Skip to main content
Brain Communications logoLink to Brain Communications
. 2021 Jan 7;3(1):fcaa190. doi: 10.1093/braincomms/fcaa190

Αlpha 5 subunit-containing GABAA receptors in temporal lobe epilepsy with normal MRI

Colm J McGinnity 1,2,3,#, Daniela A Riaño Barros 1,2,#, Rainer Hinz 4, James F Myers 1, Siti N Yaakub 3, Charlotte Thyssen 5, Rolf A Heckemann 6, Jane de Tisi 7, John S Duncan 7, Josemir W Sander 7,8, Anne Lingford-Hughes 9, Matthias J Koepp 7, Alexander Hammers 1,2,3,10,
PMCID: PMC7811756  PMID: 33501420

Abstract

GABAA receptors containing the α5 subunit mediate tonic inhibition and are widely expressed in the limbic system. In animals, activation of α5-containing receptors impairs hippocampus-dependent memory. Temporal lobe epilepsy is associated with memory impairments related to neuron loss and other changes. The less selective PET ligand [11C]flumazenil has revealed reductions in GABAA receptors. The hypothesis that α5 subunit receptor alterations are present in temporal lobe epilepsy and could contribute to impaired memory is untested. We compared α5 subunit availability between individuals with temporal lobe epilepsy and normal structural MRI (‘MRI-negative’) and healthy controls, and interrogated the relationship between α5 subunit availability and episodic memory performance, in a cross-sectional study. Twenty-three healthy male controls (median ± interquartile age 49 ± 13 years) and 11 individuals with MRI-negative temporal lobe epilepsy (seven males; 40 ± 8) had a 90-min PET scan after bolus injection of [11C]Ro15-4513, with arterial blood sampling and metabolite correction. All those with epilepsy and six controls completed the Adult Memory and Information Processing Battery on the scanning day. ‘Bandpass’ exponential spectral analyses were used to calculate volumes of distribution separately for the fast component [VF; dominated by signal from α1 (α2, α3)-containing receptors] and the slow component (VS; dominated by signal from α5-containing receptors). We made voxel-by-voxel comparisons between: the epilepsy and control groups; each individual case versus the controls. We obtained parametric maps of VF and VS measures from a single bolus injection of [11C]Ro15-4513. The epilepsy group had higher VS in anterior medial and lateral aspects of the temporal lobes, the anterior cingulate gyri, the presumed area tempestas (piriform cortex) and the insulae, in addition to increases of ∼24% and ∼26% in the ipsilateral and contralateral hippocampal areas (P < 0.004). This was associated with reduced VF:VS ratios within the same areas (P < 0.009). Comparisons of VS for each individual with epilepsy versus controls did not consistently lateralize the epileptogenic lobe. Memory scores were significantly lower in the epilepsy group than in controls (mean ± standard deviation −0.4 ± 1.0 versus 0.7 ± 0.3; P = 0.02). In individuals with epilepsy, hippocampal VS did not correlate with memory performance on the Adult Memory and Information Processing Battery. They had reduced VF in the hippocampal area, which was significant ipsilaterally (P = 0.03), as expected from [11C]flumazenil studies. We found increased tonic inhibitory neurotransmission in our cohort of MRI-negative temporal lobe epilepsy who also had co-morbid memory impairments. Our findings are consistent with a subunit shift from α1/2/3 to α5 in MRI-negative temporal lobe epilepsy.

Keywords: GABAA, α5, memory, [11C]Ro15-4513, PET


GABAA receptors containing the α5 subunit (α5) mediate tonic inhibition and impair memory. McGinnity et al. imaged α5 and compared individuals with TLE, but normal structural imaging, and healthy controls. Patients had reduced memory performance and bilaterally higher temporal lobe α5, but lower α1/2/3 in the ipsilateral hippocampus.

Graphical Abstract

Graphical Abstract.

Graphical Abstract

Introduction

Gamma-aminobutyric acid (GABA) is the main inhibitory neurotransmitter in the brain (Curtis et al., 1970) and mediates neurotransmission at 25–50% of central nervous system synapses. GABAA receptors are ligand-gated chloride ion channels mediating phasic (synaptic) inhibitory neurotransmission and tonic (extrasynaptic) neurotransmission. The pentameric GABAA receptor is assembled from 5 of 19 known protein subunit types α1–6, β1–3, δ, ε, γ1–3, π, ρ1-3 and θ (Barnard et al., 1998). These receptor subunits exhibit distinct but overlapping distributions within the brain (Sieghart and Sperk, 2002), confer the pharmacological properties of the receptor, and have roles that change during development and with pathologies (Galanopoulou, 2008).

Approximately 5% of GABAA receptors contain the α5 subunit (review: Jacob, 2019); they are predominantly, but not solely, extrasynaptic in localization (Brunig et al., 2002; Brady and Jacob, 2015) and mediate tonic inhibitory currents (Caraiscos et al., 2004).

Experiments in animals suggest that activation of receptors containing this subunit impairs hippocampus-dependent learning and memory: inverse agonism of the subunit has a positive effect on spatial learning (Chambers et al., 2004; Sternfeld et al., 2004; Dawson et al., 2006) and antagonism enhances object recognition memory (Ling et al., 2015; Gacsalyi et al., 2017; 2018). Similar to the pharmacological studies, knock-out studies have shown that in α5 null mutant mice, associative learning (Yee et al., 2004), spatial learning (Collinson et al., 2002) and trace fear conditioning (Crestani et al., 2002) are enhanced; conversely the amnestic effect of the anaesthetic etomidate on spatial and non-spatial learning is reduced (Cheng et al., 2006). Data in humans are scarce; however, the amnestic effect of alcohol on word list learning is reduced by pre-treatment with an inverse α5 agonist (Nutt et al., 2007). This suggests that α5 subunit availability is likely to adversely affect learning and memory, possibly by regulation of the threshold required for long-term potentiation (Martin et al., 2010; Pofantis and Papatheodoropoulos, 2014).

The hippocampus and neighbouring medial temporal lobe structures are crucial to episodic memory function (Eichenbaum et al., 2012). Focal epilepsies and in particular temporal lobe epilepsy (TLE) are strongly associated with verbal and visual memory disturbances, which were present in over half of a large cohort of individuals with focal epilepsy in Hoppe et al. (2007). In view of these learning and memory studies, one might hypothesize increased α5 subunit availability in individuals with TLE, either causally or as an epiphenomenon. Data on the expression of the α5 subunit in brain tissue resected from individuals with TLE is lacking. Alpha 5 subunit availability was, however, found to be decreased in the hippocampus proper (cornu ammonis CA1–CA4; Lorente de Nó, 1934) during the chronic state, via in situ hybridization and immunohistochemistry, in rodent chemoconvulsant models using kainic acid (Schwarzer et al., 1997; Tsunashima et al., 1997; Sperk et al., 1998) or pilocarpine (Houser and Esclapez, 1996; Brooks-Kayal et al., 1998; Fritschy et al., 1999; Houser and Esclapez, 2003; Scimemi et al., 2005; Bethmann et al., 2008), as well as in amygdala kindling (Bethmann et al., 2008) and hippocampal electrically induced status epilepticus models (Nishimura et al., 2005). The decreases occur in association with neuronal loss and also in its absence (Rice et al., 1996; Houser and Esclapez, 2003). Increased α5 subunit availability has been reported in the molecular and, less consistently, granule cell layers of the dentate gyrus in kainic acid (Schwarzer et al., 1997; Bouilleret et al., 2000) and pilocarpine models (Rice et al., 1996; Fritschy et al., 1999; Houser and Esclapez, 2003).

Previous human studies have used the PET radioligand [11C]flumazenil ([11C]FMZ, [11C]Ro15-1788), which is selective for the GABAA receptor subunits α1–3 and α5 (Maziere et al., 1984). [11C]FMZ total volume of distribution (VT) was reduced in the hippocampi and other temporal lobe regions of individuals with refractory TLE and normal MRI, i.e. without structural correlate of memory impairment (Henry et al., 1993; Savic et al., 1993; Savic and Thorell, 1996; Szelies et al., 1996; Ryvlin et al., 1998; Koepp et al., 2000; Lamusuo et al., 2000; Szelies et al., 2000; Hammers et al., 2002). [11C]FMZ binding was positively correlated with interictal interval (i.e. latency since last seizure; Bouvard et al., 2005) and negatively correlated with seizure frequency (Savic et al., 1996; Laufs et al., 2011). In individuals who had paired [11C]FMZ scans 1 week apart, the binding was lowest for the scan that was associated with the shorter interictal interval (Bouvard et al., 2005). Binding of this radioligand is, however, more indicative of the expression of α1 rather than α5 subunits given the former’s 10-fold higher relative concentration (Sieghart and Sperk, 2002).

The imidazobenzodiazepine Ro15-4513 (ethyl-8-azido-5,6-dihydro-5-methyl-6-oxo-4H-imidazo-1,4-benzodiazepine-3-carboxylate; F. Hoffmann-La Roche AG, Basel, Switzerland) behaves as a (competitive) α5 subunit inverse agonist (Bonetti et al., 1988) at pharmacological doses; it can lower the threshold for seizures at high doses (Lister and Nutt, 1988). Receptors that express α5 subunits have 10–15 times higher affinity to Ro15-4513 than those that do not (Luddens et al., 1994). Competition studies in the rat in vivo showed that radiolabelled Ro15-4513 uptake was reduced to non-specific levels only by drugs that have affinity for the α5 subtype (flunitrazepam, RY80, Ro15-4513, L655,708), but not by the α1 selective agonist zolpidem (Lingford-Hughes et al., 2002). In healthy human volunteers, pre-scan blocking of α1 by administration of zolpidem in healthy human volunteers did not significantly decrease [11C]Ro15-4513 VT (Myers et al., 2012).

The hippocampus is the structure with the highest concentration of α5 subunit-containing receptors in the human brain (Sur et al., 1998); expression is also high throughout the other parts of the limbic system. An ex vivo study with the α5 subunit-selective radioligand [3H]L-655,708 suggested the presence of α5 subunits in ∼28% of GABAA receptors in the human hippocampus (Sur et al., 1998); they appeared especially concentrated in CA 1 and 3 (Pirker et al., 2000). [11C]Ro15-4513 PET offers a unique means of investigating, in vivo, GABAA receptors that contain the α5 subunit in particular, unlike [11C]FMZ PET and [123I]iomazenil SPECT, which are mainly indicative of the distribution of α1 subtype. [11C]Ro15-4513 quantified with several methods including spectral analysis has excellent test–retest reliability (McGinnity et al., 2017).

We used [11C]Ro15-4513 to test

  1. whether there are changes in GABAA α5 subunit availability in individuals with TLE but unremarkable MRI,

  2. whether these changes lateralize TLE and

  3. whether increased α5 subunit availability is inversely related to memory performance in tests known to involve the hippocampus.

Materials and methods

Participants

The primary inclusion criterion for the epilepsy group was TLE with normal MRI results. Eleven such individuals (median ± interquartile range age 40 ± 7.5 years, 7 males, 9 right-handed, interictal interval 6 ± 25 days, all taking anti-epileptic mediation) were included (Table 1); they had been recruited from outpatient epilepsy clinics at the National Hospital for Neurology and Neurosurgery (Queen Square, London) and the Chalfont Centre for Epilepsy (Chalfont St Peter). For this study, six healthy male controls were recruited via local advertising, of whom five completed test–retest [11C]Ro15-4513 PET (McGinnity et al., 2017). Imaging data were available for another 17 healthy male controls from other studies (Stokes et al., 2014) for a total of 23 subjects (49.0 ± 13.0 years). The sample size was decided based on preliminary analyses which predicted a power of 92% to detect a 10% difference in hippocampal total VT with an α of 0.05 for one individual with TLE against 10 controls. We defined the minimum difference of interest as 10% based on our experience with other radiotracers, reasoning that lesser differences would not be ‘meaningful’ or indeed appreciable on visual analyses (which would be important if the radiotracer is to have clinical utility). We used the hippocampus as the region of interest in our sample size calculation because of its importance in TLE and in episodic memory, as described in the Introduction section.

Table 1.

Individuals with MRI-negative TLE and healthy controls—clinical and demographic details

ID Age/sex/ handedness Semiology Interictal EEG ± PET ± EEG-fMRI Ictal EEG Consensus TLE lateralization Onset/ duration (years) Interictal interval (days)/seizure frequency (per month) Antiepileptic drugs Summary AMIPB score Hippocampal volume Left/right (mm3) Global total VT
LTLE1 39/M/L FA aura (déjà vu); FIA with orofacial automatisms (swallowing); manual automatisms (scratching nose); anomia and neologism L T (scalp) Unclear (scalp) Left 19/20 2/1.5 VAL, ZON 0.70 1672/1840 3.24
LTLE2 40/M/R FA aura (déjà vu, epigastric); FIA with orofacial automatisms (lip smacking, facial grimacing); left and separately right bimanual automatisms (fidgeting); leg fidgeting; GM Anterior T L > R (scalp); L T ([18F]FDG PET); EEG-fMRI bi-T L T and separately R T (scalp) Left 26/14 16/9 LEV, VAL −2.02 2207/2472 2.98
LTLE3 40/M/R FIA with left manual automatisms (fidgeting) and right arm posturing; speech arrest then dysarthria; facial grimacing, short-term memory deficit; GM L T and R FT (scalp) L T (scalp) Left 18/22 2/15.5 PGB, VAL −1.54 2262/2351 2.84
LTLE4 48/F/L FA aura (‘sickness’); FIA with dysarthria and dysphasia; GM L T and generalized (scalp); normal ([11C]flumazenil PET) L T (scalp); widespread L TP (intracranial) Left 17/31 115/0.2 CBZ, LEV 0.45 1348/1485 2.40
LTLE5 64/M/R FIA with speech arrest, dysarthria, orofacial automatisms (swallowing), right arm movements, confusion L mid-T (scalp) L T (scalp) Left 21/43 6/6.5 CBZ −1.60 1817/2213 3.16
LTLE6 40/M/R FIA with agrammatical speaking; manual automatisms; orofacial automatisms (lip smacking, tongue protrusion); GM L T (scalp ); L T ([18F]FDG PET) R T (scalp) Left 37/3 30/1 CBZ, LEV 0.11 2059/2012 3.23
RTLE1 46/F/R

FA aura (déjà vu); FIA;

 

GM

T R > L (scalp) R (scalp) Right 15/31 2/3 LTG, VAL 0.02 2270/2288 2.88
RTLE2 32/F/R FA aura (déjà vu); FIA with orofacial automatisms (swallowing and lip smacking); bimanual automatisms L > R mid-T, post-T, sphenoidal (scalp); R T ([18F]FDG PET) R ant-T and sphenoidal (scalp) Right 21/11 5/6.5 CBZ, VAL 0.29 2187/1998 3.36
RTLE3 42/M/R FA aura (nausea); FIA with short-term memory deficits; GM R T (scalp interictal) Right 36/6 304/0.2 LEV 0.25 1926/2037 2.62
RTLE4 39/M/R FA aura (epigastric); FIA with bimanual (L > R) automatisms (jaw rubbing); orofacial automatisms; versive head turn to left; tonic left arm posturing R mid T (scalp); R T ([18F]FDG PET) R T (scalp) Right 27/12 1/4.5 CBZ, intermittent CLB, LAC, OXC, VAL 1.00 2010/2353 2.98
RTLE5 54/M/R FIA with orofacial automatisms (lip smacking); left manual automatisms; right arm posturing; repetitive inappropriate words; shaking limbs or walking; L T (scalp); normal ([18F]FDG PET) R T and separately L T (scalp); L (scalp) Right 22/32 24/2.5 PHB, VAL −1.27 1838/1885 2.72
Median ± iqr/total

40.0 ± 7.5/

 

7 M, 4 F

2 FIA only;

 

3 FA and FIA;

 

2 FIA and GM;

 

4 FIA, FA and GM

6 Left,

 

5 Right

21.0 ± 6.5/

 

20.0 ± 19.5

6.0 ± 25.0/

 

3.0 ± 5.30

5 CBZ, 1 CLB, 1 LAC, 4 LEV, 1 LTG, 1 OXC, 1 PBG, 1 PHB, 7 VAL, 1 ZON 0.11 ±. 1.78

2010 ± 370/

 

2037 ± 378

2.98 ± 0.42
Healthy controls

49 ± 13/

 

23 M, 0 F

-- -- -- -- --

0.65 ± 0.85

 

(n = 6)

1657 ± 481/

 

1940 ± 484

2.86 ± 0.37
Mann– Whitney P 0.36 -- 0.06 0.003/0.03 0.31

AMIPB, Adult Memory and Information Processing Battery; CBZ, carbamazepine; CLB, clobazam, last taken ∼3.5 days prior to scan; EEG, electroencephalography; F, female; FIA, focal seizures with impaired awareness; FA, focal seizures with awareness; fMRI, functional MRI; FT, fronto-temporal; GM, generalized motor seizures; ID, identifying code; iqr, interquartile range; L, left; LAC, lacosamide; LEV, levetiracetam; LTG, lamotrigine; M, male; mm, millimetres; OXC, oxcarbazepine; PHB, phenobarbitone; PGB, pregabalin; R, right; T, Temporal; TP, temporoparietal; VAL, valproate; VT, volume of distribution

The diagnosis of TLE was based on history and seizure semiology, as well as prolonged and repeated interictal and ictal electroencephalography recordings (where available). The semiology that we deemed suggestive of TLE included déjà vu, epigastric sensation, impairment of awareness, orofacial automatisms, manual automatisms and arm posturing. Ictal electroencephalography data were available for 10 of the 11 individuals who were subsequently enrolled in the study. Exclusion criteria for both groups were history of substance abuse, inability to provide informed consent, suffering any contraindication for undergoing PET or MRI, positive screening result for illicit drugs (see below for details), general practitioner’s (family doctor’s) advice against participation and pathological modified Allen’s test for patency of the ulnar artery (Allen, 1929). Additional exclusion criteria for controls were history of either psychiatric or systemic medical condition, or regular medication(s). Participants underwent a urine drug cassette test for 11-nor-Δ9-11-nor-Δ9-tetrahydrocannabinol, morphine, amphetamine, benzoylecgonine (the main metabolite of cocaine), methamphetamine and oxazepam (Monitect©; BMC, CA, USA) before PET scanning. One individual with TLE (RTLE4) tested positive for oxazepam as he had been prescribed clobazam 10 mg nocte four times a week, which had last been taken 3.5 days prior to the scan. The remaining participants had negative tests. Five of the healthy controls were scanned twice for test–retest analyses (McGinnity et al., 2017; only the first scan was used for the analyses described in this study); all other participants were scanned only once.

Ethical considerations

The London—Riverside Health Research Ethics Committee granted ethical approval for this cross-sectional PET study (08/H0706/30). The UK’s Administration of Radioactive Substances Advisory Committee granted permission for the tracer use. All participants provided written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.

Radiochemistry

[11C]Ro15-4513 was produced on site by Hammersmith Imanet as described previously (Myers et al., 2012).

Dynamic PET and memory score data acquisition

PET scans were acquired at Hammersmith Imanet on a Siemens/CTI ECAT EXACT HR+ 962 camera (Knoxville, TN, USA; Adam et al., 1997; Brix et al., 1997) in 3D mode, as described previously (McGinnity et al., 2017). Each participant had a 10-min transmission scan for attenuation correction, followed by a 90-min dynamic emission scan consisting of 24 frames of increasing length. [11C]Ro15-4513 was injected as an intravenous bolus of median 450 MBq (interquartile range 45.1 MBq) 30 s after the dynamic emission scan start. The individuals with TLE were closely observed for evidence of seizures throughout the scan (none detected). Frames were reconstructed using Fourier rebinning (FORE; Defrise et al., 1997) and 2D filtered backprojection (ramp filter, kernel 2.0 mm full-width at half-maximum) into 63 transaxial images.

Neuropsychological assessment

All 11 individuals with TLE and six controls completed the Adult Memory and Information Processing Battery (AMIPB; Coughlan and Hollows, 1985), which consists of verbal and non-verbal subtests, on the day of scanning. The battery, which is used clinically in the epilepsy surgery programme at the National Hospital for Neurology and Neurosurgery (UK), has been shown to distinguish groups with left TLE, right TLE and healthy controls (Bonelli et al., 2010). The assessments were performed in a quiet room by a clinician who had been trained in use of the AMIPB (authors A.H. and D.R.B.), in accordance with the procedure described in Baxendale (2010).

For the verbal list-learning task, each participant was read a list of 15 common words, some of which are semantically related and after each presentation they were asked to recall aloud as many words as possible. Immediately after the fifth trial, the participant was read a second list of 15 ‘distractor’ words, and again asked to recall aloud as many of these as possible. Immediately following this, the participant was then asked to recall aloud as many of the original 15 words (that were read five times) as possible, without further repetition (reading) by the clinician. We report as ‘List Learning’ the total number of words correctly recalled over the five trials, with a maximum score of 75 (5 × 15). We report as ‘List Recall’ the number of the original 15 words correctly recalled without repetition, out of a maximum of 15.

For the design learning task, each participant was asked to study an abstract line drawing consisting of nine distinct features presented on a grid, for 10 s. The participant was then asked to draw the same design on a blank grid. Similar to the verbal list-learning task, this procedure was repeated for further four trials. Immediately after the fifth trial, the participant was asked to study a new distractor design, and then asked to draw it on a blank grid. Immediately following this, the participant was then asked to draw the original design (that had been studied five times), without further repetition (presentation) by the clinician. We report as ‘Figure Learning’ the total number of distinct features correctly drawn over the five trials, with a maximum score of 45 (5 × 9). We report as ‘Figure Recall’ the number of the original distinct features correctly drawn without repetition, out of a maximum of 9.

Input function derivation

Continuous and intermittent discrete blood samples were collected to allow the subsequent generation of arterial parent plasma input functions as described previously. In brief, during the first 15 min, blood radioactivity was continuously monitored in a bismuth germanate detection system (Ranicar et al., 1991). Intermittent discrete (10 ml) samples were taken before the scan (baseline) and at 10 time points after the scan start. These discrete samples were used to quantify plasma and whole-blood radioactivity, as well as to quantify the parent fraction of the radioligand via high-performance liquid chromatography. The plasma-over-blood ratio model and the metabolite model were fitted and also applied to the whole blood measurements between 0 and 15 min. Continuous parent plasma input functions were then derived as described previously(Hinz et al., 2007; Hammers et al., 2007b; Riaño Barros et al., 2014; McGinnity et al., 2017).

MRI data acquisition and analysis

All participants had 3D T1-weighted MRI scans with approximately millimetric isotropic voxel sizes for co-registration and region definition. There was no gross cerebral abnormality on any of the T1-weighted images. The hippocampal volumes were calculated for each participant using multi-atlas propagation with enhanced registration (Heckemann et al., 2010).

PET data quantification

The dynamic PET images were de-noised and corrected for frame-by-frame movement using wavelets (Turkheimer et al., 1999) as described previously (Hammers et al., 2007b; McGinnity et al., 2017).

Summation images (also known as ‘add’ or ‘static’ images) were created for the realigned frames 1–24, using Modelling, Input functions and Compartmental Kinetics—Parametric Maps version 5.4 software (in-house MATLAB-based software available on request from the developer and co-author Rainer Hinz) for use as the reference image during co-registration of the T1-weighted MRI data.

Quantification of VT was performed on a voxel-by-voxel basis using Modelling, Input functions and Compartmental Kinetics—Parametric Maps to generate parametric images with spectral analysis (Cunningham and Jones, 1993) modified as ‘bandpass’ spectral analysis (Myers et al., 2017) to allow separation of distinct, subunit-tailored tracer kinetics. First, a binary mask of the brain was created by omitting the lowest 5% of signal from the corresponding summation images, to spatially constrain the subsequent spectral analyses to voxels with sufficient signal. The slow component volume of distribution [VS; mostly reflective of binding to α5 subunit-containing receptors (Myers et al., 2017)] was calculated using a frequency interval of 0.00063–0.00137 s−1, whereas the fast component volume of distribution (VF; mostly reflective of binding to α1/α2/α3 subunit-containing receptors (Myers et al., 2012)] was calculated using a frequency interval of 0.00137–0.1 s−1. We derived these boundaries empirically via examination of spectra derived from regional spectral analyses of the hippocampi (α5 rich) and occipital lobes and cerebella (α5 poor).

Spatial image manipulation

The VT images for each participant were co-registered to their corresponding T1-weighted MR image via the corresponding summation images using SPM12 (www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm). To enable voxel-by-voxel analyses, images acquired from individuals with TLE in whom the epileptogenic zone was presumed to lie within the right temporal lobe (five individuals) were right-left flipped before spatial normalization, so the epileptogenic side appeared on the left side in all. To ensure like-for-like comparison and to exclude spurious differences arising from the flipping of individuals with right TLE, a similar proportion of controls (10 of 23) also needed to be right-left flipped. Therefore, random subsets of images acquired from ten controls were right-left flipped before spatial normalization (10 permutations). T1-weighted MR images were normalized to a symmetrical template (Didelot et al., 2010) in Montreal Neurological Institute space using the deformation fields generated via Unified Segmentation (Ashburner and Friston, 2005) as implemented in SPM12. The same deformation fields were applied to the co-registered VS and VF images, which were subsequently smoothed (SPM12) using an isotropic Gaussian kernel of 12 mm full width at half maximum.

Statistical analyses

Age, Adult Memory and Information Processing Battery scores and hippocampal volumes

We compared age and hippocampal volumes (ipsilateral and contralateral, separately) between the groups by Mann–Whitney U-tests. As there were significant negative correlations between the list learning and list recall AMIPB subtests and age (both P = 0.04), we compared the AMIPB scores by Mann–Whitney U-tests after regression of age. To allow subsequent interrogation of the relationship between AMIPB scores and VS, whilst keeping the number of analyses acceptably low, we summarized the age-regressed AMIPB scores as a single component by principal component analysis. We also compared this component between groups via an additional Mann–Whitney U-test. We used a significance threshold of P < 0.05.

V S and VF comparisons of groups (TLE versus controls); of each case versus 23 controls; and relationship with Adult Memory and Information Processing Battery scores

We compared VS and separately VF between the TLE and control groups on a voxel-by-voxel basis using the normalized, smoothed Vs and VF images. We performed non-parametric two-sample pseudo-T tests (10 000 permutations) using the SnPM toolbox in SPM12 (Nichols and Holmes, 2002), with variance smoothing (8 mm full width at half maximum), and with a global total VT (i.e. α1/2/3/5 subunits) covariate (Table 1). The images were (explicitly) masked at a relative threshold of 0.8. Ten tests were performed, corresponding to 10 control group flipping permutations. We chose to adopt the two-step ‘suprathreshold’ cluster test approach to significance testing as it is known to be more sensitive than the single threshold approach (Friston et al., 1994; Nichols and Holmes, 2002). We report differences at a visualization pseudo-T threshold set arbitrarily to 2.5 followed by a conservative cluster threshold of P < 0.05 (family-wise error corrected). In a post-hoc analysis, we repeated the above comparison for flipping permutation 1, using threshold-free cluster enhancement as implemented in FSL version 5.0.9 (https://fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/fslwiki) with the ‘randomise’ tool, version 2.9.

Non-flipped VS were also compared for each individual participant with TLE to controls (i.e. 1 versus 23) by non-parametric two-sample pseudo-T tests as described above.

We interrogated the relationship between summary AMIPB scores (Shapiro–Wilk, P = 0.02) and hippocampal VS (ipsilateral and contralateral averaged) for the individuals with TLE separately, and in combination with the controls as a single group, using Spearman’s rank correlation coefficient. In a post-hoc analysis, we also performed a voxel-by-voxel conjunction analysis using all four AMIPB subtests, with correction for multiple comparisons using a false discovery rate of 0.05 (see Supplementary Material).

Exploratory analysis of interictal interval

Based on a reported association between [11C]FMZ binding and the interval between last seizure and PET (Savic et al., 1996; Bouvard et al., 2005; Laufs et al., 2011), we interrogated the relationship between interictal interval (transformed via natural logarithm) and global VS using Pearson’s correlation coefficient.

Post-hoc analysis

We performed a post-hoc comparison of VS between the TLE and control groups using flipping permutation one, as described above but after the application of a wavelet-based resolution recovery technique (iterative SFS-RR; (Shidahara et al., 2012; McGinnity et al., 2013; Silva-Rodríguez et al., 2016; see Supplementary Material). The results did not differ substantially from those derived from the original comparison (presented below), in either directionality or significance.

Data availability

Anonymized data are available from the corresponding author upon request.

Results

All 34 participants completed the PET and MRI scans. Their medication usage at the time of scanning is summarised in Table 1.

Between-group differences in baseline variables

Injectate

There was a small (∼8%) but significant difference in injected dose between individuals with TLE and controls, but injected dose was not correlated with global VS (P = 0.27). Median occupancies were at tracer doses, i.e. below 2.5%, in individuals with TLE and controls. Controls had a median 0.9% point higher occupancy, but occupancy was not correlated with global VS (P = 0.29).

Age and hippocampal volumes

There was no significant difference between the groups in age. The individuals with TLE had significantly larger hippocampi than controls (left P = 0.003, right P = 0.03) (Table 1).

AMIPB scores

Individuals with TLE had worse memory performance than controls, with significant differences evident overall and on the ‘List’ subtests (all P = 0.02; Table 2). Individuals with left TLE performed worse than those with right TLE on all subtests.

Table 2.

AMIPB scores for the individuals with MRI-negative TLE and healthy controls

LTLE RTLE TLE—all Controls (n = 6) Mann–Whitney U, P; Cohen’s d

List Learning (maximum attainable = 75)

42.9 ± 9.6 46.3 ± 9.6 44.5 ± 9.3  57.3 ± 9.3 9.0, 0.02, 1.4

List Recall (maximum attainable = 15)

7.3 ± 2.8 9.6 ± 2.8 8.3 ± 2.9 12.1 ± 2.9 9.5, 0.02, 1.3

Figure Learning (maximum attainable = 45)

29.0 ± 12.7 30.9 ± 12.7 29.9 ± 12.2 38.9 ± 12.2 19.0, 0.16, 0.8

Figure Recall (maximum attainable = 9)

5.6 ± 2.7 8.5 ± 2.7 6.9 ± 2.9 8.5 ± 2.9 22.0, 0.27, 0.6
Summary AMIPB score −0.7 ± 1.3 0.0 ± 0.6 −0.4 ± 1.0 0.7 ± 0.3 9.0, 0.02, 1.2

Scores are provided as estimated marginal means ± standard deviations (after regression of age), for the individual subtests. Higher values indicate better performance. Mann–Whitney U-tests were performed on the residuals (all individuals with epilepsy versus controls). The ‘Summary AMIPB’ score was generated via principal component analyses of the four age-regressed AMIPB subtest scores, see ‘Materials and Methods’.

Volumes of distribution

The global total VT for RTLE4, who had taken intermittent clobazam ∼3.5 days before the scan, did not significantly differ from those of the remaining individuals with TLE (Table 1). When included, the global total VT for RTLE4 was the median value for the group.

The TLE group had significantly higher VS than the control group, with two similar patterns of clusters observed depending on random flipping. ‘Pattern 1’, which was returned for eight of the 10 random flips, consisted of one very large cluster with the peak voxel in the contralateral, anterior hippocampus in all eight random flips (83 492 ± 4650 mm3; cluster P = 0.002 ± 0.001 family-wise error corrected; Table 3; Fig. 1). This cluster also included a secondary peak in the ipsilateral, anterior hippocampus in all eight of these random flips. ‘Pattern 2’, which was returned for the remaining two random flips, consisted instead of two separate large clusters, one with peak voxel in the contralateral, anterior hippocampus for both flips (46  960 ± 3776 mm3, cluster P = 0.005 ± 0.001) and the other with peak in the ipsilateral hippocampus for both flips (46 960 ± 3776mm3, cluster P = 0.008 ± 0.001). Despite variation in the number of clusters, the increases in VS were bilateral and extensive across all flips, and consistently encompassed anterior medial and lateral aspects of the temporal lobes, the anterior cingulate gyri, the presumed areas tempestas (piriform cortex) and the insulae (Fig. 1). The TLE group did not have any foci of significantly lower VS than controls. Exclusion of the two individuals with TLE who had the longest interictal interval did not substantially alter these results.

Table 3.

Significant differences in [11C]Ro15-4513 VS between individuals with MRI-negative TLE and healthy controls

Cluster level
Voxel level
Volume (mm3) P  FWE-corr Pseudo-T x, y, z, mm Region name Proportion of SnPM analyses
TLE > controls Pattern 1 (8/10 flips) Cluster 1 83 492 ± 4 650 0.002 ± 0.001 5.21 ± 0.31 32 −17 −16 Contralateral hippocampus 8/8
Secondary maxima:
4.51 ± 0.26 −32 −15 −16 Ipsilateral hippocampus/temporal horn 8/8
4.56 ± 0.28 12 10 −02 Contralateral caudate 6/8
4.29 ± 0.00 −08 25 −02 Corpus callosum near ipsilateral caudate 2/8
Pattern 2 (2/10 flips) Cluster 1 46 960 ± 3776 0.005 ± 0.001 5.00 ± 0.10 33 −18 −18 Contralateral hippocampus 2/2
Secondary maxima:
4.27 ± 0.06 10 12 −02 Contralateral caudate 2/2
4.38 −06 28 −02 Corpus callosum near ipsilateral caudate 1/2
4.02 04 26 −02 Corpus callosum near contralateral caudate 1/2
Cluster 2 36 840 ± 712 0.008 ± 0.001 4.77 ± 0.05 −32 −16 −14 Ipsilateral hippocampus/temporal horn 2/2
Secondary maxima:
4.01 ± 0.19 −39 03 −36 Ipsilateral anterior medial temporal lobe 2/2
3.99 ± 0.12 −60 −27 −16 Ipsilateral inferior/middle temporal gyrus / posterior temporal lobe 2/2
TLE < controls

mm(3), (cubic) millimetres; pFWE-corr, P-value corrected for multiple comparisons via family-wise error correction. Pattern 1 refers to a single cluster extending to both ipsilateral and contralateral temporal lobes; Pattern 2 describes a very similar pattern where ipsilateral and contralateral clusters are not connected. See text for details.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Significant differences in [11C]Ro15-4513 VS (individuals with MRI-negative TLE versus healthy controls).  Top row—median for the TLE group (with flipping; ipsilateral is on the right of the image/left of the brain throughout); middle row—median for the control group; Bottom row—significant differences in VS between the groups (red/yellow colour scale—TLE > Controls; cluster pseudo-T threshold 2.5). Bottom row depicts the median pseudo-T statistic image (10 random control flips). C, contralateral; I, ipsilateral

Summary statistics for the ipsilateral and contralateral hippocampal areas of increase [isolated by the union between the clusters and hippocampal regions of the latest version of the Hammersmith maximum probability atlas (Faillenot et al., 2017); https://brain-development.org/brain-atlases/adult-brain-atlases/] are provided in Table 4. In individuals with TLE, VS was significantly higher than in controls in ipsilateral and contralateral hippocampal areas; VF was significantly lower in the ipsilateral hippocampal area, and the VF:VS ratio was significantly lower in ipsilateral and contralateral hippocampal areas.

Table 4.

[11C]Ro15-4513 VTs within hippocampal areas

Hippocampal area VS
Hippocampal area VF
Hippocampal area VF: VS ratios
Ipsilateral Contralateral Ipsilateral Contralateral Ipsilateral Contralateral
TLE 6.21 ± 0.91 6.57 ± 0.86 1.35 ± 0.43 1.39 ± 0.41 0.23 ± 0.15 0.22 ± 0.14
Controls 5.02 ± 0.91 5.21 ± 0.85 1.72 ± 0.43 1.67 ± 0.40 0.38 ± 0.17 0.35 ± 0.14

Mann–Whitney

 

U, P; Cohen’s d

45.0 0.003, 1.3 27.0, 0.0002, 1.6 66.0, 0.03, 0.8 81.0, 0.09, 0.7 52.0, 0.006, 0.9 54.0, 0.008, 0.9

The areas of VS increase were isolated by the union between the SPM clusters and hippocampal regions of the Hammersmith maximum probability atlas (see text). VS and VF are summarized as estimated marginal means ± standard deviations after regression of global total VT; Mann–Whitney U-tests and calculations of Cohen’s d were performed on the residuals. VF: VS ratios are expressed as means ± standard deviations.

There were no significant differences in VF between the TLE group and the control group (Fig. 2).

Figure 2.

Figure 2

[11C]Ro15-4513 VF (A) and VS: VF ratio (B; individuals with MRI-negative TLE versus healthy controls). (A) median VF for the TLE group (upper row; with flipping; ipsilateral is on the right of the image/left of the brain throughout) and for the control group (lower row); (B) median VS: VF ratio for the TLE group (upper row; with flipping; ipsilateral is on the right of the image/left of the brain throughout) and for the control group (lower row). Note that in B, for illustrative purposes, we depict VS: VF ratio rather than VF: VS ratio. C, contralateral; I, ipsilateral

The post-hoc analysis for flipping permutation 1 using threshold-free cluster enhancement yielded results that were virtually identical to those generated using the two-step ‘suprathreshold’ cluster test approach (see Supplementary Fig. 3).

Comparisons of VS in each case versus 23 controls

Two controls had areas of VS that significantly differed from that of the remaining controls (one control with an increase, one control with a decrease; i.e. two of 46 comparisons, 4.3%; cluster P < 0.05 family-wise error corrected). At the same thresholds, two of the individuals with TLE had a significant increase each, one of which was localized to the ipsilateral frontal and temporal lobes and the other to ipsi- and contralateral frontal and temporal lobes (but with an ipsilateral frontal peak; see Supplementary Figs 1 and 2). There were no significant decreases in VS.

V S and AMIPB scores

There were no significant correlations between the summary AMIPB scores and mean hippocampal VS (individuals with TLE ρ = −0.09, P = 0.79; controls and individuals with TLE combined as a single group ρ = −0.48, P = 0.05). The negative associations between summary AMIPB score and hippocampal VS are illustrated in Fig. 3.

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Hippocampal VS versus summary AMIPB scores. AMIPB—Adult Memory And Information Processing Battery. The VS (i.e. slow component volume of distribution) was summarized as the mean of the left and right hippocampi. The age-regressed AMIPB scores were summarized as a single component by principal component analyses. Higher AMIPB summary scores indicate better memory performance

The post-hoc voxel-by-voxel analysis did not yield any clusters of conjunction of significance.

Exploratory analysis of VS and interictal interval

The analysis revealed negative correlations between interictal interval transformed by natural logarithm and global VS (r = −0.77, P = 0.005, Fig. 4). Additional post-hoc tests showed a significant negative correlation with the mean hippocampal area VS (ipsilateral and contralateral averaged; r = −0.65, P = 0.03), but not with mean hippocampal area VF or mean hippocampal area VF:VT ratio (r = 0.32, P = 0.34; and r = 0.52, P = 0.10).

Figure 4.

Figure 4

Global VS versus interictal interval. The interictal interval has been transformed via the natural logarithm (Ln). Pearson’s correlation coefficient (r) was −0.77 (P = 0.005). VS, slow component volume of distribution.

Discussion

We used bandpass spectral analysis to reveal, for the first time in humans, several alterations of α subunit binding in MRI-negative TLE, including higher VS, i.e. mostly α5 hippocampal binding of [11C]Ro15-4513.

Strengths and limitations

Our study’s principal strength is its robust PET methodology. [11C]Ro15-4513 has approximately nanomolar affinity for GABAA receptors, with approximately 10–15 times higher affinity for those receptors containing α5 than for those that do not (Luddens et al., 1994). We obtained metabolite-corrected arterial plasma input functions, the gold-standard approach for quantitative PET and report the first use of voxel-by-voxel bandpass spectral analysis, going beyond previous region-of-interest-based approaches (Mendez et al., 2013; Stokes et al., 2013; 2014; Horder et al., 2018). Bandpass spectral analysis allows approximate separation of slow (VS; mostly α5) from other binding on the basis of cerebral tissue kinetics. This approach has permitted parametric imaging of two targets using [11C]Ro15-4513, and could conceivably be applied to other radioligands with selectivity for more than one target.

An additional strength is that we deliberately excluded individuals with visually apparent MRI abnormality, such as hippocampal sclerosis, to minimize the confounding influence of structural changes which might explain memory difficulties, and of partial volume effect. This may have biased our sample towards patients with larger-than-usual hippocampi, and we unexpectedly found that individuals with TLE in our sample had slightly larger hippocampi than controls (Table 1). However, in the post-hoc analysis, the application of a wavelet-based resolution recovery technique had no effect on the directionality or significance of the difference in VS between the TLE and control groups. The difference between groups is also readily apparent even on visual inspection of the unfiltered images in native space. Therefore, we are confident that our findings are not driven by the partial volume effect.

Six of our individuals with TLE have had [18F]fluorodeoxyglucose or [11C]FMZ PET, which was localizing in four. Only one of our individuals with TLE has had intracranial electroencephalography; therefore mis-localization in some cases is possible. This limits the inference that can be made on the lateralizing capacity of [11C]Ro15-4513 VS, but does not invalidate the association we have observed between refractory focal epilepsy and increased α5 subunit expression.

Four of our individuals with TLE were female, whereas all of our controls were male; we are unaware of any studies on the effect of gender or the menstrual cycle on α5 subunit expression, but cannot exclude this as a potential confound.

High-dose valproic acid was associated with an unexpected doubling of hippocampal and amygdala [11C]Ro15-4513 standardized (α1/2/3/5) uptake in rats, using standardized uptake values (α1/2/3/5) without an arterial input function to correct for possible differences in metabolism or blood flow (Bertelsen et al., 2018). We checked whether the individuals with TLE who were taking sodium valproate at the time of the scan in our study had different VS, VF and VF:VS ratios in the hippocampal areas compared to the other individuals, but did not find a significant difference (P ≥ 0.19).

α5 subunit expression and seizures

As noted in the Introduction, increases and decreases in α5 subunit expression have been reported in animal models. The reason for discrepant findings in the preclinical literature is not clear, however, differences in experimental procedure such as in the timing of sacrifice and the region selected for examination might be important. Our finding of substantial increases (∼25%) in hippocampal limbic lobe VS in human TLE is consistent with the reports of increased dentate gyrus α5 subunit expression in models using kainic acid (Schwarzer et al., 1997; Bouilleret et al., 2000) and pilocarpine (Rice et al., 1996; Fritschy et al., 1999; Houser and Esclapez, 2003). Notably, the increases we describe extend well beyond the hippocampus into other regions of the lateral temporal lobe, which have not been scrutinized in animal models. Our findings highlight the need for validation of data derived from experimental models in humans, in vivo.

The finding of higher limbic lobe VS in those with refractory TLE suggests either: (i) upregulation of α5 subunit expression occurring in as a consequence of spontaneous epileptic seizures; or (ii) chronic upregulation of α5 subunit expression resulting in a vulnerability to seizures. Longitudinal study would allow these hypotheses to be formally tested. From our data, the finding of a negative correlation of VS with interictal interval favours the first explanation. We interpret the increase in α5 binding we observed in individuals with TLE as an increase in receptor concentration, but it could also reflect slowing of the cerebral tissue kinetics.

Upregulation of expression during the early interictal phase would result in increased tonic inhibitory neurotransmission, which we suggest may contribute to suppression of further seizures (Sackeim et al., 1987). Our findings complement earlier reports of dynamic alterations in cannabinoid (Goffin et al., 2011), GABAA (Savic et al., 1996; Bouvard et al., 2005) and opioid receptor binding in association with temporal lobe seizures (Hammers et al., 2007a; McGinnity et al., 2013).

Receptor upregulation following seizures is not invariable; it should be noted, for example, that hippocampal [11C]FMZ binding, reflective of availability of α subunits 1–3 and 5, was actually lower in individuals with MRI-negative TLE (Koepp et al., 2000; Hammers et al., 2002) and in individuals with TLE with hippocampal sclerosis (Koepp et al., 1996; 1997; Hammers et al., 2001). In six individuals with TLE (three of whom were MRI-negative) who had paired [11C]FMZ scans one week apart, the binding was lowest for the scan that was associated with the shorter interictal interval (Bouvard et al., 2005), and the extent of reduction in [11C]FMZ binding in the PET abnormality (relative to the contralateral homologue) was positively correlated with seizure frequency in 17 individuals with MRI-negative focal (mostly temporal lobe) epilepsy (Savic et al., 1996). [11C]FMZ binding in the presumed area tempestas, near the piriform cortex, was also negatively correlated with seizure frequency in 18 individuals with MRI-negative focal (mostly frontal) lobe epilepsy (Laufs et al., 2011).

Together, therefore, these data suggest that a ‘subunit shift’ may effectively occur in TLE, from α subunits 1/2/3 to 5, in response to seizures. The results of our exploratory and post-hoc analyses of the associations between global and, separately, hippocampal area VS and interictal interval support this hypothesis, and were complemented by the identification of significantly lower VFs and VF:VS ratios for the individuals with TLE than for controls in the same hippocampal areas. Such a shift might be expected to predispose the individual to learning and memory impairments, consistent with the TLE clinical phenotype (Giovagnoli and Avanzini, 1999).

[11C]Ro15-4513 VS does not appear to lateralize MRI-negative TLE, which, while disappointing in terms of usefulness for pre-surgical evaluation, is consistent with asymmetric but to a degree bilateral decreases in [11C]FMZ binding (Ryvlin et al., 1998; Hammers et al., 2002). Caution is required in the interpretation of this result due to potential mis-localization.

α5 subunit expression and memory impairment

Our finding of substantial increases (∼25%) in hippocampal limbic lobe VS in human TLE is also consistent with co-morbid memory impairments in individuals with TLE, given the extensive literature summarized in the Introduction which suggests activation of receptors containing the α5 subunit impairs hippocampus-dependent learning and memory. Contrary to our expectations and findings in individuals with alcohol dependence (Lingford-Hughes et al., 2012), however, we did not detect any significant correlation between summary AMIPB scores and hippocampal VS for the individuals with TLE (but observed a borderline significant correlation when pooled with the controls). Caution is required in the interpretation of this result due to the sample size, pooling of individuals with left and right TLE, and potential mis-localization. Whilst the performance of our individuals with TLE on the List Learning and Figure Learning AMIPB subtests was similar to those reported in a larger study (Bonelli et al., 2010), our ability to detect impairments was compromised by a ceiling effect observed in the non-verbal (figure) recall tests, where several individuals were able to attain the maximum possible score. The use of a delayed recall task might have better indexed hippocampal-dependent memory than the AMIPB subtests, which quantify immediate/short-term recall.

Conclusion

Our results provide evidence for increased tonic inhibitory neurotransmission in MRI-negative TLE. The increases in VS were bilateral and therefore not useful for lateralization of TLE. While causal inferences cannot be made, the finding of increased [11C]Ro15-4513 VS is consistent with the co-morbid memory impairments in this population. Exploratory analyses suggested a relationship with time since last seizure.

Supplementary material

Supplementary material is available at Brain Communications online.

Supplementary Material

fcaa190_Supplementary_Data

Acknowledgements

We are grateful to the participants, and to the staff of Hammersmith Imanet Limited and the Hammersmith Hospital MRI Unit for their assistance with data acquisition and preparation.

Funding

This study was supported by the Medical Research Council (MRC; United Kingdom) Clinical Sciences Centre (MC_U120085812). Authors affiliated with (a) are grateful for support from the Department of Health National Institute of Health Research (NIHR) Biomedical Research Centre funding scheme. C.J.M. was supported by a Medical Research Council Doctoral Training Account (1 + 3) studentship that was awarded by Imperial College London, and subsequently by the Medical Research Council (MR/N013042/1), the Wellcome Trust/Engineering and Physical Sciences Research Council (EPSRC) Centre for Medical Engineering (WT 203148/Z/16/Z) and the Engineering and Physical Sciences Research Council Centre for Doctoral Training in Medical Imaging (EP/L015226/1; all based in the United Kingdom). J.dT., J.S.D., J.W.S. and M.J.K. are based at National Institute of Health Research University College London Hospitals Biomedical Research Centre, which receives a proportion of funding from the United Kingdom Department of Health’s Research Centres funding scheme. J.W.S. receives research support from the Marvin Weil Epilepsy Research Fund and Christelijke Vereniging voor de Verpleging van Lijders aan Epilepsie (The Netherlands). J.W.S. and M.J.K. received research support from the United Kingdom Epilepsy Society. A.H. was supported by a Medical Research Council (United Kingdom) Clinician Scientist Fellowship (G108/585) and the Neurodis Foundation (France). The funders had no involvement in the conduct of the study or preparation of the manuscript.

Competing Interests

The authors do not have any competing interests to declare in relation to this work.

Glossary

[11C]FMZ =

[11C]flumazenil

AMIPB =

Adult Memory and Information Processing Battery

CA =

cornu ammonis

GABA =

gamma-aminobutyric acid

LTLE =

left temporal lobe epilepsy

MR =

magnetic resonance

TLE =

temporal lobe epileps

y

VF =

fast component volume-of-distribution

VS =

slow component volume-of-distribution

VT =

(total) volume-of-distribution

References

  1. Adam L, Zaers J, Ostertag H, Trojan H, Bellemann ME, Brix G.  Performance evaluation of the whole-body PET scanner ECAT EXACT HR+ following the IEC standard. IEEE Trans Nucl Sci  1997; 44: 1172–9. [Google Scholar]
  2. Allen EV.  Thromboangiitis obliterans: Methods of diagnosis of chronic occlusive arterial lesions distal to the wrist with illustrative cases. Am J Med Sci  1929; 178: 237–44. [Google Scholar]
  3. Ashburner J, Friston KJ.  Unified segmentation. Neuroimage  2005; 26: 839–51. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Barnard EA, Skolnick P, Olsen RW, Mohler H, Sieghart W, Biggio G, et al.  International Union of Pharmacology. XV. Subtypes of gamma-aminobutyric acidA receptors: classification on the basis of subunit structure and receptor function. Pharmacol Rev  1998; 50: 291–313. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Baxendale S.  The Flynn effect and memory function. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol  2010; 32: 699–703. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bertelsen F, Landau AM, Vase KH, Jacobsen J, Scheel-Krüger J, Møller A.  Acute in vivo effect of valproic acid on the GABAergic system in rat brain: A [11C]Ro15-4513 microPET study. Brain Res  2018; 1680: 110–4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bethmann K, Fritschy JM, Brandt C, Loscher W.  Antiepileptic drug resistant rats differ from drug responsive rats in GABA A receptor subunit expression in a model of temporal lobe epilepsy. Neurobiol Dis  2008; 31: 169–87. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Bonelli SB, Powell RHW, Yogarajah M, Samson RS, Symms MR, Thompson PJ, et al.  Imaging memory in temporal lobe epilepsy: predicting the effects of temporal lobe resection. Brain  2010; 133: 1186–99. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bonetti EP, Burkard WP, Gabl M, Hunkeler W, Lorez HP, Martin JR, et al.  Ro 15-4513: partial inverse agonism at the BZR and interaction with ethanol. Pharmacol Biochem Behav  1988; 31: 733–49. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Bouilleret V, Loup F, Kiener T, Marescaux C, Fritschy J-M.  Early loss of interneurons and delayed subunit-specific changes in GABAA-receptor expression in a mouse model of mesial temporal lobe epilepsy. Hippocampus  2000; 10: 305–24. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Bouvard S, Costes N, Bonnefoi F, Lavenne F, Mauguiere F, Delforge J, et al.  Seizure-related short-term plasticity of benzodiazepine receptors in partial epilepsy: a [11C]flumazenil-PET study. Brain  2005; 128: 1330–43. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Brady ML, Jacob TC.  Synaptic localization of α5 GABA (A) receptors via gephyrin interaction regulates dendritic outgrowth and spine maturation. Dev Neurobio  2015; 75: 1241–51. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Brix G, Zaers J, Adam LE, Bellemann ME, Ostertag H, Trojan H, et al.  Performance evaluation of a whole-body PET scanner using the NEMA protocol. National Electrical Manufacturers Association. J Nucl Med  1997; 38: 1614–23. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Brooks-Kayal AR, Shumate MD, Jin H, Rikhter TY, Coulter DA.  Selective changes in single cell GABAA receptor subunit expression and function in temporal lobe epilepsy. Nat Med  1998; 4: 1166–72. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Brunig I, Scotti E, Sidler C, Fritschy JM.  Intact sorting, targeting, and clustering of gamma-aminobutyric acid A receptor subtypes in hippocampal neurons in vitro. J Comp Neurol  2002; 443: 43–55. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Caraiscos VB, Elliott EM, You-Ten KE, Cheng VY, Belelli D, Newell JG, et al.  Tonic inhibition in mouse hippocampal CA1 pyramidal neurons is mediated by α5 subunit-containing γ-aminobutyric acid type A receptors. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA  2004; 101: 3662–7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Chambers MS, Atack JR, Carling RW, Collinson N, Cook SM, Dawson GR, et al.  An orally bioavailable, functionally selective inverse agonist at the benzodiazepine site of GABAA α5 receptors with cognition enhancing properties. J Med Chem  2004; 47: 5829–32. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Cheng VY, Martin LJ, Elliott EM, Kim JH, Mount HT, Taverna FA, et al.  Alpha5GABAA receptors mediate the amnestic but not sedative-hypnotic effects of the general anesthetic etomidate. J Neurosci  2006; 26: 3713–20. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Collinson N, Kuenzi FM, Jarolimek W, Maubach KA, Cothliff R, Sur C, et al.  Enhanced learning and memory and altered GABAergic synaptic transmission in mice lacking the alpha 5 subunit of the GABAA receptor. J Neurosci  2002; 22: 5572–80. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Coughlan AK, Hollows SE, The adult memory and information processing battery. Leeds: St James University Hospital; 1985. [Google Scholar]
  21. Crestani F, Keist R, Fritschy J-M, Benke D, Vogt K, Prut L, et al.  Trace fear conditioning involves hippocampal α5 GABAA receptors. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA  2002; 99: 8980–5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Cunningham VJ, Jones T.  Spectral analysis of dynamic PET studies. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab  1993; 13: 15–23. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Curtis DR, Felix D, McLellan H.  GABA and hippocampal inhibition. Br J Pharmacol  1970; 40: 881–3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Dawson GR, Maubach KA, Collinson N, Cobain M, Everitt BJ, MacLeod AM, et al.  An inverse agonist selective for α5 subunit-containing GABAA receptors enhances cognition. J Pharmacol Exp Ther  2006; 316: 1335–45. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Defrise M, Kinahan PE, Townsend DW, Michel C, Sibomana M, Newport DF.  Exact and approximate rebinning algorithms for 3-D PET data. IEEE Trans Med Imaging  1997; 16: 145–58. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Didelot A, Mauguiere F, Redoute J, Bouvard S, Lothe A, Reilhac A, et al.  Voxel-based analysis of asymmetry index maps increases the specificity of 18F-MPPF PET abnormalities for localizing the epileptogenic zone in temporal lobe epilepsies. J Nucl Med  2010; 51: 1732–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Eichenbaum H, Sauvage M, Fortin N, Komorowski R, Lipton P.  Towards a functional organization of episodic memory in the medial temporal lobe. Neurosci Biobehav Rev  2012; 36: 1597–608. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Faillenot I, Heckemann RA, Frot M, Hammers A.  Macroanatomy and 3D probabilistic atlas of the human insula. Neuroimage  2017; 150: 88–98. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Friston KJ, Worsley KJ, Frackowiak RS, Mazziotta JC, Evans AC.  Assessing the significance of focal activations using their spatial extent. Hum Brain Mapp  1994; 1: 210–20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Fritschy JM, Kiener T, Bouilleret V, Loup F.  GABAergic neurons and GABAA-receptors in temporal lobe epilepsy. Neurochem Int  1999; 34: 435–45. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Gacsalyi I, Moricz K, Gigler G, Megyeri K, Machado P, Antoni FA.  Persistent therapeutic effect of a novel alpha5-GABAA receptor antagonist in rodent preclinical models of vascular cognitive impairment. Eur J Pharmacol  2018; 834: 118–25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Gacsalyi I, Moricz K, Gigler G, Wellmann J, Nagy K, Ling I, et al.  Behavioural pharmacology of the alpha5-GABAA receptor antagonist S44819: Enhancement and remediation of cognitive performance in preclinical models. Neuropharmacology  2017; 125: 30–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Galanopoulou AS.  GABAA receptors in normal development and seizures: friends or foes?  CN  2008; 6: 1–20. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Giovagnoli AR, Avanzini G.  Learning and memory impairment in patients with temporal lobe epilepsy: relation to the presence, type, and location of brain lesion. Epilepsia  1999; 40: 904–11. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Goffin K, Van Paesschen W, Van Laere K.  In vivo activation of endocannabinoid system in temporal lobe epilepsy with hippocampal sclerosis. Brain  2011; 134: 1033–40. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Hammers A, Asselin MC, Hinz R, Kitchen I, Brooks DJ, Duncan JS, et al.  Upregulation of opioid receptor binding following spontaneous epileptic seizures. Brain  2007a; 130: 1009–16. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Hammers A, Asselin MC, Turkheimer FE, Hinz R, Osman S, Hotton G, et al.  Balancing bias, reliability, noise properties and the need for parametric maps in quantitative ligand PET: [11C]diprenorphine test-retest data. Neuroimage  2007b; 38: 82–94. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Hammers A, Koepp MJ, Hurlemann R, Thom M, Richardson MP, Brooks DJ, et al.  Abnormalities of grey and white matter [11C]flumazenil binding in temporal lobe epilepsy with normal MRI. Brain  2002; 125: 2257–71. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Hammers A, Koepp MJ, Labbe C, Brooks DJ, Thom M, Cunningham VJ, et al.  Neocortical abnormalities of [11C]-flumazenil PET in mesial temporal lobe epilepsy. Neurology  2001; 56: 897–906. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Heckemann RA, Keihaninejad S, Aljabar P, Rueckert D, Hajnal JV, Hammers A.  Improving intersubject image registration using tissue-class information benefits robustness and accuracy of multi-atlas based anatomical segmentation. Neuroimage  2010; 51: 221–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Henry TR, Frey KA, Sackellares JC, Gilman S, Koeppe RA, Brunberg JA, et al.  In vivo cerebral metabolism and central benzodiazepine-receptor binding in temporal lobe epilepsy. Neurology  1993; 43: 1998–2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Hinz R, Bhagwagar Z, Cowen PJ, Cunningham VJ, Grasby PM.  Validation of a tracer kinetic model for the quantification of 5-HT2A receptors in human brain with [11C]MDL 100,907. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab  2007; 27: 161–72. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Hoppe C, Elger CE, Helmstaedter C.  Long-term memory impairment in patients with focal epilepsy. Epilepsia  2007; 48: 26–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Horder J, Andersson M, Mendez MA, Singh N, Tangen A, Lundberg J, et al.  GABAA receptor availability is not altered in adults with autism spectrum disorder or in mouse models. Sci Transl Med  2018; 10: eaam8434. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Houser CR, Esclapez M.  Vulnerability and plasticity of the GABA system in the pilocarpine model of spontaneous recurrent seizures. Epilepsy Res  1996; 26: 207–18. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Houser CR, Esclapez M.  Downregulation of the alpha5 subunit of the GABAA receptor in the pilocarpine model of temporal lobe epilepsy. Hippocampus  2003; 13: 633–45. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Jacob TC.  Neurobiology and therapeutic potential of alpha5-GABA type A receptors. Front Mol Neurosci  2019; 12: 179. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Koepp MJ, Hammers A, Labbe C, Woermann FG, Brooks DJ, Duncan JS.  11C-flumazenil PET in patients with refractory temporal lobe epilepsy and normal MRI. Neurology  2000; 54: 332–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Koepp MJ, Richardson MP, Brooks DJ, Poline JB, Van Paesschen W, Friston KJ, et al.  Cerebral benzodiazepine receptors in hippocampal sclerosis. An objective in vivo analysis. Brain  1996; 119: 1677–87. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Koepp MJ, Richardson MP, Labbe C, Brooks DJ, Cunningham VJ, Ashburner J, et al.  11C-flumazenil PET, volumetric MRI, and quantitative pathology in mesial temporal lobe epilepsy. Neurology  1997; 49: 764–73. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Lamusuo S, Pitkanen A, Jutila L, Ylinen A, Partanen K, Kalviainen R, et al. [ 11C]Flumazenil binding in the medial temporal lobe in patients with temporal lobe epilepsy: correlation with hippocampal MR volumetry, T2 relaxometry, and neuropathology. Neurology  2000; 54: 2252–60. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Laufs H, Richardson MP, Salek-Haddadi A, Vollmar C, Duncan JS, Gale K, et al.  Converging PET and fMRI evidence for a common area involved in human focal epilepsies. Neurology  2011; 77: 904–10. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Ling I, Mihalik B, Etherington LA, Kapus G, Palvolgyi A, Gigler G, et al.  A novel GABAA alpha 5 receptor inhibitor with therapeutic potential. Eur J Pharmacol  2015; 764: 497–507. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Lingford-Hughes A, Hume SP, Feeney A, Hirani E, Osman S, Cunningham VJ, et al.  Imaging the GABA-benzodiazepine receptor subtype containing the alpha5-subunit in vivo with [11C]Ro15 4513 positron emission tomography. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab  2002; 22: 878–89. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Lingford-Hughes A, Reid AG, Myers J, Feeney A, Hammers A, Taylor LG, et al.  A [11C]Ro15 4513 PET study suggests that alcohol dependence in man is associated with reduced alpha5 benzodiazepine receptors in limbic regions. J Psychopharmacol  2012; 26: 273–81. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Lister RG, Nutt DJ.  Interactions of the imidazodiazepine Ro 15-4513 with chemical convulsants. Br J Pharmacol  1988; 93: 210–4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Lorente de Nó R.  Studies on the structure of the cerebral cortex. II. Continuation of the study of the ammonic system. J Psychol Neurol  1934; 46: 113–177. [Google Scholar]
  58. Luddens H, Seeburg PH, Korpi ER.  Impact of beta and gamma variants on ligand-binding properties of gamma-aminobutyric acid type A receptors. Mol Pharmacol  1994; 45: 810–4. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Martin LJ, Zurek AA, MacDonald JF, Roder JC, Jackson MF, Orser BA.  Alpha5GABAA receptor activity sets the threshold for long-term potentiation and constrains hippocampus-dependent memory. J Neurosci  2010; 30: 5269–82. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Maziere M, Hantraye P, Prenant C, Sastre J, Comar D.  Synthesis of ethyl 8-fluoro-5,6-dihydro-5-[11C]methyl-6-oxo-4H-imidazo [1,5-a] [1,4]benzodiazepine-3-carboxylate (RO 15.1788-11C): a specific radioligand for the in vivo study of central benzodiazepine receptors by positron emission tomography. Int J Appl Radiat Isot  1984; 35: 973–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. McGinnity CJ, Riano Barros DA, Rosso L, Veronese M, Rizzo G, Bertoldo A, et al.  Test-retest reproducibility of quantitative binding measures of [11C]Ro15-4513, a PET ligand for GABAA receptors containing alpha5 subunits. Neuroimage  2017; 152: 270–82. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. McGinnity CJ, Shidahara M, Feldmann M, Keihaninejad S, Riano Barros DA, Gousias IS, et al.  Quantification of opioid receptor availability following spontaneous epileptic seizures: correction of [11C]diprenorphine PET data for the partial-volume effect. Neuroimage  2013; 79: 72–80. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Mendez MA, Horder J, Myers J, Coghlan S, Stokes P, Erritzoe D, et al.  The brain GABA-benzodiazepine receptor alpha-5 subtype in autism spectrum disorder: a pilot [11C]Ro15-4513 positron emission tomography study. Neuropharmacology  2013; 68: 195–201. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Myers JF, Comley RA, Gunn RN.  Quantification of [11C]Ro15-4513 GABAAalpha5 specific binding and regional selectivity in humans. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab  2017; 37: 2137–48. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Myers JF, Rosso L, Watson BJ, Wilson SJ, Kalk NJ, Clementi N, et al.  Characterisation of the contribution of the GABA-benzodiazepine alpha1 receptor subtype to [11C]Ro15-4513 PET images. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab  2012; 32: 731–44. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Nichols TE, Holmes AP.  Nonparametric permutation tests for functional neuroimaging: a primer with examples. Hum Brain Mapp  2002; 15: 1–25. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Nishimura T, Schwarzer C, Gasser E, Kato N, Vezzani A, Sperk G.  Altered expression of GABAa and GABAb receptor subunit mRNAs in the hippocampus after kindling and electrically induced status epilepticus. Neuroscience  2005; 134: 691–704. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Nutt DJ, Besson M, Wilson SJ, Dawson GR, Lingford-Hughes AR.  Blockade of alcohol's amnestic activity in humans by an α5 subtype benzodiazepine receptor inverse agonist. Neuropharmacology  2007; 53: 810–20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Pirker S, Schwarzer C, Wieselthaler A, Sieghart W, Sperk G.  GABAA receptors: immunocytochemical distribution of 13 subunits in the adult rat brain. Neuroscience  2000; 101: 815–50. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Pofantis H, Papatheodoropoulos C.  The alpha5GABAA receptor modulates the induction of long-term potentiation at ventral but not dorsal CA1 hippocampal synapses. Synapse  2014; 68: 394–401. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Ranicar AS, Williams CW, Schnorr L, Clark JC, Rhodes CG, Bloomfield PM, et al.  The on-line monitoring of continuously withdrawn arterial blood during PET studies using a single BGO/photomultiplier assembly and non-stick tubing. Med Prog Technol  1991; 17: 259–64. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Riaño Barros DA, McGinnity CJ, Rosso L, Heckemann RA, Howes OD, Brooks DJ, et al.  Test–retest reproducibility of cannabinoid-receptor type 1 availability quantified with the PET ligand [11C]MePPEP. Neuroimage  2014; 97: 151–62. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Rice A, Rafiq A, Shapiro SM, Jakoi ER, Coulter DA, DeLorenzo RJ.  Long-lasting reduction of inhibitory function and gamma-aminobutyric acid type A receptor subunit mRNA expression in a model of temporal lobe epilepsy. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA  1996; 93: 9665–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Ryvlin P, Bouvard S, Le BD, De LG, Gregoire MC, Kahane P, et al.  Clinical utility of flumazenil-PET versus [18F]fluorodeoxyglucose-PET and MRI in refractory partial epilepsy. A prospective study in 100 patients. Brain  1998; 121: 2067–81. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Sackeim HA, Decina P, Portnoy S, Neeley P, Malitz S.  Studies of dosage, seizure threshold, and seizure duration in ECT. Biol Psychiatry  1987; 22: 249–68. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Savic I, Ingvar M, Stone-Elander S.  Comparison of [11C]flumazenil and [18F]FDG as PET markers of epileptic foci. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry  1993; 56: 615–21. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Savic I, Svanborg E, Thorell JO.  Cortical benzodiazepine receptor changes are related to frequency of partial seizures: a positron emission tomography study. Epilepsia  1996; 37: 236–44. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Savic I, Thorell JO.  Localized cerebellar reductions in benzodiazepine receptor density in human partial epilepsy. Arch Neurol  1996; 53: 656–62. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Schwarzer C, Tsunashima K, Wanzenbock C, Fuchs K, Sieghart W, Sperk G.  GABAA receptor subunits in the rat hippocampus II: altered distribution in kainic acid-induced temporal lobe epilepsy. Neuroscience  1997; 80: 1001–17. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Scimemi A, Semyanov A, Sperk G, Kullmann DM, Walker MC.  Multiple and plastic receptors mediate tonic GABAA receptor currents in the hippocampus. J Neurosci  2005; 25: 10016–24. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Shidahara M, Tsoumpas C, McGinnity CJ, Kato T, Tamura H, Hammers A, et al.  Wavelet-based resolution recovery using an anatomical prior provides quantitative recovery for human population phantom PET [11C]raclopride data. Phys Med Biol  2012; 57: 3107–22. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Sieghart W, Sperk G.  Subunit composition, distribution and function of GABAA receptor subtypes. CTMC  2002; 2: 795–816. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Silva-Rodríguez J, Cortés J, Rodríguez-Osorio X, López-Urdaneta J, Pardo-Montero J, Aguiar P, et al.  Iterative structural and functional synergistic resolution recovery (iSFS-RR) applied to PET-MR images in epilepsy. IEEE Trans Nucl Sci  2016; 63: 2434–42. [Google Scholar]
  84. Sperk G, Schwarzer C, Tsunashima K, Kandlhofer S.  Expression of GABAA receptor subunits in the hippocampus of the rat after kainic acid-induced seizures. Epilepsy Res  1998; 32: 129–39. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Sternfeld F, Carling RW, Jelley RA, Ladduwahetty T, Merchant KJ, Moore KW, et al.  Selective, orally active gamma-aminobutyric acidA alpha5 receptor inverse agonists as cognition enhancers. J Med Chem  2004; 47: 2176–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Stokes PR, Benecke A, Myers J, Erritzoe D, Watson BJ, Kalk N, et al.  History of cigarette smoking is associated with higher limbic GABAA receptor availability. Neuroimage  2013; 69: 70–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Stokes PR, Myers JF, Kalk NJ, Watson BJ, Erritzoe D, Wilson SJ, et al.  Acute increases in synaptic GABA detectable in the living human brain: a [11C]Ro15-4513 PET study. Neuroimage  2014; 99: 158–65. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Sur C, Quirk K, Dewar D, Atack J, McKernan R.  Rat and human hippocampal alpha5 subunit-containing gamma-aminobutyric acidA receptors have alpha5 beta3 gamma2 pharmacological characteristics. Mol Pharmacol  1998; 54: 928–33. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Szelies B, Weber-Luxenburger G, Mielke R, Pawlik G, Kessler J, Pietrzyk U, et al.  Interictal hippocampal benzodiazepine receptors in temporal lobe epilepsy: comparison with coregistered hippocampal metabolism and volumetry. Eur J Neurol  2000; 7: 393–400. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Szelies B, Weber-Luxenburger G, Pawlik G, Kessler J, Holthoff V, Mielke R, et al.  MRI-guided flumazenil- and FDG-PET in temporal lobe epilepsy. Neuroimage  1996; 3: 109–18. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Tsunashima K, Schwarzer C, Kirchmair E, Sieghart W, Sperk G.  GABAA receptor subunits in the rat hippocampus III: altered messenger RNA expression in kainic acid-induced epilepsy. Neuroscience  1997; 80: 1019–32. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Turkheimer FE, Brett M, Visvikis D, Cunningham VJ.  Multiresolution analysis of emission tomography images in the wavelet domain. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab  1999; 19: 1189–208. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Yee BK, Hauser J, Dolgov VV, Keist R, Mohler H, Rudolph U, et al.  GABA receptors containing the alpha5 subunit mediate the trace effect in aversive and appetitive conditioning and extinction of conditioned fear. Eur J Neurosci  2004; 20: 1928–36. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

fcaa190_Supplementary_Data

Data Availability Statement

Anonymized data are available from the corresponding author upon request.


Articles from Brain Communications are provided here courtesy of Oxford University Press

RESOURCES