Abstract
Features of sleep were shown to reflect aging, typical sex differences and cognitive abilities of humans. However, these measures are characterized by redundancy and arbitrariness. Our present approach relies on the assumptions that the spontaneous human brain activity as reflected by the scalp-derived electroencephalogram (EEG) during non-rapid eye movement (NREM) sleep is characterized by arrhythmic, scale-free properties and is based on the power law scaling of the Fourier spectra with the additional consideration of the rhythmic, oscillatory waves at specific frequencies, including sleep spindles. Measures derived are the spectral intercept and slope, as well as the maximal spectral peak amplitude and frequency in the sleep spindle range, effectively reducing 191 spectral measures to 4, which were efficient in characterizing known age-effects, sex-differences and cognitive correlates of sleep EEG. Future clinical and basic studies are supposed to be significantly empowered by the efficient data reduction provided by our approach.
Subject terms: Neuroscience, Circadian rhythms and sleep, Computational neuroscience
Introduction
The frequency characteristics of sleep-dependent neuronal oscillations as recorded by scalp EEG are increasingly recognized as potent markers of aging1–3, health and disease4, typical and atypical development and maturation5,6, as well as of neurocognitive features of high practical relevance7–9. However, many of these studies are suffering from increased susceptibility to Type I error as a result of an inherently increased level of “researcher degrees of freedom”. That is, EEG data can be analysed in almost infinite different ways, by focusing on one or another specific electrophysiological phenomenon9,10. Instead of focusing on multiple frequencies or phenomena, our aim is to provide an overall characterization of the broadband NREM sleep EEG. Our data-driven approach is based on the statistical properties of the signal, in order to assess the intercept and the slope, as well as the most prominent/important spectral peak of the Fourier spectrum.
Evidence suggests the linear relationship between the logarithmic amplitude or power of EEG and the logarithm of frequency11–13. Such power law scaling is a general, state-independent feature of cortical EEG, suggesting that the Fourier spectrum can be reliably described by an approximation of the parameters of the following function:
1 |
where P is power (P ≥ 0) as a function of frequency (0 ≤ f ≤ fNyquist), C is the constant (or the intercept) expressing the overall, frequency-independent EEG amplitude (C > 0), whereas α is the spectral exponent indicating the decay rate (slope) of power as a function of frequency. Reported values for the spectral exponent are − 4 < α < − 1, with lower values indicating lower arousal/(deeper) sleep14,15,18. That is, instead of providing 191 values for the power spectra of 0.5–48 Hz activity in bins of 0.25 Hz, the background slope of the spectrum of scale-free activity can be characterized just by two parameters (C and α). Most notably, if reliable, this function suggests that classical bandwise or binwise spectral analyses are not considering the frequency-determined nature of power values when applying statistical tests focusing on specific oscillatory phenomena. Similar views were expressed and successfully implemented for the analysis of various time series characterizing the power law scaling of brain activity in humans, some of them emphasizing the conceptual and methodological aspects12–14,19–23, while others focusing on sleep cycle effects, arousal and consciousness15,16,24, but none of them specifically targeting the issue of interindividual differences. In addition, available reports do not consider the constant (C) or the intercept as a variable of interest in describing sleep–wake EEG features.
Besides the spectral slope and the constant, there are further specific features of the EEG spectrum, known as spectral peaks12,19, which are upward deflections in the decreasing power law trend described by function (1) above. Peaks reflect oscillatory activities of specific frequencies25, which might prevent the reliable estimation of α if they are not considered24,26. In order to deliberately describe the power spectrum by taking into account its prominent peaks, we suggest the inclusion of a peak power function in the formula as follows:
2 |
Peak power (PPeak) at frequency f equals 1 if there is no peak and is larger than 1 if there is a spectral peak at that frequency. Thus, the number of parameters is increased by considering spectral peaks, but is still lower than the values included in the original spectra, as putative “no peak regions” can be compressed in series of all ones. It has to be noted, that PPeak(f) is a whitened power measure, because it is characterized by roughly equal power along the frequency axis and is thus statistically independent from the spectral slope (α) and intercept (C), which constitute the colored-noise or power-law noise part of the spectrum, characterized by an exponential decrease of power with increasing frequencies (Fig. 1). Although it is known that most subjects might have several peaks in their NREM sleep EEG spectra and the peak that is greatest can vary between individuals and recording locations, in the following we only consider the maximal peak in the 9–18 Hz range, emerging at a specific fmaxPeak frequency, with an amplitude exceeding all other potential peak amplitudes (PPeak(fmaxPeak) > PPeak(f) for any 9 < f < 18). No multiple peaks are analysed in this report.
As a proof of concept, we apply these measures on a large sleep EEG dataset with previously demonstrated effects of age, sex, and general intelligence. The issue of individual differences in NREM sleep was not explicitly addressed in former reports on spectral parameters of power law scaled sleep EEG15,16, with the exception of a report focusing exclusively on whitened spectral peak sizes in the spindle range27. Our intention is to fill this gap and broaden the validity of the power law scaling-type of spectral EEG parameters, as well as to provide a set of non-redundant measures of individual differences in NREM sleep EEG.
Age was reported to correlate negatively with NREM sleep EEG slow wave activity, but positively with high frequency activity in healthy adult subjects28. In addition, steeper spectral slopes of wakefulness and NREM sleep-derived EEG were found in young as compared to older subjects11,22.
Thus, we hypothesize (H1) that the slope of the Fast Fourier Transformation (FFT)-based spectrum of NREM sleep EEG is characterized by age-dependent flattening (α closer to 0). In addition, aging was shown to be associated with decreased sleep spindle activity29,30, thus we hypothesize (H2) a negative correlation between age and spectral peak amplitude as measured by PPeak(fmaxPeak) value. In addition to decreased spindle activity, the increase in intra-spindle oscillatory frequency (Hz) was shown to be a characteristic feature of aging according to some29,31, but not all30 reports. As a consequence, we hypothesize (H3) that maximal spectral peak amplitudes in the spindle range emerge at higher fmaxPeak values in aged, as compared to young subjects.
Reported sex differences in NREM and REM sleep EEG indicate higher spectral power in several frequency bands in women, as compared to men28,32. Such broad band and state-independent differences suggest a general tendency for a higher EEG amplitude in women, due to a contamination with non-neuronal factors, like skull thickness and bone mineral density32,33. As a consequence, we hypothesize (H4) that women are characterized by higher spectral intercepts, than men (C♀> C♂). Furthermore, we will reanalyze some of the reported sex differences in sleep spindle density/power, indicating increased sleep spindling in women as compared to men28,32,34,36, by relying on whitened spectral peak amplitudes of the spindle range (PPeak(fmaxPeak)), the latter being a measure which is independent of overall EEG-amplitude (C).
Based on a largely overlapping dataset, formerly we reported another sex difference in terms of sleep spindle frequency: women were shown to be characterized by higher oscillatory frequencies as compared to men36. Thus, our explicit intention is to provide convergent validity of the present method, by testing the following hypothesis (H5): maximal spectral peaks occur at higher frequencies in women as compared to men (fmaxPeak♀ > fmaxPeak♂).
Intelligence was shown to correlate positively with NREM sleep EEG sleep spindle activity7. Although, a recent metaanalysis casts doubt on the sexual dimorphism of this relationship9, the dataset we analyse in our current report is characterized by a clear difference among women and men: women were characterized by positive correlation between sleep spindle amplitude/power and IQ, whereas null correlations were reported for men8,36. As our current analyses are based on the same dataset, we aim to provide convergent validity of our current method by testing the hypothesis (H6): PPeak(fmaxPeak) values of the sleep spindle range (9–18 Hz) correlate positively with IQ in women, but not in men. Intelligence was also reported to modulate the relationship between the decrease in NREM sleep EEG slow activity associated with aging: participants showing average IQ (AIQ) scores were characterized by significant negative correlations regarding age vs. slow wave activity, whereas no such correlations were found in individuals with high IQ (HIQ) in an overlapping sample1. As the original report provided overwhelming evidence for an age vs relative delta power correlation as being modulated by IQ range, whereas weaker evidence was found for absolute power1, we do not know if this finding reflects the age-dependency of slow wave activity per se, or the combined age-dependency of slow wave activity and slow/high activity ratio. The former scenario would fit with a null effect for IQ-modulation of age vs spectral slope correlation, whereas the latter would lead to an IQ-dependence of age vs spectral slope relationship (H7).
Results
Goodness of fit: Is the logarithm of spectral power a linear function of the logarithm of frequency?
NREM sleep EEG spectra of 175 healthy subjects (81 females, age range 17–60 years), with a maximum of 18 available (artefact-free) common recording locations were included in our analyses. Linears were fitted to the equidistant log–log plots of the EEG power spectra below 48 Hz, excluding the 0–2 and the 6–18 Hz range, both known to be characterized by spectral peaks (slow oscillation and spindles) in NREM sleep (Fig. 1, see details in section “Methods”). The sample mean of fitted slopes () varied between − 2.73 (SD = 0.22) and − 2.33 (SD = 0.22) for the frontocentral (Fz) and left posterior temporal (T5) region, respectively. In turn, the sample mean of the intercepts () varied between 3.74 (SD = 0.73) and 5.76 (SD = 0.69) for recording locations T5 and Fz, respectively (Suppl. table 1). Goodness of fit (R2) of the linear model of the equidistant 2–6 and 18–48 Hz spectral data varied in the range of 0.8955–0.9997 across subjects and EEG recording locatios. The square of the Fisher Z-transformed, averaged and back-transformed Pearson correlations between the fitted linear and the spectral data is = 0.9952 (SD = 0.1578).
Here we claim that the spectral slope (α) and the intercept (ln C) carry meaningful information. In order to demonstrate that the present method of determining slope and intercept is comparable with existing methods, we tested these parameters against the respective outputs of a recently published method termed fitting oscillations & one over f (FOOOF)23. Our spectral slopes and intercepts correlated significantly with FOOOF slopes and intercepts, respectively. Mean correlation (Fisher-transformed, averaged and back-transformed) over recording locations was r = 0.90 for spectral slopes and r = 0.92 for intercepts (see Suppl. Figure 1).
Spectral peaks in the 9–18 Hz range
Spectral peaks in the alpha/sigma range were determined by a combination of the first and the second derivative tests indicating local maxima in mathematical terms (see details in section “Methods”). Detected peaks were ranked according to their whitened amplitude (coloured noise characterized by the spectral slope (α) and intercept (C) was removed before ranking). At least one peak was detected in 81.16–100% of the subjects, depending on recording location (relatively lower values were found in the temporal locations T3, and T4, whereas above 90% was the rule for other regions, see details in Suppl. Table 2). Spectral peaks with maximal amplitudes in the 9–18 Hz range were found to conform the overall topography vs frequency relationship of sleep spindles. That is, anterior spectral peaks were slower than the posterior ones (Suppl. Figure 2). The total antero-posterior frequency increase of maximal spectral peaks in the 9–18 Hz range (fmaxPeak) equalled 1.99 Hz (sample mean). However, the above change was largely non-continuous along the antero-posterior cortical axis, as more than 83% (1.67 Hz) of the upward shift in spectral peak frequency emerged in a single, maximal value characterizing the frontal to central (54.77% of the subjects), frontopolar to frontal (36.94%), central to parietal (6.36%) or parietal to occipital (1.91%) shifts. Spectral peak frequencies (fmaxPeak) which were detected rostral to the maximal antero-posterior upward frequency shift are hypothesized to reflect slow sleep spindles (100% of frontopolar, 63.05% of frontal, 8.28% of central, 1.91% of parietal and 0% of occipital recording sites), whereas the caudal ones are reflections of putative fast sleep spindles (0, 36.95, 91.72, 98.09, and 100% of frontopolar, frontal, central, parietal and occipital regions, respectively) (Fig. 2).
A second spectral peak with roughly half of the amplitude of the first was detected in a subgroup of subjects/EEG recording locations (6.29–50.64%) (Suppl. Table 2). However, in most of the cases the second peaks were roughly 1.5 Hz slower than the reported slow sleep spindle frequencies in an overlapping sample36, thus it seems that these peaks reflected rather alpha activity (~ 10 Hz) instead of true slow or fast sleep spindles (see also Suppl. Figure 2). That is the method used in the present study was robust enough in terms of the reliable detection of the dominant spectral peak of sleep spindling in the given location, but not sufficiently sensitive in testing the non-dominant sleep spindle peaks (fast spindles in the anterior and slow spindles in the posterior locations, see Suppl. Table 2 for details). A third peak was only detected in a few instances (between 1 and 6 cases, depending on recording locations, data not shown). Given the fact that we only focus on the maximal spectral peak parameters in the following parts of our paper, we can conclude that these parameters reflect the prevailing anterior slow and the posterior fast spindles, depending on recording location. However, the anatomical boundary between prevailing anterior slow and posterior fast sleep spindles varies among subjects, leading to some uncertainty in the frontal leads, which can express both slow and fast sleep spindles (in 63.05% and 36.95% of the cases, respectively).
H1: Age-associated flattening of spectral slope
Positive association between age (years) and NREM sleep EEG spectral exponents (α), indicating age-associated flattening of slopes were found at all recording locations (Suppl. Table 3a). The Rüger’s area (consisting of spatially contingent recording locations characterized by uncorrected significances) including all recording locations in this specific case, proved to be significant at both of the new critical probability (p) levels (0.025 and 0.017). Thus, based on the Descriptive Data Analysis (DDA, see details in section “Methods”) procedure37,38, this area can be considered as a significant one (see also Fig. 3A).
H2: Age-dependent decrease in spectral peak amplitude
Maximal whitened spectral peak amplitudes of NREM sleep EEG spindle frequencies (PPeak(fmaxPeak)) and age correlated negatively at 10 recording locations covering the frontocentral, parietal and posterior temporal areas (F3, F4, Fz, C3, Cz, C4, T5, T6, P3, and P4). The above region remained significant after the control of multiple testing (see a representative example in Fig. 3B).
H3: No age-related increase in spectral peak frequency was found
Maximal sleep spindle spectral peak emerge at lower fmaxPeak values in the frontal region of aged, as compared to young subjects. This finding evidently contrasts our prediction. Peak frequency and age correlated negatively at 8 recording locations covering the frontal and the right temporal areas (Fp1, Fp2, F3, F4, Fz, F7, F8 and T4). This Rüger’s area was significant, as all correlations conformed to both of the new critical probabilities (Fig. 3D; Suppl. Table 3c).
In order to test if changes in slow/fast spindle peak sizes could underlie these effects, that is if the maximal spindle peak “jumps” from the fast to the slow spindle peak more frequently in frontal recording sites of aged individuals, we compared the age of the following groups of subjects. Group F-Fp was characterized by a maximal antero-posterior frequency increase of fmaxPeak between the frontopolar and frontal recording sites, whereas for the C-F group this frequency shift was measured between the frontal and the central region. Mann–Whitney U-test revealed higher age in the C-F, as compared to the F-Fp group (U = − 2.41; η2 = 0.713; p = 0.015). That is the age-associated dampening of fmaxPeak might indicate a decrease in the emergence of fast sleep spindles in the frontal region in aged subjects.
H4: Spectral intercepts, but not peak amplitudes are higher in women as compared to men
The spectral intercept is the power value at which the spectral slope crosses the y-axis. Women are characterized by significantly higher spectral intercepts [the natural logarithm of C values in formula (1) and (2)] compared to men at all recording locations (see an example at location C4 as an example: Fig. 4).
After correction for multiple testing the Rüger-area remained significant (Suppl. table 4). As predicted women and men did not differ in NREM sleep EEG maximal spectral peak amplitudes of the spindle range (PPeak(fmaxPeak)) at any of the recording locations (Fig. 4; Suppl. table 4b).
H5: Women are characterized by faster sleep spindles
Women were characterized by significantly higher fmaxPeak values as compared to men (Fig. 4), except temporal recording locations T3 and T4. The area remained significant after the correction for multiple testing (Table 1).
Table 1.
Recording location | U (η2) |
p | N♀ | Md♀ (Q1–Q3)♀ |
N♂ | Md♂ (Q1–Q3) ♂ |
Md♀–Md♂ |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Fp1 |
1888 (.076) |
.001 | 67 |
11.97 (11.36–12.45) |
83 |
11.32 (10.87–11.93) |
0.65 |
Fp2 |
1864 (.100) |
< .001 | 68 |
12.00 (11.33–12.44) |
87 |
11.29 (10.83–11.81) |
0.71 |
F3 |
2191 (.095) |
< .001 | 75 |
12.80 (12.10–13.25) |
91 |
11.86 (11.14–12.86) |
0.94 |
F4 |
2217 (.088) |
< .001 | 76 |
12.98 (12.06–13.35) |
89 |
11.80 (11.04–12.97) |
1.18 |
Fz |
2259 (.028) |
.041 | 66 |
13.06 (11.75–13.41) |
85 |
12.51 (11.10–13.13) |
0.55 |
F7 |
1492 (.090) |
< .001 | 59 |
12.23 (11.60–12.66) |
78 |
11.59 (11.19–12.04) |
0.64 |
F8 |
1608 (.088) |
< .000 | 63 |
12.14 (11.53–12.59) |
78 |
11.49 (11.13–12.14) |
0.65 |
C3 |
2651 (.058) |
.002 | 80 |
13.53 (13.20–13.96) |
92 |
13.26 (12.86–13.59) |
0.27 |
C4 |
2502 (.075) |
< .001 | 79 |
13.60 (13.33–14.04) |
93 |
13.28 (12.96–13.61) |
0.32 |
Cz |
1830 (.107) |
< .001 | 68 |
13.68 (13.39–14.13) |
87 |
13.33 (13.05–13.64) |
0.35 |
P3 |
2290 (.118) |
< .001 | 81 |
13.71 (13.38–14.12) |
94 |
13.36 (13.03–13.68) |
0.35 |
P4 |
2368 (.102) |
< .001 | 81 |
13.70 (13.37–14.12) |
93 |
13.38 (13.06–13.68) |
0.32 |
T3 |
1829 (.002) |
.635 | 55 |
12.80 (12.19–13.42) |
70 |
12.91 (11.79–13.37) |
− 0.11 |
T4 |
1942 (.005) |
.440 | 57 |
12.94 (12.14–13.39) |
74 |
12.93 (11.65–13.29) |
0.01 |
T5 |
1893 (.084) |
< .001 | 68 |
13.62 (13.27–14.06) |
84 |
13.32 (13.00–13.63) |
0.27 |
T6 |
1730 (.108) |
< .001 | 66 |
13.63 (13.35–14.11) |
85 |
13.33 (12.97–13.62) |
0.30 |
O1 |
2282 (.111) |
< .001 | 80 |
13.65 (13.34–14.10) |
93 |
13.33 (12.96–13.64) |
0.32 |
O2 |
2253 (.112) |
< .001 | 80 |
13.65 (13.33–14.12) |
92 |
13.35 (12.99–13.64) |
0.30 |
Mann–Whitney U test indicates that women as compared to men are characterized by higher fmaxPeak values at which spindle range PPeak(f) maxima emerge. The Rüger area containes 16 nominally significant effects. 15 of these women vs men differences were significant at both of the more stringent criteria (p < .025 and p < .017), which supports the significance of the area.
Italic and bold italic values indicate statistical significance at p < .05, and p < .017, respectively.
Md median.
These findings might be confounded by the factor spindle type in the case if fast spindles are dominant in more anterior leads in females as compared to males. However, the analysis of the localization of the major antero-posterior frequency shift in fmaxPeak of women and men did not reveal a significant difference (χ2 = 0.42; p = 0.51).
H6: IQ correlates positively with spectral peak amplitude in women
Pearson correlations revealed significant associations of whitened maximal spectral peak amplitudes (PPeak(fmaxPeak)) pertaining to NREM sleep EEG spindle activity with IQ at recording locations C3 (N = 67, r = 0.33, p = 0.007), C4 (N = 66, r = 0.34, p = 0.005), Cz (N = 55, r = 0.34, p = 0.010), P3 (N = 68, r = 0.26, p = 0.031), P4 (N = 68, r = 0.28, p = 0.020), and T3 (N = 45, r = 0.32, p = 0.031) in women (Fig. 5; Suppl. table 5). The Rüger area at this centroparietal-left temporal region remained significant after the control for multiple testing (4/6 correlations are significant at 0.05/2 and 3/6 correlations at 0.05/3). No significant correlations of whitened spectral peak amplitude and IQ were found in men.
H7: Do age-related flattenings of spectral slopes differ among subjects with average and high IQ?
As already presented in the former subheadings (H1) an age-associated flattening of spectral slopes characterizes the NREM sleep EEG of adult volunteers. This effect was separately assessed in subjects with average and high IQ, and results were compared. Age and slopes of the NREM sleep EEG spectra (α) were significantly associated in both subgroups (AIQ and HIQ). We found no significant difference between these correlations, however (Table 2). That is, age-associated flattening of the slopes of the NREM sleep EEG spectra are independent of the subjects’ IQ (Fig. 3C).
Table 2.
Recording location | ρAIQ | pAIQ | NAIQ | ρHIQ | pHIQ | NHIQ | pdifference |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Fp1 | .44 | < .001 | 79 | .40 | .001 | 60 | .787 |
Fp2 | .44 | < .001 | 85 | .45 | < .001 | 63 | .901 |
F3 | .48 | < .001 | 84 | .41 | .001 | 64 | .622 |
F4 | .52 | < .001 | 83 | .42 | .001 | 64 | .476 |
Fz | .57 | < .001 | 70 | .45 | < .001 | 60 | .370 |
F7 | .39 | .001 | 70 | .45 | < .001 | 58 | .660 |
F8 | .45 | < .001 | 69 | .43 | .001 | 59 | .900 |
C3 | .44 | < .001 | 84 | .45 | < .001 | 64 | .956 |
C4 | .45 | < .001 | 85 | .43 | < .001 | 64 | .896 |
Cz | .47 | < .001 | 70 | .37 | .004 | 60 | .507 |
P3 | .39 | < .001 | 85 | .42 | .001 | 64 | .801 |
P4 | .42 | < .001 | 85 | .41 | .001 | 64 | .927 |
T3 | .43 | < .001 | 70 | .49 | < .001 | 59 | .640 |
T4 | .51 | < .001 | 70 | .42 | .001 | 60 | .507 |
T5 | .32 | .007 | 70 | .45 | < .001 | 58 | .412 |
T6 | .40 | .001 | 70 | .42 | .001 | 60 | .896 |
O1 | .31 | .004 | 85 | .40 | .001 | 64 | .549 |
O2 | .34 | .002 | 84 | .41 | .001 | 64 | .610 |
Spearman rank correlations (ρ) were significant in both intelligence groups, however, the differences between the higher (HIQ) and average (AIQ) intelligence groups was not significant (pdifference). Bold italic values indicate statistical significance at p < .017.
Overcoming model redundancy by determining the alternative intercept of the spectra
Although our model resulted in good fit with empirical data in terms of background (scale-free) activity and the majority of our hypotheses (including the ones regarding peak power features) were supported by parameters derived from Eq. (2), the spectral slope and the intercept are far from being independent in statistical terms. That is, although women vs men differences emerged in our spectral intercepts (ln C♀ > ln C♂) as predicted in H4 (see Suppl. table 4), and no sex differences in NREM sleep EEG spectral slopes (α) were observed (Suppl. table 6a), the intercepts and the slopes are negatively correlated in our database (Suppl. table 6b): subjects with steeper spectral slopes (lower α exponents) are characterized by higher intercepts (apparently higher EEG amplitudes). This might reflect the position of the intercept, which is at ln f = 0 (f = 1 Hz). The interpolated 1 Hz power (based on the fitted line in the double logarithmic plots) partially reflects the steepness of the slope of the spectrum.
In order to overcome the above issue of parameter-interdependency, we derived alternative intercepts with the aim of determining parts of the interpolated scale-free spectrum at which our parameter do not reflects the steepness of the slope (α). We based our search for this alternative intercept on two assumptions: (1) the alternative (“slope-free”) intercept is situated at the border of low and high frequency activities, delineated by the reported sleep deprivation-induced increases and decreases of spectral power, respectively; (2) intercepts below the border mentioned in point 1. correlate negatively with the spectral slopes, whereas intercepts above this border correlate positively with slopes. Extended wakefulness of human adults is known to increase the NREM sleep EEG spectral power below the sleep spindle frequencies, that is the power of 1–9, 1–12 or 1–13 Hz according to different studies39–43, whereas power above 10 or 13 Hz was shown to be decreased during recovery sleep40,42,43. Thus, we used our fitted model parameters α and ln C, as well as the modified version of formula (3), with the last term (ln PPeak(f)) omitted (see “Methods”) to determine the interpolated scale-free natural logarithm power ln P(f) at frequencies of f = 7.4, 10, 12.2, 13.5, 15 and 20 Hz corresponding to natural logarithm values of ln f = 2, 2.3, 2.5, 2.6, 2.7, and 3, respectively. These alternative intercepts representing different scenarios of y-axis crosses (changing the position of the y-axis) were tested for their independence from the slopes (α) by Pearson correlations (Fig. 6). The pattern of correlations supported our assumptions: alternative intercepts below 12.2 Hz were found to correlate negatively with spectral slopes, whereas above 12.2 or 13.5 Hz (depending on electrode location) positive correlations were found. That is the best “slope-free” intercepts in the scale-free part of the parametrized NREM sleep EEG spectra are emerging at 12.2 Hz and the 13.5 Hz for anterior and posterior recording locations, respectively (ln C2.5 and ln C2.6). The original intercept derived at ln f = 0 could be termed as ln C0, according to this terminology. We reanalyzed our hypothesis based on the assumption of higher intercepts in women as compared to men, which is the only hypothesis involving term C of the formula (H4). Substituting ln C0 with ln C2.5 and ln C2.6 resulted in increased mean effects sizes (larger intercepts in women) from = 0.084 to = 0.118 (both averaged over recording locations).
Discussion
When analyzing the Fourier spectra of EEG records performed for long periods of sleep, researchers and clinicians rely on statistics. That is, the periodograms of short modified EEG segments are averaged in order to obtain the averaged spectra44. As a consequence, the spectral profiles are inherently statistical in nature. In coherence with former reports focusing on sleep stages and arousal15,16 our current approach provides a characterization of the NREM sleep EEG Fourier spectra by taking into account their inherent electrophysiological and statistical regularities based on its power law scaling properties. Our focus is on individual differences in NREM sleep and we assume that the approach we follow results in an integrated characterization of NREM sleep EEG, which is superior in terms of construct validity and accuracy. First of all, a frequency-independent amplitude measure potentially reflecting a contamination of neuronal and non-neuronal factors, like brain activity and skull anatomy, can be reliably separated and is not mixed up in power spectral values focusing on specific oscillatory phenomena. Although the natural logarithm of term C derived from formula (1) and (2) (ln C0) reliably reflects the hypothesized sex differences, the model could be refined by using alternative intercepts, which were independent from the slopes (ln C2.5 and ln C2.6) (Fig. 6). Thus, we were able to determine the slope free intercepts, which—according to our best knowledge – is a first explicit and successful attempt to build a non-redundant, power law scaling-based mathematical model of sleep EEG spectra. The slope free intercept might constitute an ideal normalization value for NREM sleep EEG (spectra) in future basic and clinical studies.
In addition to the spectral intercepts, the power law functions describing the sleep EEG spectra appropriately address the issue of the ratio of EEG power at different frequencies, providing a single measure (α), instead of several ones scattered redundantly in all frequency bins and bands. This approach was found to be effective in deriving measures of consciousness24, sleep stages15 and arousal16 from sleep EEG records, as well as to index aging as a function of scale free wake EEG features22. Here we complete these studies with the individual differences approach of NREM sleep EEG, which was suggested to be a seminal perspective of all sleep studies45.
Last, but not least, spectral peak amplitudes (PPeak(f)) are whitened in our approach, that is, the scale-free part of the spectrum is effectively controlled, which might enable researchers to differentiate background and oscillatory activities at specific frequencies.
The findings derived from our approach of parametrizing the NREM sleep EEG spectra clearly supports the robustness and validity of the method presented in this paper, which was inspired by studies aiming to whiten the spectral power in the sleep spindle frequency27,46. As predicted (H1), age correlates positively with NREM sleep EEG spectral exponents (Suppl. Table 3a), indicating that aging is associated with flattening of the Fourier spectra (i.e. exponents closer to 0) (Fig. 3). This finding coheres with reports of bandwise power spectral analyses of NREM sleep EEG, indicating decreased low and increased high frequency activity in the NREM sleep EEG of healthy aged subjects28. Moreover, the steepness of the slope of the linear describing the relationship between the log-amplitude and the log-frequency of NREM sleep EEG revealed the same age-dependency11. Thus, our method is capable of extracting spectral slope information with sufficient precision and is a valid and simple approach to be used in future (translational) studies. The slope of the spectrum is basically a measure of the constant ratio between low and high frequency activities, which was hypothesized to reflect the ratio between inhibition and excitation, the depth of sleep and/or the level of conscious awareness15,24,47,48. Findings might indicate that aged subjects have lower sleep depth, but might also open new avenues beyond the exclusive focus on sleep slow waves/oscillation when studying the relationship between aging and sleep. The latter point is supported by our finding on the lack of a difference in the age-dependency of the NREM sleep EEG spectral slopes in subjects with average and high intelligence (Table 2). This finding apparently contrasts the outcomes of our previous report on the significant differences in age-dependent declines in NREM sleep EEG slow wave/oscillation of average and high IQ subjects. That is in terms of NREM sleep EEG slow waves high IQ subjects tend to age at a slower pace than average IQ subjects1. In spite of the fact that the database we used in the two studies are the same, the methods (band-limited spectral analysis focusing on specific frequencies vs. spectral exponent extraction) yield different results. Besides trivial methodological differences (spectral power vs exponent), our current approach of excluding the 0–2 Hz range from slope-fitting might contribute to this difference. That is, our present findings indicate that average and high IQ subjects tend to age at a same pace, at least in terms of their NREM sleep EEG spectral exponents. These contrasting results indicate that our former findings are preferentially reflecting the age- and IQ-dependency of the NREM sleep EEG slow oscillatory mechanism per se, but not the scale-free activity and/or the constant ratio of slow and high frequency activities. The latter could be a subject of aging which is at least partially independent from the well characterized age-dependent decreases in slow oscillations49 and is equally present in average and high IQ subjects. Recent findings and considerations suggest that the spectral slope derived from an electrophysiological signal indicates the ratio of excitation and inhibition in the underlying neural tissue47. Thus, according to our current findings and previously published modeling data47 aging is characterized by a relative increase in excitation over inhibition during the state of night time NREM sleep, and this effect seems to be relatively independent from the decreased slow oscillation reported in former studies1,49.
Aging was also shown to be associated with decreased sleep spindle frequency activity and decreased phasic sleep spindles in former studies30. These findings cohere with our current report of an age-associated decrease in whitened spectral peak amplitudes of NREM sleep EEG spindle frequency range (Suppl. Table 3b). Reports suggest that the age-dependent decrease in sleep spindles recorded over the prefrontal regions mediates the cognitive decline in later ages50. Moreover, it was suggested that this effect reflects the disruption of thalamocortical regulatory mechanisms involved in sleep spindle rhythmogenesis51. Thus, our index of whitened NREM sleep EEG spectral peak amplitude in the spindle frequency range could serve as a potential biomarker of the above mentioned50,51 neurocognitive aspects of aging.
The age-associated increases in the frequency of sleep spindle oscillations (also known as intraspindle frequencies) were reported in several former reports31, although the largest study did not reveal such changes in adulthood30. Our present findings reveal a non-predicted decrease in maximal frontal spectral peak amplitude in the spindle frequency range of NREM sleep EEG. The range of the spindle frequency changes clearly indicate a change from the predominant fast (~ 14 Hz) to predominant slow (~ 12 Hz) sleep spindle spectral peaks during aging. That is, our finding indicates a decrease in relative frontal emergence of fast sleep spindles during aging, rather than a deceleration of sleep spindles at a rate of 0.5 Hz/decade (Fig. 3). This post-hoc assumption is supported by our additional empirical findings indicating the increasing rates of frontal slow sleep spindle dominance in the aged. In addition, the parietal recording locations, which are almost uniformly characterized by fast sleep spindle types of spectral peaks over the whole sample, do not provide any evidence for an age-dependent acceleration of intraspindle oscillatory frequencies. That is, our findings clearly do not support this hypothesis.
Women were shown to be characterized by significantly higher NREM sleep EEG spectral intercepts as compared to men. This difference is not seen in the spectral slopes and is sharpened when using the alternative (“slope-free”) intercepts (ln C2.5 and ln C2.6 instead of ln C0). To the best of our knowledge this is the first report explicitly targeting these issues. We based our hypothesis on findings suggesting that women vs men differences in EEG power are largely frequency-independent28, thus indicating an overall amplitude effect captured by the term C in formula (1) and (2). That is, previous reports focusing on specific frequency ranges and oscillatory phenomena are confounded by overall amplitude differences in the EEG of women and men. Examples for such potentially confounded findings are reports on women vs men differences in sleep spindle densities/occurrences. Spindles detected by fixed thresholds34,35 or raw (non-whitened) spectral power values of the spindle frequency range28,32 indicate sex differences (increased sleep spindle density/activity in women), but are not controlled for overall amplitude differences. It has to be noted however, that one of the early publications cited above hypothesized that women vs men differences in sleep EEG spectral power might reflect sex differences in skull thickness32, but—at least to our best knowledge—this hypothesis remained largely unexplored from the electrophysiological point of view. Our current approach considers this issue and provides a reliable and potentially useful method for controlling non-specific effects, potentially contaminated with non-neuronal issues in EEG amplitude. The estimation of the spectral intercept provides a simple index which can be included in future biophysical, electrophysiological-modeling studies of the skull-thickness-EEG power issue. Our current findings clearly indicate the lack of sex differences in sleep spindle power when overall amplitude women vs men differences are controlled (Fig. 4; Suppl. table 4).
Women were shown to be characterized by higher frequency sleep spindle oscillations as compared to men according to our former study based on the individual adjustment of sleep spindle frequencies and amplitudes36. This finding was strengthened by our current report based on the detection of whitened spectral peak location with 0.0052 Hz resolution (Table 1). That is, our current finding strengthens the validity of our spectral parametrization approach. In addition, the hypotheses suggesting that sleep spindle frequency is accelerated by either progesterone and its neuroactive, indirect GABA-agonist metabolite allopregnanolone52 or the progesterone-induced hyperthermia53 during the follicular phase of the menstrual cycle in women are indirectly supported by our present findings. Although our participants were not controlled for menstrual cycle phases and oral contraceptive use, we can assume that at least some of the female subjects were examined during the follicular phase of their menstrual cycle. Furthermore, oral contraceptive use involve the intake of progestagenic compounds, which might induce some of the neural effects of endogenous progesterone in naturally cycling women.
Here we reveal a positive correlation between whitened spectral peak amplitude of sleep spindle frequency activity during NREM sleep and IQ in women, but not in men (Fig. 5; Suppl. table 5). Intelligence was shown to be reflected in the intensity (amplitude and/or density) of phasic sleep spindle events or alternatively in the spectral power of sleep spindle frequency activity during NREM sleep7–10,36. In the database we use in our present study a marked sexual dimorphism of this effect was also revealed: women but not men were shown to be characterized by the sleep spindle amplitude/power vs IQ correlations8,36. Although this latter effect was not unequivocally reflected in a significant meta-regression between effect size and % female in the sample in a subsequent metaanalysis9, here we refer to it because convergent findings obtained by different methods used on the same dataset are an issue of validity of the methods. That is, we reproduced the positive sleep spindle vs. IQ correlation in women by using a linear fitting approach to the log–log spectra of NREM sleep and a concurrent whitening of spectral peaks, without assumptions on time-domain sleep spindle features. Again, this finding might strengthen our views on the reliability of the method of analyzing the constant, the slope and the (whitened) peak attributes of the NREM sleep EEG in human subjects.
Among the shortcomings of our work we would emphasize the lack of slow vs fast sleep spindle differentiation by the current version of our method, as well as the fact that we disregarded low frequency power (< 2 Hz) when fitting the slopes. Fitting of two slightly overlapping spectral peaks instead of just one, would increase considerably the complexity of the approach, whereas our intention was to keep the process as simple and intuitive as possible. Moreover, we intended to follow the already published method of finding the maximal peak in the spindle frequency range and correlating its amplitude/power with neurological-clinical and cognitive data27,46. Similarly, the potential and largely unpredictable contamination of low frequency power with sweating artefacts, as well as the high-pass filtering effects of gold-coated electrodes54 we used in our studies precluded us from a precise measurement of the power law scaling at low frequencies below 2 Hz. Our approach of excluding the alpha and spindle frequencies before fitting a linear to the equidistant double logarithmic NREM sleep EEG power spectra requires a priori knowledge on the position of the spectral peaks and, as a consequence, increases the researchers degrees of freedom. In addition, this approach inherently omits a wide range of frequencies when fitting the linear. Although, there are reported methods for handling the above issues14, here we focused on the current method because of our the explicit intention of comparability with former reports focusing on NREM sleep EEG power spectra and neurocognition27,46.
In sum, the parametrization of NREM sleep EEG of healthy adult subjects by relying on the power law scaling behavior of the electrical activity of the brain, as well as by completing this statistical property with the prominent spectral peak at the sleep spindle range, provides an integral method of describing and characterizing individual differences in sleep and cognition. Here we show, that most of the features of NREM sleep EEG can be efficiently compressed in the spectral intercepts, slopes and peaks, at least in terms of demographic (age, sex) and cognitive (IQ) correlates of sleep. It remains to be determined, if known arousal and sleep state-dependent changes15,16 or overnight sleep dynamics48 can be efficiently completed with measures of sleep regulatory mechanisms (e.g. homeostatic and circadian factors) derived from our integrative parameters of NREM sleep EEG spectra. In addition, further studies are needed for an adequate handling of multiple spectral peaks and low frequency (< 2 Hz) oscillations in the non-full-band EEG.
Methods
Subjects/databases
Data was combined from multiple databases (Max Planck Institute of Psychiatry, Munich, Germany; Institute of Behavioural Sciences of Semmelweis University, Budapest, Hungary) for this retrospective multicenter study3,8. Polysomnography data were recorded from 175 participants 81 females, 94 males, mean age 29.57 years, age range 17–60 years) and IQ scores were measured for 149 participants (68 females, 81 males, mean age 29.23 years, age range 17–60 years). Volunteers were recruited also via Mensa Germany and Mensa Hungary to increase the number of highly intelligent individuals. As some of the participants have missing data of some electrodes and/or IQ scores the data numbers from which the statistical analysis was conducted are always reported in the results.
Based on self-reports, none of the participants had a history of psychiatric or neurological disorders. Alcohol consumption was restricted before recording, but a small amount of caffeine (max. 2 cups of coffee before noon) was allowed to the participants. Based on self reports 8 participants were light or moderate smokers. Data were combined from multiple databases (Max Planck Institute of Psychiatry, Munich, Germany; Institute of Behavioural Sciences of Semmelweis University, Budapest, Hungary). The experiment was conducted in full accordance with the World Medical Association Helsinki Declaration and all applicable national laws and it was approved by the institutional review board, the Ethical Committee of the Semmelweis University, Budapest, or the Ludwig Maximilian University, Munich. Written informed consent was obtained from adults participants and parents/guardians of the children (age: 17 years).
Psychometric intelligence
Standardized nonverbal intelligence tests were recorded from 149 participants: 70 of them completed the Culture Fair Test (CFT)55,56 and 39 of them completed the Raven Advanced Progressive Matrices (Raven APM)57 test. 40 participants completed both tests. These tests have been shown to similarly measure abstract pattern completion and are particularly good measures of the general factor of intelligence58–60. A composite raw intelligence test score was calculated, expressed as a Raven equivalent score (RES)1. RES for Raven APM tests was equal to the actual raw test score, whereas RES of the CFT test raw scores were equal to the Raven APM score corresponding to the IQ percentile derived from CFT performance and the age of the participant. Scores were averaged for participants who completed both tests. Standardization of APM was applied according to 1993 Des Moines (Iowa). Based on their mean IQ score, the sample was split into an average (AIQ: 88 < IQ < 120; = 106.9; N = 85) and a high intelligence (HIQ: 120 ≤ IQ < 156; = 130.38; N = 64) subgroup1.
Polysomnography recordings
Detailed data recording procedures and power spectral analysis are also reported in published studies2,8. Sleep data were recorded on two consecutive nights by standard polysomnography including EEG, electro-occulography (EOG), electrocardiography (ECG) and bipolar submental electromyography (EMG). EEG channels were placed according to the international 10–20 system (Fp1, Fp2, F3, F4, Fz, F7, F8, C3, C4, Cz, P3, P4, T3, T4, T5, T6, O1, O2 and left and right mastoids)61. Impedances for the EEG electrodes were kept below 8 kΩ. The sampling frequency was either 249 Hz, 250 Hz or 1024 Hz, depending on recording site (Suppl. table 7). Data were offline re-referenced to the average of the mastoid signals and notch filtered at 50 Hz. Electrodes excluded from the analysis due to artifacts and/or recording failures were treated as missing data. The number missing data for the total 175 participants is reported in Supplementary Table 8, separately for each electrode. Recordings of the first night were used for habituation and therefore were not included in further analyses. Sleep data of the second night in the laboratory were scored for sleep-waking states and stages according to standard AASM criteria on a 20-s basis62 by an expert. Furthermore, artefactual segments were marked on a 4-s basis and excluded from further analyses.
Power spectral analysis
Power spectral densities were calculated for the NREM (N2 and N3) sleep, in 0.25 Hz bins from 0 Hz to the Nyquist frequency (fNyquist) by relying on 4 s Hanning-tapered, non-artefactual windows. A 50% overlap was used for consecutive windows, whereas mixed-radix FFT for calculating power spectral densities. Power spectral densities from all 4 s windows were then averaged. As data were recorded with different EEG devices producing different analog filter characteristics, average power spectral densities were corrected as follows1: An analog waveform generator was connected to the C3 and C4 electrode positions of all EEG devices and sinusoid signals of various frequencies (0.05 Hz, every 0.1 Hz between 0.1–2 Hz, every 1 Hz between 2–20 Hz, every 10 Hz between 10–100 Hz) were generated with 40 and 355 μV amplitudes. The amplitude reduction rate of each recording system at each frequency was determined by calculating the proportion between digital (measured) and analog (generated) amplitudes of sinusoid signals at the corresponding frequency. The amplitude reduction rate was averaged for the 40 and 355 μV at each frequency. The reduction rate at the intermediate frequencies were interpolated by spline interpolation. The measured power spectral density values were corrected with the device-specific amplitude reduction rate by dividing the original value with the squared amplitude reduction rate at the corresponding frequency according to previous suggestions63,64.
Estimation of the spectral intercepts and slopes
Basically our approach is based on obtaining the power spectrum of the EEG signal (see above), fitting a line to the log–log power and performing a peak detection. In order to manage the second step the power law function (formula (2)) was transformed to one which fits in the double logarithmic plots as follows (Fig. 1C):
3 |
This means that the natural logarithm of spectral power (P) is expressed as a linear function of the natural logarithm of frequency (f). In addition, there are two terms in the equation: the natural logarithm of the constant (C) and the natural logarithm of peak power (PPeak, see Fig. 1). If the latter equals 1 (PPeak = 1), that is, there is no peak at a given frequency f, the value is 0 (ln 1 = 0). The logarithmic frequency scale inherently induces increasing data density at higher frequencies. Thus, a linear fit to this data would induce a strong bias against low frequency bins, which would contribute less to the determination of slopes compared to the higher frequency bins. In order to manage this problem and obtain an equal distribution of the data points, power values were interpolated up to the smallest frequency step (0.0052 Hz) by the piecewise cubic Hermite interpolation method. In the next step a linear was fitted to the 2–48 Hz frequency range of this equidistant log–log plot, excluding the 6.0052–17.9948 Hz frequency range corresponding to the alpha and spindle bands (in order to avoid those parts of the NREM sleep EEG spectra which are characterized by oscillatory activities as well). This part of our procedure was inspired by two former studies using a similar approach for whitening of the NREM sleep spindle spectra27,46. The slope of the linear is α, whereas its intercept is ln C.
Our intention to determine alternative, slope-free intercepts of the linear function in the double logarithmic plot was performed by using individually fitted α and C values in an alternative version of formula (3). In this case the term expressing spectral peak power PPeak(f) was omitted, and ln P(f) values were calculated for f values equalling 7.4, 10, 12.2, 13.5, 15 or 20 Hz. The goal of this step was the determination of the scale-free part of the spectrum at which the line cross of the y-axis is statistically independent from the slope of the line (α) across subjects.
Estimation of the spectral peak frequencies
Spectral peak frequency was determined in the 9–18 Hz range, separately for each EEG recording location by automatically defining local maxima in mathematical terms. That is, we used the first derivative test in order to find the critical points, followed by the second derivative test to differentiate local maxima and minima. A spectral peak was accepted if the first order derivative was zero and the second order derivative was negative. Calculations were performed as follows: a second-degree polynomial curve fitting was performed using all sets of successive bin triplets (0.75 Hz), with an overlap of 2 bins (0.5 Hz) in the 9–18 Hz range resulting in equations of the following type:
4 |
P: power; f: frequency (9–18 Hz); a, b, and c: fitted parameters.
The first derivative of these functions were calculated for each triplet, resulting in:
5 |
The slope of the function described in formula (5) is 2a, which was considered as the derivative at the middle of the triplets, resulting in the first derivative function of the spectra. The procedure was repeated for calculating the second derivatives: in this case the first order derivative function served as an input for fitting the quadratic polynomials.
Zero-crossings of the first derivatives were determined by spline interpolation (interpolating the series between the bins of 0.25 Hz). In addition, the second derivative was interpolated by the spline method at each detected zero crossing of the first derivatives. The cases which were characterized by the co-ocurrences of the two criteria below were considered as spectral peak frequencies:
6.1 |
6.2 |
Estimation of the spectral peak amplitudes
Spectral power at peak frequencies were estimated by spline interpolation of the double logarithmic plots of the power spectra. The spectral peak amplitude was then whitened by subtracting the estimated power based on the fitted linear function from the peak power containing both arrhythmic and rhythmic activity (Fig. 2):
7 |
In order to avoid negative amplitudes due to the logarithmic scale, the power values were shifted for being all positive before this subtraction by adding a constant. This latter step was applied for the calculation of the amplitude measures only. As multiple spectral peaks were detected for some of the participants/EEG recording locations, the one with the largest amplitude was determined and used in this study. If no spectral peak was found in the spindle frequency range, peak values were considered as missing data (see Suppl. table 8). Data analysis was performed by MATLAB 9.5 (Mathworks Inc., https://www.mathworks.com).
Antero-posterior changes in prevailing spectral peak frequency
Frequency measures of spectral peaks with maximal amplitudes (PPeak(fmaxPeak)) were analyzed in terms of antero-posterior changes as follows. First, we formed (para)sagittal regions by averaging fmaxPeak values in frontopolar (Fp1, Fp2), frontal (F3, F4, Fz), central (C3, C4, Cz), parietal (P3, P4, Pz), as well as occipital (O1, O2) recording locations. In the following, the regional means of fmaxPeak values were serially subtracted in consecutive antero-posterior regions as follows: frontal-frontopolar, central-frontal, parietal-central, occipital-parietal. Outputs express the antero-posterior shifts in fmaxPeak (Hz), with positive values indicating antero-posterior increases in frequency (Fig. 2). The successive frequency shifts were summed for each subject, whereas the results of this addition were averaged over the whole sample. In a separate analysis maximal frequency shifts were determined and localized in each subject, resulting a sample mean of maximal antero-posterior frequency shift and a topographical distribution of this shifts.
FOOOF analyses
In order to test the convergent validity of our analyes we run separate analyses with a recently published method23. The same frequency range (2–48 Hz) was analyzed and the so-called knee-parameter, unique to the FOOOF-method was omitted, because the latter was specifically designed to describe the lowest frequency end of the spectrum (which we did not include in our analyses).
Statistical analyses
Goodness of fit of the linear to the equidistant log–log spectral data was assessed by Pearson product moment correlations, which were Fisher Z-transformed, averaged and back-transformed according to Silver and Dunlap65. Last, but not least the resulting average R-value were squared in order to determine the shared variance. Standard deviation (SD) was assessed from the Fisher-Z-transformed dataset, and the resulting value was back-transformed as well.
We used parametric tests (Pearson correlation, independent sample t-test) on normally distributed data and non-parametric tests (Spearman’s rank correlation, Mann–Whitney U test) when the distribution of the data was not Gaussian. The normality of the distributions was analysed by Shapiro–Wilk tests. In order to control Type 1 statistical errors due to multiple electrodes/hypothesis, we used a version of the Descriptive Data Analysis (DDA) protocol37 adapted for neurophysiological data38,66. This procedure tests the global null hypothesis (“all individual null hypotheses in the respective region are true”) at level 0.05, against the alternative that at least one of the null hypotheses is wrong. DDA considers the intercorrelations between the different electrodes and is based on defining Rüger’s areas67, which are sets of spatially contingent conventionally (descriptively) significant (p < 0.05) results. The global significance of the Rüger area means that at least 1/3 of the descriptive significances are significant at a p = 0.05/3 = 0.017 and/or 1/2 of the descriptive significances are significant at p = 0.05/2 = 0.025. We used both criteria simultaneously (the “and” operator) in this study. In order to obtain a better localization of regions with significant correlations, associations between NREM sleep EEG spindle frequency whitened spectral peak amplitudes and IQ were represented by significant probability maps68.
Ethical statements
We confirm that we have read the Journal’s position on issues involved in ethical publication and affirm that this report is consistent with those guidelines.
Supplementary Information
Acknowledgements
We would like to thank Mensa Germany and Mensa Hungary for their help in volunteer recruitment and Bence Schneider for his help in FOOOF analyses. Research supported by the Hungarian Medical Research Council (ETT-162/2003; https://ett.aeek.hu/en/secretariat/), the Hungarian National Research, Development and Innovation Office (K-128117; https://nkfih.gov.hu/about-the-office), the Bial Foundation (https://www.bial.com/com/bial-foundation/) the Higher Education Institutional Excellence Program of the Ministry of Human Capacities in Hungary, within the framework of the Neurology thematic program of the Semmelweis University (http://semmelweis.hu/english/), the Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (NWO; https://www.nwo.nl/en), the European Cooperation in Science and Technology (COST Action CA18106; https://www.cost.eu/), as well as the general budgets of the Institute of Behavioural Sciences, Semmelweis University (http://semmelweis.hu/magtud/en/) and the Max Planck Institute of Psychiatry (https://www.psych.mpg.de/en). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
Author contributions
R.B. and O.S. conceived the study. R.B., P.S. M.Z., M.D. acquired data. R.B., O.S., C.H., P.P.U., F.G., A.P. analyzed data. All authors contributed to the interpretation of the findings as well as to the writing and revision of the manuscript.
Data availability
All corrected power spectral data, as well as the fitted parameters and the program code used are available at https://osf.io/c487g/.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher's note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary Information
The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1038/s41598-021-81230-7.
References
- 1.Pótári A, et al. Age-related changes in sleep EEG are attenuated in highly intelligent individuals. Neuroimage. 2017;146:554–560. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.09.039. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Ujma PP, Simor P, Steiger A, Dresler M, Bódizs R. Individual slow-wave morphology is a marker of aging. Neurobiol. Aging. 2019;80:71–82. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2019.04.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Ujma PP, et al. Sleep EEG functional connectivity varies with age and sex, but not general intelligence. Neurobiol. Aging. 2019;78:87–97. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2019.02.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Kaskie RE, Ferrarelli F. Sleep disturbances in schizophrenia: what we know, what still needs to be done. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 2019;34:68–71. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2019.09.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Campbell IG, Grimm KJ, de Bie E, Feinberg I. Sex, puberty, and the timing of sleep EEG measured adolescent brain maturation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2012;109:5740–5743. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1120860109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Bódizs R, Gombos F, Kovács I. Sleep EEG fingerprints reveal accelerated thalamocortical oscillatory dynamics in Williams syndrome. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2012;33:153–164. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2011.09.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Bódizs R, et al. Prediction of general mental ability based on neural oscillation measures of sleep. J. Sleep Res. 2005;14:285–292. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2005.00472.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Ujma PP, et al. The sleep EEG spectrum is a sexually dimorphic marker of general intelligence. Sci. Rep. 2017;7:1807. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-18124-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Ujma PP. Sleep spindles and general cognitive ability—a meta-analysis. Sleep Spindles Cortical Up States. 2018 doi: 10.1556/2053.2.2018.01. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Ujma PP, Bódizs R, Dresler M. Sleep and intelligence: critical review and future directions. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 2020;33:109–117. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2020.01.009. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Feinberg I, March JD, Fein G, Aminoff MJ. Log amplitude is a linear function of log frequency in NREM sleep EEG of young and elderly normal subjects. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 1984;58:158–160. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(84)90029-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Pritchard WS. The brain in fractal time: 1/f-like power spectrum scaling of the human electroecephalogram. Int. J. Neurosci. 1992;66:119–129. doi: 10.3109/00207459208999796. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Pereda E, Gamundi A, Rial R, Gonzalez J. Non-linear behaviour of human EEG: fractal exponent versus correlation dimension in awake and sleep stages. Neurosci. Lett. 1998;250:91–94. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3940(98)00435-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Wen H, Liu Z. Separating fractal and oscillatory components in the power spectrum of neurophysiological signal. Brain Topogr. 2016;29:13–26. doi: 10.1007/s10548-015-0448-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Miskovic V, MacDonald KJ, Rhodes LJ, Cote KA. Changes in EEG multiscale entropy and power-law frequency scaling during the human sleep cycle. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2019;40:538–551. doi: 10.1002/hbm.24393. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Lendner JD, et al. An electrophysiological marker of arousal level in humans. eLife. 2020;9:e55092. doi: 10.7554/eLife.55092. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Freeman WJ, Holmes MD, West GA, Vanhatalo S. Fine spatiotemporal structure of phase in human intracranial EEG. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2006;117:1228–1243. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2006.03.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Lázár, A. S., Lázár, Z. I. & Bódizs, R. Frequency characteristics of sleep. In Oxford Handbook of EEG frequency(in press)
- 19.Dummermuth G, et al. Studies on EEG activities in the beta band. Eur. Neurol. 1977;16:197–202. doi: 10.1159/000114900. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Miller KJ, Sorensen LB, Ojemann JG, den Nijs M. Power-law scaling in the brain surface electric potential. PLoS Comput. Biol. 2009;5:e1000609. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000609. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.He BJ, Zempel JM, Snyder AZ, Raichle M. The temporal structures and functional significance of scale-free brain activity. Neuron. 2010;66:353–369. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.04.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Voytek B, et al. Age-related changes in 1/f neural electrophysiological noise. J. Neurosci. 2015;35:13257–13265. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2332-14.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Donoghue T, et al. Parameterizing neural power spectra into periodic and aperiodic components. Nat. Neurosci. 2020;23:1655–1665. doi: 10.1038/s41593-020-00744-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Colombo MA, et al. The spectral exponent of the resting EEG indexes the presence of consciousness during unresponsiveness induced by propofol, xenon, and ketamine. Neuroimage. 2019;189:631–644. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.01.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Gao R. Interpreting the electrophysiological power spectrum. J. Neurophysiol. 2016;115:628–630. doi: 10.1152/jn.00722.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Freeman WJ, Zhai J. Simulated power spectral density (PSD) of background electrocorticogram (ECoG) Cogn. Neurodyn. 2009;3:97–103. doi: 10.1007/s11571-008-9064-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Geiger A, et al. The sleep EEG as a marker of intellectual ability in school age children. Sleep. 2011;34:181–189. doi: 10.1093/sleep/34.2.181. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Carrier J, Land S, Buysse DJ, Kupfer DJ, Monk TH. The effects of age and gender on sleep EEG power spectral density in the middle years of life (ages 20–60 years old) Psychophysiology. 2001;38:232–242. doi: 10.1111/1469-8986.3820232. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Nicolas A, Petit D, Rompré S, Montplaisir J. Sleep spindle characteristics in healthy subjects of different age groups. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2001;112:521–527. doi: 10.1016/s1388-2457(00)00556-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Purcell SM, et al. Characterizing sleep spindles in 11,630 individuals from the national sleep research resource. Nat. Commun. 2017;8:1593. doi: 10.1038/ncomms1593. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Principe JC, Smith JR. Sleep spindle characteristics as a function of age. Sleep. 1982;5:73–84. doi: 10.1093/sleep/5.1.73. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Dijk DJ, Beersma DG, Bloem GM. Sex differences in the sleep EEG of young adults: visual scoring and spectral analysis. Sleep. 1989;12:500–507. doi: 10.1093/sleep/12.6.500. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Looker AC, et al. Age, gender, and race/ethnic differences in total body and subregional bone density. Osteoporos. Int. 2009;20:1141–1149. doi: 10.1007/s00198-008-0809-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Crowley K, Trinder J, Kim Y, Carrington M, Colrain IM. The effects of normal aging on sleep spindle and K-complex production. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2002;113:1615–1622. doi: 10.1016/s1388-2457(02)00237-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Huupponen E, et al. A study on gender and age differences in sleep spindles. Neuropsychobiology. 2002;45:99–105. doi: 10.1159/000048684. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Ujma PP, et al. Sleep spindles and intelligence: evidence for a sexual dimorphism. J. Neurosci. 2014;34:16358–16368. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1857-14.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Abt K. Descriptive data analysis: a concept between confirmatory and exploratory data analysis. Methods Inf. Med. 1987;26:77–88. doi: 10.1055/s-0038-1635488. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Abt K. Statistical aspects of neurophysiologic topography. J. Clin. Neurophysiol. 1990;7:519–534. doi: 10.1097/00004691-199010000-00007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Borbély AA, Baumann F, Brandeis D, Strauch I, Lehmann D. Sleep deprivation: effect on sleep stages and EEG power density in man. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 1981;51:483–495. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(81)90225-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Finelli LA, Achermann P, Borbély AA. Individual 'fingerprints' in human sleep EEG topography. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2001;25:S57–S62. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(01)00320-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Olbrich E, Landolt HP, Achermann P. Effect of prolonged wakefulness on electroencephalographic oscillatory activity during sleep. J. Sleep Res. 2014;23:253–260. doi: 10.1111/jsr.12123. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Tarokh L, Rusterholz T, Achermann P, Van Dongen HP. The spectrum of the non-rapid eye movement sleep electroencephalogram following total sleep deprivation is trait-like. J. Sleep Res. 2015;24:360–363. doi: 10.1111/jsr.12279. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Tinguely G, Finelli LA, Landolt HP, Borbély AA, Achermann P. Functional EEG topography in sleep and waking: state-dependent and state-independent features. Neuroimage. 2006;32:283–292. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.03.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Welch PD. The use of Fast Fourier transform for the estimation of power spectra: a method based on time averaging over short, modified periodograms. IEEE Trans. Audio Electroacoust. 1967;15:70–73. doi: 10.1109/TAU.1967.1161901. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Tucker AM, Dinges DF, Van Dongen HPA. Trait interindividual differences in the sleep physiology of healthy young adults. J. Sleep Res. 2007;16:170–180. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2007.00594.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Gottselig JM, Bassetti CL, Achermann P. Power and coherence of sleep spindle frequency activity following hemispheric stroke. Brain. 2002;125:373–383. doi: 10.1093/brain/awf021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Gao R, Peterson EJ, Voytek B. Inferring synaptic excitation/inhibition balance from field potentials. Neuroimage. 2017;158:70–78. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.06.078. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Weiss B, Clemens Z, Bódizs R, Halász P. Comparison of fractal and power spectral EEG features: effects of topography and sleep stages. Brain Res. Bull. 2011;84:359–375. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2010.12.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Mander BA, et al. Prefrontal atrophy, disrupted NREM slow waves and impaired hippocampal-dependent memory in aging. Nat. Neurosci. 2013;16:357–364. doi: 10.1038/nn.3324. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Mander BA, et al. Impaired prefrontal sleep spindle regulation of hippocampal-dependent learning in older adults. Cereb. Cortex. 2014;24:3301–3309. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht188. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Clawson BC, Durkin J, Aton SJ. Form and function of sleep spindles across the lifespan. Neural Plast. 2016;2016:6936381. doi: 10.1155/2016/6936381. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Driver HS, Dijk DJ, Werth E, Biedermann K, Borbély AA. Sleep and the sleep electroencephalogram across the menstrual cycle in young healthy women. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 1996;81:728–735. doi: 10.1210/jcem.81.2.8636295. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Deboer T. Brain temperature dependent changes in the electroencephalogram power spectrum of humans and animals. J. Sleep Res. 1998;7:254–262. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2869.1998.00125.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Vanhatalo S, Voipio J, Kaila K. Full-band EEG (FbEEG): an emerging standard in electroencephalography. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2005;116:1–8. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2004.09.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Cattell R. Culture Free Intelligence Test, Scale 1, Handbook. Champaign, IL: Institute of Personality and Ability Testing; 1949. [Google Scholar]
- 56.Cattell RB, Krug SE, Barton K. Technical Supplement for the Culture Fair Intelligence Tests, Scales 2 and 3. Champaign, IL: IPAT; 1973. [Google Scholar]
- 57.Raven J, Raven JC, Court JH. Manual for Raven's Progressive Matrices and Vocabulary Scales. San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment; 2004. [Google Scholar]
- 58.Cattell RB, Cattell AKS. Measuring Intelligence with the Culture Fair Tests. Champaign, IL: IPAT; 1973. [Google Scholar]
- 59.Duncan J, et al. A neural basis for general intelligence. Science. 2000;289:457–460. doi: 10.1126/science.289.5478.457. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Prokosch MD, Yeo RA, Miller GF. Intelligence tests with higher g-loadings show higher correlations with body symmetry: evidence for a general fitness factor mediated by developmental stability. Intelligence. 2005;33:203–213. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2004.07.007. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Jasper HH. Report of the committee on methods of clinical examination in electroencephalography. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 1958;10:370–375. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(58)90053-1. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Berry RB, et al. The AASM Manual for the Scoring of Sleep and Associated Events: Rules, Terminology and Technical Specification. Version 2.5. Darien, IL: American Academy of Sleep Medicine; 2018. [Google Scholar]
- 63.Achermann P, Borbély AA. Low-frequency (< 1 Hz) oscillations in the human sleep electroencephalogram. Neuroscience. 1997;81:213–222. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4522(97)00186-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Vasko RC, Jr, et al. Power spectral analysis of EEG in a multiple-bedroom, multiple-polygraph sleep laboratory. Int. J. Med. Inform. 1997;46:175–184. doi: 10.1016/s1386-5056(97)00064-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Silver NC, Dunlap WP. Averaging correlation coefficients: Should Fisher’s z transformation be used? J. Appl. Psychol. 1987;72:146–148. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.72.1.146. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Duffy FH, et al. Quantified neurophysiology with mapping: statistical inference, exploratory and confirmatory data analysis. Brain Topogr. 1990;3:3–12. doi: 10.1007/bf01128856. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Rüger B. Das maximale Signifikanziniveau des tests: “Lehne H0 ab, wenn k unter n gegebene Tests zur ablehnung führen”. Metrika. 1978;25:171–178. doi: 10.1007/bf02204362. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Hassainia F, Petit D, Montplaisir J. Significance probability mapping: the final touch in t-statistic mapping. Brain Topogr. 1994;7:3–8. doi: 10.1007/bf01184832. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
All corrected power spectral data, as well as the fitted parameters and the program code used are available at https://osf.io/c487g/.