Skip to main content
Iranian Journal of Basic Medical Sciences logoLink to Iranian Journal of Basic Medical Sciences
. 2020 Dec;23(12):1571–1575. doi: 10.22038/ijbms.2020.44891.10468

The effect of alpha-2 adrenergic receptors on memory retention deficit induced by rapid eye movement sleep deprivation

Yaser Norozpour 1, Mohammad Nasehi 2,*, Vahid Sabouri-Khanghah 1, Mohammad Nami 3,4, Salar Vaseghi 1,2, Mohammad-Reza Zarrindast 1,5
PMCID: PMC7811809  PMID: 33489031

Abstract

Objective(s):

Evidence shows that sleep deprivation (SD) disrupts the formation of hippocampus-related memories. Moreover, α2 adrenergic receptors that are wildly expressed in the CA1 hippocampal region have a significant role in modulating both sleep and memory formation. In the present research, we wanted to investigate the effect of stimulation and blockage of CA1 α2 adrenergic receptors by clonidine (an agonist of α2 adrenergic receptor) and yohimbine (an antagonist of α2 adrenergic receptor), respectively, on memory retention impairment induced by REM SD (RSD) in rats.

Materials and Methods:

Multiple platform apparatus were used to induce RSD, and the passive avoidance task was used to assess memory consolidation. Clonidine and yohimbine were injected intra-CA1.

Results:

The results showed that RSD (for 24 and 36, but not 12 hr) decreased memory retention, with no effect on locomotion. Post-training intra-CA1 infusion of a subthreshold dose of yohimbine (0.001 μg/rat) did not alter, while clonidine (0.1 μg/rat) restored memory retention impairment induced by RSD (24 and 36 hr). Also, none of the interventions did not influence locomotor activity.

Conclusion:

Our data strongly showed that CA1 α2 adrenergic receptors have a critical role in RSD-induced memory retention impairment.

Key Words: Clonidine, Hippocampus, Memory, Sleep, Yohimbine

Introduction

Sleep is a circadian rhythm that is involved in modulating physiological processes (1). It has a main role in modulating cognitive functions (2). Sleep consists of non-rapid eye movement (non-REM) and rapid eye movement (REM) stages (3). Cognitive functions including learning, memory, decision making, reaction time, attention, etc., are affected by sleep, and one of the most important functions of sleep is mediating memory formation (3-5). Also, sleep has a critical role in the consolidation of lately acquired memories for long-term storage (6). Thus, sleep deprivation (SD) can negatively affect hippocampus-dependent learning and memory and long-term potentiation (LTP) (7). Furthermore, previous studies have reported the role of REM sleep in learning and memory consolidation (8, 9).

Noradrenaline has a critical role in SD-induced cognitive deficits (10, 11). It has been reported that locus coeruleus (LC) ascending noradrenergic projections which lead to the prefrontal cortex and the hippocampus play a key role in cognitive deficits induced by SD (12). It has been also revealed that noradrenaline and adrenergic receptors modulate learning and memory processes (13). Adrenergic receptors are highly expressed in the posterior hippocampus region (CA1) that mediates learning and memory (3). Noradrenaline, by two receptor categories named α and β that couple to a G protein, exerts its own effects (12). The α adrenergic receptors consist of α1 and α2 subgroups which are expressed in a large amount in the central nervous system (CNS). Most of the α1 adrenergic receptors are post-synaptic, while α2 adrenergic receptors are both post- and pre-synaptic (14). The α2-adrenoceptors consist of various subtypes: α2A, α2B, and α2C (15). The α2A-subtype is the key α2-adrenoceptor which has an important role in different responses that are induced by the activation of α2-adrenoceptor, including modulating nociception, cardiovascular function, sedation, and releasing noradrenaline (16).

It has been shown that α2-adrenoceptors are involved in increased sleepiness following SD (11), and the regulatory role of adrenergic receptors may induce a homeostatic response to the continuous discharge by hypocretin/orexin neurons that would occur during enforced wakefulness (17). Now the question is whether RSD attenuates cognitive functions and how can we prevent it? To achieve this goal, we designed this study to investigate the impairment effect of RSD on different brain regions and the role of direct activation or deactivation of various receptors in RSD-induced responses. According to these mentioned points, this study aims to explore the effect of α2 agonism and antagonism on memory deficits induced by RSD.

Materials and Methods

Animals

Ninety-six 220–250 g adult male Wistar rats were used. The animals were stored for one week in the lab before performing experiments to habituate with temperature, humidity, and circadian rhythm. The temperature was kept at 23 °C. The rats had free access to food and water. Each group consisted of eight rats and each rat was used only once. All experiments were carried out during the light phase. Our experimental protocol in this study was done accord with the Ethics Guidelines of the Institute for Cognitive Science Studies.

Stereotaxic surgery

All rats were anesthetized by intraperitoneal (IP) injection of ketamine hydrochloride (50 mg/kg) plus xylazine (5 mg/kg). Then, each rat was placed in a stereotaxic apparatus (Stoelting Co, Illinois, USA), the skin was incised and the skull was cleaned. Next, two steel 22-gauge guide cannulae (0.7 mm diameter) were bilaterally implanted into the CA1 region according to the atlas of Paxinos and Watson (AP: -2 mm from bregma, L: ±1.6 from the sagittal suture and V: -1.5 mm from the skull surface) (18). Cannulae were secured by dental acrylic. Rats had 7 days to recover from stereotaxic surgery and the side effects of ketamine hydrochloride and xylazine (19).

REM sleep deprivation apparatus

To induce RSD in rats, we used a multiple platform apparatus. The rats were placed in a water tank (90×50×50 cm) that had several small transparent Plexiglas platforms with a diameter of 7 cm and a height of 10 cm. These platforms were used by rats to avoid getting wet and for sleeping. The water surrounded the platforms (2 cm lower than the platforms). The rats were placed on these platforms to avoid getting wet and to sleep. As soon as the rats went to the REM phase, their muscle tone was significantly attenuated, and they fell into the water and woke up. Within this apparatus, all rats had enough food and water (20).

Intra-CA1 administrations

For the intra-CA1 administrations, all rats were gently restrained by hand and 27-gauge injection needles were placed into the guide-cannula (1 mm below the tip of the guide cannula). All infusions were manually performed at a volume of 1 µl/rat (0.5 µl in each side) within 60 sec. Then, the rats were transferred to their cages (21).

Step-through inhibitory avoidance apparatus

Step-through inhibitory avoidance (IA) apparatus consisted of a trough-shaped alley which had two compartments separated by a sliding door: a dark compartment where the rats were shocked (20 × 20 × 30 cm) and a light compartment (20 × 20 × 30 cm). A sliding door (7×9 cm) was in the center of the partition between the dark and the light compartments. Foot shocks were produced by stainless steel grids (2.5 mm in diameter) which were placed at 1 cm intervals on the dark compartment’s floor. An insulated stimulator transferred periodic electric shocks (50 Hz, 3 sec, 0.1 mA intensity) to the grid floor of the dark compartment.

Behavioral procedures

Training

The training was performed according to our previous research (22-24). All rats had enough time to habituate in the lab. On the habituation day, each rat was placed only in the light compartment of the apparatus and given 30 sec to explore. Five seconds later, the sliding door was raised and opened and the rat could explore freely. Rats that waited more than 100 sec to enter the dark compartment were eliminated from the tests. When each rat entered the dark compartment with all four paws, the sliding door closed. Then, the rat received an inescapable foot shock (50 Hz, 3 sec, 0.1 mA intensity) and immediately afterward was removed from the apparatus (Habituation trial). Thirty mins later, the acquisition trial was performed. On acquisition day, each rat was placed only in the light compartment and had five sec to explore. Five sec later, the sliding door was raised. As soon as the rat completely entered the dark compartment, the sliding door was closed and the latency to enter was recorded. A footshock (50 Hz, 3 sec, 0.1 mA intensity) was transferred 3 sec after closing the door. Twenty sec after the footshock each rat was removed and placed gently into its cage. After 2 mins, the method was repeated. The training was ended when the rat stayed in the light compartment for 120 sec. The number of entries into the dark compartment (trials) was recorded. Each rat learned with a maximum of three trials.

Retrieval test

The retrieval test was carried out 24 hr after the training. On this day, each rat was placed into the light compartment for 20 sec, while the sliding door was raised. When the rat completely entered the dark compartment, the sliding door was closed, and the latency to enter the dark compartment was recorded. Then, each rat gently returned to its cage. A cut-off time of 300 sec was considered for this test; on the retrieval test day, no shock was delivered.

Locomotor activity evaluation

The locomotion apparatus (BorjSanatAzma Co, Tehran, Iran) included a clear perspex container box (30 cm × 30 cm × 40 cm high). This apparatus had a gray perspex panel (30 cm × 30 cm × 2.2 cm thick) with sixteen photocells. Photocells divided the box into sixteen equal-sized squares. The number of entries of each rat from one square to another square within five mins determined the locomotor activity.

Drugs

Clonidine (an agonist of α2 receptor) and yohimbine (an antagonist of α2 receptor) were procured from Tocris (Tocris Bioscience United Kingdom). All drugs were dissolved in the saline solution immediately prior to administrations at the doses of 0.001, 0.01, and 0.1 µg for each rat.

Statistical analysis

For evaluating the effects of post-training intra-CA1 infusions of clonidine and yohimbine, we used one-way ANOVA. Also, for evaluating the interaction effect between drugs and sleep deprivation, we used two-way ANOVA. Following a significant F value, further analyses for paired-group comparisons were performed using a post-hoc Turkey’s test. In all analyses, P<0.05 was considered statistically significant.

Experimental design

The effects of RSD on memory consolidation and locomotor activity with or without yohimbine/clonidine

In this test, 12 groups of rats were used. All rats were treated with RSD for 0, 12, 24, and 36 hr. The rats received intra-CA1 administration of saline (saline 1 µl/rat; four groups), subthreshold dose of yohimbine (0.001 µg/rat; four groups), and subthreshold dose of clonidine (0.1 µg/rat; four groups) immediately after training.

Results

The effects of RSD on memory consolidation and locomotor activity with or without yohimbine/clonidine

One-way ANOVA and post-hoc analysis revealed that RSD for 24 and 36 but not 12 hr decreased memory consolidation [F (3, 28) = 32.21, P<0.001, Figure 1A], even though it did not influence locomotor activity [F (3, 28) =2.4, P>0.05, Figure 1B] by itself.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

The effect of rapid eye movement sleep deprivation (RSD) on memory consolidation (panel A) and locomotion (panel B) with or without yohimbine or clonidine. Each group (saline 1 µl/rat, yohimbine 0.001 µg/rat, clonidine 0.1 µg/rat) consisted of four sub-groups which were treated with RSD at different hours (0, 12, 24, and 36). Data are expressed as Mean±SEM of eight rats in each group. ***P<0.001 in comparison with saline 1 µl/rat + 0 hr RSD. +P<0.05 and +++P< 0.001 in comparison with respective sub-groups of the saline group

Two-way ANOVA showed that subthreshold dose of yohimbine (0.001 μg/rat) did not influence dose-response curve induced by RSD-treated rats on memory consolidation [treatment effect: F (1, 56) = 3.24, P>0.05; dose effect: F (3, 56) = 23.4, P<0.001; treatment and dose interaction effect: F (3, 56) =2.9, P>0.05, Figure 1A] and locomotor activity [treatment effect: F (1, 56) =0.661, P>0.05; dose effect: F (3, 56) = 4.4, P>0.05; treatment and dose interaction effect: F (3, 56) = 1.18, P> 0.05, Figure 1B].

Two-way ANOVA also showed that subthreshold dose of clonidine (0.1 μg/rat) altered response curve induced by RSD on memory consolidation [treatment effect: F (1, 56) =8.64, P<0.001; dose effect: F (3, 56) =45.24, P<0.001; treatment and dose interaction effect: F (3, 56) =5.24, P<0.05, Figure 1A], while it did not alter locomotor activity [treatment effect F (1, 56) =0.572, P>0.05; dose effect: F (3, 56) =3.62, P>0.05; treatment and dose interaction effect: F (3, 56) =2.62, P>0.05, Figure 1B]. Post hoc analysis revealed that clonidine restored memory consolidation impairment induced by RSD (24 and 36 hr).

Discussion

In line with previous studies, the results of this research affirmed that RSD impairs memory consolidation (3, 25, 26). Sleep disturbances impair different types of learning and memory in animal models, such as passive avoidance learning (27). Therefore, from a functional point of view, RSD has negative effects on various cognitive performances, especially attention, working memory, decision making, and long-term memory (7, 8). Previous studies have reported that RSD for 24 hr disrupts memory formation in inhibitory passive avoidance task (4, 28). Another study has also revealed that SD for 72 hr leads to passive avoidance retention deficit (29). It’s important to note that, SD induces oxidative stress via an increase in lipid peroxidation and nitric oxide, and via reducing the glutathione level (29). Oxidative stress interferes with important hippocampal functional roles and induces cognitive decline, including memory deficit (30). Also, increased oxidative stress in the hippocampus may have a role in the impairment of spatial memory retention (31). The side effects of oxidative stress are more prominent in the hippocampus than in the cortex. This effect reflects the great vulnerability of this brain area in comparison with the other brain regions (32).

We reported that intra-CA1 infusion of a subthreshold dose of yohimbine did not alter memory consolidation impairment induced by RSD. On the contrary, intra-CA1 infusion of a subthreshold dose of clonidine restored RSD-induced amnesia (24 and 36 hr). None of the previous interventions influenced locomotor activity. Our previous study has shown that both clonidine and yohimbine (only in 24 hr) restore memory retention impairment induced by total sleep deprivation (TSD) in rats. It has been revealed that α2-adrenoreceptors are associated with synaptic plasticity, learning, and memory (28). Clonidine, the α2-adrenergic agonist, has shown its enhancement effect on learning and memory in different studies (33, 34). The activation of neurons in the LC region undergoes certain rhythms that are significantly associated with the sleep/wakefulness cycle. These neurons fire at the highest rates when animals are active and awake, and fire at the lower rates when animals are in both non-REM and REM phases of sleep (8). Therefore, synaptic plasticity would be increased if synaptic activation synchronously enhanced in the hippocampus with elevating the spiking rate of LC neurons; for example, synaptic plasticity seems to occur more easily within active waking periods. However, a previous study showed that sleep is significantly involved in the consolidation of declarative memory (35) because SD potently impairs long-term synaptic plasticity in the rats’ hippocampus (36).

Noradrenaline may have a role in enhancing memory via an increase in general arousal (37, 38). It has been shown that noradrenaline has the main role in memory encoding and consolidation via pathways from the basolateral amygdala (BLA) to the entorhinal cortex, hippocampus, or anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) (39). On the other hand, noradrenaline is not only restricted to the amygdala, but also influences many brain areas including neocortical and hippocampal regions, and has interactions with other neurotransmitters, such as acetylcholine (ACh) (40, 41). It has been suggested that these effects of noradrenaline may be correlated with its ability to stabilize early LTP and transform it into late LTP (42).

As discovered, the LC fires tonically during wakefulness and induces stimulus-related bursts during activity (37, 43). This tonic rate is related to the alertness level. During the non-REM phase, the tonic rate of LC is slowly attenuated with sleep depth. Within the REM phase, LC neurons cease firing altogether (44, 45); however, during phasic slow-wave sleep which contains sleep spindles and slow oscillations, LC neurons exhibit burst firing activity (45). The clonidine’s restoration effect on RSD-induced memory deficit is unclear and needs further investigations, but it seems that clonidine can reverse the impairment effect of both TSD and RSD on memory formation. In addition, the role of clonidine in regulating glutamatergic transmissions through stimulation of G-protein-coupled adrenergic receptors may have a role. Although further studies are needed, the present research is an important step in investigating the role of CA1 α2 adrenoreceptor activation in RSD-induced amnesia.

Conclusion

Our results showed that RSD for 24 and 36 hr decreased memory consolidation. It seems that oxidative stress following RSD has the main role in this impairment effect. Yohimbine did not alter memory function, but clonidine restored the impairment effect of RSD on memory formation. The firing rate of LC neurons during wakefulness, Non-REM and REM sleep, and also, modulating the efficacy of glutamatergic transmissions through activation of G-protein-coupled alpha-2 adrenergic receptors by clonidine may lead to the restoration effect of clonidine on RSD-induced memory consolidation deficit.

Acknowledgment

There was no financial support provided for this project.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare that they have no conflicts of interest.

References

  • 1.Eydipour Z, Nasehi M, Vaseghi S, Jamaldini SH, Zarrindast MR. The role of 5-HT4 serotonin receptors in the CA1 hippocampal region on memory acquisition impairment induced by total (TSD) and REM sleep deprivation (RSD) Physiol Behav. 2020;215:112788. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2019.112788. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Javad-Moosavi BZ, Nasehi M, Vaseghi S, Jamaldini SH, Zarrindast MR. Activation and inactivation of nicotinic receptnors in the dorsal hippocampal region restored negative effects of total (TSD) and REM sleep deprivation (RSD) on memory acquisition, locomotor activity and pain perception. Neuroscience. 2020;433:200–211. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2020.03.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Norozpour Y, Nasehi M, Sabouri-Khanghah V, Torabi-Nami M, Zarrindast MR. The effect of CA1 alpha2 adrenergic receptors on memory retention deficit induced by total sleep deprivation and the reversal of circadian rhythm in a rat model. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2016;133:53–60. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2016.06.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Javad-Moosavi BZ, Vaezi G, Nasehi M, Haeri-Rouhani SA, Zarrindast MR. Critical role of CA1 muscarinic receptors on memory acquisition deficit induced by total (TSD) and REM sleep deprivation (RSD) Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2017;79:128–135. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2017.05.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.de Bruin EJ, van Run C, Staaks J, Meijer AM. Effects of sleep manipulation on cognitive functioning of adolescents: A systematic review. Sleep Med Rev. 2016;32:45–57. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2016.02.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Havekes R, Vecsey CG, Abel T. The impact of sleep deprivation on neuronal and glial signaling pathways important for memory and synaptic plasticity. Cell Signal. 2012;24:1251–1260. doi: 10.1016/j.cellsig.2012.02.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Ertugrul A, Rezaki M. The neurobiology of sleep and its influence on memory. Turk Psikiyatri Derg. 2004;15:300–308. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Landmann N, Kuhn M, Maier JG, Spiegelhalder K, Baglioni C, Frase L, et al. REM sleep and memory reorganization: Potential relevance for psychiatry and psychotherapy. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2015;122:28–40. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2015.01.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Kordestani-Moghadam P, Nasehi M, Khodagholi F, Vaseghi S, Zarrindast MR, Khani M. The fluctuations of metabotropic glutamate receptor subtype 5 (mGluR5) in the amygdala in fear conditioning model of male Wistar rats following sleep deprivation, reverse circadian and napping. Brain Res. 2020;1734:146739. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2020.146739. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Kim Y, Elmenhorst D, Weisshaupt A, Wedekind F, Kroll T, McCarley RW, et al. Chronic sleep restriction induces long-lasting changes in adenosine and noradrenaline receptor density in the rat brain. J Sleep Res. 2015;24:549–558. doi: 10.1111/jsr.12300. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Uschakov A, Grivel J, Cvetkovic-Lopes V, Bayer L, Bernheim L, Jones BE, et al. Sleep-deprivation regulates alpha-2 adrenergic responses of rat hypocretin/orexin neurons. PLoS One. 2011;6:e16672. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0016672. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Sirvio J, MacDonald E. Central alpha1-adrenoceptors: their role in the modulation of attention and memory formation. Pharmacol Ther. 1999;83:49–65. doi: 10.1016/s0163-7258(99)00017-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Valizadegan F, Oryan S, Nasehi M, Zarrindast MR. Interaction between morphine and noradrenergic system of basolateral amygdala on anxiety and memory in the elevated plus-maze test based on a test-retest paradigm. Arch Iran Med. 2013;16:281–287. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Kim Y, Chen L, McCarley RW, Strecker RE. Sleep allostasis in chronic sleep restriction: the role of the norepinephrine system. Brain Res. 2013;1531:9–16. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2013.07.048. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.MacDonald E, Kobilka BK, Scheinin M. Gene targeting--homing in on alpha 2-adrenoceptor-subtype function. Trends Pharmacol Sci. 1997;18:211–219. doi: 10.1016/s0165-6147(97)01063-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Kable JW, Murrin LC, Bylund DB. In vivo gene modification elucidates subtype-specific functions of alpha(2)-adrenergic receptors. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 2000;293:1–7. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Lee MG, Hassani OK, Jones BE. Discharge of identified orexin/hypocretin neurons across the sleep-waking cycle. J Neurosci. 2005;25:6716–6720. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1887-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Paxinos G, Watson C, Pennisi M, Topple A. Bregma, lambda and the interaural midpoint in stereotaxic surgery with rats of different sex, strain and weight. J Neurosci Methods. 1985;13:139–143. doi: 10.1016/0165-0270(85)90026-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Vaseghi S, Babapour V, Nasehi M, Zarrindast MR. Synergistic but not additive effect between ACPA and lithium in the dorsal hippocampal region on spatial learning and memory in rats: Isobolographic analyses. Chem Biol Interact. 2019;315 doi: 10.1016/j.cbi.2019.108895. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Machado RB, Hipolide DC, Benedito-Silva AA, Tufik S. Sleep deprivation induced by the modified multiple platform technique: quantification of sleep loss and recovery. Brain Res. 2004;1004:45–51. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2004.01.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Naseri MH, Hesami-Tackallou S, Torabi-Nami M, Zarrindast MR, Nasehi M. Involvement of the CA1 GABAA receptors in MK-801-induced anxiolytic-like effects: an isobologram analysis. Behav Pharmacol. 2014;25:197–205. doi: 10.1097/FBP.0000000000000037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Nasehi M, Sahebgharani M, Haeri-Rohani A, Zarrindast MR. Effects of cannabinoids infused into the dorsal hippocampus upon memory formation in 3-days apomorphine-treated rats. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2009;92:391–399. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2009.05.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Zarrindast MR, Farajzadeh Z, Rostami P, Rezayof A, Nourjah P. Involvement of the ventral tegmental area (VTA) in morphine-induced memory retention in morphine-sensitized rats. Behav Brain Res. 2005;163:100–106. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2005.04.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Malboosi N, Nasehi M, Hashemi M, Vaseghi S, Zarrindast MR. The neuroprotective effect of NeuroAid on morphine-induced amnesia with respect to the expression of TFAM, PGC-1alpha, DeltafosB and CART genes in the hippocampus of male Wistar rats. Gene. 2020;742:144601. doi: 10.1016/j.gene.2020.144601. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Torabi-Nami M, Nasehi M, Zarrindast MR. Sleep loss and the brain vulnerability to neurodegeneration: behavioral, biochemical and neurohistopathological observations in a rat model. EXCLI J. 2013;12:347–372. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Dorokhov VB, Kozhedub RG, Arsen’ev GN, Kozhechkin SN, Ukraintseva Iu V, Kulikov MA, et al. [Sleep deprivation effect upon spatial memory consolidation in rats after one-day learning in a Morris water maze] Zh Vyssh Nerv Deiat Im I P Pavlova. 2011;61:322–331. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Bridoux A, Laloux C, Derambure P, Bordet R, Monaca Charley C. The acute inhibition of rapid eye movement sleep by citalopram may impair spatial learning and passive avoidance in mice. J Neural Transm. 2013;120:383–389. doi: 10.1007/s00702-012-0901-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Ferry B, McGaugh JL. Involvement of basolateral amygdala alpha2-adrenoceptors in modulating consolidation of inhibitory avoidance memory. Learn Mem. 2008;15:238–243. doi: 10.1101/lm.760908. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Silva RH, Abilio VC, Takatsu AL, Kameda SR, Grassl C, Chehin AB, et al. Role of hippocampal oxidative stress in memory deficits induced by sleep deprivation in mice. Neuropharmacology. 2004;46:895–903. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2003.11.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Alzoubi KH, Rawashdeh NQ, Khabour OF, El-Elimat T, Albataineh H, Al-Zghool HM, et al. Evaluation of the effect of Moringa peregrina extract on learning and memory: Role of oxidative stress. J Mol Neurosci. 2017;63:355–363. doi: 10.1007/s12031-017-0986-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Liu YZ, Chen JK, Li ZP, Zhao T, Ni M, Li DJ, et al. High-salt diet enhances hippocampal oxidative stress and cognitive impairment in mice. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2014;114:10–15. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Bridoux A, Laloux C, Derambure P, Bordet R, Monaca Charley C. The acute inhibition of rapid eye movement sleep by citalopram may impair spatial learning and passive avoidance in mice. J Neural Transm (Vienna) 2013;120:383–389. doi: 10.1007/s00702-012-0901-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Beiranvand A, Nasehi M, Zarrindast MR, Moghaddasi M. Involvement of medial prefrontal cortex alpha-2 adrenoceptors on memory acquisition deficit induced by arachidonylcyclopropylamide, a cannabinoid CB1 receptor agonist, in rats; possible involvement of Ca2+ channels. J Psychopharmacol. 2016;30:945–954. doi: 10.1177/0269881116652585. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Li Y, Yu M, Zhao B, Wang Y, Zha Y, Li Z, et al. Clonidine preconditioning improved cerebral ischemia-induced learning and memory deficits in rats via ERK1/2-CREB/ NF-kappaB-NR2B pathway. Eur J Pharmacol. 2018;818:167–173. doi: 10.1016/j.ejphar.2017.10.041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Ellenbogen JM, Payne JD, Stickgold R. The role of sleep in declarative memory consolidation: passive, permissive, active or none? Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2006;16:716–722. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2006.10.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Ishikawa A, Kanayama Y, Matsumura H, Tsuchimochi H, Ishida Y, Nakamura S. Selective rapid eye movement sleep deprivation impairs the maintenance of long-term potentiation in the rat hippocampus. Eur J Neurosci. 2006;24:243–248. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.04874.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Aston-Jones G, Cohen JD. Adaptive gain and the role of the locus coeruleus-norepinephrine system in optimal performance. J Comp Neurol. 2005;493:99–110. doi: 10.1002/cne.20723. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Gibbs ME, Summers RJ. Role of adrenoceptor subtypes in memory consolidation. Prog Neurobiol. 2002;67:345–391. doi: 10.1016/s0301-0082(02)00023-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Roozendaal B, McEwen BS, Chattarji S. Stress, memory and the amygdala. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2009;10:423–433. doi: 10.1038/nrn2651. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Izquierdo I, Medina JH, Izquierdo LA, Barros DM, de Souza MM, Mello e Souza T. Short- and long-term memory are differentially regulated by monoaminergic systems in the rat brain. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 1998;69:219–224. doi: 10.1006/nlme.1998.3825. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Bear MF, Singer W. Modulation of visual cortical plasticity by acetylcholine and noradrenaline. Nature. 1986;320:172–176. doi: 10.1038/320172a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Frey S, Bergado-Rosado J, Seidenbecher T, Pape HC, Frey JU. Reinforcement of early long-term potentiation (early-LTP) in dentate gyrus by stimulation of the basolateral amygdala: heterosynaptic induction mechanisms of late-LTP. J Neurosci. 2001;21:3697–3703. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-10-03697.2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Berridge CW, Waterhouse BD. The locus coeruleus-noradrenergic system: modulation of behavioral state and state-dependent cognitive processes. Brain Res Brain Res Rev. 2003;42:33–84. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0173(03)00143-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Pace-Schott EF, Hobson JA. The neurobiology of sleep: genetics, cellular physiology and subcortical networks. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2002;3:591–605. doi: 10.1038/nrn895. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Aston-Jones G, Bloom FE. Activity of norepinephrine-containing locus coeruleus neurons in behaving rats anticipates fluctuations in the sleep-waking cycle. J Neurosci. 1981;1:876–886. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.01-08-00876.1981. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Iranian Journal of Basic Medical Sciences are provided here courtesy of Mashhad University of Medical Sciences

RESOURCES