Skip to main content
Springer logoLink to Springer
. 2020 Aug 4;268(1):178–188. doi: 10.1007/s00415-020-10125-w

Early discrimination of cognitive motor dissociation from disorders of consciousness: pitfalls and clues

Alessandro Pincherle 1,2,✉,#, Frederic Rossi 1,#, Jane Jöhr 1, Vincent Dunet 3, Philippe Ryvlin 1, Mauro Oddo 4, Nicolas Schiff 5, Karin Diserens 1
PMCID: PMC7815538  PMID: 32754829

Abstract

Bedside assessment of consciousness and awareness after a severe brain injury might be hampered by confounding clinical factors (i.e., pitfalls) interfering with the production of behavioral or motor responses to external stimuli. Despite the use of validated clinical scales, a high misdiagnosis rate is indeed observed. We retrospectively analyzed a cohort of 49 patients with severe brain injury admitted to an acute neuro-rehabilitation program. Patients’ behavior was assessed using the Motor Behavior Tool and Coma Recovery Scale Revised. All patients underwent systematic assessment for pitfalls including polyneuropathy and/or myopathy and/or myelopathy, major cranial nerve palsies, non-convulsive status epilepticus, aphasia (expressive or comprehensive), cortical blindness, thalamic involvement and frontal akinetic syndrome. A high prevalence (75%) of pitfalls potentially interfering with sensory afference (polyneuropathy, myopathy, myelopathy, and sensory aphasia), motor efference (polyneuropathy, myopathy, motor aphasia, and frontal akinetic syndrome), and intrinsic brain activity (thalamic involvement and epilepsy) was found. Nonetheless, the motor behavior tool identified residual cognition (i.e. a cognitive motor dissociation condition) regardless of the presence of these pitfalls in 70% of the patients diagnosed as unresponsive using the Coma Recovery Scale Revised. On one hand, pitfalls might contribute to misdiagnosis. On the other, it could be argued that they are clues for diagnosing cognitive motor dissociation rather than true disorders of consciousness given their prominent effect on the sensory–motor input–output balance.

Keywords: Disorders of consciousness, Cognitive motor dissociation, Brain injury, Motor behavior tool, Coma Recovery Scale

Introduction

One of the main challenges in neurology, often implying serious ethical consequences, is the reliable bedside clinical identification of consciousness in patients with severe brain injury [1, 2]. Functional neuroimaging can help detect residual cognitive function and awareness in some patients who appear entirely unresponsive at the bedside [3], pointing to the so-called cognitive motor dissociation (CMD) condition. This approach has, however, a limited feasibility in the neuro-intensive-care setting. Consequently, assessment of motor/verbal/visual responses to external stimuli remains the current standard in clinical practice [4], with the Coma Recovery Scale Revised (CRS-R) regarded as the gold-standard for diagnosis of disorders of consciousness (DOC) [5]. However, despite the use of validated clinical scales, an unacceptably high rate of clinical misdiagnosis is observed [6].

We recently developed a simple bedside clinical tool, the motor behavior tool (MBT) revised to its current form (MBT-r), to refine the evaluation of residual cognition in patients at an early stage after severe brain injury [7, 8]. We found that the MBT detected signs of residual cognition in almost two-thirds of patients who had been classified as unconscious using the CRS-R, suggesting that the MBT can identify a subgroup of patients with blocked motor efference/output rather than a true consciousness disorder. The high percentage of these recognized patients suggests the presence of common confounding factors that mask their accurate diagnosis, similar to the many specific factors that hamper accurate assessment of brain death exams [9]. Indeed, several factors including co-morbidities/complications that interfere with the production of appropriate behavioral or motor responses to external stimuli might affect clinical examination in the neuro-critical care setting. Moreover, patients with severe brain injury suffer from neurological deficits that often render any movement slight and inconsistent and patients become easily fatigued. These obstacles to behavioral evaluation might lead to the erroneous diagnosis of DOC.

Here, we aimed to describe the confounding clinical factors that might interfere with the clinical recognition of consciousness, and thus increase the uncertainty of clinical evaluation. Specifically, we sought to: (a) identify and describe confounding clinical factors (here defined as pitfalls) that interfere with clinical/behavioral evaluation, (b) assess the effects of these pitfalls on the diagnostic classification according to CRS-R and MBT scales, and (c) suggest simple clinical and paraclinical clues to help overcome pitfalls, which hinder recognition of residual cognition.

Methods

Patients

The local ethical committee approved this retrospective cohort study (Ref. 142-09). We consecutively enrolled 49 patients (26 men, aged 51.8 ± 19.5 years) who were admitted to the Lausanne University Hospital Acute Neuro-rehabilitation Unit between June 2012 and December 2017 after severe brain injury (traumatic and non-traumatic injuries, including vascular, anoxic, encephalopathic, and neoplastic disorders) requiring the initial treatment in the neuro-intensive-care unit (Table 1). We excluded patients with the following conditions at the time of behavioral assessments: hypo- and hyper-glycemia (70 and 200 mg/dL, respectively); hypo- and hyper-natremia (133 and 150 mmol/L, respectively); renal or liver failure.

Table 1.

Patients demographics and clinical characteristics

ID Sex Age Etiology Time to clinical appraisal (days) Time to rehab (days) Rehab stay (days) CRS-R diagnosis MBT diagnosis Negative MBT signs Pitfalls
1 F 17 Traumatic 20 33 34 Coma/UWS True DOC Yes Aphasia
2 M 17 Traumatic 15 18 14 MCS/EMCS CMD No NCSE
3 M 37 Anoxic 8 17 26 Coma/UWS True DOC Yes None
4 F 74 Traumatic 5 17 17 Coma/UWS CMD No Frontal Akinetic Syndrome
5 F 78 Vascular 6 26 21 MCS/EMCS CMD No Frontal Akinetic Syndrome
6 F 67 Traumatic 13 19 15 Coma/UWS CMD No None
7 F 40 Vascular 3 26 33 Coma/UWS True DOC No Thalamic Involvement
8 M 78 Infectious 19 48 43 Coma/UWS CMD No NCSE
9 M 59 Vascular 8 29 29 Coma/UWS CMD No Neuropathy–Myopathy
10 M 18 Traumatic 15 28 41 Coma/UWS True DOC Yes None
11 F 54 Vascular 14 23 41 Coma/UWS True DOC Yes None
12 M 45 Traumatic 11 31 9 Coma/UWS CMD No Neuropathy–Myopathy
13 F 67 Vascular 18 26 22 Coma/UWS CMD No Frontal Akinetic Syndrome
14 M 24 Traumatic 27 38 30 Coma/UWS CMD No None
15 F 58 Traumatic 13 38 20 MCS/EMCS CMD No NCSE
16 F 54 Traumatic 28 21 34 Coma/UWS CMD Yes Aphasia
17 M 24 Vascular 9 18 35 Coma/UWS True DOC Yes None
18 M 27 Traumatic 14 18 14 Coma/UWS CMD No Frontal Akinetic Syndrome
19 F 65 Anoxic 9 31 20 Coma/UWS CMD No Neuropathy–Myopathy
20 M 21 Traumatic 5 12 21 Coma/UWS CMD No None
21 F 23 Traumatic 20 33 8 Coma/UWS True DOC Yes Aphasia
22 M 50 Anoxic 15 22 50 MCS/EMCS CMD No Neuropathy–Myopathy
23 M 43 Traumatic 10 19 35 Coma/UWS CMD No None
24 F 84 Traumatic 3 21 19 Coma/UWS CMD No Neuropathy–Myopathy
25 M 80 Traumatic 2 10 32 Coma/UWS CMD No Frontal Akinetic Syndrome
26 M 43 Vascular 18 34 20 Coma/UWS CMD No Aphasia
27 M 73 Traumatic 23 28 29 Coma/UWS CMD No Aphasia
28 F 37 Traumatic 8 49 41 Coma/UWS True DOC Yes Thalamic Involvement
29 M 34 Traumatic 8 15 33 Coma/UWS CMD No Aphasia
30 F 61 Vascular 12 19 56 Coma/UWS CMD No Aphasia
31 M 61 Vascular 12 26 41 Coma/UWS CMD No Aphasia
32 F 69 Toxic-Metabolic 24 36 64 MCS/EMCS CMD No Neuropathy–Myopathy
33 F 62 Vascular 6 51 33 MCS/EMCS CMD No Aphasia
34 F 66 Traumatic 4 19 39 Coma/UWS CMD No Frontal Akinetic Syndrome
35 M 35 Toxic-Metabolic 17 17 27 Coma/UWS True DOC No None
36 M 42 Traumatic 16 23 22 Coma/UWS CMD No NCSE
37 F 75 Vascular 11 25 9 MCS/EMCS CMD No Aphasia
38 M 61 Vascular 3 15 47 Coma/UWS CMD No Frontal Akinetic Syndrome
39 M 73 Traumatic 11 25 59 Coma/UWS CMD No Frontal Akinetic Syndrome
40 F 35 Traumatic 18 33 33 Coma/UWS True Yes None
41 F 42 Vascular 5 13 34 Coma/UWS CMD No Aphasia
42 M 55 Vascular 1 24 36 MCS/EMCS CMD No Frontal Akinetic Syndrome
43 M 59 Anoxic 20 26 20 Coma/UWS True DOC Yes None
44 F 24 Traumatic 6 15 24 MCS/EMCS CMD No Frontal Akinetic Syndrome
45 M 71 Anoxic 12 61 41 MCS/EMCS CMD No None
46 F 52 Vascular 6 19 31 MCS/EMCS CMD No Aphasia
47 F 72 Vascular 2 23 28 Coma/UWS CMD No Frontal Akinetic Syndrome
48 M 73 Vascular 7 14 43 Coma/UWS CMD No Frontal Akinetic Syndrome
49 M 63 Vascular 18 34 48 MCS/EMCS CMD No Frontal Akinetic Syndrome

CRS-R Coma Recovery Scale Revised, MBT motor behavior tool, MCS minimally conscious state, UWS unresponsive wakefulness syndrome, EMCS emergence from minimally conscious state, CMD cognitive motor dissociation, DOC disorder of consciousness, NCSE non-convulsive status epilepticus

Clinicalandimagingassessment

Patients underwent neurobehavioral assessment as early as 48 h after therapeutic sedation withdrawal and within 30 days of the brain injury using the French version of the CRS-R based on the existing guidelines [10, 11] complemented by the MBT [7, 8]. As previously described in detail in [8], the MBT brings out clinical signs of preserved conscious awareness by means of careful evaluation and scoring of subtle motor behavior not adequately identified using the CRS-R only, due to severe motor defects. The MBT uses a simple dichotomous scoring method, validated in a blinded study [8], to identify “positive” signs of residual cognition/conscious awareness and “negative” signs of possible brain-stem dysfunction (Table 2). Patients showing signs of residual cognition at MBT are classified as having “clinical CMD” condition.

Table 2.

The motor behavior tool revised

Item Notes/instructions
Positive signs
1 Spontaneous non-reflexive movements Observation of the patient without any stimulation. At least one non-reflexive movements defined as intentional motor pattern non-stereotypical, not contextualized and non-repetitive
2 Response to command Any scorable response to verbal command
3 Visual fixation or visual pursuit Any clearly discernible visual fixation or visual pursuit in any direction
4 Responses in a motivational context Any increased in the frequency of non-reflexive motor responses in a salient context (e.g., mother tongue, patient's own name)
5 Defensive non-reflexive response to a noxious stimulation—Nipple Twisting the patient's nipple while keeping the patient's healthier arm between the patient's body and the examiner's arm. Any attempt to push away the examiner's arm that is not a stereotypical posture involving extension and internal rotation of the arms
6 Defensive non-reflexive response to a noxious stimulation—Nail bed Deep pressure to nail beds of four extremities. Any limb movement whose kinematics differs from a motor reflex response in terms of orientation planes and the type of elicited muscles is scored as defensive.
7 Response to a noxious stimulation—Grimace Observation of at least one grimace during administration of noxious stimulation
Negative signs
8 Abnormal motor or neurovegetative responses to stimulation Observation of slow, stereotyped flexion or extension of the upper and/or lower extremities after noxious stimulation or neurovegetative responses (i.e., tachycardia, hypo/hyper-ventilation, hypertension, excessive sweating) to stimulation.
9 Signs of roving eyes or absence of oculocephalic reflex Slowly roving eyes movements are typical of metabolic encephalopathy indicating diffuse cerebral dysfunction. Oculocephalic responses imply intact brain-stem pathways

Patients were grouped as MCS/EMCS (minimally conscious state/emerging from the minimally conscious state) or coma/UWS (unresponsive wakefulness syndrome) based on the CRS-R and as potential clinical CMD (i.e., presenting signs of residual cognition) or true-DOC (i.e., presenting no signs of residual cognition) based on the MBT.

Patients underwent a morphological brain magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) during their hospitalization in the Intensive Care or Acute Neuro-rehabilitation Unit (time from admission to the Acute Neuro-rehabilitation Unit: median 8.0 days, range min–max: 0–42 days). MRI acquisitions included T1-weighted, T2-weighted or fluid attenuated inversion recovery, T2 gradient echo or susceptibility-weighted, and diffusion-weighted and T1-weighted images after intravenous injection of gadolinated contrast media.

A neuroradiologist (VD) blinded to the results of MBT/CRS-R assessments, performed a detailed evaluation of all morphological brain MR images. Lesions in frontal, temporal, parietal, occipital, basal ganglia, thalamus, hypothalamus, mesencephalon, pons, and cerebellum regions were identified bilaterally and recorded. Special attention was given to score lesions in areas strategic for vision (calcarine cortex [12, 13]), language (dominant fronto-temporo-parietal cortices involved in language [14, 15]), arousal control (thalamic involvement [16, 17]), motor planification and execution (frontal–subcortical circuitry [18]), and cranial nerve nuclei in the brain stem [19, 20].

Confoundingclinicalfactorsorpitfalls

We identified neurological deficits/co-morbidities (i.e. pitfalls), intrinsically related to the brain injury process and, which might potentially interfere with the ability to deliver motor and verbal responses to external stimulation during the neuro-behavioral assessment. The choice of these co-morbidities was reached from a review of the literature and discussion in focus groups with experts in intensive-care and severe brain injury management. The following seven conditions were identified as pitfalls: polyneuropathy and/or myopathy and/or myelopathy, major cranial nerve palsies, non-convulsive status epilepticus (NCSE), aphasia (expressive or comprehensive), cortical blindness, thalamic involvement, and frontal akinetic syndrome (Table 3 presents the extensive clinical/paraclinical criteria used to identify the pitfalls).

Table 3.

Pitfalls description

Comorbid condition/pitfall (references) Signs/clues to identify pitfalls
Clinical Para-clinical
Polyneuropathy or myopathy Areflexia, amyotrophia, flaccidity ENG/EMG—nerve conduction and electromyographic abnormalities
Cranial nerves palsies [19, 20] Cranial nerve palsies Imaging (MRI)—brain-stem nuclear or nerve lesions
Non convulsive status epilepticus Staring, eye deviations, neglect, myoclonus EEG—epileptic potentials
Cortical blindness [12, 13] Absence of visual interaction, absence of menace reflex, absence of visual pursuit Imaging (MRI)—bilateral occipital lesions
Akinetic (frontal) Syndrome [18] Marked reduction of spontaneous movement and speech production with inconstant visual fixation and tracking, inconstant command-following and vocalization Imaging (CT or MRI)—(Bi-) cortico-subcortical frontal, cerebellar, basal ganglia lesions
Thalamic involvement [16, 17] Intermittent vigilance fluctuations, short-lasting (i.e., seconds–minutes) easily reverted with sensory stimulation Imaging (MRI)—thalamic lesions or hypothalamic lesions
Aphasia [14, 15] Anarthric or mutic patient, absence of command-following Imaging (MRI or CT)—dominant sided parietal-fronto-temporal lesions

After the neuro-radiological assessment and according to the diagnostic criteria listed in Table 3, a neurologist (FR) not involved in MBT/CRS-R administration systematically reviewed all patient records to identify the presence of any of the seven pitfalls present at the time of MBT/CRS-R testing.

Statistical analysis

Statistical analyses were performed using the Jasp software. Continuous variables are presented as mean ± standard deviation or median (range min–max). Categorical variables are presented as number and percentage. We compared categorical variables using 2 × 2 contingency tables and the Chi-squared test with Fisher’s correction applied when necessary. We used the Student’s t test to compare the means of continuous/ordinal variables after verifying normal distribution.

Results were considered significant at p < 0.05.

Results

Residual cognition and diagnosis

Among the 49 enrolled patients, 23 had a traumatic, 18 a vascular, 5 an anoxic, 2 a toxic–metabolic, and 1 an infectious etiology. The mean time to CRS-R/MBT assessment after injury was 11 days (range 1–28), while mean time from admission to the acute rehabilitation unit was 25 days (range 10–61). The mean stay in the unit was 31 days (range 8–64).

Based on the CRS-R, 37 patients were diagnosed as coma/UWS and 12 MCS/EMCS. In contrast, using the MBT, 11 patients were diagnosed as true DOC (i.e., without signs of residual cognition) and 38 as clinical CMD (i.e., with signs of residual cognition).

Of the 37 patients classified as coma/UWS using the CRS-R, 26 (70%) showed signs of residual cognition based on the MBT. Furthermore, all 12 (100%) patients classified as MCS/EMCS regarding the CRS-R showed signs of residual cognition performing the MBT.

Negative MBT test signs (signs of possible brain-stem dysfunction) were identified in 10 patients (20%); only one of these patients was diagnosed as clinical CMD.

Confounding clinical factors/pitfalls

We observed pitfalls likely able to interfere with identification of behavioral signs of cognition in 37 patients (75%). Specifically, as pitfalls, we detected 13 akinetic frontal syndrome patients (26%), 12 aphasia (24%), 6 neuropathy/myopathy (12%), 4 NCSE (8%), and 2 patients with thalamic involvement (4%). None of the patients presented with cortical blindness or major cranial nerve dysfunction. A total of 15 patients (30%) had more than one pitfall.

Pitfalls and diagnostic classification

From the CRS-R classification, we noticed pitfalls in 11/12 (91%) of the patients with MCS/EMCS and 26/37 (70%) of the patients with coma/UWS (p = 0.13) (Fig. 1a).

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

a CRS-R classification and pitfall identification. On the left, the bar shows the number of Coma/UWS patients presenting with (light gray) or without (dark gray) pitfalls; the right bar shows the number of MCS/EMCS patients presenting with (light gray) and without (dark gray) pitfalls. b MBT classification and pitfall identification. On the left, the bar shows the number of true-DOC patients presenting with (light gray) and without (dark gray) pitfalls; the right bar shows the number of potential clinical CMD patients presenting with (light gray) and without (dark gray) pitfalls. UWS unresponsive wakefulness syndrome, MCS minimally conscious state, EMCS emergence from minimally conscious state, DOC disorder of consciousness, CMD cognitive motor dissociation

On the contrary, with the MBT classification, pitfalls were recognized in only 4/11 patients (36%) with true-DOC and 33/38 patients with potential clinical CMD (86%) (p < 0.001) (Fig. 1b).

Discussion

In this retrospective cohort study on patients with severe brain injuries admitted to an acute neuro-rehabilitation program, we found a high prevalence (75%) of patients with clinical conditions potentially interfering with a behavioral response to sensory stimulation. Despite the high predominance of these so-called pitfalls, we found that signs of residual cognition could be identified using the MBT in 70% of patients diagnosed as coma or UWS using the CRS-R. This indicates the ability of the MBT to identify subtle motor behavior and that MBT test sensitivity is unaffected by the high presence of pitfalls that interfere with sensory afference (polyneuropathy, myopathy, myelopathy, and sensory aphasia), motor efference (polyneuropathy, myopathy, motor aphasia, and frontal akinetic syndrome), or intrinsic brain activity (thalamic involvement and epilepsy).

Unlike the MBT, the CRS-R appeared to be affected by pitfalls when used at a very early phase. Specifically, the CRS-R, mainly based on residual motor output, might be insufficient to assess the cognitive ability to interact in patients with impaired motor efference/output, lesions affecting strategic functional areas of the central nervous system, or altered function of the peripheral nervous system. In line with this, it was suggested that performing at least five CRS-R assessments in patients with DOC within a 2-week time interval might reduce the misdiagnosis rate [21]. However, while performing repeated tests might be feasible in stabilized patients; it might not be suitable in the intensive-care setting where rapid decisions are required.

In this study, we found three pitfalls, frontal akinetic syndrome, aphasia, and polyneuropathy, which could, in theory, coexist in a single patient, present in 60% of our patient population. Therefore, they deserve further specific consideration.

Frontal akinetic syndrome, which shares some characteristics of akinetic mutism [2224], is a condition in which patients might appear outwardly attentive and vigilant (often shown by deliberate visual tracking), but exhibit a paucity of behavioral responses, even when prompted by external stimuli. The brain injury pattern most commonly associated with this syndrome is bilateral damage of the anterior medial regions of the frontal cortex. Notably, all the patients (13/13) in our cohort with this pitfall recovered consciousness at discharge and presented signs of cognition in the early MBT evaluation (mean days after injury, 7; range 1–18). Indeed, the MBT seemed to have sufficient sensitivity for subtle motor phenomena (such as grimaces, spontaneous movements, or defensive responses to pain) that might persist in patients with CMD. On the contrary, 9 out of the 13 patients with frontal akinetic syndrome were classified as unconscious (coma/UWS) using the CRS-R. Therefore, detailed imaging analysis aiming to identify frontal cortico-subcortical motor pathway disconnections [18] might be crucial to minimize residual cognition underestimation at the very early stage after brain injury.

We found that 18% of our patients had aphasia, which is consistent with the findings of a previous series [25]. The existence of overlooked language disorders might negatively interact with a behavioral response to verbal commands, leading to underestimation of the patient’s level of consciousness, especially when using scales like CRS-R that require persistent responses. The possibility of co-occurrence of aphasia and a disorder of consciousness should particularly be considered in the context of left hemisphere focal (cerebrovascular or post-traumatic) lesions in the dominant peri-sylvian area [14, 15]. The presence of focal brain lesions here, their extent and localization, can be easily confirmed by systematic structural brain imaging examination using computer tomography or MRI scans. Functional neuroimaging appears to be particularly promising for disentangling impaired consciousness and aphasia [25].

Previous studies reported that 40–80% of critically ill patients present with acute poly-neuro-myopathy [26, 27]. Under these circumstances, it might be difficult to clinically identify the absence of motor behavioral signs due to central causes. Therefore, it is important to carefully evaluate osteo-tendon reflexes and muscular tonus with complementary electroneuromyography to assess peripheral nerve and muscle function.

Earlier studies from our group suggested that the MBT, which detects subtle motor behaviors missed by the CRS-R, can clinically discriminate patients with potential clinical CMD from those with true DOC [7, 8, 28]. The MBT was reported to detect residual cognition at very early stages after brain injury [7, 8] and the present study showed consistent findings despite the presence of pitfalls. Our data, based on careful clinical observation, are in line with increasing evidence of residual brain activation in unresponsive patients detected using more complex electroencephalographic (EEG) or functional neuroimaging protocols [2931]. Indeed, although the original definition of CMD is based on this clinical/paraclinical discordance [3], there is still no gold-standard definition for CMD. Moreover, successful completion of complex behavioral paradigms requires considerable preservation of working memory, attention, and executive functions, such that the clinical spectrum of CMD is likely to be wide, including many patients who cannot perform these tasks reliably yet show behavioral evidence of awareness, akin to the patients identified in the present study. In this respect, considering pitfalls potentially able to interfere with command-following approaches is extremely relevant and might influence positively the detection rates of CMD by subsequent EEG or functional MRI protocols. Indeed, approaches based on direct measurements of cortico-cortical connectivity using transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS)-EEG have been proven to discriminate MCS from UWS [32, 33]. Furthermore, they are theoretically unaffected by pitfalls; however, TMS-EEG has only been assessed in chronic patients and its ability to detect covert residual cognition at an early phase and feasibility in the ICU setting remain unknown.

There are several theoretical arguments describing CMD and DOC as two separate entities. According to the meso-circuit hypothesis, CMD patients probably have some functional disturbance of the forebrain systems (frontal/prefrontal, cortical-striato-pallidal, and thalamocortical loop systems) associated with motor preparation and action [34], unlike the situation in locked-in subjects, producing both fluctuations in responsiveness and varying limitations of motor control. This hypothesis suggests, therefore, that pitfalls have a relevant and direct interference on sensory–motor output and intrinsic brain activity; indeed, they could be considered CMD features rather than confounding factors (Fig. 2). Contrastingly, only widespread cortico-thalamic damage/disconnection can produce true DOC [35, 36] based on the view that conscious states do not rely on a single cortical area or network, but rather require sustained, complex, and differentiated brain-scale communication defects [3739]. Consistent with this hypothesis, we found a higher prevalence of cortical laminar necrosis and diffuse axonal injuries [40, 41] among patients with DOC compared to patients with clinical CMD (data not shown). However, as we did not design this study to address imaging patterns, this finding should be regarded cautiously and requires further analysis. In addition, it should be recognized that hypoxic–ischemic injuries and diffuse axonal injury harbor subtypes more likely to end up as CMD although infrequent in demographic distribution. Examples include diffuse axonal injury with predominant radial component disrupting brain-stem axons [42] and hypoxic injuries producing dysfunction of the motor cortex or basal ganglia with preservation of global integrative brain function [43].

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

Pitfall interference on sensory-motor output and intrinsic brain activity. Several arguments point to distinguishing CMD as a separate entity from disorders of consciousness, the latter being more the expression of widespread cortico-thalamic dysregulation directly impairing cognition and awareness. LIS is characterized by preserved consciousness and cortical functions and a complete motor output disruption. CMD reflects rather a dysfunction of the motor planning cortico-subcortical circuitries, with pitfalls negatively interacting with sensory input and/or motor output. CMD cognitive motor dissociation, PNP polyneuropathy, UWS unresponsive wakefulness syndrome, MCS minimally conscious state, LIS locked-in syndrome, DOC disorders of consciousness

Another finding that supports the discrimination of CMD from DOC is also the observed between-group difference in the frequency of negative signs measured with the MBT (90% vs. 10%). Negative signs observed in MBT testing (roving eyes or absence of oculocephalic reflex and abnormal motor or neurovegetative responses to stimulation) were interpreted as indicators of major brain-stem involvement. The higher prevalence of negative signs from the MBT among patients with DOC is expected given the role of the brain-stem arousal system as the physiological foundation of forebrain arousal control, the base of classical and modern DOC patho-physiology [44, 45].

The present study has several limitations. First, although supported by repeated observations [7, 8, 28], we categorized patients as presenting with potential clinical CMD only on clinical observations from MBT rating. We did not perform active mental-imagery tasks according to the acknowledged operational definition of CMD to confirm our clinical diagnosis [4]. We have, however, previously shown that multisensory processing of the peri-personal space (the multisensory-motor space immediately surrounding the body) was preserved in putative (based on MBT) CMD but not DOC patients [46]. Further studies are warranted to provide an objective measure of covert awareness/residual signs of cognition combining functional MRI or EEG testing and the MBT. Second, we did not validate the identification of pitfalls in an independent study. However, we based their identification on literature-based (see Table 3 for details) clinical/paraclinical criteria routinely used in our institution; furthermore, the longitudinal observation during the hospital stay allowed us to confirm clinically the presence of pitfalls putatively identified at the very early stage after brain injury using paraclinical tests. We, therefore, believe that lack of validation of our pitfall criteria did not affect our results. Third, the unbalanced sample in terms of etiology (high prevalence of traumatic and vascular compared to other etiologies) might limit the generalization of our findings to different populations. A preliminary analysis of our cohort did not show any difference (data not shown), but a larger sample is needed to stratify the results according to different etiological groups.

Conclusions

We found a high prevalence of pitfalls that could potentially interfere with production of behavioral signs when performing clinical diagnosis of DOC. Furthermore, our findings indicate that the MBT can discriminate patients with clinical CMD from those with true DOC regardless of the presence of pitfalls. A systematic global clinical screening of pitfalls (including a critical structural imaging revision to reveal potential pitfalls) is needed in the quest to minimize misdiagnosis in cases of suspected DOC. On one hand, pitfalls could contribute to misdiagnosis. On the other hand, they may point towards a potential clinical CMD diagnosis given their prominent effect on the sensory–motor input–output balance.

Acknowledgements

Open access funding provided by University of Lausanne. We would like to thank the Foundation Biaggi for supporting this research project.

Author contributions

AP, FR, JJ, PR, MO, NS, and KD have contributed to the conception and design of the study; AP, JJ, VD, and FR contributed to the acquisition and analysis of data; AP, FR, VD, and JJ contributed to drafting the text and preparing the figures.

Compliance with ethical standards

Conflicts of interest

The authors declare that they have no competing interest.

Ethics approval

This retrospective cohort study was approved by the local ethics committee review board (Ref. 142-09).

Footnotes

Alessandro Pincherle and Frederic Rossi have contributed equally.

References

  • 1.Rohaut B, Claassen J. Decision making in perceived devastating brain injury: a call to explore the impact of cognitive biases. Br J Anaesth. 2018;120:5–9. doi: 10.1016/j.bja.2017.11.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Turgeon AF, Dorrance K, Archambault P, Lauzier F, Lamontagne F, Zarychanski R, et al. Factors influencing decisions by critical care physicians to withdraw life-sustaining treatments in critically ill adult patients with severe traumatic brain injury. CMAJ. 2019;191:E652–E663. doi: 10.1503/cmaj.190154. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Schiff ND. Cognitive motor dissociation following severe brain injuries. JAMA Neurol. 2015;72:1413–1415. doi: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2015.2899. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Giacino JT, Katz DI, Schiff ND, Whyte J, Ashman EJ, Ashwal S, et al. Practice guideline update recommendations summary: disorders of consciousness: report of the guideline development, dissemination, and implementation Subcommittee of the American Academy of Neurology; the American Congress of Rehabilitation Medicine; and the National Institute on Disability, Independent Living, and Rehabilitation Research. Neurology. 2018;91:450–460. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000005926. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Seel RT, Sherer M, Whyte J, Katz DI, Giacino JT, Rosenbaum AM, et al. Assessment scales for disorders of consciousness: evidence-based recommendations for clinical practice and research. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2010;91:1795–1813. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2010.07.218. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Kondziella D, Friberg CK, Frokjaer VG, Fabricius M, Moller K. Preserved consciousness in vegetative and minimal conscious states: systematic review and meta-analysis. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2016;87:485–492. doi: 10.1136/jnnp-2015-310958. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Pignat J-M, Mauron E, Johr J, Gilart de Keranflech C, Van De Ville D, Preti MG, et al. Outcome prediction of consciousness disorders in the acute stage based on a complementary motor behavioural tool. PLoS One. 2016;11:0156882. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0156882. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Pincherle A, Johr J, Chatelle C, Pignat J-M, Du Pasquier R, Ryvlin P, et al. Motor behavior unmasks residual cognition in disorders of consciousness. Ann Neurol. 2019;85:443–447. doi: 10.1002/ana.25417. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Posner JB, Saper CB, Schiff ND, Claassen J. Plum and Posner’s diagnosis and treatment of stupor and coma. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2019. [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Giacino JT, Kalmar K, Whyte J. The JFK coma recovery scale-revised: measurement characteristics and diagnostic utility. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2004;85:2020–2029. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2004.02.033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Schnakers C, Majerus S, Giacino J, Vanhaudenhuyse A, Bruno M-A, Boly M, et al. A French validation study of the coma recovery scale-revised (CRS-R) Brain Inj. 2008;22:786–792. doi: 10.1080/02699050802403557. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Zucco GM, Priftis K, Stevenson RJ. From blindsight to blindsmell: a mini review. Transl Neurosci. 2015;6:8–12. doi: 10.1515/tnsci-2015-0002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Tong F. Primary visual cortex and visual awareness. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2003;4:219–229. doi: 10.1038/nrn1055. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Flowers HL, AlHarbi MA, Mikulis D, Silver FL, Rochon E, Streiner D, et al. MRI-based neuroanatomical predictors of dysphagia, dysarthria, and aphasia in patients with first acute ischemic stroke. Cerebrovasc Dis Extra. 2017;7:21–34. doi: 10.1159/000457810. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Yourganov G, Smith KG, Fridriksson J, Rorden C. Predicting aphasia type from brain damage measured with structural MRI. Cortex. 2015;73:203–215. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2015.09.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Schiff ND. Central thalamic contributions to arousal regulation and neurological disorders of consciousness. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2008;1129:105–118. doi: 10.1196/annals.1417.029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Schmahmann JD. Vascular syndromes of the thalamus. Stroke. 2003;34:2264–2278. doi: 10.1161/01.STR.0000087786.38997.9E. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Alexander GE. Basal ganglia-thalamocortical circuits: their role in control of movements. J Clin Neurophysiol. 1994;11:420–431. doi: 10.1097/00004691-199407000-00004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Casselman J, Mermuys K, Delanote J, Ghekiere J, Coenegrachts K. MRI of the cranial nerves—more than meets the eye: technical considerations and advanced anatomy. Neuroimaging Clin N Am. 2008;18:197–231. doi: 10.1016/j.nic.2008.02.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Benghanem S, Mazeraud A, Azabou E, Chhor V, Shinotsuka CR, Claassen J, et al. Brainstem dysfunction in critically ill patients. Crit Care. 2020;24:5. doi: 10.1186/s13054-019-2718-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Wannez S, Heine L, Thonnard M, Gosseries O, Laureys S. The repetition of behavioral assessments in diagnosis of disorders of consciousness. Ann Neurol. 2017;81:883–889. doi: 10.1002/ana.24962. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Ackermann H, Ziegler W. Akinetic mutism—a review of the literature. Fortschr Neurol Psychiatr. 1995;63:59–67. doi: 10.1055/s-2007-996603. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Ackermann H, Ziegler W. Mutism in central motor disorders—a review of the literature. Fortschr Neurol Psychiatr. 1994;62:337–344. doi: 10.1055/s-2007-999065. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Mega MS, Cohenour RC. Akinetic mutism: disconnection of frontal–subcortical circuits. Neuropsychiatry Neuropsychol Behav Neurol. 1997;10:254–259. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Majerus S, Bruno M-A, Schnakers C, Giacino JT, Laureys S. The problem of aphasia in the assessment of consciousness in brain-damaged patients. Prog Brain Res. 2009;177:49–61. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(09)17705-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Latronico N, Bolton CF. Critical illness polyneuropathy and myopathy: a major cause of muscle weakness and paralysis. Lancet Neurol. 2011;10:931–941. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(11)70178-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Intiso D, Di Rienzo F, Fontana A, Tolfa M, Bartolo M, Copetti M. Functional outcome of critical illness polyneuropathy in patients affected by severe brain injury. Eur J Phys Rehabil Med. 2017;53:910–919. doi: 10.23736/S1973-9087.17.04595-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Jöhr J, Halimi F, Pasquier J, Pincherle A, Schiff N, Diserens K. Recovery in cognitive motor dissociation after severe brain injury: a cohort study. PLoS One. 2020;15:e0228474. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0228474. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Claassen J, Doyle K, Matory A, Couch C, Burger KM, Velazquez A, et al. Detection of brain activation in unresponsive patients with acute brain injury. N Engl J Med. 2019;380:2497–2505. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa1812757. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Curley WH, Forgacs PB, Voss HU, Conte MM, Schiff ND. Characterization of EEG signals revealing covert cognition in the injured brain. Brain. 2018;141:1404–1421. doi: 10.1093/brain/awy070. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Edlow BL, Chatelle C, Spencer CA, Chu CJ, Bodien YG, O’Connor KL, et al. Early detection of consciousness in patients with acute severe traumatic brain injury. Brain. 2017;140:2399–2414. doi: 10.1093/brain/awx176. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Casarotto S, Comanducci A, Rosanova M, Sarasso S, Fecchio M, Napolitani M, et al. Stratification of unresponsive patients by an independently validated index of brain complexity. Ann Neurol. 2016;80:718–729. doi: 10.1002/ana.24779. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Comolatti R, Pigorini A, Casarotto S, Fecchio M, Faria G, Sarasso S, et al. A fast and general method to empirically estimate the complexity of brain responses to transcranial and intracranial stimulations. Brain Stimul. 2019;12:1280–1289. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2019.05.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Schiff ND. Recovery of consciousness after brain injury: a mesocircuit hypothesis. Trends Neurosci. 2010;33:1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2009.11.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Zheng ZS, Reggente N, Lutkenhoff E, Owen AM, Monti MM. Disentangling disorders of consciousness: insights from diffusion tensor imaging and machine learning. Hum Brain Mapp. 2017;38:431–443. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23370. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Laureys S, Owen AM, Schiff ND. Brain function in coma, vegetative state, and related disorders. Lancet Neurol. 2004;3:537–546. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(04)00852-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Koch C, Massimini M, Boly M, Tononi G. Neural correlates of consciousness: progress and problems. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2016;17:307–321. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2016.22. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Naccache L. Minimally conscious state or cortically mediated state? Brain. 2018;141:949–960. doi: 10.1093/brain/awx324. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Tononi G. An information integration theory of consciousness. BMC Neurosci. 2004;5:42. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-5-42. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Davceva N, Sivevski A, Basheska N. Traumatic axonal injury, a clinical–pathological correlation. J Forensic Leg Med. 2017;48:35–40. doi: 10.1016/j.jflm.2017.04.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Samain JL, Haven E, Gille M, Mathys P. Typical CT and MRI features of cortical laminar necrosis. JBR-BTR. 2011;94:357. doi: 10.5334/jbr-btr.713. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Bardin JC, Fins JJ, Katz DI, Hersh J, Heier LA, Tabelow K, et al. Dissociations between behavioural and functional magnetic resonance imaging-based evaluations of cognitive function after brain injury. Brain. 2011;134:769–782. doi: 10.1093/brain/awr005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Forgacs PB, Fridman EA, Goldfine AM, Schiff ND. Isolation syndrome after cardiac arrest and therapeutic hypothermia. Front Neurosci. 2016;10:259. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2016.00259. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Edlow BL, Takahashi E, Wu O, Benner T, Dai G, Bu L, et al. Neuroanatomic connectivity of the human ascending arousal system critical to consciousness and its disorders. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol. 2012;71:531–546. doi: 10.1097/NEN.0b013e3182588293. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Moruzzi G, Magoun HW. Brain stem reticular formation and activation of the EEG. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1949;1:455–473. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(49)90219-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Noel J-P, Chatelle C, Perdikis S, Johr J, LopesDaSilva M, Ryvlin P, et al. Peri-personal space encoding in patients with disorders of consciousness and cognitive–motor dissociation. Neuroimage Clin. 2019;24:101940. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2019.101940. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Journal of Neurology are provided here courtesy of Springer

RESOURCES