting them with an executive function problem: the A-not-B barrier task58, a test that has been widely used to test inhibitory control in humans and other animals59. In this task, dogs are presented with a barrier with a gap at one end and food on the other side. After having four trials to learn to go through the gap in order to obtain the food, dogs are presented with four test trials where the barrier is slid across so that the gap is now on the opposite side. In order to retrieve the reward, the dogs must inhibit their learnt response to approach the original gap, and instead approach the new gap. However, both dogs58 and children60 have a tendency to initially move towards the original gap, and so exhibit a perseveration error. In order to investigate whether scaffolding behaviour improves dogs’ performance in an executive function task, we had dogs take part in one of three conditions during the test trials. In the control condition, dog took part in four control trials where they received no scaffolding behaviour. In the pointing condition, dogs took part in four pointing trials where an experimenter dynamically pointed towards the new gap before the dog was released, and finally, in the demonstration condition, dogs took part in four demonstration trials where the experimenter walked through the new gap before the dog was released (see Supplementary video S1 for examples). Both pointing and demonstrating the route are forms of scaffolding behaviour as the experimenter uses ostensive-communication cues to gain the dogs’ attention before directing that attention towards salient parts of the task through their actions. If dogs are sensitive to scaffolding behaviour, we predict that dogs in the control condition would be substantially more likely to show the perseveration error than dogs in either the pointing or demonstration conditions.

Results

Dogs in all three conditions were substantially more likely than chance to commit the perseveration error on the first trial (Control Condition: 9/10 dogs, Bayesian Binomial test: BF = 18.5; Pointing Condition: 9/10 dogs, Bayesian Binomial test: BF = 18.5; Demonstration Condition: 9/10 dogs, Bayesian Binomial test: BF = 18.5; Fig. 1) and whether the experimenter engaged in scaffolding behaviour or not had no effect on how likely the dogs were to initially move toward gap A (Pointing vs Control Bayesian contingency test: BF = 0.329; Demonstration vs Control Bayesian contingency test: BF = 0.329).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Dogs in all conditions were equally likely to commit the perseveration error on the first trial and as likely to learn to avoid committing it over all four trials. Each separate block represents a condition (with 10 dogs in each condition.) Each row represents an individual dog and each column represents an individual trial. 9/10 dogs in each condition showed the perseveration error on the first trial.

Across all four trials (Fig. 1), the 10 dogs in each condition were substantially less likely than chance to commit the perseveration error (Control Condition: 11/40 trials, Bayesian Binomial Test: BF = 0.050; Pointing Condition: 10/40 trials, Bayesian Binomial Test: BF = 0.046; Demonstration Condition: 13/40 trials, Bayesian Binomial Test: BF = 0.061), suggesting that, over the course of the test trials, the dogs learnt to inhibit their initial response to move towards the original gap’s location. Dogs in either the Pointing (Pointing vs Control Bayesian contingency test: BF = 0.299) or Demonstration conditions (Demonstration vs Control Bayesian contingency test: BF = 0.176) were no less likely to commit the perseveration error than dogs in the Control condition. As such, the scaffolding behaviour directed towards the dogs appeared to have no effect on whether they initially showed the perseveration error on the first trial or how likely they were to learn to avoid making the error across all four trials.

Additionally, we ran exploratory analyses to examine if there were any differences between our conditions in how long dogs took to solve the task on their first trial, or if dogs differed in their route towards the barrier, which might indicate they were more hesitant in one condition than another. After controlling for how long dogs took to solve the task in the final learning trial, dogs’ latency to solve the task in the first test trial did not differ from the control condition (mean ± 95% CI: 4.59 ± 2.94 s) in either the pointing condition (2.32 ± 0.76 s; Bayesian independent t-test: BF = 0.812) or the demonstration condition (2.07 ± 0.51 s; Bayesian independent t-test: BF = 1.002). Similarly, in the first test trial, there was no difference in how long it took dogs to change direction after their initial movement towards the barrier in the control condition (2.37 ± 0.97 s) compared to either the pointing condition (1.96 ± 0.55 s; Bayesian independent t-test: BF = 0.480) or demonstration condition (1.67 ± 0.37 s; Bayesian independent t-test: BF = 0.710). Both these findings are consistent with the null hypothesis that scaffolding behaviour has no effect on dogs’ performance in an inhibition task.

Discussion

Our results demonstrate that scaffolding behaviour such as pointing to a gap or demonstrating a route had no effect on how likely dogs were to commit the perseveration error in an A-not-B detour task. Furthermore, scaffolding behaviour did not improve dog’s efficiency at solving the task on the first test trial. These results cannot be explained in terms of a floor effect, where there is no difference between the conditions simply because the task was too difficult for the dogs to solve regardless of human intervention. Firstly, dogs clearly understood the contingencies of the task as, in the learning phase, all subjects were readily approaching the treat through the original gap by the end of this phase. Secondly, in the test trials, all dogs successfully inhibited the learned response to approach the original location of gap A by the final trial. Similarly, a ceiling effect, where there was no difference between conditions because the task was too easy for the dogs, also cannot account for this result. Across all three conditions, dogs were substantially more likely than chance to commit the perseveration error on the first trial, suggesting that inhibiting the initial movement towards the original gap was challenging for the dogs. Furthermore, dogs’ failure to use the actions of the experimenter to improve performance in the pointing and demonstration conditions does not appear to be attributable to a lack of sensitivity to these actions. Past research has shown that dogs are highly sensitive to pointing gestures from humans6163, including when humans point towards locations rather than objects56, and are able to learn from the routes taken by humans to solve detour tasks64,65. Therefore, our results strongly suggest that, despite sensitivity to referential communication from humans43,44, dogs do not respond to scaffolding behaviour by showing better inhibitory control in an A-not-B detour task.

Dog’s lack of sensitivity to an experimenter’s scaffolding behaviour in this current study contrasts sharply with the improvement in children’s performance in an A-not-B task after an experimenter points at the correct location20. This difference in responsiveness may appear surprising, considering that simple social learning mechanisms such as social facilitation or stimulus enhancement seem to be sufficient to explain the effectiveness of scaffolding behaviour. However, our results suggest that responding to scaffolding takes more than simple social learning mechanisms and a sensitivity to ostensive-communicative cues. In humans, this scaffolding behaviour is embedded within a context where infants have an expectation that these cues will provide relevant and generalizable information34,35 that refers to a specific object or location3638. Without that expectation51, it seems dogs’ personal experience of having previously completed the task in the learning trials resulted in the dogs regarding the experimenter’s ostensive-communicative gestures as not relevant, resulting in the dogs not attending to the salient part of the task and thus being less able to inhibit their learned response to approach the original location of the gap.

In sum, while it has been suggested that children’s attentiveness to ostensive-communication cues is merely the by-product of general social learning mechanisms and children’s particular socio-cultural environment6668, our results support the claim made by natural pedagogy theory: children’s responsiveness to ostensive-communication cues is an innate, species-specific adaptation30,31. Furthermore, our findings suggest that the natural pedagogy adaptations of humans appear to not only help with the learning of complex information but also play a role in the development of executive function in young children.

Methods

Ethics statement

The present study was approved by the University of Auckland Animal Ethics Committee R001826 and the University of Auckland Human Ethics Committee R018410. All work with the dogs was in accordance with the guidelines of the New Zealand National Animals Ethics Advisory Committee. Dogs were recruited through owners’ responses to online applications. Written informed consent for participating in this study was obtained from the owners.

Participants

A total of 30 dogs were recruited, with ten dogs in each condition. To determine sample size, we used a Bayesian stopping rule where we collected data until we had at least 10 dogs in each condition and Bayes Factors of > 3 or < 0.333 in our contingency tests. All dogs were pet dogs (aged 2–10 years old) which were accompanied to the lab by their owners (see Supplementary Dataset S1 for details for dogs included in the study).

Experimental set-up

The experiment took place in a 3.6 m × 3.4 m testing room. A 2.9 m barrier was placed across the middle of the room, with a 0.5 m gap between either the left or right end of the barrier (gap A) and the wall. Which side the original gap was on was counterbalanced across dogs (see Fig. 2 for set up of testing room).

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Set up of experimental room. The subject was placed on the starting position opposite the experimenter. In the learning trials, the experimenter would get the dog’s attention, show it the treat, place the treat in a bowl, and place the bowl on the target. After the experimenter gave the release command, the owner would release the dog and the dog would have to enter through the gap in the barrier to reach the treat. The test trials were the same as the learning trials, but the gap was moved to the other side. The experimental procedure was the same except that in the scaffolding conditions, the experimenter would either point to the gap or demonstrate the route before giving the release command.

Protocol

Dogs took part in four initial learning trials. Using the same approach as previous research58, the experimenter entered the room first and positioned themselves in the middle of the room, directly behind the barrier. The dog was then positioned on a mark opposite the experimenter, 1.6 m away from the barrier. After the dog was settled and facing the experimenter, the experimenter would show the dog a treat before placing it in a bowl and putting the bowl on the ground on their side of the barrier. Once the bowl was placed on the ground, the experimenter called the dog’s name and gave the release command “ok, go!”. In order to avoid inadvertently cuing the dog, both the owner and experimenter were asked to stare straight ahead and avoid looking towards the gap. Additionally, both the owner and experimenter were blind to hypotheses. After the dog was released, the trial ended after the dog obtained the reward. Dogs received four learning trials before moving onto the test trials.

Dogs took part in four test trials in one of three conditions. In all conditions, the barrier would be slid across the room so that the gap was now at the opposite end of the barrier (gap B). In all conditions, the dog would be brought back into the testing room and positioned on the mark as before. In the Control condition, the test trials were carried out in the same manner as the learning trials. In the pointing condition, the experimenter would bait the bowl as before but after placing the bowl on the ground, the experimenter would call the dog’s name, look it in the eyes, point to the new gap, and then call the dog’s name again whilst alternating their gaze between the gap and the dog. After calling the dog’s name, the experimenter gave the release cue as in the learning trials and maintained the pointing gesture until the dog had retrieved the reward. In the Demonstration condition, the dog would enter the room and be placed on the mark as before, but the experimenter would be standing, opposite gap B, on the same side of the barrier as the dog. Once the dog was settled, the experimenter would show the dog the treat, call its name and make eye contact with dog twice. Once the dog was attending to the experimenter, the experimenter would walk through the new gap to their original position facing the dog. Afterwards, the experimenter would place the food down and give the release cue as in the leaving trials.

Analysis

The initial movement that the dogs made was coded (either towards the original location of gap A or the new gap B). Bayesian binomial tests were used to determine whether dogs were more likely than chance to commit the perseveration error and move towards the original location of gap A in each condition, and Bayesian contingency tests were used to test whether the number of dogs committing the perservation error differed between the Control condition and the two scaffolding conditions. Bayes Factors > 3 indicate substantial support for the alternative hypothesis, while Bayes Factors < 0.333 indicate substantial support for the null hypothesis. As dogs tend to show a learning effect over the course of detour trials69, we primarily focused on dogs’ performance in the first trial for our analyses. The Bayesian binomials had Beta priors of 1 for both parameter a and parameter b. The Bayesian contingency test was an independent multinomial with rows fixed and had a prior concentration of 1.

Alongside our confirmatory analyses, we also carried out exploratory analyses looking at whether the experimenters’ scaffolding behaviour improved dog’s efficiency at solving the task, regardless of whether they showed the perseveration error or not. As the majority of dogs learned to avoid committing the perseveration error by the second test trial, we focused on the first test trial for these analyses. Firstly, we compared the how quickly it took dogs to solve the first test trial across condition. In order to control for individual differences in motivation, we divided the latency for dogs to complete the first test trial by the latency of dogs to complete the final learning trial. Secondly, we compared how long dogs persisted in moving in the initial direction they chose towards the barrier before changing the direction. Both comparisons were made using Bayesian independent t-tests. The priors for both t-tests were Cauchy distributions centred around a mean effect size of zero. The full, rather than directional Cauchy distribution was used to reflect the exploratory nature of these analyses.

All analyses were carried out in Jasp 0.10 (Jasp Team, 2019).

Supplementary Information

Supplementary Video 1. (52.5MB, mp4)

Acknowledgements

We thank Zoe Tai for help with data collection, and the owners of our participants for volunteering both their time and their dogs. This work was funded by a Rutherford Discovery Fellowship award and a MacDiarmid Emerging Scientist Prime Minister science prize awarded to AHT by the Royal Society of New Zealand and a PBRF grant from the University of Auckland.

Author contributions

P.N. conceptualized the study. P.N. & A.H.T. designed the experiment. P.N. & O.K.S. collected the data and P.N. analysed the data. All authors were involved in writing the manuscript.

Data availability

All data generated and used for analysis in this study are found in Supplementary Dataset S1 in the Supplementary Information Files.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Footnotes

Publisher's note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1038/s41598-020-79557-8.

References

  • 1.Diamond A. Executive functions. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2013;64:135–168. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Diamond, A. The early development of executive functions. In Lifespan Cognition: Mechanisms of Change (eds. Bialystok, E. & Craik, F. I. M.) 70–95 (Oxford University Press, Oxford, 2006). 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195169539.003.0006.
  • 3.Miyake A, Friedman NP. The nature and organization of individual differences in executive functions: Four general conclusions. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2012;21:8–14. doi: 10.1177/0963721411429458. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Zelazo, P. D., Carlson, S. M. & Kesek, A. The development of executive function in childhood. In Handbook of Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience (eds. Nelson, C. A. & Luciana, M.) 553–574 (The MIT Press, Cambridge, 2008). 10.7551/mitpress/7437.001.0001.
  • 5.Miller EK, Cohen JD. An intergrative theory of prefrontal cortext function. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2001;24:167–202. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.167. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Koechlin E, Summerfield C. An information theoretical approach to prefrontal executive function. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2007;11:229–235. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.04.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Domenech P, Koechlin E. Executive control and decision-making in the prefrontal cortex. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 2015;1:101–106. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2014.10.007. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Rose J, Colombo M. Neural correlates of executive control in the avian brain. PLoS Biol. 2005;3:1139–1146. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0030190. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Borella E, Carretti B, Pelegrina S. The specific role of inhibition in reading comprehension in good and poor comprehenders. J. Learn. Disabil. 2010;43:541–552. doi: 10.1177/0022219410371676. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Gathercole SE, Pickering SJ, Knight C, Stegmann Z. Working memory skills and educational attainment: Evidence from national curriculum assessments at 7 and 14 years of age. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 2004;18:1–16. doi: 10.1002/acp.934. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Riggs NR, Spruijt-Metz D, Sakuma KL, Chou CP, Pentz MA. Executive cognitive function and food intake in children. J. Nutr. Educ. Behav. 2010;42:398–403. doi: 10.1016/j.jneb.2009.11.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Miller HV, Barnes JC, Beaver KM. Self-control and health outcomes in a nationally representative sample. Am. J. Health Behav. 2011;35:15–27. doi: 10.5993/AJHB.35.1.2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Will Crescioni A, et al. High trait self-control predicts positive health behaviors and success in weight loss. J. Health Psychol. 2011;16:750–759. doi: 10.1177/1359105310390247. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Denson TF, Pedersen WC, Friese M, Hahm A, Roberts L. Understanding impulsive aggression: Angry rumination and reduced self-control capacity are mechanisms underlying the provocation-aggression relationship. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2011;37:850–862. doi: 10.1177/0146167211401420. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Friedman NP, et al. Individual differences in executive functions are almost entirely genetic in origin. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2008;137:201–225. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.137.2.201. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Fay-Stammbach T, Hawes DJ, Meredith P. Parenting influences on executive function in early childhood: A review. Child Dev. Perspect. 2014;8:258–264. doi: 10.1111/cdep.12095. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Bernier A, Carlson SM, Whipple N. From external regulation to self-regulation: Early parenting precursors. Child Dev. 2010;81:326–339. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01397.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Hammond SI, Müller U, Carpendale JIM, Bibok MB, Liebermann-Finestone DP. The effects of parental scaffolding on preschoolers’ executive function. Dev. Psychol. 2012;48:271–281. doi: 10.1037/a0025519. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Marcovitch S, Zelazo PD. A hierarchical competing systems model of the emergence and early development of executive function. Dev. Sci. 2009;12:1–18. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00754.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Miller SE, Marcovitch S. Toddlers benefit from labeling on an executive function search task. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2011;108:580–592. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2010.10.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Hughes CH, Ensor RA. How do families help or hinder the emergence of early executive function? New Dir. Child Adolesc. Dev. 2009 doi: 10.1002/cd.234. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Conway A, Stifter CA. Longitudinal antecedents of executive function in preschoolers. Child Dev. 2012;83:1022–1036. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01756.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Hopkins J, Lavigne JV, Gouze KR, Lebailly SA, Bryant FB. Multi-domain models of risk factors for depression and anxiety symptoms in preschoolers: Evidence for common and specific factors. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 2013;41:705–722. doi: 10.1007/s10802-013-9723-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Matte-Gagné C, Bernier A. Prospective relations between maternal autonomy support and child executive functioning: Investigating the mediating role of child language ability. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2011;110:611–625. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2011.06.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Valcan DS, Davis H, Pino-Pasternak D. Parental behaviours predicting early childhood executive functions: A meta-analysis. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 2018;30:607–649. doi: 10.1007/s10648-017-9411-9. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Roskam I, Stievenart M, Meunier JC, Noël MP. The development of children’s inhibition: Does parenting matter? J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2014;122:166–182. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2014.01.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Mermelshtine R. Parent–child learning interactions: A review of the literature on scaffolding. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 2017;87:241–254. doi: 10.1111/bjep.12147. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Kirkham NZ, Cruess L, Diamond A. Helping children apply their knowledge to their behavior on a dimension-switching task. Dev. Psychol. 2003;6:449–467. [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Galef BG. Social learning and traditions in animals: Evidence, definitions, and relationship to human culture. Wiley Interdiscip. Rev. Cogn. Sci. 2012;3:581–592. doi: 10.1002/wcs.1196. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Csibra G, Gergely G. Natural pedagogy. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2009;13:148–153. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.01.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Csibra G, Gergely G. Natural pedagogy as evolutionary adaptation. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 2011;366:1149–1157. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2010.0319. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Király I, Csibra G, Gergely G. Beyond rational imitation: Learning arbitrary means actions from communicative demonstrations. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2013;116:471–486. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2012.12.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Csibra G. Recognizing communicative intentions in infancy. Mind Lang. 2010;25:141–168. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-0017.2009.01384.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Gergely G, Egyed K, Király I. On pedagogy. Dev. Sci. 2007;10:139–146. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00576.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Yoon JMD, Johnson MH, Csibra G. Communication-induced memory biases in preverbal infants. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 2008;105:13690–13695. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0804388105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Csibra G, Volein Á. Infants can infer the presence of hidden objects from referential gaze information. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 2008;26:1–11. doi: 10.1348/026151007X185987. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Moll H, Tomasello M. 12- and 18-Month-old infants follow gaze to spaces behind barriers. Dev. Sci. 2004;7:1–9. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2004.00315.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Gliga T, Csibra G. One-year-old infants appreciate the referential nature of deictic gestures and words. Psychol. Sci. 2009;20:347–353. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02295.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Christiansen FO, Bakken M, Braastad BO. Social facilitation of predatory, sheep-chasing behaviour in Norwegian Elkhounds, grey. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2001;72:105–114. doi: 10.1016/S0168-1591(00)00208-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Kubinyi E, Miklósi Á, Topál J, Csányi V. Dogs (Canis familiaris) learn from their owners via observation in a manipulation task. J. Comp. Psychol. 2003;117:156–165. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.117.2.156. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Miller HC, Rayburn-Reeves R, Zentall TR. Imitation and emulation by dogs using a bidirectional control procedure. Behav. Processes. 2009;80:109–114. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2008.09.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Topál J, Byrne RW, Miklósi Á, Csányi V. Reproducing human actions and action sequences: ‘Do as I do!’ in a dog. Anim. Cogn. 2006;9:355–367. doi: 10.1007/s10071-006-0051-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Kaminski J, Schulz L, Tomasello M. How dogs know when communication is intended for them. Dev. Sci. 2012;15:222–232. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01120.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Téglás E, Gergely A, Kupán K, Miklósi Á, Topál J. Dogs’ gaze following is tuned to human communicative signals. Curr. Biol. 2012;22:209–212. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.12.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Tomasello M, Carpenter M, Call J, Behne T, Moll H. Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition. Behav. Brain Sci. 2005;28:675–691. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X05000129. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Tomasello M, Carpenter M. Shared intentionality. Dev. Sci. 2007;10:121–125. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00573.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Liszkowski U, Carpenter M, Striano T, Tomasello M. 12- and 18-month-olds point to provide information for others. J. Cogn. Dev. 2006;7:173–187. doi: 10.1207/s15327647jcd0702_2. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Kovács ÁM, Tauzin T, Téglás E, Gergely G, Csibra G. Pointing as epistemic request: 12-month-olds point to receive new information. Infancy. 2014;19:543–557. doi: 10.1111/infa.12060. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Topal J, Gergely G, Miklosi A, Erdohegyi A, Csibra G. Infants’ perseverative search. Science (80-). 2008;321:1831–1834. doi: 10.1126/science.1161437. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Butler LP, Markman EM. Preschoolers use pedagogical cues to guide radical reorganization of category knowledge. Cognition. 2014;130:116–127. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2013.10.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Kaminski J, Neumann M, Bräuer J, Call J, Tomasello M. Dogs, Canis familiaris, communicate with humans to request but not to inform. Anim. Behav. 2011;82:651–658. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.06.015. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Scheider L, Kaminski J, Call J, Tomasello M. Do domestic dogs interpret pointing as a command? Anim. Cogn. 2013;16:361–372. doi: 10.1007/s10071-012-0577-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Sümegi Z, Kis A, Miklósi Á, Topál J. Why do adult dogs (Canis familiaris) commit the A-not-B search error? J. Comp. Psychol. 2014;128:21–30. doi: 10.1037/a0033084. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Marshall-Pescini, S., Passalacqua, C., Miletto Petrazzini, M. E., Valsecchi, P. & Prato-Previde, E. Do dogs (Canis lupus familiaris) make counterproductive choices because they are sensitive to human ostensive cues? PLoS One7(4), e35437 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 55.Tauzin T, Csík A, Kis A, Topál J. What or where? The meaning of referential human pointing for dogs (Canis familiaris) J. Comp. Psychol. 2015;129:334–338. doi: 10.1037/a0039462. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Scheider, L., Grassmann, S., Kaminski, J. & Tomasello, M. Domestic dogs use contextual information and tone of voice when following a human pointing gesture. PLoS One6(7), e21676 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 57.Pongrácz P, Miklósi Á, Kubinyi E, Topál J, Csányi V. Interaction between individual experience and social learning in dogs. Anim. Behav. 2003;65:595–603. doi: 10.1006/anbe.2003.2079. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Osthaus B, Marlow D, Ducat P. Minding the gap: Spatial perseveration error in dogs. Anim. Cogn. 2010;13:881–885. doi: 10.1007/s10071-010-0331-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Osthaus, B. A-Not-B Problem. In Encyclopedia of Animal Cognition and Behaviour (eds. Vonk, J. & Shackleford, T.) (Springer, New York, 2017). 10.1007/978-3-319-47829-6.
  • 60.McKenzie BE, Bigelow E. Detour behaviour in young human infants. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 1986;4:139–148. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-835X.1986.tb01005.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Miklósi Á, Soproni K. A comparative analysis of animals’ understanding of the human pointing gesture. Anim. Cogn. 2006;9:81–93. doi: 10.1007/s10071-005-0008-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Soproni K, Miklósi Á, Topál J, Csányi V. Dogs’ (Canis familiaris) responsiveness to human pointing gestures. J. Comp. Psychol. 2002;116:27–34. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.116.1.27. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Kirchhofer KC, Zimmermann F, Kaminski J, Tomasello M. Dogs (Canis familiaris), but not chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), understand imperative pointing. PLoS ONE. 2012;7:e30913. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0030913. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Pongracz P, et al. Social learning in dogs: The effect of a human demonstrator on the performance of dogs in a detour task. Anim. Behav. 2001;62:1109–1117. doi: 10.1006/anbe.2001.1866. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Pongrácz P, Miklósi Á, Vida V, Csányi V. The pet dogs ability for learning from a human demonstrator in a detour task is independent from the breed and age. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2005;90:309–323. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2004.08.004. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Heyes C. Born pupils? Natural pedagogy and cultural pedagogy. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2016;11:280–295. doi: 10.1177/1745691615621276. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Gredebäck G, Astor K, Fawcett C. Gaze following is not dependent on ostensive cues: A critical test of natural pedagogy. Child Dev. 2018;89:2091–2098. doi: 10.1111/cdev.13026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Skerry AE, Lambert E, Powell LJ, Mcauliffe K. The origins of pedagogy: Developmental and evolutionary perspectives. Evol. Psychol. 2013;11:550–572. doi: 10.1177/147470491301100306. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Kabadayi C, Bobrowicz K, Osvath M. The detour paradigm in animal cognition. Anim. Cogn. 2018;21:21–35. doi: 10.1007/s10071-017-1152-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary Video 1. (52.5MB, mp4)

Data Availability Statement

All data generated and used for analysis in this study are found in Supplementary Dataset S1 in the Supplementary Information Files.


Articles from Scientific Reports are provided here courtesy of Nature Publishing Group

RESOURCES