Abstract
Contrary to spontaneous yawning—an ancient phenomenon common to vertebrates—contagious yawning (elicited by others’ yawns) has been found only in highly social species and may reflect an emotional inter-individual connection. We investigated yawn contagion in the domestic pig, Sus scrofa. Owing to the complex socio-emotional and cognitive abilities of Sus scrofa, we posited that yawn contagion could be present in this species (Prediction 1) and influenced by individual/social factors (Prediction 2). In June-November 2018, on 104 semi-free ranging adolescent/adult pigs, 224 videos were recorded for video analysis on yawning. Kinship information was refined via genetic analyses. Statistical elaboration was conducted via GLMMs and non-parametric/randomization/cross-tabulation tests. We found yawn contagion in Sus scrofa, as it was more likely that pigs yawned when perceiving rather than not perceiving (yawning/control condition) others’ yawns (response peak in the first out of three minutes). Yawn contagion was more likely: (1) in response to males’ yawns; (2) as the age increased; (3) within short distance (1 m); (4) between full siblings, with no significant association between kinship and distance. The influence of kinship suggests that—as also hypothesized for Homo sapiens—yawn contagion might be linked with emotional communication and possibly contagion.
Subject terms: Zoology, Animal behaviour, Anthropology, Mimicry, Social evolution
Introduction
Contrary to spontaneous yawning—not elicited by others’ yawns—contagious yawning takes place when the yawn emitted by a subject (trigger) induces yawning in another subject (responder)1. In this respect, yawning acts as a releasing stimulus (sensu Tinbergen and Perdeck2). Despite some morphological variants especially described in primates (e.g. chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes3; geladas, Theropithecus gelada4; Tonkean macaques, Macaca tonkeana and Japanese macaques, M. fuscata5; humans, Homo sapiens6,7), spontaneous yawning is considered a plesiomorphic (ancient) trait because its basic motor pattern has been observed in a wide array of vertebrates8. On the other hand, contagious yawning between conspecifics has been observed so far in a relatively low number of species, which suggests that this phenomenon may have appeared more recently in vertebrate evolution9. In particular, the presence of yawn contagion has been found only in highly social species and seems to be linked to the type of sociality more than to the phylogenetic closeness9. For example, in primates yawn contagion seems not to be expressed in species with relatively low levels of affiliation or tolerance (strepsirrhines: ring-tailed lemurs, Lemur catta, and ruffed lemurs, Varecia variegata10,11; the cercopithecid, Japanese macaque12; the hominid, lowland gorilla; Gorilla gorilla13,14) whereas it is present in other (in some cases phylogenetically close) species showing higher levels of social affiliation (cercopithecids: Tonkean macaque12; geladas4; hominids: chimpanzees15–17; bonobo, Pan paniscus13,18,19; humans1,6). Yawn contagion has been also found in non-primate species characterized by high inter-individual cohesion, including mammals (wolves, Canis lupus lupus20; sheep, Ovis aries21; some groups of elephant seals, Mirounga leonina22), and one social bird species (budgerigar, Melopsittacus undulates23).
It has been hypothesized that the emergence of yawn contagion has been favored by natural selection in highly social species to enhance synchronization between individuals (spatial ranging, coordinated foraging, and sleep/wake rhythms9). Indeed, in both human and non-human mammals, spontaneous yawning can be linked to physiological and behavioral transitions, such as those occurring over the sleep–wake cycle or during the transitions around emotional arousal (humans, Homo sapiens24; South American sea lions, Otaria flavescens25; Verreaux’ sifaka, Propithecus verreauxi and ring-tailed lemurs26; rats, Rattus norvegicus27).
In humans and great apes individual features can influence yawn contagion. These features include: (1) age (with the yawning response increasing from the immature period to adulthood; humans28; chimpanzees29; geladas4); (2) sex of the trigger (females can elicit more yawns in bonobos18; males can elicit more yawns in chimpanzees and in humans for yawns that are heard but not seen30,31); (3) sex of the responder (with human females showing highest frequencies of response at least under certain conditions32). Moreover, social factors, such as familiarity between trigger and responder, can also affect yawn contagion (humans33; bonobos18,34; chimpanzees17; geladas4; wolves20). Finally, different levels in the detectability of the yawning stimulus have been hypothesized to influence the response9,35.
The species considered in this study is the domestic pig (Sus scrofa), characterized by complex cognition, psychology and sociality, with stable relationships established early in life and, at least in part, retained in adulthood36–41. Hence, Sus scrofa is a particularly suitable model to investigate yawn contagion, addressed here for the first time in this species. As the phenomenon of yawn contagion might be related to emotional communication and/or inter-individual synchronization in both humans and other animals9, this study on the domestic pig can be relevant to both applied research in animal welfare (especially livestock) and theoretical studies on the evolutionary convergences underlying the behavior of both humans and non-human social species.
Based on the previous framework, we formulated the following predictions.
Prediction 1: presence of yawn contagion
Yawning in pigs can express emotional arousal and chronic stress42. Domestic pigs can be influenced by the emotional state of others43,44, and use facial expressions to communicate emotional states to others (e.g. aggressive intent) and convey information about emotional responses39. Hence, we predicted that domestic pigs could be influenced by others’ yawns and, more specifically, that yawn contagion could be present in Sus scrofa.
Prediction 2: modulation of yawn contagion
Although in a variable way depending on the species, individual, perceptive and social factors can modulate yawn contagion9. We therefore predicted the levels of yawn contagion could vary depending on individual features (i.e., sex and age) and on the social and spatial proximity between pigs.
Methods
Study group and site
This study was conducted from June to November 2018 on semi-free ranging domestic pigs (Sus scrofa) at the “Ethical Farm Parva Domus”, in Cavagnolo, Turin (Italy). The animals could freely move and forage in a 13 ha area of natural grassland/woodland habitat. The study group was composed by 104 pigs (7–22 month old), 54 males and 50 females coming from 14 different litters and belonging to three mixed breeds: Parma Black, Large White, and Piedmont Black. Depending on the age, the animals had been together from 3 to 14 months. No animal showed stereotypic behavior.
The males had been all castrated via the removal of testes within their first days of life; all females were potentially reproductive. Reproductive males stayed separated from the group and therefore no reproductive male was present in the study group. Four feeding spots were available in the area where the animals were provided with food (Ciclo Unico P, SILDAMIN©) every day from 8:30 to 10:30; water was available ad libitum. The pigs could integrate their diet with roots, leaves or fruits that they could find in the environment. For individual recognition, the pigs were marked with spray Raidex© for livestock, used by the farmers to mark animals, with no manipulation because the pigs were habituated to the human presence. Different spray colors and different signs on the back and sides of the pigs were used. As a result, each individual had a unique marking, renewed every 4–7 days depending on weather (observations could not be carried out during rainy days). Due to the slaughtering suspension from June to September and the low slaughter rates (usually one individual per week), all but eight pigs were available for the whole data collection period.
Observational data, operational definitions, and video analysis
Video recording occurred on a daily basis from around 06:00 am to 05:00 pm, spanning morning and afternoon, on 80 days in total (avoiding rainy days). Video registration occurred when animals were active and records of the observational effort/animal were kept to give priority to less observed animals so to reduce observational imbalance. HD/Full HD videos were recorded by 2 people (E.C. and a field assistant) each day via Panasonic HC-V380/V180/Sony HDR-PJ240E. Extra information, when necessary, was audio-recorded on camera, with the help of a third field assistant. As a general rule, videos lasted 10 min but always included three minutes after the last yawn. For the purpose of this study, we carried out the video-analysis on 224 videos, corresponding to 42.67 h of registration and including 493 yawning bouts.
The video-analysis started after a training phase with supervisors (IN and GC), when inter-observer reliability scores measured via Cohen’s k reached 0.81 (R function “cohen.cappa”; libraries "irr" and "psych"; R version 3.5.3; https://cran.r-project.org). The video-analyses were carried out via freeware VLC 3.0.6 and extension Jump-to-Time, allowing frame-to-frame analysis.
Via the all occurrences sampling method45 (applied on videos, on the present animals), we extracted yawning bouts in absence of external unexpected events possibly generating anxiety responses (i.e. displacement activities) in the study animals. The yawning pattern involved mouth opening, with inhalation and a more rapid closing and exhalation46 (Fig. 1). No yawn was vocalized (via vocal folds). The possible yawning response was collected in blocks of three minutes, used in previous studies15,18,33, to reduce autocorrelation probability31,47. For each yawn, we recorded the identity of the yawner (trigger) and the identity of the animals present on videos (hereafter potential responders). For each trigger/potential responder dyad, we recorded: (1) time of the last consecutive yawn emitted by the trigger (hereafter triggering yawn); (2) number of yawns in the three minutes preceding the triggering yawn and whether they came from the trigger or not; (3) sight condition (whether the triggering yawn or other yawns emitted in the three minutes preceding the triggering yawn fell within the visual range of the potential responder or not); (4) sex, age and breed of each individual; (5) distance between trigger and potential responder (≤ 1 m, 1 < distance ≤ 10 m, > 10 m); (6) kinship (weak kinship: no parent in common; half-siblings: one parent in common; full-siblings: both parents in common); (7) minute of the yawning response in the three minutes following the triggering yawn (first/second/third). Multiple yawns from a responder in the three minutes following the triggering yawn were considered as a single response.
Owing to the wide visual range of Sus scrofa, a yawn emitted by an individual was considered as not visible to the other pigs only if a sight shielding, physical obstacle (e.g. fence, large tree) was present between yawner and potential responder. Doubtful cases (e.g. if it was unclear whether a pig was in the blind spot of another pig, at the back) were excluded.
Because pigs were mixed-breed, the breed was assigned based on the mother’s breed. Yawning was considered as spontaneous when a pig emitted a yawn with no previous yawns being emitted from other pigs in the preceding three minutes.
Kinship and genetic analyses
Owing to controlled reproduction, close kinship (full siblings, maternal/paternal siblings) was known from the birth record. The different breeds, size, and marks allowed the reliable identification of the different generations. However, different mothers and fathers could be related (e.g. siblings or cousins). Therefore, to distinguish between distantly related animals (weak kinship) from more closely related ones, genetic analyses were carried out on 31 pigs, (2–3 individuals picked from different sibling generations) at the forensic genetics lab of the Department of Public Health Sciences and Pediatrics. DNA was extracted by hair bulbs (collected during the study period) via QIAmp DNA Investigator Kit (Qiagen; www.qiagen.com), provided by the farmer, following the provider’s protocol. 11 autosomic STRs were amplified via multiplex PCR Animal Type Pig PCR amplification kit (http://www.biotype.de; Biotype AG, Dresden, Germany). Genetic profile typing was obtained via capillary electrophoresis with SeqStudio system (Thermo Fisher Scientific; www.thermofisher.com). Allele frequencies and kinship index (0.08) were set on the basis of a mixed sample of domestic pigs (n = 412), consisting of commercial lines commonly used in the production process48. The mutation rate for all markers was set at 0.002.
For each possible dyad of pigs an unspecific kinship search was performed using Familias 3.1.5 “Blind Search” module49. Likelihood ratio (LR) was calculated for siblings, half-siblings, 1st cousins, and 2nd cousins relationships, scaled versus unrelated. Relationship was assigned according to the maximum LR value observed among the tested relationships.
Statistical elaboration
The first GLMM (Model1) was run to test for the possible presence of yawn contagion and to verify whether yawning in the study pigs was influenced by the yawns previously emitted by group mates (n = 706). The presence/absence of yawns in the three minutes following a previous yawn was included as dependent, binomial variable (presence = 1; absence = 0). The following fixed factors were included: sight condition (factorial: 0 = previous yawning non-visible; 1 = previous yawning visible); additional yawns preceding the last triggering yawn (factorial: 0 = no previous additional yawn; 1 = one additional yawn; 2 = two additional yawns; 3 = three or more additional yawns); time slot (factorial; 1 = 6:01–9:00am; 2 = 9:01–12:00am; 3 = 12:01–15:00 pm; 4 = 15:01–17:00 pm). The identity of the potential responder was included as random factor (nominal variable).
We ran a second GLMM (Model2) to check what individual and social factors could influence yawn contagion. We included yawn contagion as dependent, binomial variable (1 = present; 0 = absent). As individual fixed factors we included: trigger’ and potential responder’s sex (factorial, M = male; F = female); trigger and potential’s responder age (months, numeric variable), trigger’ and potential responder’s breed (factorial variable: 1 = Piedmont Black, 2 = Parma Black, 3 = Large White). As social fixed factors we included: kinship (obtained from both genetic analyses and information on birth register: factorial; 1 = weak kinship; 2 = half siblings; 3 = full siblings); and distance (factorial; 1 = within 1 m; 2 = between 1 and 10 m; 3 = more than 10 m). The combination between trigger’ and potential responder’s identity (nominal variable) was included as random factor. For this analysis we selected the cases (n = 351) in which the triggering yawn came by one specific pig in the three min slot considered, so to single out the individual features of the trigger, and the kinship and distance between a given trigger and the potential responder. To additionally reduce the autocorrelation effect during yawn chains (subsequent yawns occurring within 3 min), only the first yawn following the last trigger’s yawn was considered as a response.
We selected the individuals with at least three yawning responses (n = 19) and calculated their yawn contagion frequencies, to check for yawn contagion distribution across the three minutes following the triggering stimulus. Owing to the non-normality of frequency distribution in the third minute (Kolmogorov–Smirnov, p = 0.001), via SPSS 20.0 we applied a non-parametric Friedman’s test for k ≥ 2 dependent samples50. Then, we applied the Bonferroni-Dunn post-hoc test for pairwise comparisons between minutes.
To check whether the same individual factors possibly affecting yawn contagion could also affect spontaneous yawning, a LMM (Model3) was run on the log10-transformed frequencies of spontaneous yawning emitted in each 3-min time slot by the pigs under study (n = 244; transformation applied to reach normality: Kolmogorov–Smirnov, p = 0.052). The yawner’s sex (factorial, M = male; F = female), age (months, numeric variable), and breed (factorial: 1 = Piedmont Black, 2 = Parma Black, 3 = Large White) were included as fixed factor and the yawner’s identity as random factor.
The GLMM/LMMs were fitted in R (R Core Team, 2019; version 3.5.3; https://www.R-project.org) by using the function glmer (for binary dependent variable) or lmer (for numeric dependent variable) of the R-package lme451. As a first step it was verified if the full model significantly differed from the null model only including the random factors52. The likelihood ratio test53 was used to test this significance (ANOVA with argument 'Chisq'). Subsequently, by using the R-function “drop1”, the p-values for the individual predictors based on likelihood ratio tests between the full and the null model were calculated54. A binomial error distribution was used for binary response variables (Model1/2; link function: logit) whereas a Gaussian distribution was used for the normal response variable (Model3). A multiple contrast package (multcomp) was used to perform all pairwise comparisons between categories for significant multinomial factors with the Tukey test55. The Bonferroni-adjusted p-values are reported, along with estimate (Est), standard error (S.E.), and z-values.
Owing to data pseudoreplication (same individuals observed at different age), via the freeware Resampling Procedures 1.3 (package by David C. Howell) we applied a randomized bivariate correlation test (10,000 permutations) to check for the correlation between pig age and the frequency of yawning response (number of yawning responses normalized over the total number of opportunities at each age). Via SPSS 20.0, we ran the Kruskal’s lambda test to test for the association between the nominal variables distance and kinship (included in Model2, n = 351). P-values < 0.05 were considered as statistically significant for all tests, unless when Bonferroni correction was applied (as indicated above).
Ethics approval
This research was purely observational and no animal manipulation was required during the study. Hence, no ethical approval was necessary according to the current regulation.
Results
We ran the first GLMM (Model1) to check whether yawning could be transferred from one pig to another and to verify what contextual factors could influence it. The dependent variable was the yawning performed by a pig within three minutes from others’ yawns (binomial: present = 1; absent = 0). We found a significant difference between the full model including all fixed factors (sight condition, daily time slot, and previously emitted yawns) and the null model, only containing the random factor (potential responder’s identity) (likelihood ratio test: χ2 = 72.192; df = 7; p < 0.001). Hence, we moved on with a drop1 procedure. All the tested fixed factors had a significant main effect on the dependent variable (results included in Table 1). In particular, it was significantly more likely that a pig yawned after that other pigs had yawned within their visual range rather than when the previous yawns occurred outside their visual range (Table 1; Fig. 2). Hence, yawning was contagious in the study group. Moreover, it was more likely that a pig yawned in response to others’ yawns when previous yawns had been emitted in addition to the last triggering yawn (Table 1; Fig. 3).
Table 1.
Estimate | SE | z-value | P | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Model1—(GLMM) Dependent variable = yawning performed by a subject within three minutes from others’ yawns (binomial: present = 1; absent = 0). Random factors = potential responder identity Full versus null model: χ2 = 72.192, df = 7, p < 0.001 | ||||
(Intercept)a | − 1.912 | 0.283 | A | a |
Time slot (2)b,c | − 0.666 | 0.209 | − 3.192 | 0.001 |
Time slot (3)b,c | − 0.139 | 0.408 | − 0.342 | 0.733 |
Time slot (4)b,c | 0.248 | 0.285 | 0.871 | 0.384 |
Previous yawns (1) b,c | 0.817 | 0.236 | 3.456 | 0.001 |
Previous yawns (2)b,c | 1.052 | 0.250 | 4.202 | < 0.001 |
Previous yawns (3)b,c | 0.865 | 0.283 | 3.060 | 0.002 |
Sight condition (1) b,c | 1.170 | 0.220 | 5.332 | < 0.001 |
Pairwise comparisons (Tukey test) for the multinomial fixed factors of Model1 having a significant main effect on the dependent variable Bonferroni-adjusted p-values are reported | ||||
Time slot (1 = 6:01–9:00am; 2 = 9:01–12:00am; 3 = 12:01–15:00 pm, 4 = 15:01–17:00 pm) | ||||
2 versus 1 | − 0.666 | 0.209 | − 3.192 | 0.007 |
3 versus 1 | − 0.139 | 0.408 | − 0.342 | 0.985 |
4 versus 1 | 0.248 | 0.285 | 0.871 | 0.811 |
3 versus 2 | 0.527 | 0.413 | 1.275 | 0.564 |
4 versus 2 | 0.914 | 0.295 | 3.096 | 0.010 |
4 versus 3 | 0.388 | 0.455 | 0.852 | 0.821 |
Previous yawns (0 = no previous yawns; 1 = one previous yawn; 2 = two previous yawns; 3 = three or more previous yawns) | ||||
1 versus 0 | 0.817 | 0.236 | 3.456 | 0.003 |
2 versus 0 | 1.052 | 0.250 | 4.202 | < 0.001 |
3 versus 0 | 0.865 | 0.283 | 3.060 | 0.012 |
2 versus 1 | 0.235 | 0.249 | 0.943 | 0.780 |
3 versus 1 | 0.048 | 0.279 | 0.143 | 0.998 |
3 versus 2 | − 1.186 | 0.291 | − 0.641 | 0.918 |
aNot shown as not having a meaningful interpretation.
bEstimate ± SE refer to the difference of the response between the reported level of this categorical predictor and the reference category of the same predictor.
cThese predictors were dummy coded. Time slot was coded as: 1 = 6:01–9:00am; 2 = 9:01–12:00am; 3 = 12:01–15:00 pm, 4: 15:01–17:00 pm. Previous yawns were coded as: 0 = no previous additional yawns; 1 = one previous yawn; 2 = two previous yawns; 3 = three or more previous yawns. Sight condition was coded as: 0 = yawn(s) outside the visual range; 1 = yawn(s) within the visual range.
The pairwise comparison indicates that the yawning response was significantly more probable when at least one extra yawn had been emitted by other pigs (Tukey test; one additional yawn: p = 0.003; two additional yawns: p < 0.001; three or more additional yawns: p = 0.012) compared to none. No difference was found between other categories (Tukey test; p = ns; see Table 1 for full results). Finally, the occurrence of yawn contagion decreased in late morning, in the time slot 9:01–12:00 (Table 1). The pairwise comparisons revealed that the yawning response was significantly lower in the late morning (09:01–12:00am) compared to early morning (06:01–09:00am) (Tukey test; p = 0.007) and late afternoon (15:01–17:00 pm) (Tukey test; p = 0.010). No significant difference was found between the other time slots (Tukey test; p = ns; see Table 1 for full results).
The frequencies of yawn contagion were significantly different across the three minutes from the triggering stimulus (Friedman’s test; N = 19; χ2 = 19.279; df = 2; p < 0.001). The pairwise comparisons revealed a significant difference between first minute and the other minutes (Bonferroni-Dunn post-hoc test; third versus first min: Q = 1.263; p < 0.001; second versus first min: Q = 0.789; p = 0.045) but not between third and second minute (Bonferroni-Dunn post-hoc test: Q = 0.474; p = 0.433). Specifically, yawn contagion was highest during the first minute (Fig. 4). Video 1 shows yawning and yawning response in two study pigs lying in contact.
As a second analysis, we carried out a GLMM (Model2) to verify what individual and social factors could modulate yawn contagion, whose occurrence was introduced as binomial, dependent variable (presence = 1; absence = 0). We found a significant difference between the full model, including all fixed factors (individual factors: trigger and potential responder sex, age, and breed; social factors: kinship, inter-individual distance) and the null model, only containing the random factors (trigger’ * responder’s identity) (likelihood ratio test: χ2 = 61.439; df = 12; p < 0.001). Hence, we moved on with a drop1 procedure.
With respect to social factors, as shown in Table 2, both kinship and inter-individual distance had a significant effect on yawn contagion. In particular, post-hoc pairwise comparisons between kinship categories revealed that yawn contagion was more likely between full siblings rather than between other categories (Tukey test; full siblings versus weak kin: p = 0.031; full versus half-siblings: p = 0.033) (Fig. 5a). No other difference was significant (see Table 2 for full results).
Table 2.
Estimate | SE | z-value | P | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Model2—(GLMM) Dependent variable = yawn contagion (binomial: presence = 1; absence = 0) Random factors = trigger identity*responder identity Full versus null model: χ2 = 61.439, df = 12, p < 0.001 | ||||
(Intercept)a | 0.806 | 2.360 | A | a |
Trigger sex (M)b,c | 1.867 | 0.792 | 2.357 | 0.018 |
Potential responder sex (M)b,c | 0.166 | 0.409 | 0.407 | 0.684 |
Trigger age | − 0.183 | 0.117 | − 1.563 | 0.118 |
Potential responder age | 0.220 | 0.067 | 3.295 | 0.001 |
Trigger breed (2) | − 1.425 | 1.320 | − 1.080 | 0.280 |
Trigger breed (3) | − 1.670 | 1.225 | − 1.363 | 0.173 |
Responder breed (2) | − 0.329 | 0.852 | − 0.386 | 0.700 |
Responder breed (3) | 0.190 | 0.844 | 0.225 | 0.822 |
Kinship (2)b,c | 0.449 | 0.800 | 0.561 | 0.574 |
Kinship (3)b,c | 2.206 | 0.873 | 2.527 | 0.012 |
Distance (2) b,c | − 2.453 | 0.503 | − 4.872 | < 0.001 |
Distance (3) | − 2.736 | 0.601 | − 4.550 | < 0.001 |
Pairwise comparisons (Tukey test) for the multinomial fixed factors of Model2 having a significant main effect on the dependent variable Bonferroni-adjusted p-values are reported | ||||
Kinship (1 = weak kinship; 2 = half-siblings; 3 = full-siblings) | ||||
2 versus 1 | 0.449 | 0.800 | 0.561 | 0.840 |
3 versus 1 | 2.206 | 0.873 | 2.527 | 0.031 |
3 versus 2 | 1.757 | 0.702 | 2.502 | 0.033 |
Distance (1 = within 1 m; 2 = from 1 to 10 m; 3 = more than 10 m) | ||||
2 versus 1 | − 2.453 | 0.503 | − 4.872 | < 0.001 |
3 versus 1 | − 2.736 | 0.602 | − 4.550 | < 0.001 |
3 versus 2 | − 0.284 | 0.557 | − 0.510 | 0.865 |
aNot shown as not having a meaningful interpretation.
bEstimate ± SE refer to the difference of the response between the reported level of this categorical predictor and the reference category of the same predictor.
cThese predictors were dummy coded. For both trigger and potential responder: M = male, F = female; Breed: 1 = Piedmont Black; 2 = Parma Black; 3 = Large White. Kinship: 1 = weak kinship; 2 = half-siblings; 3 = full-siblings; Distance: 1 = within 1 m; 2 = from 1 to 10 m; 3 = more than 10 m.
Post-hoc pairwise comparisons on inter-individual distance indicate that yawn contagion is significantly higher when individuals are within one meter from each other, compared to further distances (Tukey test; one meter versus within 10 m: p < 0.001; one meter versus more than 10 m: p < 0.001) (Fig. 5b). No difference was found between other distances (see Table 2 for full results). Goodman and Kruskal’s lambda test revealed no significant association between the two nominal fixed factors kinship and distance (λ = 0.010; T = 0.200; p = 0.841).
As concerns the individual factors, the trigger and responder breed had no effect on yawn contagion (Table 2). Instead the sex of the trigger had a significant effect on yawn contagion, which was significantly more likely if the yawning stimulus was emitted by a male rather than by a female (Table 2; Fig. 6a). Moreover the responder’s age had a significant main effect on yawn contagion (Table 2). In particular, yawn contagion increased as age increased (bivariate correlation via randomization: n = 13; r = 0.600; p = 0.031; Fig. 6b).
Finally, a LMM (Model3) was run as a further control, to check whether spontaneous yawning was affected by the same individual variables affecting yawn contagion. Individual spontaneous yawning frequencies in the different time slots were included as dependent, numeric variable. We found no significant difference between the full model (including all fixed factors: yawner’s sex, age, and breed) and the null model including only the identity of the yawner (likelihood ratio test: χ2 = 4.924; df = 4; p = 0.295). Therefore, we proceeded no further, as the considered fixed factors had no significant effect on spontaneous yawning (sex: F = 1.302; p = 0.257; age: F = 3.419; p = 0.067; breed: F = 0.098; p = 0.907).
Discussion
Our results show that yawn contagion is present in the domestic pig. As a matter of fact, it was significantly more likely that the pigs yawned after that at least one yawn had been emitted by another pig within their visual range (sight condition) rather than when a physical obstacle prevented the potential responder to see the yawning stimulus (control condition; Prediction 1 confirmed; Table 1; Fig. 2). Moreover, similarly as in other mammalian species21,34, pig yawn contagion was most likely in the first minute from the emission of the triggering stimulus (Fig. 4).
Pigs—as other highly social mammal species showing yawn contagion (for a review9)—can naturally form groups and engage in stable social relationships40,41. Immature individuals start to fine tune their relationship with others via play, first with littermates and later also with individuals from other litters37–39. As it has been hypothesized for other species9, it is possible that yawning has been co-opted during evolution to become a signal of behavioral/physiological change that other group mates could catch and replicate for reciprocal synchronization, important for social life.
Our results converge in indicating that the detection of the yawning stimulus has a crucial role in determining the yawning response, which is significantly enhanced if more than one triggering yawn is emitted (Table 1, Fig. 3) and when the potential responder is in proximity to the trigger (≤ 1 m, Table 2; Fig. 5b). This finding can be explained by the fact that pigs (which also rely on olfaction, touch, and hearing to orient themselves in the environment) possess a broad visual range (owing to lateralized eyes) but poor visual acuity56–60. Considering that no yawn was vocalized, pigs could only rely on vision to spot mouth opening. In this respect, any element increasing stimulus detectability (such as more yawning stimuli or short distances) could enhance the yawning response.
In humans, yawn contagion significantly fluctuates during the day, with peacks in the morning and in the evening, possibly associated with the wake-sleep cycle61. In chimpanzees, weak variations in yawn contagion have been observed16. In pigs, we observed that yawn contagion was lowest in late morning (Table 1) probably because the pigs were fed in this time slot and spent most of the time feeding and, then, sleeping. This difference is likely to disappear in the wild, where animals are not provisioned with food. In our study, the variation in the occurrence of yawn contagion was not significant across the other time slots. Thus, we cannot state that there is an appreciable variation of the phenomenon across the whole day, as expected for a species that—although preferring crepuscular and night activity—can be active over the 24h56.
Our findings indicate that the breed did not influence contagious yawning (Table 2). To our knowledge there is no study addressing the possible effect of breeds on yawn contagion in mammals. With the present data, it is not possible to determine whether our results mean that the yawning response is not affected by breed or whether the absence of a significant effect is due to the fact that the breeds of our study individuals were mixed.
Taken together, our results also show that while contagious yawning was significantly influenced by the sex of the trigger (with males eliciting more yawns than females; Table 2, Fig. 6a) and by the age of the responder (with yawn contagion increasing as age increased, Table 2, Fig. 6b), spontaneous yawning was not. Hence, yawn contagion probably was not enhanced by trigger males or responder’s age as a result of generally higher yawning levels in males or older pigs. Inter-sexual biases in the power of eliciting a yawning response have been found in bonobos, where yawning in group mates can be induced most frequently by females18, and in humans (for vocalized yawns perceived only by hearing31) and chimpanzees30 where males as triggers are particularly effective in triggering others’ yawns. In humans, for vocalized yawns that are heard but not seen, it is possible that men’s vocalizations are better heard than women’s in natural settings, often characterized by background noises31. In chimpanzees and bonobos, the sex bias has been related to the dominance or social relevance of the triggering subjects, considering that in chimpanzees males are dominant30 whereas in bonobos females acquire leadership by forming coalitions18. Even though castrated males can still fight for dominance62, it is unlikely that dominance provides a possible explanation for enhanced yawning response in Sus scrofa. As a matter of fact, at the adaptive level the dominant status of males may be not as much relevant for yawn contagion in the light of the species biology. Under natural conditions, in both feral pigs and wild boars females with offspring form matrilineal units and join together in stable groups of variable size whereas solitary adult males live isolated and only temporarily join female groups40,63,64. This social structure can explain why adult females and not males form a linear hierarchy, with older sows being dominant over younger sows65–68. In this perspective, a possible explanation for adult males being best triggers might be the necessity of other pigs to synchronise with reproductive males when they temporarily join the social groups composed by sows and offspring. However, further investigation on this issue is necessary to verify this possibility or formulate other hypotheses.
With respect to age, in pigs we observed a significant increase of yawn contagion as age increased (Table 2; Fig. 6b). In some primate species, yawn contagion is present in adults and absent in infants (humans28; chimpanzees30; geladas4). In humans, yawn contagion seems also to decline with age in adults69 although no conclusive results are available (e.g. see32). Our study subjects had passed the age in which pigs (if reproductive) reach sexual maturity (around 6 months), but it should be considered that in pigs body development continues up to 18 months, with the period between 6 and 18 months sometimes referred to as adolescence70. Pigs and humans are thought to share similar brain growth and development patterns, with pig brain considered closer to the human brain than other non-human animal models in terms of size, structure, and composition71–73. Ryan et al.74 found that longitudinal effects in magnetic resonance spectroscopy (MRS) measurements in adolescent female pigs (from pubescent to sexually mature) were similar to those reported in adolescent humans. Moreover, the domestic pigs possess complex cognitive and affective skills, including object discrimination, spatial learning, understanding human cues, emotional sharing, and possible elements of perspective taking43,75,76 (for review36). Hence, as it has been hypothesized for other species, the increased rates of yawn contagion in pigs with age might also suggest ongoing maturation of socio-cognitive skills and/or neural networks involved in the elaboration of social cues, developmental changes in action-understanding or identification of others’ affective state4,29.
Finally, in the pigs under study yawn contagion was highest between full siblings than in more weakly related individuals (Table 2; Fig. 5a). Among our study animals, 88% of full siblings came from the same litter and therefore they had spent more time than others in close association. Even though kinship and social bonding are distinct aspects41,77, the kinship bias observed in pigs might have a similar effect on yawn contagion as the relationship quality bias observed in other species, in which yawn contagion was highest between strongly bonded individuals (humans33; bonobos18,34; chimpanzees17; geladas4; wolves20). Even if the issue is still under debate78, it has been hypothesized that the familiarity and/or kinship bias observed in yawn contagion might reflect a form of emotional contagion, a basic building block of empathy (for review9). Indeed, domestic pigs show emotional contagion potentials and the ability to convey social information via facial expressions39,43.
As explained above, in our study we found that the spatial proximity between pigs also enhanced yawn contagion (Fig. 5b), probably by increasing the detection probability of the yawning stimulus. Social closeness and tight kinship often go in tandem with spatial closeness, with physical proximity being frequently used as a measure of social and emotional engagement in social animals79. We found that spatial distance and kinship between trigger and potential responder were not significantly associated with one another. Consistently, a recent study41 found that social proximity and relatedness were not correlated in pigs. Thus, the yawn contagion bias observed in pigs, might be linked to inter-individual relatedness, and not just proximity. This possibility can have interesting repercussions for animal welfare studies investigating emotional connection between individuals. However, no conclusion can be drawn at this stage of knowledge and further investigations, experimentally disentangling distance, kinship and social bond, are necessary to determine whether yawn contagion may be driven by familiarity per se or not.
Supplementary Information
Acknowledgements
This study is part of the broader project “So.Pig” (Socially Pig) aimed at investigating how social interactions affect the quality of life in pigs raised under naturalistic conditions. The authors wish to thank Cristina Desdera and Davide Lovera, who own and manage the ethical farm “Parva Domus” located in Cavagnolo (Turin, Italy) for their interest, encouragement and availability since the study start. The authors also thank Parva Domus staff for the practical help during the field work. Finally, a special thanks go to Edoardo Collarini, Marta Comin, and Marika Gioia, Anna Zanoli, and Marta Caselli who provided support during part of the fieldwork.
Author contributions
Data collection: E.C.; conceptualization: I.N., G.C.; methodology: I.N., G.C.; formal analysis and investigation: I.N., G.C., E.C.; genetic analyses: C.R., E.C.; writing—original draft preparation: I.N., G.C.; writing—review and editing: I.N., G.C.
Funding
Funding was provided by Department of Life Sciences and Systems Biology, University of Torino, Italy (Grant No. NORI_RILO_18_01).
Data availability
The datasets generated during and/or analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher's note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Contributor Information
Ivan Norscia, Email: ivan.norscia@unito.it.
Giada Cordoni, Email: giada.cordoni@unito.it.
Supplementary Information
The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1038/s41598-020-80545-1.
References
- 1.Provine RR. Faces as releasers of contagious yawning: An approach to face detection using normal human subjects. Bull. Psychonom. Soc. 1989;27(3):211–214. doi: 10.3758/BF03334587. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Tinbergen, N., & Perdeck, A. C. On the stimulus situation releasing the begging response in the newly hatched Herring Gull chick (Larus argentatus argentatus Pont.). Behaviour, 3(1), 1–39. 10.1163/156853951X00197 (1951).
- 3.Vick SJ, Paukner A. Variation and context of yawns in captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) Am. J. Primatol. 2010;72(3):262–269. doi: 10.1002/ajp.20781. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Palagi E, Leone A, Mancini G, Ferrari PF. Contagious yawning in gelada baboons as a possible expression of empathy. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 2009;106(46):19262–19267. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0910891106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Zannella A, Stanyon R, Palagi E. Yawning and social styles: Different functions in tolerant and despotic macaques (Macaca tonkeana and Macaca fuscata) J. Comp. Psychol. 2017;131(3):179. doi: 10.1037/com0000062. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Provine RR. Yawning as a stereotyped action pattern and releasing stimulus. Ethology. 1986;72(2):109–122. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.1986.tb00611.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Provine, R. R. Curious behavior: Yawning, laughing, hiccupping, and beyond. Harvard University Press (2012).
- 8.Baenninger R. Some comparative aspects of yawning in Betta splendens, Homo sapiens, Panthera leo, and Papio sphinx. J. Comp. Psychol. 1987;101(4):349. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.101.4.349. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Palagi E, Celeghin A, Tamietto M, Winkielman P, Norscia I. The neuroethology of spontaneous mimicry and emotional contagion in human and non-human animals. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2020;111:149–165. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.01.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Reddy RB, Krupenye C, MacLean EL, Hare B. No evidence for contagious yawning in lemurs. Anim. Cogn. 2016;19(5):889–898. doi: 10.1007/s10071-016-0986-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Pereira ME, Seeligson ML, Macedonia JM. The behavioral repertoire of the black-and-white ruffed lemur, Varecia variegata variegata (Primates: Lemuridae) Folia Primatol. 1988;51(1):1–32. doi: 10.1159/000156353. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Palagi E, Norscia I. Etologia dell'emozione animale: Studio e interpretazione. Sistemi intelligenti. 2019;31(1):11–32. doi: 10.1422/92933. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Amici F, Aureli F, Call J. Response facilitation in the four great apes: Is there a role for empathy? Primates. 2014;55(1):113–118. doi: 10.1007/s10329-013-0375-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Palagi E, Norscia I, Cordoni G. Lowland gorillas (Gorilla gorilla gorilla) failed to respond to others’ yawn: Experimental and naturalistic evidence. J. Comp. Psychol. 2019;133(3):406–416. doi: 10.1037/com0000175. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Anderson, J. R., Myowa–Yamakoshi, M., & Matsuzawa, T. Contagious yawning in chimpanzees. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. Ser. B Biol. Sci.271(suppl 6), S468-S470. 10.1098/rsbl.2004.0224 (2004). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 16.Campbell MW, Cox CR. Observational data reveal evidence and parameters of contagious yawning in the behavioral repertoire of captive-reared chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) Sci. Rep. 2019;9(1):1–13. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-49698-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Campbell MW, De Waal FB. Ingroup-outgroup bias in contagious yawning by chimpanzees supports link to empathy. PLoS ONE. 2011;6(4):1. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0018283. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Demuru E, Palagi E. In bonobos yawn contagion is higher among kin and friends. PLoS ONE. 2012;7(11):1. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0049613. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Tan J, Ariely D, Hare B. Bonobos respond prosocially toward members of other groups. Sci. Rep. 2017;7(1):1–11. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-15320-w. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Romero T, Ito M, Saito A, Hasegawa T. Social modulation of contagious yawning in wolves. PLoS ONE. 2014;9(8):1. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0105963. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Yonezawa T, Sato K, Uchida M, Matsuki N, Yamazaki A. Presence of contagious yawning in sheep. Anim. Sci. J. 2017;88(1):195–200. doi: 10.1111/asj.12681. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Wojczulanis-Jakubas K, Plenzler J, Jakubas D. Indications of contagious behaviours in the southern elephant seal: An observational study. Behaviour. 2019;156(1):59–77. doi: 10.1163/1568539X-00003530. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Gallup AC, Swartwood L, Militello J, Sackett S. Experimental evidence of contagious yawning in budgerigars (Melopsittacus undulatus) Anim. Cogn. 2015;18(5):1051–1058. doi: 10.1007/s10071-015-0873-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Guggisberg AG, Mathis J, Schnider A, Hess CW. Why do we yawn? Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2010;34(8):1267–1276. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.03.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Palagi E, Guillén-Salazar F, Llamazares-Martín C. Spontaneous yawning and its potential functions in South American Sea Lions (Otaria flavescens) Sci. Rep. 2019;9(1):1–8. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-53613-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Zannella A, Norscia I, Stanyon R, Palagi E. Testing yawning hypotheses in wild populations of two strepsirrhine species: Propithecus verreauxi and Lemur catta. Am. J. Primatol. 2015;77(11):1207–1215. doi: 10.1002/ajp.22459. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Kubota N, Amemiya S, Yanagita S, Nishijima T, Kita I. Emotional stress evoked by classical fear conditioning induces yawning behavior in rats. Neurosci. Lett. 2014;566:182–187. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2014.02.064. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Millen A, Anderson JR. Neither infants nor toddlers catch yawns from their mothers. Biol. Lett. 2011;7(3):440–442. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2010.0966. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Madsen EA, Persson T. Contagious yawning in domestic dog puppies (Canis lupus familiaris): The effect of ontogeny and emotional closeness on low-level imitation in dogs. Anim. Cogn. 2013;16(2):233–240. doi: 10.1007/s10071-012-0568-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Massen JJ, Vermunt DA, Sterck EH. Male yawning is more contagious than female yawning among chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) PLoS ONE. 2012;7(7):1. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0040697. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Norscia I, Zanoli A, Gamba M, Palagi E. Auditory contagious yawning is highest between friends and family members: Support to the emotional bias hypothesis. Front. Psychol. 2020;11:442. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00442. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Norscia I, Demuru E, Palagi E. She more than he: Gender bias supports the empathic nature of yawn contagion in Homo sapiens. R. Soc. Open Sci. 2016;3(2):150459. doi: 10.1098/rsos.150459. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Norscia I, Palagi E. Yawn contagion and empathy in Homo sapiens. PLoS ONE. 2011;6(12):1. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0028472. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Palagi E, Norscia I, Demuru E. Yawn contagion in humans and bonobos: emotional affinity matters more than species. PeerJ. 2014;2:e519. doi: 10.7717/peerj.519. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Massen JJ, Gallup AC. Why contagious yawning does not (yet) equate to empathy. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2017;80:573–585. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.07.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Marino, L., & Colvin, C. M. Thinking pigs: A comparative review of cognition, emotion, and personality in Sus domesticus. Int. J. Compar. Psychol.https://escholarship.org/uc/item/8sx4s79c (2015).
- 37.Weller JE, Camerlink I, Turner SP, Farish M, Arnott G. Socialisation and its effect on play behaviour and aggression in the domestic pig (Sus scrofa) Sci. Rep. 2019;9(1):1–11. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-40980-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Newberry RC, Wood-Gush DGM. Social relationships of piglets in a semi-natural environment. Anim. Behav. 1986;34(5):1311–1318. doi: 10.1016/S0003-3472(86)80202-0. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Camerlink I, Farish M, D’Eath RB, Arnott G, Turner SP. Long term benefits on social behaviour after early life socialization of piglets. Animals. 2018;8(11):192. doi: 10.3390/ani8110192. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Podgórski T, Lusseau D, Scandura M, Sönnichsen L, Jędrzejewska B. Long-lasting, kin-directed female interactions in a spatially structured wild boar social network. PLoS ONE. 2014;9(6):1. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0099875. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Goumon S, Illmann G, Leszkowová I, Dostalová A, Cantor M. Dyadic affiliative preferences in a stable group of domestic pigs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2020;1:105045. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2020.105045. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Zhang MY, et al. Effects of confinement duration and parity on stereotypic behavioral and physiological responses of pregnant sows. Physiol. Behav. 2017;179:369–376. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2017.07.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Reimert I, Bolhuis JE, Kemp B, Rodenburg TB. Indicators of positive and negative emotions and emotional contagion in pigs. Physiol. Behav. 2013;109:42–50. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2012.11.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Goumon S, Špinka M. Emotional contagion of distress in young pigs is potentiated by previous exposure to the same stressor. Anim. Cogn. 2016;19(3):501–511. doi: 10.1007/s10071-015-0950-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Altmann J. Observational study of behavior: Sampling methods. Behaviour. 1974;49(3–4):227–266. doi: 10.1163/156853974X00534. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Baenninger R. On yawning and its functions. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 1997;4(2):198–207. doi: 10.3758/BF03209394. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Kapitány R, Nielsen M. Are yawns really contagious? A critique and quantification of yawn contagion. Adapt. Hum. Behav. Physiol. 2017;3(2):134–155. doi: 10.1007/s40750-017-0059-y. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Caratti S, et al. Analysis of 11 tetrameric STRs in wild boars for forensic purposes. For. Sci. Int. Genet. 2010;4(5):339–342. doi: 10.1016/j.fsigen.2010.07.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Kling D, Tillmar AO, Egeland T. Familias 3: Extensions and new functionality. For. Sci. Int. Genet. 2014;13:121–127. doi: 10.1016/j.fsigen.2014.07.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Siegel, S., & Castellan, N. J. Non parametric statistics for the behavioural sciences, 2nd Edition. Book (1988).
- 51.Bates, D et al. Package ‘lme4’. Convergence, 12(1), 2. http://lme4.r-forge.r-project.org/ (2015).
- 52.Forstmeier W, Schielzeth H. Cryptic multiple hypotheses testing in linear models: overestimated effect sizes and the winner's curse. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 2011;65(1):47–55. doi: 10.1007/s00265-010-1038-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Dobson, A. J., & Barnett, A. G. An introduction to generalized linear models. CRC press. (2018).
- 54.Barr DJ, Levy R, Scheepers C, Tily HJ. Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. J. Mem. Lang. 2013;68(3):255–278. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2012.11.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Bretz, F., Hothorn, T., & Westfall, P. Multiple comparisons using R. CRC Press (2016).
- 56.Zonderland JJ, Cornelissen L, Wolthuis-Fillerup M, Spoolder HA. Visual acuity of pigs at different light intensities. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2008;111(1–2):28–37. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2007.05.010. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Lutwyche, R. The Pig: A Natural History. Ivy Press (2019).
- 58.Koba Y, Tanida H. How do miniature pigs discriminate between people?: Discrimination between people wearing coveralls of the same colour. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2001;73(1):45–58. doi: 10.1016/S0168-1591(01)00106-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Arave CW. Assessing sensory capacity of animals using operant technology. J. Anim. Sci. 1996;74(8):1996–2009. doi: 10.2527/1996.7481996x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Lomas CA, Piggins D, Phillips CJC. Visual awareness. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 1998;57(3–4):247–257. doi: 10.1016/S0168-1591(98)00100-2. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Giganti F, Zilli I. The daily time course of contagious and spontaneous yawning among humans. J. Ethol. 2011;29(2):215–219. doi: 10.1007/s10164-010-0242-0. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Foister S, Doeschl-Wilson A, Roehe R, Arnott G, Boyle L, Turner S. Social network properties predict chronic aggression in commercial pig systems. PLoS ONE. 2018;13(10):e0205122. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0205122(2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Maselli V, et al. Wild boars’ social structure in the Mediterranean habitat. Ital. J. Zool. 2014;81(4):610–617. doi: 10.1080/11250003.2014.953220. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 64.D’Eath, R. B., & Turner, S. P. The natural behaviour of the pig. In The welfare of pigs (pp. 13–45). Springer, Dordrecht. 10.1007/978-1-4020-8909-1_2 (2009).
- 65.Meikle DB, Drickamer LC, Vessey SH, Arthur RD, Rosenthal TL. Dominance rank and parental investment in swine (Sus scrofa domesticus) Ethology. 1996;102(8):969–978. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.1996.tb01174.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Mauget, R. Behavioural and reproductive strategies in wild forms of Sus scrofa (European wild boar and feral pigs). In The welfare of pigs (pp. 3–15). Springer, Dordrecht. 10.1007/978-94-011-9574-4_1 (1981).
- 67.Gonyou HW. The social behaviour of pigs. Soc. Behav. Farm Anim. 2001;1:147–176. doi: 10.1079/9780851993973.0147. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Jensen P, Wood-Gush DG. Social interactions in a group of free-ranging sows. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 1984;12(4):327–337. doi: 10.1016/0168-1591(84)90125-4. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Bartholomew AJ, Cirulli ET. Individual variation in contagious yawning susceptibility is highly stable and largely unexplained by empathy or other known factors. PLoS ONE. 2014;9(3):1. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0091773. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Reiland S. Growth and skeletal development of the pig. Acta Radiol. Suppl. 1978;358:15–22. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Conrad MS, Dilger RN, Johnson RW. Brain growth of the domestic pig (Sus scrofa) from 2 to 24 weeks of age: a longitudinal MRI study. Dev. Neurosci. 2012;34(4):291–298. doi: 10.1159/000339311. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Kinder HA, Baker EW, West FD. The pig as a preclinical traumatic brain injury model: current models, functional outcome measures, and translational detection strategies. Neural Regen. Res. 2019;14(3):413. doi: 10.4103/1673-5374.245334. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Sauleau P, Lapouble E, Val-Laillet D, Malbert CH. The pig model in brain imaging and neurosurgery. Animal. 2009;3(8):1138–1151. doi: 10.1017/S1751731109004649. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Ryan MC, et al. Miniature pig magnetic resonance spectroscopy model of normal adolescent brain development. J. Neurosci. Methods. 2018;308:173–182. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2018.08.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Gerencsér L, Fraga PP, Lovas M, Újváry D, Andics A. Comparing interspecific socio-communicative skills of socialized juvenile dogs and miniature pigs. Anim. Cogn. 2019;22(6):917–929. doi: 10.1007/s10071-019-01284-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Nawroth C, Ebersbach M, von Borell E. Are juvenile domestic pigs (Sus scrofa domestica) sensitive to the attentive states of humans? The impact of impulsivity on choice behaviour. Behav. Proc. 2013;96:53–58. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2013.03.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Durrell JL, Sneddon IA, Oconnell NE, Whitehead H. Do pigs form preferential associations? Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2004;89:41–52. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2004.05.003(2004). [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Adriaense JEC, Koski SE, Huber L, Lamm C. Challenges in the comparative study of empathy and related phenomena in animals. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2020;112:62–82. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.01.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Dunbar RI, Shultz S. Bondedness and sociality. Behaviour. 2010;1:775–803. doi: 10.1163/000579510X501151. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The datasets generated during and/or analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.