Abstract
Background:
Dysregulation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis is associated with poor physical and mental health. Early-life adversity may dysregulate cortisol response to subsequent stress. The current study examines the association between patterns of maternal behavior and infant stress response to a stressor. Specifically, we test whether infant exposure to unpredictable maternal sensory signals is related to the cortisol response to a painful stressor.
Method:
Participants were 102 mothers and their children enrolled in a longitudinal study. Patterns of maternal sensory signals were evaluated at 6 and 12 months during a 10-minute mother-infant play episode. Entropy rate was calculated as a quantitative measure of the degree of unpredictability of maternal sensory signals (visual, auditory, and tactile) exhibited during the play episode. Infant saliva samples were collected for cortisol analysis before and after inoculation at 12 months.
Results:
Unpredictable patterns of maternal sensory signals were associated with a blunted infant cortisol response to a painful stressor. This relation persisted after evaluation of covariates including maternal sensitivity and maternal psychological distress.
Conclusions:
The current study provides evidence that unpredictable patterns of maternal sensory signals is one process through which caregiving affects the function of infant stress response systems.
Keywords: Parental Care, HPA Axis, Infant
The hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis is a major neuroendocrine system that is key in physiological responses to challenge. The HPA axis response involves a cascade of biochemical events resulting in the release of cortisol from the adrenal cortex (Selye, 1950). Cortisol plays a critical role in prenatal and postnatal brain development (Joëls, Karst, & Sarabdjitsingh, 2018; Kim et al., 2017) and regulates many functions of the central nervous system (e.g., metabolic, endocrine, immune) throughout the lifespan (Adam et al., 2017; Martins et al., 2017; Müller & Quinkler, 2018). Dysregulation of the HPA axis confers risk for a range of physical and mental illnesses (Moisiadis & Matthews, 2014; van Bodegom, Homberg, & Henckens, 2017). The HPA axis is particularly susceptible to early experiences (for review, see Howland, Sandman, & Glynn, 2017; Noroña, Doom, Davis, & Gunnar, 2020). Parental care is one potent factor that shapes the developing HPA axis, and an extensive literature shows that quality of maternal caregiving as well as the nature of the attachment relationship is associated with infant and child cortisol regulation (Blair et al., 2008; Feldman et al., 2009; Martinez-Torteya et al., 2014; McLaughlin et al., 2015; Spangler & Zimmermann, 2014). However, the process by which parental signals affect HPA axis development remains poorly understood.
Rodent models have been leveraged to shed light on the role of maternal care in shaping the HPA axis. In the context of caregiving, the dam provides critical sensory signals to the pup (Champagne & Meaney, 2001; Eghbal-Ahmadi, Avishai-Eliner, Hatalski, & Baram, 1999; Levine, 2005). Disruption to essential aspects of the dam’s caregiving behaviors and sensory signals, including nursing and licking and grooming, leads to stress in the offspring (Champagne, Francis, Mar, & Meaney, 2003). In addition to the quantity of maternal sensory signals, the patterns of maternal sensory signals profoundly impact the pup (Baram et al., 2012; Chen & Baram, 2016). Manipulations to the dam’s environment that create a poverty of bedding and nesting materials alter the care she provides to her pup. Specifically, dams exposed to impoverished cage conditions provide unpredictable sensory signals to their pups (Ivy, Brunson, Sandman, & Baram, 2008; Rice, Sandman, Lenjavi, & Baram, 2008). These unpredictable signals provoke stress in the pup, and may also directly influence the maturation of brain circuits (Glynn & Baram, 2019). For example, excitatory synapses onto stress-sensitive hypothalamic neurons that control the response of the HPA axis to subsequent stress are augmented (Gunn et al., 2013). In addition, the maturation of hippocampal circuits, which might influence stress memory, is affected long-term. This includes stunting of dendrites and alterations to synapse number and function in the hippocampus (Ivy et al., 2010; Molet et al., 2016).
Evidence that patterns of sensory signals to the rodent pup impact brain systems involved in regulation of the HPA axis is highly consistent with neurobiological principles governing brain development. It is established that patterns of sensory signals are essential for the maturation of neural circuits involved in processing sensory information. For example, exposure to light patterns and sound and tone patterns is required for maturation of the visual and auditory systems, respectively (Espinosa & Stryker, 2012; Takesian, Bogart, Lichtman, & Hensch, 2018). Patterns of sensory signals, and particularly their unpredictability, additionally affect the development of systems involved in memory (Molet et al., 2016) and reward (Bolton, Molet, et al., 2018). Recent cross-species (rodents and humans) evidence suggests unpredictable maternal sensory signals affect cognitive function, with replication across continents (North America and Europe) (Davis et al., 2019, 2017).
Because of the importance of patterns of sensory information for neural maturation, and because early-life stressors may lead to dysfunction of the HPA axis, we examine whether aberrant patterns of maternal sensory signals comprise a stressor as indicated by alterations to HPA axis regulation. Specifically, the current study tests whether infant exposure to unpredictable maternal sensory signals is related the cortisol response to a painful stressor in human infants.
Method
Participants
Study participants were 102 mothers and their children (52% male) participating in a longitudinal study evaluating the role of early experiences in development. Inclusion criteria for the sample were over 18 years of age, English ability, and singleton intrauterine pregnancy. Exclusion criteria were tobacco, alcohol, or drug use during pregnancy, and medical conditions involving dysregulated neuroendocrine, cardiovascular, hepatic, or renal functioning. The study participants included in these analyses were the 102 dyads for whom video recordings of maternal-child interactions and child outcomes were available.
Demographic information for the sample appears in Table 1. All study procedures were approved by the Institutional Review Board for Protection of Human Subjects at the University of California Irvine. Each mother provided written, informed consent for herself and her child.
Table 1.
Factor | M/% | SD |
---|---|---|
Maternal Education (Years) | 16.0 | 2.1 |
Maternal Racea | 55% | - |
Household Income (USD) | $66,374 | $32,754 |
Maternal Age (Years) | 31.1 | 4.8 |
Maternal Marital Statusb | 79% | - |
Mother Cohabitation with Partner | 91% | - |
Mother Parity History | 0.7 | 0.8 |
Child Biological Sexc | 52% | - |
Notes.
White Non-Hispanic = 1, Non-White or White Hispanic = 0
Married = 1, Single = 0
Male = 1; Female = 0.
Mother-child interaction procedure
The degree of unpredictability of maternal sensory signals as well as maternal global sensitivity were derived from behavioral coding of a play interaction of mother and child as previously described (Davis et al., 2017). Briefly, mothers and infants attended laboratory visits at infant ages 6 and 12 months. These visits included a semi-structured play interaction between the mother and her infant. Mothers were given a standard set of age-appropriate toys and were instructed to play with their infants as they would at home for 10 minutes (NICHD Early Childcare Research Network, 1999).
Measures
Unpredictability of maternal sensory signals.
Maternal behaviors that provide auditory, visual, or tactile sensory signals to the child were coded on a moment-to-moment basis from digital video recordings of the 6-and 12-month mother-child play interactions using The Observer XT 11 (Noldus). Auditory signals included all maternal vocalizations (e.g., talking, laughing). Visual signals were maternal manipulations of a toy or object while the infant was visually attending. Tactile signals involved all instances of physical contact (e.g., holding, touching) initiated by the mother. Coders were blind to all other information on study participants. Interrater reliability was calculated for 20% of the videos and averaged 89%.
Quantification of the unpredictability of maternal sensory signals was determined by the calculation of the conditional probabilities of transitions between each of the eight possible combinations of maternal visual, auditory and tactile sensory signals (i.e., presence/absence of input of each of the three types of sensory signals). For example, a mother who is speaking to the child while showing her a toy provides both auditory and visual stimulation. If she additionally picks up the child, she now provides tactile stimulation to the child. This is coded as a new state. The transitions among states are modeled as changes in the state of a discrete-state first-order Markov process, and the entropy rate of the process was taken as a measure of unpredictability, as described in Supplementary material of Davis et al. (2017). Alternative Markov chain models (2nd order and 3rd order), as well as a non-parametric approaches confirmed the reliability of the entropy measure (Spearman’s rank correlations for the resulting entropy measures ranged from .91 to .98) (Vegetabile, Stout-Oswald, Davis, Baram, & Stern, 2017). The entropy rate of a sequence of behaviors can be conceptualized as a measure of how random or unpredictable a mother’s next behavior would appear to an observer who was making a guess based only on the most recently observed behavior. If one behavior always followed another (e.g., speech was always followed by touch), then this would be highly predictable and there would be little uncertainty for the observer (low entropy rate). In contrast, if the next behavior appeared to be a random choice, then this would be unpredictable (high entropy rate). Entropy rate can vary between a minimum value of zero, when a process is perfectly predictable, to a maximum value of 2.807 (the logarithm [base two] of the number of possible transitions [7] of sensory signals at each step), when all possible transitions in this coding scheme are equally likely and maternal signals are most unpredictable. The entropy rates of maternal sensory signals during the 6- and 12-month mother-infant play interactions were correlated (r = .46, p < .01) and consistent with prior research averaged to create a composite measure of unpredictable maternal sensory signals in infancy (Davis et al., 2019).
Additional details regarding the behavioral coding, calculation of entropy, and a description of an R software package for calculating entropy rate are provided in Davis et al. (2017) and available at https://contecenter.uci.edu/shared-resources/ .
Infant salivary cortisol response to the painful stress of an inoculation.
Study researchers attended routine 12-month pediatric well-child visits with participating families. Saliva samples were collected from the infants for cortisol analysis at baseline and in response to an intramuscular injection. The baseline sample was obtained upon arrival to the waiting room and before entering the examination room. The response sample was collected 20 minutes post-inoculation to capture the peak cortisol response. Saliva was obtained (without any stimulant) by placing a swab in the infant’s mouth for up to one minute, and time of day was recorded. After collection, the swab was inserted into a saliva extraction tube (Roche Diagnostics, Indianapolis, IN).
Saliva samples were spun and stored at −20C until assayed. Cortisol levels were determined by a competitive luminescence immunoassay (LIA; IBL-America, Minneapolis, MN) with reported detection limits of 0.015 μg ⁄ dl. The cross reactivity of the assay was < 2.5% with cortisone, prednisone, and corticosterone and < 0.1% with other naturally occurring steroids. The intra- and inter-assay coefficients of variance are 5.5% and 7.6%, respectively. Data reduction for the LIA assay was done by an automated four-parameter logistics computer program (software Mikro Win 2000; Berthold Microplate Luminometer, Berthold Detection Systems GmbH; Pforzheim, Germany). Six infants were excluded from analyses for having cortisol concentrations more than four standard deviations above the mean. The change in cortisol concentrations in response to the inoculation was calculated by subtracting baseline levels from response cortisol levels at 20 minutes after the start of the inoculation.
Maternal sensitivity.
Maternal behaviors during the 6- and 12-month mother-child interactions described above were scored from video using a coding system developed for the NICHD Study of Early Child Care and Youth Development (NICHD Early Childcare Research Network, 1999). Based on standard procedures, a composite rating of maternal sensitivity was created by summing 4-point ratings of sensitivity to nondistress, positive regard, and intrusiveness (reverse-coded). A composite maternal sensitivity score was calculated by averaging 6- and 12-month scores. Sensitivity scores at 6 and 12 months were correlated (r = .47, p < .01) and averaged to create an index of maternal sensitivity over the first postnatal year consistent with prior research (Sulik et al., 2015). All coders were blind to other data gathered on study participants. Twenty percent of sessions were selected at random, without coder knowledge, and coded again by a second independent coder to obtain an index of inter-rater reliability. Reliability for each of the subscales were: sensitivity to nondistress (90%), intrusiveness (90%), and positive regard (93%).
Maternal distress.
Maternal symptoms of anxiety and depression were assessed with the Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale (EPDS; Cox & Holden, 2003) and the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI; Spielberger, 2010) at 6 and 12 months. Each measure was averaged across the two time points to create a composite score for maternal depression (EPDS) and maternal anxiety (STAI) during infancy. The two composites were highly correlated (r = .76, p < .01) and thus were averaged to create an index of maternal distress.
Data analytic plan.
First, covariates were identified. Potential covariates were identified based on the literature and included maternal sensitivity, maternal psychological distress (i.e., anxiety and depression symptoms) and demographic factors (e.g., maternal marital status, household income, maternal age). Any of these variables associated (p < .1) with the predictor or the outcome were included as covariates in the analysis. Maternal sensitivity and family income-to-needs ratio met criteria for covariate inclusion and thus were included in the regression model (See Table 2). Although it did not meet covariate criteria, time of day of the cortisol sample was also included due to associations with cortisol values in prior research (e.g., Martin, Kim, Bruce, & Fisher, 2014).
Table 2.
M | SD | 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 | 9 | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1. Infant cortisol response (μg/dl) | 0.16 | 0.30 | 1 | ||||||||
2. Unpredictability of maternal sensory signals | 0.79 | 0.15 | −0.22* | 1 | |||||||
3. Infant baseline cortisol (μg/dl) | 0.23 | 0.20 | −0.31* | 0.13 | 1 | ||||||
4. Saliva collection time | 11:32 | 2:32 | 0.08 | 0.09 | −0.26* | 1 | |||||
5. Maternal distress (anxiety and depression) | −0.16 | 0.81 | −0.09 | 0.01 | 0.00 | 0.09 | 1 | ||||
6. Maternal sensitivity | 9.92 | 1.17 | −0.05 | −0.21* | −0.11 | −0.14 | −0.15 | 1 | |||
7. Maternal marital status (1 = married) | 0.79 | 0.41 | −0.08 | −0.11 | −0.11 | −0.21* | −0.10 | 0.31* | 1 | ||
8. Maternal education (years) | 15.83 | 2.44 | −0.12 | −0.16 | −0.06 | −0.29* | −0.05 | 0.35* | 0.43* | 1 | |
9. Family income-to-needs ratio (percent) | 434.34 | 248.22 | −0.02 | −0.28* | 0.04 | −0.13 | −0.28* | 0.33* | 0.33* | 0.54* | 1 |
10. Maternal age (years) | 30.82 | 5.06 | 0.11 | −0.08 | 0.01 | −0.19 | −0.07 | 0.20* | 0.46* | 0.34* | 0.16 |
Note.
p < .05.
First, we examined whether the inoculation stressor led to an increase in cortisol using a repeated measures ANCOVA adjusting for time of day. Next, to examine the association between unpredictability of maternal sensory signals and infant cortisol response to the inoculation stressor, we employed bivariate correlations followed by linear regression with covariates. We also examined the association between exposure to unpredictable maternal sensory signals and infant baseline (pre-inoculation) cortisol using a bivariate correlation. In addition, we tested sex differences in the relation between exposure to unpredictable maternal sensory signals and cortisol response through inclusion of an interaction term (sex X unpredictability of sensory signals) in the second step of a stepwise regression.
Results
On average, cortisol levels of infants exposed to the pain of inoculation increased from 0.26 μg/dl to 0.51μg/dl (F = 14.55, p < .001), illustrating that the manipulation effectively produced a cortisol increase. Infant exposure to unpredictable maternal sensory signals was associated with a blunted cortisol response to inoculation (r = −.22, p = .026). In contrast, unpredictable maternal sensory signals did not have a significant relation to baseline cortisol concentrations (r = .13, p = .206). The association between infant exposure to unpredictable maternal signals and cortisol response to inoculation remained after accounting for covariates: maternal sensitivity, family income-to-needs ratio, and saliva collection time (β = −0.261, B = −0.538); see Table 3. Note that maternal psychological distress was not included in the regression model due to non-significant (p > .1) bivariate correlations with cortisol response and unpredictability of maternal sensory signals. The association between unpredictable maternal signals and child cortisol was not moderated by sex (B = 0.646, p = .293).
Table 3.
B | SE | β | p | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Saliva collection time | < 0.001 | <.001 | 0.104 | 0.302 |
Family income-to-needs ratio | < 0.001 | < 0.001 | −0.049 | 0.654 |
Maternal sensitivity | −0.020 | 0.028 | −0.074 | 0.486 |
Unpredictability of maternal sensory signals | −0.538 | 0.214 | −0.261 | 0.014 |
Notes. Significant (p < .05) effects are bolded for emphasis.
Discussion
The principle findings of this study are (1) infants exhibited a significant increase in cortisol following exposure to the painful stimulation of inoculation at 12 months, and (2) unpredictable maternal sensory signals were associated with a blunted cortisol response to a painful stressor. Importantly, established factors that shape child development broadly including maternal anxiety and depression, maternal sensitivity, and family socioeconomic status did not account for this finding. Unpredictable maternal sensory signals were not related to baseline cortisol levels, only to the change in cortisol following a painful stressor. These findings support the notion that infant exposure to unpredictable maternal sensory signals blunts either the ability of the brain components regulating the stress response to sense the painful stimulation or the magnitude of the hormonal output that is generated. In support of the former possibility, there is evidence that the connectivity of corticotropin releasing hormone-expressing cells in the hypothalamus, which govern cortisol secretion, is altered by patterns of maternal sensory signals (Gunn et al., 2013; Korosi et al., 2010). Indeed, it is interesting to speculate that as patterns of sensory signals (light and sound) contribute to the normal maturation of visual and auditory circuitries (Espinosa & Stryker, 2012; Takesian et al., 2018; Wiesel & Hubel, 1963), then aberrant patterns of sensory signals from the mother may impact the development of stress-related circuitry (Chen & Baram, 2016; Glynn & Baram, 2019).
Prior human studies have found that the quality of early life experiences, such as caregiver sensitivity, influences HPA axis development. However, variability in both study design and results has made it difficult to draw firm conclusions about how caregiving affects HPA axis function (Blair et al., 2008; Martinez-Torteya et al., 2014). A contribution of the present investigation to this line of inquiry is use of a painful stressor to assess infant cortisol response. Physical pain is an objective, universal stressor that has been established as a precipitant of infant HPA axis response (Davis & Granger, 2009; Gunnar, Porter, Wolf, Rigatuso, & Larson, 1995). In contrast to social stressor tasks (e.g., still-face procedure; Tronick, Als, Adamson, Wise, & Brazelton, 1978) in which an infant’s cortisol response may vary as a function of the infant’s past interactions with the caregiver, infants are expected to exhibit a cortisol increase in response to a painful event. Thus, the current study demonstrates the utility of an objective challenge to examine caregiver influences on HPA axis development and indicates that unpredictable maternal behavior is associated with a blunting of the cortisol response.
Experimental animal work that manipulates patterns of maternal sensory signals affords direct examination of mechanisms underlying the relation between caregiving patterns and pup HPA axis response (Bolton, Ruiz, et al., 2018; Korosi & Baram, 2010; Plotsky & Meaney, 1993; Singh-Taylor, Korosi, Molet, Gunn, & Baram, 2014). These studies demonstrate that unpredictable patterns of maternal care lead to changes in DNA methylation and aberrant development of neural systems involved in the regulation of the HPA axis. For example, there are observed disruptions in dendritic structure, connectivity, and glucocorticoid receptor gene expression in the hippocampus (Fenoglio, Brunson, & Baram, 2006; Molet et al., 2016; Weaver et al., 2004). In addition, unpredictable maternal sensory signals modulate the number and efficacy of synaptic inputs onto stress-sensitive neurons (Gunn et al., 2013). Thus, it is possible that human infant exposure to unpredictable patterns of maternal sensory signals affects the development of cortisol responsivity through these neurological and epigenetic pathways.
These results should be interpreted in the context of several important study limitations. First, the current study employed an observational design, which does not allow for causal inference and makes it challenging to completely rule-out alternative explanations. For example, it is difficult to separate genetic influences from experiences in observational human studies. However, experimental animal models involving cross-fostering, which obviates potential genetic confounding, have demonstrated effects of patterns of maternal sensory information on neurological systems underlying HPA responsivity (Molet et al., 2016). Second, the current study included measurement of cortisol, which is only one component of the HPA axis response. The benefit of studying cortisol in young children is that it can be measured noninvasively (e.g., saliva collection); however, future studies are encouraged to consider inclusion of additional hormones involved in HPA axis activity, such as ACTH. Last, infants were only followed to one year of age. Additional work is needed to determine whether the effect of unpredictable maternal sensory signals in infancy persists across developmental stages.
In summary, findings of the present study suggest that unpredictable patterns of maternal sensory signals may presage blunted infant cortisol response. This result builds upon an emerging human literature indicating that unpredictability is a type of early adversity with implications for brain and behavioral development (Baram et al., 2012; Davis et al., 2019, 2017; Glynn & Baram, 2019; Glynn et al., 2018; Glynn et al., 2019).
Acknowledgements:
This research was supported by the National Institutes of Health awards P50MH096889, HD051852, NS041298, HD02413, HD050662, and HD065823. This work was also partially supported by a training fellowship through the National Institute of Mental Health (T32MH015442). We are indebted to the families that participated in this research study.
Footnotes
Data Availability Statement:
The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.
References
- Adam EK, Quinn ME, Tavernier R, McQuillan MT, Dahlke KA, & Gilbert KE (2017). Diurnal cortisol slopes and mental and physical health outcomes: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 83, 25–41. 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2017.05.018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baram TZ, Davis EP, Obenaus A, Sandman CA, Small SL, Solodkin A, & Stern H (2012). Fragmentation and unpredictability of early-life experience in mental disorders. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 169(9), 907–915. 10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.11091347 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blair C, Granger DA, Kivlighan KT, Mills-Koonce R, Willoughby M, Greenberg MT, … Fortunato CK (2008). Maternal and child contributions to cortisol response to emotional arousal in young children from low-income, rural communities. Developmental Psychology, 44(4), 1095. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bolton JL, Molet J, Regev L, Chen Y, Rismanchi N, Haddad E, … Baram TZ (2018). Anhedonia following early-life adversity involves aberrant interaction of reward and anxiety circuits and is reversed by partial silencing of amygdala corticotropin-releasing hormone gene. Biological Psychiatry, 83(2), 137–147. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bolton JL, Ruiz CM, Rismanchi N, Sanchez GA, Castillo E, Huang J, … Mahler SV (2018). Early-life adversity facilitates acquisition of cocaine self-administration and induces persistent anhedonia. Neurobiology of Stress, 8, 57–67. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Champagne FA, Francis DD, Mar A, & Meaney MJ (2003). Variations in maternal care in the rat as a mediating influence for the effects of environment on development. Physiology & Behavior, 79(3), 359–371. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Champagne F, & Meaney MJ (2001). Like mother, like daughter: Evidence for non-genomic transmission of parental behavior and stress responsivity In Progress in brain research (Vol. 133, pp. 287–302). Elsevier. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chen Y, & Baram TZ (2016). Toward understanding how early-life stress reprograms cognitive and emotional brain networks. Neuropsychopharmacology, 41(1), 197. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cox J, & Holden J (2003). Perinatal mental health: A guide to the Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale (EPDS). Royal College of Psychiatrists. [Google Scholar]
- Davis EP, & Granger DA (2009). Developmental differences in infant salivary alpha-amylase and cortisol responses to stress. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 34(6), 795–804. 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.02.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davis EP, Korja R, Karlsson L, Glynn LM, Sandman CA, Vegetabile B, … Pelto J (2019). Across continents and demographics, unpredictable maternal signals are associated with children’s cognitive function. EBioMedicine, 46, 256–263. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davis EP, Stout SA, Molet J, Vegetabile B, Glynn LM, Sandman CA, … Baram TZ (2017). Exposure to unpredictable maternal sensory signals influences cognitive development across species. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 114(39), 10390–10395. 10.1073/pnas.1703444114 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eghbal-Ahmadi M, Avishai-Eliner S, Hatalski CG, & Baram TZ (1999). Differential Regulation of the Expression of Corticotropin-Releasing Factor Receptor Type 2 (CRF2) in Hypothalamus and Amygdala of the Immature Rat by Sensory Input and Food Intake. Journal of Neuroscience, 19(10), 3982–3991. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-10-03982.1999 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Espinosa JS, & Stryker MP (2012). Development and plasticity of the primary visual cortex. Neuron, 75(2), 230–249. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Feldman R, Granat A, Pariente C, Kanety H, Kuint J, & Gilboa-Schechtman E (2009). Maternal Depression and Anxiety Across the Postpartum Year and Infant Social Engagement, Fear Regulation, and Stress Reactivity. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 48(9), 919–927. 10.1097/CHI.0b013e3181b21651 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fenoglio KA, Brunson KL, & Baram TZ (2006). Hippocampal neuroplasticity induced by early-life stress: Functional and molecular aspects. Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology, 27(2), 180–192. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Glynn LM, & Baram TZ (2019). The influence of unpredictable, fragmented parental signals on the developing brain. Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology, 53, 100736 10.1016/j.yfrne.2019.01.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Glynn LM, Howland MA, Sandman CA, Davis EP, Phelan M, Baram TZ, & Stern HS (2018). Prenatal maternal mood patterns predict child temperament and adolescent mental health. Journal of Affective Disorders, 228, 83–90. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Glynn LM, Stern HS, Howland MA, Risbrough VB, Baker DG, Nievergelt CM, … & Davis EP (2019). Measuring novel antecedents of mental illness: the Questionnaire of Unpredictability in Childhood. Neuropsychopharmacology, 44(5), 876–882. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gunn BG, Cunningham L, Cooper MA, Corteen NL, Seifi M, Swinny JD, … Belelli D (2013). Dysfunctional Astrocytic and Synaptic Regulation of Hypothalamic Glutamatergic Transmission in a Mouse Model of Early-Life Adversity: Relevance to Neurosteroids and Programming of the Stress Response. Journal of Neuroscience, 33(50), 19534–19554. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1337-13.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gunnar MR, Porter FL, Wolf CM, Rigatuso J, & Larson MC (1995). Neonatal Stress Reactivity: Predictions to Later Emotional Temperament. Child Development, 66(1), 1–13. 10.2307/1131186 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Howland MA, Sandman CA, & Glynn LM (2017). Developmental origins of the human hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis. Expert Review of Endocrinology & Metabolism, 12(5), 321–339. 10.1080/17446651.2017.1356222 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ivy AS, Brunson KL, Sandman C, & Baram TZ (2008). Dysfunctional nurturing behavior in rat dams with limited access to nesting material: A clinically relevant model for early-life stress. Neuroscience, 154(3), 1132–1142. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ivy AS, Rex CS, Chen Y, Dubé C, Maras PM, Grigoriadis DE, … Baram TZ (2010). Hippocampal Dysfunction and Cognitive Impairments Provoked by Chronic Early-Life Stress Involve Excessive Activation of CRH Receptors. Journal of Neuroscience, 30(39), 13005–13015. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1784-10.2010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Joëls M, Karst H, & Sarabdjitsingh RA (2018). The stressed brain of humans and rodents. Acta Physiologica, 223(2), e13066 10.1111/apha.13066 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim D-J, Davis EP, Sandman CA, Sporns O, O’Donnell BF, Buss C, & Hetrick WP (2017). Prenatal Maternal Cortisol Has Sex-Specific Associations with Child Brain Network Properties. Cerebral Cortex, 27(11), 5230–5241. 10.1093/cercor/bhw303 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Korosi A, & Baram TZ (2010). Plasticity of the stress response early in life: Mechanisms and significance. Developmental Psychobiology, 52(7), 661–670. 10.1002/dev.20490 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Korosi A, Shanabrough M, McClelland S, Liu Z-W, Borok E, Gao X-B, … Baram TZ (2010). Early-Life Experience Reduces Excitation to Stress-Responsive Hypothalamic Neurons and Reprograms the Expression of Corticotropin-Releasing Hormone. Journal of Neuroscience, 30(2), 703–713. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4214-09.2010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levine S (2005). Developmental determinants of sensitivity and resistance to stress. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 30(10), 939–946. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martinez-Torteya C, Dayton CJ, Beeghly M, Seng JS, McGinnis E, Broderick A, … Muzik M (2014). Maternal Parenting predicts Infant Biobehavioral Regulation among Women with a History of Childhood Maltreatment. Development and Psychopathology, 26(2), 379–392. 10.1017/S0954579414000017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martins CS, Elias D, Colli LM, Couri CE, Souza MCLA, Moreira AC, … Castro M de. (2017). HPA axis dysregulation, NR3C1 polymorphisms and glucocorticoid receptor isoforms imbalance in metabolic syndrome. Diabetes/Metabolism Research and Reviews, 33(3), e2842 10.1002/dmrr.2842 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McLaughlin KA, Sheridan MA, Tibu F, Fox NA, Zeanah CH, & Nelson CA (2015). Causal effects of the early caregiving environment on development of stress response systems in children. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 112(18), 5637–5642. 10.1073/pnas.1423363112 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moisiadis VG, & Matthews SG (2014). Glucocorticoids and fetal programming part 1: Outcomes. Nature Reviews Endocrinology, 10(7), 391 10.1038/nrendo.2014.73 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Molet J, Maras PM, Kinney-Lang E, Harris NG, Rashid F, Ivy AS, … Baram TZ (2016). MRI uncovers disrupted hippocampal microstructure that underlies memory impairments after early-life adversity. Hippocampus, 26(12), 1618–1632. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Müller L, & Quinkler M (2018). Adrenal disease: Imitating the cortisol profile improves the immune system. Nature Reviews Endocrinology, 14(3), 137–139. 10.1038/nrendo.2018.5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Noroña A, Doom JR, Gunnar MR, & Davis EP (n.d.). The Effects of Stress on Early Brain and Behavioral Development In Neural Circuit and Cognitive Development. Amsterdam: Elsevier Health Sciences. [Google Scholar]
- Plotsky PM, & Meaney MJ (1993). Early, postnatal experience alters hypothalamic corticotropin-releasing factor (CRF) mRNA, median eminence CRF content and stress-induced release in adult rats. Molecular Brain Research, 18(3), 195–200. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rice CJ, Sandman CA, Lenjavi MR, & Baram TZ (2008). A novel mouse model for acute and long-lasting consequences of early life stress. Endocrinology, 149(10), 4892–4900. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Selye H (1950). Stress. Montreal. Acta, Inc, 143. [Google Scholar]
- Singh-Taylor A, Korosi A, Molet J, Gunn BG, & Baram TZ (2014). Synaptic rewiring of stress-sensitive neurons by early-life experience: A mechanism for resilience? Neurobiology of Stress, 1, 109–115. 10.1016/j.ynstr.2014.10.007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spangler G, & Zimmermann P (2014). Emotional and adrenocortical regulation in early adolescence: Prediction by attachment security and disorganization in infancy. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 38(2), 142–154. 10.1177/0165025414520808 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Spielberger CD (2010). State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. In The Corsini Encyclopedia of Psychology (pp. 1–1). 10.1002/9780470479216.corpsy0943 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Sulik MJ, Blair C, Mills-Koonce R, Berry D, Greenberg M, Family Life Project Investigators, … & Frank JL (2015). Early parenting and the development of externalizing behavior problems: Longitudinal mediation through children’s executive function. Child Development, 86(5), 1588–1603. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Takesian AE, Bogart LJ, Lichtman JW, & Hensch TK (2018). Inhibitory circuit gating of auditory critical-period plasticity. Nature Neuroscience, 21(2), 218. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tronick E, Als H, Adamson L, Wise S, & Brazelton TB (1978). The infant’s response to entrapment between contradictory messages in face-to-face interaction. Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 17(1), 1–13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Bodegom M, Homberg JR, & Henckens MJAG (2017). Modulation of the Hypothalamic-Pituitary-Adrenal Axis by Early Life Stress Exposure. Frontiers in Cellular Neuroscience, 11 10.3389/fncel.2017.00087 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vegetabile BG, Stout-Oswald SA, Davis EP, Baram TZ, & Stern HS (2017). Estimating the Entropy Rate of Finite Markov Chains With Application to Behavior Studies. Journal of Educational and Behavioral Statistics, 1076998618822540. [Google Scholar]
- Weaver ICG, Cervoni N, Champagne FA, D’Alessio AC, Sharma S, Seckl JR, … Meaney MJ (2004). Epigenetic programming by maternal behavior. Nature Neuroscience, 7(8), 847–854. 10.1038/nn1276 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wiesel TN, & Hubel DH (1963). Single-cell responses in striate cortex of kittens deprived of vision in one eye. Journal of Neurophysiology, 26(6), 1003–1017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]