Skip to main content
PLOS One logoLink to PLOS One
. 2021 Jan 27;16(1):e0245444. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0245444

The relationship between social support, stressful events, and menopause symptoms

Megan Arnot 1,*, Emily H Emmott 1, Ruth Mace 1
Editor: Martha Asuncion Sánchez-Rodríguez2
PMCID: PMC7840006  PMID: 33503073

Abstract

Many women going through the menopausal transition experience vasomotor symptoms (VMS), and research has shown that there is a large amount of variation in their frequency and severity. Many lifestyle factors have been found to co-vary with VMS, including the level of social support received by the woman, and how stressed she is. Stress is well documented to worsen menopause symptoms, and there is some evidence that support eases them; however, there is little research into whether support is an effective buffer against the negative effects of stress on VMS. Using nine years of data from the Study of Women’s Health Across the Nation (n = 2718), we use multilevel Poisson regression with random effects to test: 1) if more social support is associated with decreased VMS frequency, 2) if increased life stress worsens VMS, and 3) if support acts as a buffer against stress. After adjusting for age, marital status, smoking, self-perceived overall health, ethnicity, and menopausal status, we find that stress increases the frequency of VMS. Contrary to our hypothesis, we did not find strong evidence that emotional support led to lower VMS frequency, or that support buffers against the effects of stress. Experience of a stressful event, but not amount of social support, was included in the best fitting model; with the degree to which the woman was upset by the life stressor having the largest effect on menopause symptoms. Here, women who said they were currently upset by a stressful event experienced 21% more VMS than women who had experienced no life stressor. This research highlights that social factors may impact the menopausal transition.

Introduction

Menopause is often a significant event for women; and, for many, the process is not smooth. Rather, it is accompanied by a multitude of physical symptoms, with vasomotor symptoms (VMS) being the most frequently reported, which include hot flashes, night sweats, and cold sweats [1]. In addition to VMS being generally bothersome, they have also been observed to have a negative impact on women’s mental health and wellbeing [2]. Typically, menopause symptoms are confined to the perimenopausal period; however, they are known to occasionally persist beyond final menstruation [3]. They are thought to be primarily the result of hormonal fluctuations that occur during the menopausal transition, with higher levels of follicular stimulating hormone and lower oestradiol being associated with a greater likelihood of reporting VMS [4]. However, while all women experience these hormonal changes during the menopausal transition, not all report VMS [5]: while population estimates vary [6], it is thought that over 70% of all women experience VMS at some point during the menopausal transition [3,7], meaning the minority of women do not experience VMS. Further, even amongst the women who do report VMS, the symptomatology is not uniform within and between populations, with women reporting them to varying degrees of severity and frequency [3,813]. Given the negative impact of VMS reported by women [14], it is important to understand what co-varies with VMS prevalence and severity, as it may better equip women to deal with the menopausal transition.

In recent years, different social and ecological factors have been found to associate with VMS, including ethnicity, living arrangements, and socioeconomic position (SEP) [3,12,15]. Importantly, it has been argued that stress may mediate the relationship between such factors and the women’s reported menopause symptoms [12,16]. Experience of stressors and increased perceived stress have been linked to higher levels of cortisol and fibrinogen, with the former being associated with the suppression of the immune, reproductive, and digestive systems, and the latter with increased inflammation [17]. As such, stress has causally been associated with a greater risk of cardiovascular disease [18], faster progression of HIV [19], delayed wound healing [20,21], decreased receptivity to the influenza vaccine [22], and a greater risk of upper respiratory tract infection [23,24]. Moreover, a number of studies have found that increased stress (both perceived stress and stressful experiences) can worsen the frequency and severity of menopause symptoms [3,2528]; however, the causal pathway underlying this particular association is currently unclear [16].

The potential effect of stress on menopause symptoms is of importance to understand: midlife is often a highly stressful time for women, with many major life events coinciding with one another, such as parental death, children leaving home, and divorce [2931]. Further, going through menopause can itself be a stressful event, as it marks a transition to a new phase of life, with many women reporting a sense of loss accompanying the end of their fertility [32]. Previous research suggests that a strong social network and higher levels of social support throughout the menopausal transition may reduce menopause symptom frequency and severity [3,3335]. Similar associations have also been observed between social relationships and other health outcomes [36,37], with strong social ties being related to an increased likelihood of cancer recovery [38,39], a reduced risk of Alzheimer’s and dementia [40,41], and also protect against cardiovascular disease [42,43]. Exactly how social ties work to sustain and/or improve health and well-being is not completely clear [37]. However, possible pathways include lower social support manifesting itself in more negative behavioural (i.e. poorer health behaviours) and psychological (i.e. depression) processes [36].

In addition to the noted direct benefits of social support, it is also thought that support can act as a buffer against the negative effects of stress on health [44]. Here, a stronger support system is thought to lessen the impact of stress, decrease its intensity, and also help to correct any maladaptive behaviours forming as a result of stress (e.g. alcoholism) [44]. Hence, social support may weaken the association between stress and negative health outcomes by moderating the actor’s reaction or perception of the stress they are experiencing. Despite the fact that stress has been widely associated with worse menopause symptoms, to our knowledge, the stress-buffering hypothesis for social support has not been explicitly tested in relation to VMS.

In this study, we build on previous research into stress, support and menopause symptoms by testing whether social support acts as an effective buffer against the negative influence of stress on VMS. First, we test whether higher levels of social support and stress associate with less frequent VMS and more frequent VMS, respectively. Further, we test whether social support buffers against the proposed negative impact of stress on VMS. This research aims to clarify how support and stress are associated with one another in regards to VMS.

Material and methods

Study sample

Data from the Study of Women’s Health Across the Nation (SWAN) were used in this study. SWAN is an ongoing, multi-site, longitudinal cohort study currently being carried out in the US. Data collection began in 1996/97, with women aged between 42 and 52 being recruited initially. Criteria to be part of the baseline cohort including having an intact uterus, at least one ovary, experience of a menstrual period in the past three months, and not currently being pregnant. 11 years’ worth of data is currently publicly available for analysis [4553], and this present study utilises data from nine years of interviews based on data available at the time of analysis (visits 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 6, 8, 9 and 10). Baseline data (visit 0) was omitted as the stress measure was not comparable to the question patterning in later years, and data from visit 7 was also not included as participants were not asked any questions on social support in that year of interviews.

Variables

Vasomotor symptoms

VMS served as the outcome variable in all analyses, and was measured through combining how often women experienced hot flashes, cold sweats and night sweats in the two weeks approaching the interview. These were measured individually on a Likert scale (not at all; 1–5 days; 6–8 days; 9–13 days; every day), and we then assigned each of these responses a value ranging from 0 to 5 (i.e. 0 = not at all; 1 = 1–5 days; and so on) and summed the woman’s experience of the three symptoms. This left each respondent with a score between 0 and 15, in which a higher score indicated more frequent VMS.

Social support

SWAN provides four measures of social support which include how often the respondent feels she has someone to confide in, someone to listen to her, take her to the doctors, and help out when sick (see S1 Text Figure A). These were measured on a 5-point Likert scale (none of the time; a little of the time; some of the time; most of the time; all of the time), and taken from the Medical Outcome Study: Social Support Survey which is one of the most widely used instruments for measuring social support [54], and shows high levels of validity and reliability [5558]. An average measure of support was created, in which the Likert scales for each measure of support were converted to numeric (i.e. none of the time = 1; a little of the time = 2, and so on), then summed and averaged to create a value ranging from 1–5, where a higher value indicated greater social support [54].

Stress

For this study, experience of stressful events was used as a metric of how stressed the participant was. SWAN asks participants about the occurrence of, and perceived stressfulness of, 18 life stresses that have occurred over the past year. These include the death of close kin, employment changes, and changes in household structure (see Fig 1 for the specific stressors and individual relationships). This is a modified version of the Psychiatric Epidemiology Research Interview life events scale [59], which has been validated across multiple racial and ethnic groups and shows high internal consistency [60]. Each stressful event was measured on a 5-point Likert scale which indicated whether she had experienced the event or not and how much it affected her (no–did not experience; yes–not upsetting; yes–somewhat upsetting; yes–very upsetting; yes–very upsetting and still upsetting). From these 18 questions, we derived two variables:

Fig 1. Relationship between the 18 individual measures of stress and vasomotor symptoms.

Fig 1

Models are unadjusted, with a higher Incidence Rate Ratio indicating more frequent vasomotor symptoms.

  1. A ‘stress index’ which is the maximum amount of stress the participant experienced across all of the 18 stressful events. It can be seen in Fig 1 that all the events individually present a similar relationship with VMS, with increased upset being associated with more VMS, and therefore we created a composite measure from these which indicates the maximum amount of stress experienced by the respondent. For example, if a woman answered “No” to 17 of the events, and stated that one event had “Somewhat upset her”, her stress index would be “Yes–somewhat upsetting”. An outline of the derivation of this variable can be found in the S1 Text Table A. In summary, a woman’s stress index can be seen as:

Did the participant experience a stressful event over the past year, and was she upset by it?

  • With the possible response being either: No; Yes, but she did not find it upsetting; Yes, and she found it somewhat upsetting; Yes, and she found it very upsetting, or; Yes, and she found it very upsetting at the time and is still upset by it.

  • 2

    A ‘stress dose variable’ was also created to measure the woman’s ‘stress dose’, or how many stressors she has experienced. This was therefore a count variable which ranged from 0 to 18, with a higher number indicating that she has experienced more stressful events. An explanation of how this variable was derived is presented in S1 Text Table B.

Both the stress index and stress dose variables were included as time varying. A detailed description of the creation of the variables and variable selection can be found in S1 Text (Tables A and B, and Figure C).

Covariates

Covariates were selected based on existing research into VMS and included as time varying. These included age, marital status (divorced/separated, married/in a relationship, widowed) [61], smoking habits (never smoked, ever smoked) [62], maximum level of education (less than high school, high school, some college/technical college, college degree, post-graduate degree) [63], ethnicity (self-identified as Black or African American, Chinese American, Japanese American, non-Hispanic Caucasian, Hispanic) [3], self-perceived overall health (poor, fair, good, very good, excellent) [64], and menopausal status (pre-menopausal, early perimenopause, late perimenopause, post-menopausal, and not menstruating for another reason). In all analyses, age was centred on the mean, with cubic and quadratic terms also included.

Analyses

To test whether support and stress influence vasomotor symptom experience throughout the menopausal transition, multivariable, multilevel Poisson regression with mixed effects was used. This method was deemed as appropriate as the VMS in our data follows a Poisson distribution, with most women reporting infrequent symptoms. In total, 8 models were made, which are shown in Table 1. An initial model that comprised solely of the covariates (the Base model) was first created as a unit of comparison. Social support, the stress index, and stress dose variables were then subsequently added to the base model (the Support model, Stress model 1 and Stress model 2, respectively) individually, and then together (Full model 1 and 2; stress measures were always included in separate models due to collinearity). Finally, to measure whether support buffers against stress, two models were made with an interaction effect between support and the two measures of stress (Interaction model 1 and 2).

Table 1. Candidate models.

Model name Covariates
Base model Age + Age2 + Age3 + Marital status + Smoking habits + Education + Ethnicity + Overall health + Menopause status
Support model Social support + Age + Age2 + Age3 + Marital status + Smoking habits + Education + Ethnicity + Overall health + Menopause status
Stress model 1 Stress index + Age + Age2 + Age3 + Marital status + Smoking habits + Education + Ethnicity + Overall health + Menopause status
Stress model 2 Stress dose + Age + Age2 + Age3 + Marital status + Smoking habits + Education + Ethnicity + Overall health + Menopause status
Full model 1 Social support + Stress index + Age + Age2 + Age3 + Marital status + Smoking habits + Education + Ethnicity + Overall health + Menopause status
Full model 2 Social support + Stress dose + Age + Age2 + Age3 + Marital status + Smoking habits + Education + Ethnicity + Overall health + Menopause status
Interaction model 1 Social support*Stress index + Age + Age2 + Age3 + Marital status + Smoking habits + Education + Ethnicity + Overall health + Menopause status
Interaction model 2 Social support*Stress dose + Age + Age2 + Age3 + Marital status + Smoking habits + Education + Ethnicity + Overall health + Menopause status

Model fitting was then carried out using the Akaike Information Criterion (AIC), which has been shown to be appropriate for Poisson regression with random effects [65]. The AIC shows which model is a relatively better fit to the data, and also penalises models for complexity. The model with the lowest AIC value is deemed to best fit the data, and a ΔAIC increase of more than 3 signifies weaker support for the model [66]. Models were then weighted based on how much their AIC value changed relative to the best fitting model [66]. All analyses were performed in R version 4.0.2 [67] using the packages lme4 [68] and ggeffects [69]. Model fitting was carried out using AICcmodavg [70], and data visualisations created using ggplot2 [71].

Results

Descriptive statistics

In the first round of interviews used in this analysis (n = 2718) women were aged between 42 and 54 (mean [M]: 46.91; standard deviation [SD]: 2.69), with the majority of participants either being married or in a relationship (76.09%). A greater proportion of women had never smoked (58.06%), and very few women were not educated to a high school standard, with most women having attended some kind of college or technical school (32.38%). Half of the women in the sample were white (49.71%), and at this point in the study very few women were post-menopausal (1.51%), with most of them being in early perimenopause (60.60%). Women were receiving high levels of social support on average (M: 4.18; SD: 0.78, see S1 Text Figure B), most had experienced a stressful event that upset them to some degree (76.49%), and the stress dose variable was positively skewed (S1 Text Figure D), with women experiencing on average 3 (SD: 2.45) stressful events in the past year. At this stage, the majority of women were not reporting frequent menopause symptoms (M: 1.21; SD: 2.05). Full descriptive statistics are presented in Table 2.

Table 2. Characteristics of the participants in visit 1.

Variable n (%) Mean (S.D)
Vasomotor symptoms 1.21 (2.05)
Social support 4.18 (0.78)
Stress index (experienced a stressful life event in past year)
    No 388 (14.3)
    Yes—not upsetting 251 (9.2)
    Yes—somewhat upsetting 766 (28.2)
    Yes—very upsetting 642 (23.6)
    Yes—very upsetting and still upsetting 671 (24.7)
Stress dose 3.07 (2.45)
Age 46.91 (2.69)
Marital status
    Divorced/Separated/Single 601 (22.1)
    Married/In a relationship 2068 (76.1)
    Widowed 49 (1.8)
Smoking habits
    Ever smoked 1140 (41.9)
    Never smoked 1578 (58.1)
Education
    Less than high school 169 (6.2)
    High school 443 (16.3)
    Some college/technical school 880 (32.4)
    College degree 569 (20.9)
    Post-graduate education 657 (24.2)
Ethnicity
    African American 687 (25.3)
    Chinese American 220 (8.1)
    Japanese American 262 (9.6)
    White 1351 (49.7)
    Hispanic 198 (7.3)
Self-perceived overall health
    Poor 50 (1.8)
    Fair 294 (10.8)
    Good 820 (30.2)
    Very good 1073 (39.5)
    Excellent 481 (17.7)
Menopausal status
    Pre-menopausal 676 (24.9)
    Early peri 1647 (60.6)
    Late peri 112 (4.1)
    Menopaused 41 (1.5)
    Other 242 (8.9)

Base model

The Base model, which comprised of all the covariates, was created to serve as a unit of comparison (see S1 Text Table C for full results). Within this model, women who were married experienced more frequent VMS compared to divorced or separated women (IRR: 1.10; 95% CI: 1.05–1.16), as did women who had ever smoked (IRR: 1.22; 95% CI: 1.12–1.32). There was a near linear relationship between level of education and reporting of menopause symptoms, with a longer time spent in education being associated with decreased VMS frequency (IRR: 0.72; 95% CI: 0.59–0.88). As in previous studies [3], African American reported VMS most often, with Japanese American women experiencing them least (IRR: 0.41; 95% CI: 0.35–0.48); and women who perceived themselves to be healthier also reported less frequent VMS (IRR: 0.65; 95% CI: 0.58–0.72). Women who were in late perimenopause (IRR: 1.87; 95% CI: 1.79–1.96) or had experienced menopause (IRR: 1.44; 95% CI: 1.37–1.51) reported symptoms more often.

Support and stress

Our study is testing the prediction that social support and stress influence symptom severity, and that support would act as a buffer against the effects of stress. Should this be the case, we would expect models with these variables included to better fit the data, in addition to an interaction effect. Results from model fitting are presented in Table 3, where it can be seen that the best fitting model is Stress model 1 (ΔAIC = 0.00), which included the stress index and covariates. The inclusion of social support (Full model 1 ΔAIC = +0.66) and an interaction term (Interaction model 1 ΔAIC = +16.13) worsened the model fit. The model that included just the covariates worst fit the data (Base model ΔAIC = +91.52).

Table 3. Results from model fitting based on Akaike Information Criterion (AIC) value.

Model K AIC ΔAIC wi
Stress model 1 28 64167.97 0.00 0.58
Full model 1 29 64168.64 0.66 0.42
Interaction model 1 29 64184.11 16.13 0.00
Stress model 2 25 64187.53 19.55 0.00
Full model 2 26 64187.96 19.99 0.00
Interaction model 2 25 64204.12 36.14 0.00
Support model 25 64258.49 90.52 0.00
Base model 24 64259.50 91.52 0.00

A lower AIC value indicates better fit for the model, and a ΔAIC increase of more than 3 signifying weaker support for the model. wi shows the Akaike weights that indicate the level of support for the model, or the ‘model probabilities’.

In the best fitting model (Stress model 1) it can be seen that the degree to which the woman was affected by the stressor was most critical. Fig 2 displays the predicted VMS at any given age based on experience of a stressor. Here, being still upset by a stressful event increased VMS frequency by 21% (95% CI: 1.15–1.26), and women who found the stressful event very upsetting at the time (but were no longer upset by it) experienced a 7% increase in VMS frequency (IRR: 1.03–1.13). However, those who were not upset by the stressful event (IRR: 1.01; 95% CI: 0.95–1.06) or only found it somewhat upsetting (IRR: 1.03; 95% CI: 0.98–1.07) did not experience a statistically significant increase in VMS at the time of reporting (see Table 4).

Fig 2. Predicted vasomotor symptoms any given age based on the best fitting model.

Fig 2

Model adjusted for adjusted for marital status, education, smoking habits, ethnicity, self-perceived overall health, and menopause status.

Table 4. Results from multilevel Poisson regression models showing the Incidence Rate Ratio and 95% confidence interval.

Support model Stress model 1 Stress model 2 Full model 1 Full model 2 Interaction model 1 Interaction model 2
Stress index (ref.: No)
    Yes—not upsetting 1.01 (0.95–1.06) 1.01 (0.95–1.06) 1.11 (0.78–1.60)
    Yes—somewhat upsetting 1.03 (0.98–1.07) 1.03 (0.98–1.07) 1.02 (0.79–1.30)
    Yes—very upsetting 1.07** (1.03–1.13) 1.07** (1.03–1.13) 1.31* (1.01–1.69)
    Yes—very upsetting and still upsetting 1.21*** (1.15–1.26) 1.20*** (1.15–1.26) 1.57*** (1.23–2.00)
Stress dose 1.03*** (1.02–1.03) 1.03*** (1.02–1.03) 1.05*** (1.02–1.07)
Support 0.98 (0.96–1.00) 0.99 (0.96–1.01) 0.99 (0.96–1.01) 1.02 (0.97–1.07) 1.00 (0.97–1.03)
Social support:Stress index (ref.: No)
    Social support:Yes—not upsetting 0.98 (0.90–1.06)
    Social support:Yes—    somewhat upsetting 1.00 (0.95–1.06)
    Social support:Yes–very upsetting 0.96 (0.90–1.01)
    Social support:Yes—very upsetting and still upsetting 0.94* (0.89–0.99)
Social support:Stress dose 1.00 (0.99–1.00)

Note:

* P≤0.05

** P≤0.01

*** P≤0.001.

The dependent variable for all models is vasomotor symptoms, and all models are adjusted for age (linear, cubic and quadratic terms), marital status, smoking habits, education, ethnicity, self-perceived overall health, and menopausal status.

As our current understanding of the physiological pathways around stress and VMS leads us to predict that current stress is an important determinant of VMS, we then also conducted an additional post hoc analysis with a time-lagged stress measure to test whether current stress is a better predictor of VMS than historical stress. Results (see S1 Text Table C) show that experience of stressful life events from a year prior to VMS reporting have a weaker relationship with VMS frequency than currently reported stressful events. Here, a very upsetting event from a year ago is not associated with more frequent VMS (IRR: 1.00; 95% CI: 0.95–1.05), and a very upsetting and still upsetting event from a year ago being associated with a 7% increase in VMS compared to women who had experienced no stressor a year ago (95% CI: 1.01–1.12).

Discussion

Support is often thought to buffer against the negative health impacts of stress [44], however, to our knowledge, this has not been tested with respect to menopause symptoms. It is well documented that stress associates with more severe symptoms that occur at a higher frequency [3,2528], with some evidence that a supportive environment decreases symptoms [3,33,35,72]. In our analyses of a sample of US women, we did not find evidence that increased social support is associated with decreased menopause symptoms, nor that it moderates the negative relationship between stress and menopause symptoms. Rather, we find that a model that does not include social support or its interaction with stress best fits the data, with this model suggesting that it is not necessarily the stressful event but the degree to which it upsets the woman (i.e. perceived stress) that impacts VMS.

This is interesting for a number of reasons. These results suggest that experiencing a stressful event in and of itself does not worsen menopause symptoms; rather, it is the woman’s psychological reaction to the stressful event that has the largest effect on VMS frequency. For example, based on our findings, if the period of upset after a stressful event is short, then menopause symptoms may not be significantly impacted. This is in line with findings that perceived stress is influential over symptomology [25], and that women who are deemed to be more resilient experience fewer menopause symptoms (i.e. women who experienced a stressful event but were not upset by it) [33]. Further, as we predict that current stress influences VMS, we carried out an additional post hoc analysis in which we lagged the measure of stress in the best fitting model. If VMS are partially exacerbated by current stress, we would expect a lagged variable that indicates historical stress to be a poor predictor, which we found to be the case (S1 Text Table C). There was a significant effect in the lagged variable amongst women who a year ago had reported that they had experienced a life stressor that was upsetting at the time, and there are two reasons that might explain this relationship in the lagged variable: firstly, the upsetting event that happened last year could still feasibly be upsetting them now (e.g., if it was the death of a close relative); secondly, women who reported being still upset by a life stressor may be less resilient to their effects, meaning that they would be more likely to report life stressors in subsequent years. Nonetheless, these results are more evidence for a possible causal relationship between stress and VMS.

Contrary to our hypothesis, our results suggest that support may not effectively protect against the effects of stress in our sample of US women. Elsewhere in the literature, there are reports that various kinds of support (familial, emotional, etc.) are associated with fewer menopause symptoms [33,35,72], but we did not replicate these effects in our analysis. When looking at other domains of health, researchers have found that social support is not always protective against stress [73,74]. It may be in this case that the support questions were not specific enough to capture their effect on menopause symptoms (e.g. women could have been asked “how often do you have someone to talk to about your health”), and to alleviate symptoms, it could be that women need to receive targeted support [35]. Alternatively, it may be that talking about the menopause does not have a positive impact. There is evidence that the anticipation of menopause symptoms, which might occur through frequent discussion, can in fact worsen the woman’s experience of such symptoms [26]. Understanding how support and discussion relate to symptom expectation is critical to understand. As the discussion of menopause becomes more normalised, so may the discourse on negative symptoms. While one could expect that normalising the menopause may ease the transition, it may in fact have the converse effect if it does cause women to expect that hot flashes and such are an inevitable negative outcome of the menopause. Thus, further research is required to understand how menopause discussion and symptoms interact with one another.

Our results also showed that women from certain ethnic backgrounds experienced worse symptoms. Here, African American women experienced the most frequent symptoms, while Chinese and Japanese Americans reported symptoms less often, which is in line with findings from previous research [3,9,13,75,76]. It has been suggested that the relationship between stress and menopause symptoms may help to explain some of this variation. In the US (where data used in this study was collected), African American individuals present more markers of stress than other ethnicities [77], likely due to economic and systematic factors [75]. However, results in this study suggest that stress does not mediate the relationship between ethnicity and VMS, with the relationship persisting after the inclusion of the stress index and stress dose within the model (S1 Text Table C). Additionally, attributing ethnic differences to stress does not account for why women from an East Asian background in this sample report fewer symptoms, as–due to being an ethnic minority–it would be predicted that due to higher levels of stress [75] they would report more symptoms than white women. Therefore, it is unlikely that stress explains the ethnic differences in menopause symptoms, and further research is still required to understand this variation.

How stress affects the menopausal transition is vital to understand, as, for many women, midlife is accompanied by many negative events such as the death of parents, children leaving home, and relationship problems [78]. The menopausal transition is also accompanied by a shift in their societal role: as fertility wanes and children move away, women experience a sense of loss of identity, which can also be highly stressful for women [7982]. As we have shown, there is a dose-effect in regards to stress and menopause symptoms, and therefore the accumulation of emotionally stressful events can be expected to worsen the menopausal transition. Furthermore, these stressors that coincide with menopause are largely unavoidable. Familial death is an inevitability, and children will generally move away at some point. The coincidence of these events with menopause is somewhat a product of social norms associated with age of reproduction, and as a result, this is unlikely to change soon. As a means of tackling the emotional burden of midlife for women, the attitude towards ageing may require a shift. Cultures in which age is revered often associate with less severe menopause symptoms (e.g. traditional East Asian cultures) [83]; furthermore, when women report menopause as being empowering they are less likely to report symptoms [84].

Mechanistically, why stress worsens menopause symptoms is less clear. Increased levels of cortisol produced in response to stress might mimic the neuroendocrinological changes that occur alongside VMS. However, recent research has shown that increased perceived stress is not associated with higher inflammation (measured through fibrinogen), so this may not be the case [16]. Alternatively, it might be that symptoms that would have otherwise have been mild or relatively unnoticeable are exacerbated by stressors, much like how pain has been observed to be worsened by stress [85].

Limitations

While we show a possible causal relationship between stress and VMS, further research using targeted data collection is likely to establish the precise causal pathway and mechanisms. Nonetheless, the persisting relationship between stress and menopause symptoms allows us to consider possible behavioural interventions women could use to deal with the symptoms associated with the menopausal transition. We found no relationship between our support measure and VMS. We chose to use an aggregated measure of support, which means we could not identify which measures of social support were associated with more or less frequent menopause symptoms (e.g. two women could have the same support score, but completely different support experiences). Further research may wish to look at support specific to the menopausal transition (e.g. menopause cafes), or conduct qualitative research to find which forms of support women themselves find most vital throughout menopause.

Conclusion

This research contributes to the growing body of research linking stress with a worse menopausal transition. For most, stress is an unavoidable aspect of daily life, and our results confirm an association between stressful events and worse menopause symptoms. However, our results suggest it is not necessarily the occurrence of a stressor that exacerbates menopause symptoms, but how the woman psychologically responds to it. In our sample, women who were “still upset” by a stressful event reported the most severe symptoms, whereas women who had experienced the same event but were not upset by it did not report significantly worse symptoms. This suggests that resilience may be important in the relationship between stress and menopause symptoms; however, further research is required to establish this. Little evidence was found for support reducing menopause symptoms, or acting as a buffer against the effects of stress. This may be relevant for understanding treatments and interventions for menopause symptoms, in particular, whether support-based treatments to tackle menopause symptoms are effective. Future research should focus on whether social interventions do have the desired effect on menopause symptoms, and also whether certain personality types–in regards to resilience–experience menopause in a different way.

Supporting information

S1 Text. Supporting text.

Supporting information containing information on the selection of the social support variable (Figure A), and the derivation of the stress index (Table A) and stress dose (Table B). Figure B demonstrates the justification for how missing data in the stress dose variable was dealt with, and Figure C shows the distribution of the stress dose variable by wave and age. Complete model results are presented in Table C.

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

The authors are grateful to the study stuff at each site and all the women who participated in SWAN. Clinical Centres: University of Michigan, Ann Arbor—Siobán Harlow, PI 2011–present, Mary Fran Sowers, PI 1994–2011; Massachusetts General Hospital, Boston, MA—Joel Finkelstein, PI 1999–present; Robert Neer, PI 1994–1999; Rush University, Rush University Medical Center, Chicago, IL—Howard Kravitz, PI 2009–present; Lynda Powell, PI 1994–2009; University of California, Davis/Kaiser—Ellen Gold, PI; University of California, Los Angeles—Gail Greendale, PI; Albert Einstein College of Medicine, Bronx, NY—Carol Derby, PI 2011–present, Rachel Wildman, PI 2010–2011; Nanette Santoro, PI 2004–2010; University of Medicine and Dentistry—New Jersey Medical School, Newark—Gerson Weiss, PI 1994–2004; and the University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA—Karen Matthews, PI. NIH Program Office: National Institute on Aging, Bethesda, MD—Chanda Dutta 2016–present; Winifred Rossi 2012–2016; Sherry Sherman 1994–2012; Marcia Ory 1994–2001; National Institute of Nursing Research, Bethesda, MD—Program Officers. Central Laboratory: University of Michigan, Ann Arbor—Daniel McConnell (Central Ligand Assay Satellite Services). Coordinating Centre: University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA—Maria Mori Brooks, PI 2012–present; Kim Sutton-Tyrrell, PI 2001–2012; New England Research Institutes, Watertown, MA—Sonja McKinlay, PI 1995–2001. Steering Committee: Susan Johnson, Current Chair, Chris Gallagher, Former Chair. The authors also thank one anonymous reviewer for their helpful comments.

Data Availability

All data are publicly available at https://www.icpsr.umich.edu/web/ICPSR/series/00253 with no restrictions. Data were cleaned for use within this study.

Funding Statement

MA is funded by the ESRC-BBSRC Soc-B Centre for Doctoral Training (grant no. ES/P000347/1). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to public, or preparation of the manuscript. https://www.ucl.ac.uk/soc-b-biosocial-doctoral-training/.

References

  • 1.Avis NE, Brockwell S, Colvin A. A universal menopausal syndrome? Am J Med. 2005;118 Suppl 12B:37–46. Epub 2006/01/18. 10.1016/j.amjmed.2005.09.057 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Genazzani AR, Gambacciani M. Vasomotor symptoms and negative affect: time to act. Menopause-the Journal of the North American Menopause Society. 2011;18(12):1265–6. 10.1097/gme.0b013e31823767b6 WOS:000297914800002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Gold EB, Colvin A, Avis N, Bromberger J, Greendale GA, Powell L, et al. Longitudinal analysis of the association between vasomotor symptoms and race/ethnicity across the menopausal transition: study of women's health across the nation. Am J Public Health. 2006;96(7):1226–35. Epub 2006/06/01. 10.2105/AJPH.2005.066936 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Randolph JF Jr., Sowers M, Bondarenko I, Gold EB, Greendale GA, Bromberger JT, et al. The relationship of longitudinal change in reproductive hormones and vasomotor symptoms during the menopausal transition. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2005;90(11):6106–12. Epub 2005/09/08. 10.1210/jc.2005-1374 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Thurston RC, Joffe H. Vasomotor symptoms and menopause: findings from the Study of Women's Health across the Nation. Obstet Gynecol Clin North Am. 2011;38(3):489–501. Epub 2011/10/04. 10.1016/j.ogc.2011.05.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Melby MK, Lock M, Kaufert P. Culture and symptom reporting at menopause. Hum Reprod Update. 2005;11(5):495–512. Epub 2005/05/28. 10.1093/humupd/dmi018 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Williams RE, Kalilani L, DiBenedetti DB, Zhou X, Granger AL, Fehnel SE, et al. Frequency and severity of vasomotor symptoms among peri- and postmenopausal women in the United States. Climacteric. 2008;11(1):32–43. Epub 2008/01/19. 10.1080/13697130701744696 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Anderson D, Yoshizawa T, Gollschewski S, Atogami F, Courtney M. Menopause in Australia and Japan: effects of country of residence on menopausal status and menopausal symptoms. Climacteric. 2004;7(2):165–74. Epub 2004/10/23. 10.1080/13697130410001713760 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Gold EB, Sternfeld B, Kelsey JL, Brown C, Mouton C, Reame N, et al. Relation of demographic and lifestyle factors to symptoms in a multi-racial/ethnic population of women 40–55 years of age. Am J Epidemiol. 2000;152(5):463–73. Epub 2000/09/12. 10.1093/aje/152.5.463 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Li L, Wu J, Pu D, Zhao Y, Wan C, Sun L, et al. Factors associated with the age of natural menopause and menopausal symptoms in Chinese women. Maturitas. 2012;73(4):354–60. Epub 2012/10/03. 10.1016/j.maturitas.2012.09.008 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Melby MK. Vasomotor symptom prevalence and language of menopause in Japan. Menopause. 2005;12(3):250–7. Epub 2005/05/10. 10.1097/01.gme.0000146108.27840.d9? . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Yang Y, Arnot M, Mace R. Current ecology, not ancestral dispersal patterns, influences menopause symptom severity. Ecol Evol. 2019;9(22):12503–14. Epub 2019/12/04. 10.1002/ece3.5705 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Avis NE, Stellato R, Crawford S, Bromberger J, Ganz P, Cain V, et al. Is there a menopausal syndrome? Menopausal status and symptoms across racial/ethnic groups. Social Science & Medicine. 2001;52(3):345–56. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Thurston RC, Bromberger JT, Joffe H, Avis NE, Hess R, Crandall CJ, et al. Beyond frequency: who is most bothered by vasomotor symptoms? Menopause. 2008;15(5):841–7. Epub 2008/06/04. 10.1097/gme.0b013e318168f09b [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Gold EB, Block G, Crawford S, Lachance L, FitzGerald G, Miracle H, et al. Lifestyle and demographic factors in relation to vasomotor symptoms: baseline results from the Study of Women's Health Across the Nation. Am J Epidemiol. 2004;159(12):1189–99. Epub 2004/06/12. 10.1093/aje/kwh168 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Falconi AM, Gold EB, Janssen I. The longitudinal relation of stress during the menopausal transition to fibrinogen concentrations: results from the Study of Women's Health Across the Nation. Menopause. 2016;23(5):518–27. Epub 2016/02/18. 10.1097/GME.0000000000000579 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Segerstrom SC, Miller GE. Psychological stress and the human immune system: a meta-analytic study of 30 years of inquiry. Psychol Bull. 2004;130(4):601–30. Epub 2004/07/15. 10.1037/0033-2909.130.4.601 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Steptoe A, Kivimaki M. Stress and cardiovascular disease. Nat Rev Cardiol. 2012;9(6):360–70. Epub 2012/04/05. 10.1038/nrcardio.2012.45 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Leserman J, Petitto JM, Golden RN, Gaynes BN, Gu H, Perkins DO, et al. Impact of stressful life events, depression, social support, coping, and cortisol on progression to AIDS. Am J Psychiatry. 2000;157(8):1221–8. Epub 2000/07/27. 10.1176/appi.ajp.157.8.1221 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Kiecolt-Glaser JK, Page GG, Marucha PT, MacCallum RC, Glaser R. Psychological influences on surgical recovery. Perspectives from psychoneuroimmunology. Am Psychol. 1998;53(11):1209–18. Epub 1998/11/27. 10.1037//0003-066x.53.11.1209 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Marucha PT, Kiecolt-Glaser JK, Favagehi M. Mucosal wound healing is impaired by examination stress. Psychosom Med. 1998;60(3):362–5. Epub 1998/06/13. 10.1097/00006842-199805000-00025 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Kiecolt-Glaser JK, Glaser R, Gravenstein S, Malarkey WB, Sheridan J. Chronic stress alters the immune response to influenza virus vaccine in older adults. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1996;93(7):3043–7. Epub 1996/04/02. 10.1073/pnas.93.7.3043 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Cohen S, Frank E, Doyle WJ, Skoner DP, Rabin BS, Gwaltney JM. Types of stressors that increase susceptibility to the common cold in healthy adults. Health Psychology. 1998;17(3):214–23. 10.1037//0278-6133.17.3.214 WOS:000073598500002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Cohen S, Tyrrell DA, Smith AP. Psychological stress and susceptibility to the common cold. N Engl J Med. 1991;325(9):606–12. Epub 1991/08/29. 10.1056/NEJM199108293250903 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Nosek M, Kennedy HP, Beyene Y, Taylor D, Gilliss C, Lee K. The effects of perceived stress and attitudes toward menopause and aging on symptoms of menopause. J Midwifery Womens Health. 2010;55(4):328–34. Epub 2010/07/16. 10.1016/j.jmwh.2009.09.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Bauld R, Brown RF. Stress, psychological distress, psychosocial factors, menopause symptoms and physical health in women. Maturitas. 2009;62(2):160–5. Epub 2009/01/27. 10.1016/j.maturitas.2008.12.004 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Sood R, Kuhle CL, Kapoor E, Thielen JM, Frohmader KS, Mara KC, et al. Association of mindfulness and stress with menopausal symptoms in midlife women. Climacteric. 2019;22(4):377–82. Epub 2019/01/18. 10.1080/13697137.2018.1551344 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Jalava-Broman J, Junttila N, Sillanmaki L, Makinen J, Rautava P. Psychological behavior patterns and coping with menopausal symptoms among users and non-users of hormone replacement therapy in Finnish cohorts of women aged 52–56 years. Maturitas. 2020;133:7–12. Epub 2020/02/02. 10.1016/j.maturitas.2019.12.009 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Perrig-Chiello P, Hopflinger F. Aging parents and their middle-aged children: Demographic and psychosocial challenges. European Journal of Ageing. 2005;2:183–91. 10.1007/s10433-005-0003-z [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Dare JS. Transitions in midlife women's lives: contemporary experiences. Health Care Women Int. 2011;32(2):111–33. Epub 2011/01/14. 10.1080/07399332.2010.500753 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Sakraida TJ. Common themes in the divorce transition experience of midlife women. Journal of Divorce & Remarriage. 2005;43(1/2):69–88. [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Glazer G, Zeller R, Delumba L, Kalinyak C, Hobfoll S, Winchell J, et al. The Ohio Midlife Women's Study. Health Care Women Int. 2002;23(6–7):612–30. Epub 2002/11/07. 10.1080/07399330290107377 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Zhao D, Liu C, Feng X, Hou F, Xu X, Li P. Menopausal symptoms in different substages of perimenopause and their relationships with social support and resilience. Menopause. 2019;26(3):233–9. Epub 2018/09/27. 10.1097/GME.0000000000001208 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Dimkpa DI. Psychosocial adjustment needs of menopausal women. International Multidisciplinary Journal, Ethiopia. 2011;5(5):288–302. [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Yazdkhasti M, Keshavarz M, Khoei EM, Hosseini A, Esmaeilzadeh S, Pebdani MA, et al. The Effect of Support Group Method on Quality of Life in Post-menopausal Women. Iran J Public Health. 2012;41(11):78–84. Epub 2013/01/11. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Uchino BN. Social support and health: a review of physiological processes potentially underlying links to disease outcomes. J Behav Med. 2006;29(4):377–87. Epub 2006/06/08. 10.1007/s10865-006-9056-5 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Thoits PA. Mechanisms linking social ties and support to physical and mental health. J Health Soc Behav. 2011;52(2):145–61. Epub 2011/06/16. 10.1177/0022146510395592 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Boen CE, Barrow DA, Bensen JT, Farnan L, Gerstel A, Hendrix LH, et al. Social Relationships, Inflammation, and Cancer Survival. Cancer Epidemiol Biomarkers Prev. 2018;27(5):541–9. Epub 2018/02/25. 10.1158/1055-9965.EPI-17-0836 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Ikeda A, Kawachi I, Iso H, Iwasaki M, Inoue M, Tsugane S. Social support and cancer incidence and mortality: the JPHC study cohort II. Cancer Causes Control. 2013;24(5):847–60. Epub 2013/04/04. 10.1007/s10552-013-0147-7 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Fratiglioni L, Wang HX, Ericsson K, Maytan M, Winblad B. Influence of social network on occurrence of dementia: a community-based longitudinal study. Lancet. 2000;355(9212):1315–9. Epub 2000/04/25. 10.1016/S0140-6736(00)02113-9 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Dyer AH, Murphy C, Lawlor B, Kennelly SP, Study Group For The N. Social networks in mild-to-moderate Alzheimer disease: longitudinal relationships with dementia severity, cognitive function, and adverse events. Aging Ment Health. 2020:1–7. Epub 2020/04/08. 10.1080/13607863.2020.1745146 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Shaya FT, Chirikov VV, Daniel Mullins C, Shematek J, Howard D, Foster C, et al. Social networks help control hypertension. J Clin Hypertens (Greenwich). 2013;15(1):34–40. Epub 2013/01/04. 10.1111/jch.12036 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Ford ES, Ahluwalia IB, Galuska DA. Social relationships and cardiovascular disease risk factors: findings from the third national health and nutrition examination survey. Prev Med. 2000;30(2):83–92. Epub 2000/02/05. 10.1006/pmed.1999.0606 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Cohen S, Wills TA. Stress, social support, and the buffering hypothesis. Psychol Bull. 1985;98(2):310–57. Epub 1985/09/01. 10.1037/0033-2909.98.2.310 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Sutton-Tyrell K, Selzer F, R. Sowers M, Neer R, Powell L, Gold EB, et al. Study of Women's Health Across the Nation (SWAN), 2001–2003: Visit 05 Dataset. Inter-university Consortium for Political and Social Research; [distributor]. 2018. 10.3886/ICPSR30501.v2 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Sutton-Tyrell K, Selzer F, Sowers MR, Finkelstein J, Powell L, Gold E, et al. Study of Women's Health Across the Nation (SWAN), 2004–2006: Visit 08 Dataset. Inter-university Consortium for Political and Social Research; [distributor]. 2018. 10.3886/ICPSR32122.v2 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Sutton-Tyrell K, Selzer F, Sowers MR, Finkelstein J, Powell L, Gold E, et al. Study of Women's Health Across the Nation (SWAN), 2005–2007: Visit 09 Dataset. Inter-university Consortium for Political and Social Research; [distributor]. 2018. 10.3886/ICPSR32721.v2 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Sutton-Tyrell K, Selzer F, Sowers MR, Finkelstein J, Powell L, Gold EB, et al. Study of Women's Health Across the Nation (SWAN), 2002–2004: Visit 06 Dataset. Inter-university Consortium for Political and Social Research; [distributor]. 2018. 10.3886/ICPSR31181.v2 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Sutton-Tyrrell K, Selzer F, Sowers M, Finkelstein J, Powell L, Gold E, et al. Study of Women's Health Across the Nation (SWAN), 2006–2008: Visit 10 Dataset. Inter-university Consortium for Political and Social Research; [distributor]. 2018. 10.3886/ICPSR32961.v2 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Sutton-Tyrrell K, Selzer F, Sowers M, R., Neer R, Powell L, Gold EB, et al. Study of Women's Health Across the Nation (SWAN): Visit 04 Dataset, [United States], 2000–2002. Inter-university Consortium for Political and Social Research; [distributor]. 2019. 10.3886/ICPSR30142.v4 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Sutton-Tyrrell K, Selzer F, Sowers M, R., Neer R, Powell L, Gold EB, et al. Study of Women's Health Across the Nation (SWAN): Visit 03 Dataset, [United States], 1999–2001. Inter-university Consortium for Political and Social Research; [distributor]. 2019. 10.3886/ICPSR29701.v4 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Sutton-Tyrrell K, Selzer F, Sowers M, R., Neer R, Powell L, Gold EB, et al. Study of Women's Health Across the Nation (SWAN): Visit 02 Dataset, [United States], 1998–2000. Inter-university Consortium for Political and Social Research; [distributor]. 2019. 10.3886/ICPSR29401.v4 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Sutton-Tyrrell K, Selzer F, Sowers M, R., Neer R, Powell L, Gold EB, et al. Study of Women's Health Across the Nation (SWAN): Visit 01 Dataset, [United States], 1997–1999. Inter-university Consortium for Political and Social Research; [distributor]. 2019. 10.3886/ICPSR29221.v3 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Sherbourne CD, Stewart AL. The MOS social support survey. Soc Sci Med. 1991;32(6):705–14. Epub 1991/01/01. 10.1016/0277-9536(91)90150-b . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Yu DS, Lee DT, Woo J. Psychometric testing of the Chinese version of the medical outcomes study social support survey (MOS-SSS-C). Res Nurs Health. 2004;27(2):135–43. Epub 2004/03/26. 10.1002/nur.20008 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Saddki N, Sulaiman Z, Abdullah S, Zakaria N, Mohamad N, Ab Razak A, et al. Psychometric properties of the Malay version of the Medical Outcomes Study Social Support Survey (MOS-SSS) in a sample of patients with HIV. J Hiv-Aids Soc Serv. 2017;16(1):60–74. 10.1080/15381501.2015.1107801 WOS:000396709400006. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Robitaille A, Orpana H, McIntosh CN. Psychometric properties, factorial structure, and measurement invariance of the English and French versions of the Medical Outcomes Study social support scale. Health Rep. 2011;22(2):33–40. Epub 2011/08/19. . [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Khuong LQ, Vu TT, Huynh VN, Thai TT. Psychometric properties of the medical outcomes study: social support survey among methadone maintenance patients in Ho Chi Minh City, Vietnam: a validation study. Subst Abuse Treat Prev Policy. 2018;13(1):8 Epub 2018/02/16. 10.1186/s13011-018-0147-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Bromberger JT, Assmann SF, Avis NE, Schocken M, Kravitz HM, Cordal A. Persistent mood symptoms in a multiethnic community cohort of pre- and perimenopausal women. Am J Epidemiol. 2003;158(4):347–56. Epub 2003/08/14. 10.1093/aje/kwg155 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Vernon SW, Roberts RE. Measuring nonspecific psychological distress and other dimensions of psychopathology. Further observations on the problem. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1981;38(11):1239–47. Epub 1981/11/01. 10.1001/archpsyc.1981.01780360055005 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Augoulea A, Moros M, Kokras N, Karageorgiou V, Paschou SA, Lymberi R, et al. Association of menopausal symptoms with sociodemographic factors and personality traits. Menopause Rev. 2019;18(4):191–7. Epub 2020/03/07. 10.5114/pm.2019.93117 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Gallicchio L, Miller SR, Visvanathan K, Lewis LM, Babus J, Zacur H, et al. Cigarette smoking, estrogen levels, and hot flashes in midlife women. Maturitas. 2006;53(2):133–43. Epub 2005/12/22. 10.1016/j.maturitas.2005.03.007 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Avis NE, Crawford SL, Green R. Vasomotor Symptoms Across the Menopause Transition: Differences Among Women. Obstet Gynecol Clin North Am. 2018;45(4):629–40. Epub 2018/11/08. 10.1016/j.ogc.2018.07.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Del Sueldo M, Martell-Claros N, Abad-Cardiel M, Zilberman JM, Marchegiani R, Fernandez-Perez C. Health perception in menopausal women. Int J Womens Health. 2018;10:655–61. Epub 2018/11/15. 10.2147/IJWH.S173891 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Lian H. A note on conditional Akaike information for Poisson regression with random effects. Electron J Stat. 2012;6:1–9. 10.1214/12-Ejs665 WOS:000298886400001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Burnham KP, Anderson DR. Model selection and multimodel inference: a practical information-theoretic approach. New York, N.Y: Spinger-Verlag; 2002. [Google Scholar]
  • 67.R Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical coding. R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2020:Vienna, Austria: See https://www.R-project.org/. [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Bates D, Machler M, Bolker BM, Walker SC. Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using lme4. J Stat Softw. 2015;67(1):1–48. 10.18637/jss.v067.i01 WOS:000365981400001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Lüdecke D. ggeffects: Tidy Data Frames of Marginal Effects from Regression Models. Journal of Open Source Software. 2018;3(26):772. [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Mazerolle MJ. AICcmodavg: Model selection and multimodel inference based on (Q)AIC(c). R package version 23–1. 2020;https://cran.r-project.org/package=AICcmodavg.
  • 71.Wickham H. ggplot2: Elegant Graphics for Data Analysis. New York: Springer-Verlag; 2016. [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Ayers B, Forshaw M, Hunter MS. The impact of attitudes towards the menopause on women's symptom experience: a systematic review. Maturitas. 2010;65(1):28–36. Epub 2009/12/04. 10.1016/j.maturitas.2009.10.016 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Krause N. Social support, stress, and well-being among older adults. J Gerontol. 1986;41(4):512–9. Epub 1986/07/01. 10.1093/geronj/41.4.512 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Rook KS. The negative side of social interaction: impact on psychological well-being. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1984;46(5):1097–108. Epub 1984/05/01. 10.1037//0022-3514.46.5.1097 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Brown LL, Mitchell UA, Ailshire JA. Disentangling the Stress Process: Race/Ethnic Differences in the Exposure and Appraisal of Chronic Stressors Among Older Adults. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2020;75(3):650–60. Epub 2018/06/08. 10.1093/geronb/gby072 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Gold EB, Bromberger J, Crawford S, Samuels S, Greendale GA, Harlow SD, et al. Factors associated with age at natural menopause in a multiethnic sample of midlife women. Am J Epidemiol. 2001;153(9):865–74. Epub 2001/04/27. 10.1093/aje/153.9.865 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Chyu L, Upchurch DM. Racial and ethnic patterns of allostatic load among adult women in the United States: findings from the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey 1999–2004. J Womens Health. 2011;20(4):575–83. Epub 2011/03/25. 10.1089/jwh.2010.2170 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Bove R, Vaughan T, Chitnis T, Wicks P, De Jager PL. Women's experiences of menopause in an online MS cohort: A case series. Mult Scler Relat Dis. 2016;9:56–9. Epub 2016/09/21. 10.1016/j.msard.2016.06.015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.de Bruin JP, Bovenhuis H, van Noord PA, Pearson PL, van Arendonk JA, te Velde ER, et al. The role of genetic factors in age at natural menopause. Hum Reprod. 2001;16(9):2014–8. Epub 2001/08/31. 10.1093/humrep/16.9.2014 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Sergeant J, Rizq R. 'Its all part of the big CHANGE': a grounded theory study of women's identity during menopause. J Psychosom Obstet Gynaecol. 2017;38(3):189–201. Epub 2017/01/10. 10.1080/0167482X.2016.1270937 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Wadsworth G, Green E. Changing Women: An Analysis of Difference and Diversity in Women's Accounts of Menopause. In: Earle S, Letherby G, editors. Gender, Identity & Reproduction London: Palgrave Macmillan; 2003. [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Duffy O, Iversen L, Hannaford PC. The menopause 'It's somewhere between a taboo and a joke'. A focus group study. Climacteric. 2011;14(4):497–505. 10.3109/13697137.2010.549974 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Minkin MJ, Reiter S, Maamari R. Prevalence of postmenopausal symptoms in North America and Europe. Menopause. 2015;22(11):1231–8. Epub 2015/05/07. 10.1097/GME.0000000000000464 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Deeks AA, McCabe MP. Well-being and menopause: an investigation of purpose in life, self-acceptance and social role in premenopausal, perimenopausal and postmenopausal women. Qual Life Res. 2004;13(2):389–98. Epub 2004/04/17. 10.1023/B:QURE.0000018506.33706.05 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Ahmad AH, Zakaria R. Pain in Times of Stress. Malays J Med Sci. 2015;22(Spec Issue):52–61. Epub 2016/03/24. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

Martha Asuncion Sánchez-Rodríguez

30 Oct 2020

PONE-D-20-28399

The relationship between social support, stressful events, and menopause symptoms

PLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Arnot,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

I know that the review has taken a long time, but is has been difficult, given the world situation, to find reviewers who accept. In general, the study is interesting, but it is necessary to attend to several theoretical and methodological issues to better support it; thus, it is important to consider the observations made to the manuscript by the reviewer in all its sections.

Please submit your revised manuscript by Dec 14 2020 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Martha Asuncion Sánchez-Rodríguez, PhD

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments:

I know that the review has taken a long time, but is has been difficult, given the world situation, to find reviewers who accept. In general, the study is interesting, but it is necessary to attend to several theoretical and methodological issues to better support it; thus, it is important to consider the observations made to the manuscript by the reviewer in all its sections.

Journal Requirements:

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

2. Please include captions for your Supporting Information files at the end of your manuscript, and update any in-text citations to match accordingly. Please see our Supporting Information guidelines for more information: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/supporting-information.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Partly

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: This paper is focused in a very interesting study field, relationship between social support with stressful events and menopause symptoms. However, the manuscript has several weaknesses in the theoretical-support, methods and discussion. Therefore, it cannot be accepted for publication in its current form.

Major comments

1.Introduction.

• The text included in this section is too long and imprecise. In this sense, the authors should only include the theoretical support related to the study. It is suggested that you review the following articles:

(i) Thoits, P.A. Mechanisms linking social ties and support to physical and mental health. J. Health Soc. Behav. 2011, 52, 145–161. ,

(ii) Uchino, B.N., et al. The relationship between social support and physiological processes: A review with emphasis on underlying mechanisms and implications for health. Psychol. Bull. 1996, 119, 488–531.,

(iii) Uchino, B.N. Social support and health: A review of physiological processes potentially underlying links to disease outcomes. J. Behav. Med. 2006, 29, 377–387.,

(iv) Smith, K.P. and Christakis, N.A. Social networks and health. Annu. Rev. Sociol. 2008, 34, 405–429.

•It is not necessary to make the hypotheses explicit and the objective must be concise and precise.

2. Methods.

• What is the reliability of the instruments used to measure social support and stressful events? C

3. Results

• Los títulos de los cuadros deben ser menos extesos y más precisos. No es necesario que incluya • The titles of the tables should be less long and more precise. You do not need to include the reference of the statistical analysis (this was already done in the methods section).

4. Discussion.

• The authors do not analyze or compare their results with similar studies. In this sense, you must consider the similarity of the instruments used to measure the variables.

• The authors should consider that the self-report of stressful events are not necessarily related to the perception and response to stress.

•Study limitations should be included.

5. The conclusions are not based on the results.

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2021 Jan 27;16(1):e0245444. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0245444.r002

Author response to Decision Letter 0


19 Nov 2020

We thank the reviewer for their helpful comments, and have addressed each one below.

Introduction

• The text included in this section is too long and imprecise. In this sense, the authors should only include the theoretical support related to the study. It is suggested that you review the following articles:

i. Thoits, P.A. Mechanisms linking social ties and support to physical and mental health. J. Health Soc. Behav. 2011, 52, 145–161. ,

ii. Uchino, B.N., et al. The relationship between social support and physiological processes: A review with emphasis on underlying mechanisms and implications for health. Psychol. Bull. 1996, 119, 488–531.,

iii. Uchino, B.N. Social support and health: A review of physiological processes potentially underlying links to disease outcomes. J. Behav. Med. 2006, 29, 377–387.,

iv. Smith, K.P. and Christakis, N.A. Social networks and health. Annu. Rev. Sociol. 2008, 34, 405–429

We have completely rewritten the introduction to ensure precision in language, and to not deviate from the topic at hand. We have also incorporated some of the suggested references as appropriate.

• It is not necessary to make the hypotheses explicit and the objective must be concise and precise.

We have removed the explicit hypothesis and rewritten this paragraph:

“In this study, we build on previous research into stress, support and menopause symptoms by testing whether social support acts as an effective buffer against the negative influence of stress on VMS. First, we test whether higher levels of social support and stress associate with less frequent VMS and more frequent VMS, respectively. Further, we test whether social support buffers against the proposed negative impact of stress on VMS.”

Methods

• What is the reliability of the instruments used to measure social support and stressful events?

We had already highlighted the reliability of the measure of stressful events (L293-295):

“This is a modified version of the Psychiatric Epidemiology Research Interview life events scale (69), which has been validated across multiple racial and ethnic groups and shows high internal consistency (70).”

A few sentences have now been added about the reliability of our social support measure (L283-286):

“This measure is taken from the Medical Outcome Study: Social Support Survey (MOS-SSS) which is one of the most widely used instruments for measuring social support (69). The MOS-SSS has high levels of validity and reliability and has been translated into multiple languages (70-73).”

Results

• The titles of the tables should be less long and more precise.

Thank you for this suggestion. While recognising the value of concise titles, we have decided to keep the titles of the tables as they are. This is because we are aware that readers might just look at the tables for the results/quick summary of the paper (e.g., tables and graphs are frequently shared on social media or in talks without manuscript text). Therefore, we want to ensure the tables make sense in their own right, and we have deemed the titles of the tables necessary.

• You do not need to include the reference of the statistical analysis (this was already done in the methods section).

This has been removed.

Discussion

• The authors do not analyze or compare their results with similar studies. In this sense, you must consider the similarity of the instruments used to measure the variables

Thank you for this suggestion. We believe we have already done this in the following sentences:

• “This is in line with findings that perceived stress is influential over symptomology (23), and that women who are deemed to be more resilient experience fewer menopause symptoms (31).”

• “Elsewhere in the literature, there are reports that various kinds of support (familial, emotional, etc.) are associated with fewer menopause symptoms (31, 33, 70), but we did not replicate these effects in our analysis. When looking at other domains of health, researchers have found that social support is not always protective against stress (71, 72). It may be in this case that the support question was not specific enough to capture its effect on menopause symptoms (e.g. women could have been asked “how often do you have someone to talk to about your health”),”

Though, for clarity, we have added in the following sentence:

“with previous research sometimes using combined measures of support in studies.”

• The authors should consider that the self-report of stressful events are not necessarily related to the perception and response to stress.

We are not completely clear what is meant by this comment, however, the measure of stress used in this study is self-reported and includes how upset the respondent is, which is surely subjective and therefore related to perception. For example, how upset someone is by a life stressor will be relative to past experience of stressful events, general resilience, etc. (Dohrenwend, 2006), and therefore we think that the measure of stress used in this study would be related to perception and response to stress.

• Study limitations should be included.

Thank you for this suggestion. Limitations have been added in as follows:

“This study is correlative, and therefore we cannot definitely say that stressful events found upsetting by a woman result in worse menopause symptoms. Further, the precise causal mechanism that might underly the relationship is not clear. Nonetheless, the persisting relationship between stress and menopause symptoms allows us to consider possible behavioural interventions women could use to deal with the symptoms associated with the menopausal transition.”

Conclusion

• The conclusions are not based on the results

Thank you for highlighting this. The conclusion has been rewritten to remove any speculative conclusions, or where appropriate we have highlighted when they are speculative inferences of results.

References

DOHRENWEND, B. P. 2006. Inventorying stressful life events as risk factors for psychopathology: Toward resolution of the problem of intracategory variability. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 477-495.

Attachment

Submitted filename: MR_Arnot et al. 2020_Reviewer reply.docx

Decision Letter 1

Martha Asuncion Sánchez-Rodríguez

2 Dec 2020

PONE-D-20-28399R1

The relationship between social support, stressful events, and menopause symptoms

PLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Arnot,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

==============================

This corrected version has been revised by a new reviewer to complete two reviewers,  which contributes with very important comments that may be improve the manuscript. I suggest attending to the observations, mainly those of the data analysis to better support the findings.

==============================

Please submit your revised manuscript by Jan 16 2021 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Martha Asuncion Sánchez-Rodríguez, PhD

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments (if provided):

This corrected version has been revised by a new reviewer to complete two reviewers, which contributes with very important comments that may be improve the manuscript. I suggest attending to the observations, mainly those of the data analysis to better support the findings.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #1: All comments have been addressed

Reviewer #2: (No Response)

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Partly

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: No

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: (No Response)

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

6. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: In general, the authors have made corrections to the manuscript considering the comments. However, I reiterate that it is necessary that the titles of tables 2 and 3 should be more concise. In this regard, the specifications can be included at the bottom of the table.

Reviewer #2: The authors are seeking to examine support buffering hypothesis in stress and menopause symptoms. I did not review the original submission – so I am responding to the revisions that were requested and made and the revised manuscript. The topic is interesting and important and the authors use a wonderful dataset to conduct their analyses. However, I think that the authors stop short in their analyses to actually advance the research in a substantial way.

First, the Introduction is well written – so no major issues there and I think that the authors addressed the prior reviewers’ comments adequately. However, the final sentence in the Introduction threw me. The authors state: “This research aims to clarify how support and stress are associated with one another in regards to VMS, as well as informing the potential efficacy of support-based treatments – such as mindfulness and mediation – to manage menopause symptoms.” This makes it sound like the authors are going to be looking at mindfulness and meditation (I think they meant this instead of “mediation”) in their analyses. However, in reading the rest of the manuscript, there is nothing about these concepts in the analyses. Also, this is the first mention of these constructs in the Introduction. I suggest that this sentence be removed and maybe moved to the Discussion as a future direction.

With respect to the social support measure, I am concerned about the authors letting the data drive their analyses. In my reading, they first tested which social support measures best predicted VMS and then chose the one that best predicted VMS to go forward with in their analyses. First, this feels like data mining to me. Second, if social support is a moderator, there is no reason that it needs to be related to the outcome (see Baron & Kenny, 1986) – in other words, a moderator is supposed to be orthogonal to the predictor and outcome.

Additionally, the authors highlight the race/ethnic differences in levels VMS symptoms and then speculate what that might mean. But, they have the data to conduct analyses to see if the link between stress and VMS differs by race/ethnicity and whether the support buffer is found for some race/ethnic groups as opposed to others.

Finally, they mention that a limitation of their analyses is that they are correlative. However, they have multiple waves of data and could easily have conducted lagged analyses to determine whether causation would be supported.

Overall, I feel that the authors need to do more in their analyses and that as of now, they essentially only have shown that stress is connected with VMS symptoms and I am not sure that is substantive enough for publication. They have the data to do a lot more – and it should not be too hard to do these additional analyses.

Minor Points:

In the first paragraph, first sentence, the authors say “The menopause is often a significant event for women…” I find the use of “The” to be awkward – I think it should just say “Menopause is often a significant event for women…”

In Table 2, I think there is a typo – half the sample is Chinese American and only 7% white but that does not match with the text. Also, Caucasian is no longer used (it is actually attached to white supremacists: https://www.latimes.com/opinion/story/2019-09-10/race-caucasian-myth-racism) – instead, it should be listed as “White”.

**********

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: Yes: Víctor Manuel Mendoza-Núñez

Reviewer #2: No

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2021 Jan 27;16(1):e0245444. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0245444.r004

Author response to Decision Letter 1


14 Dec 2020

We would like to thank the reviewers for their helpful comments. We have addressed each one below.

Reviewer #1

• In general, the authors have made corrections to the manuscript considering the comments. However, I reiterate that it is necessary that the titles of tables 2 and 3 should be more concise. In this regard, the specifications can be included at the bottom of the table.

Excess information has been moved to the bottom of the table.

Reviewer #2

• The authors state: “This research aims to clarify how support and stress are associated with one another in regards to VMS, as well as informing the potential efficacy of support-based treatments – such as mindfulness and mediation – to manage menopause symptoms.” This makes it sound like the authors are going to be looking at mindfulness and meditation (I think they meant this instead of “mediation”) in their analyses. However, in reading the rest of the manuscript, there is nothing about these concepts in the analyses. Also, this is the first mention of these constructs in the Introduction. I suggest that this sentence be removed and maybe moved to the Discussion as a future direction.

This sentence has been removed from the introduction.

• With respect to the social support measure, I am concerned about the authors letting the data drive their analyses. In my reading, they first tested which social support measures best predicted VMS and then chose the one that best predicted VMS to go forward with in their analyses. First, this feels like data mining to me. Second, if social support is a moderator, there is no reason that it needs to be related to the outcome (see Baron & Kenny, 1986) – in other words, a moderator is supposed to be orthogonal to the predictor and outcome.

Thank you for pointing this out. We were by no means trying to data mine. Rather, as it’s survey data with hundreds of possible variables, we were using pre-specified variable selection strategies. However, we have taken this comment on board and changed the measure of social support used in the analysis, and instead used a composite measure that includes the four forms of support collected by SWAN. This has not changed the overall results of the analysis, and we have included the individual measures in the S1 Text.

• Additionally, the authors highlight the race/ethnic differences in levels VMS symptoms and then speculate what that might mean. But, they have the data to conduct analyses to see if the link between stress and VMS differs by race/ethnicity and whether the support buffer is found for some race/ethnic groups as opposed to others.

Thank you for highlighting this. We don’t wish to specifically add this as a research question as it is not one of our aims; however, we do now mention it more specifically in the results and comment on how our data fit the claims that stress underlies the relationship between ethnicity and VMS: “Here, African American women experienced the most frequent symptoms, while Chinese and Japanese Americans reported symptoms less often, which is in line with findings from previous research (3, 9, 13, 75, 76). It has been suggested that the relationship between stress and menopause symptoms may help to explain some of this variation. In the US (where data used in this study was collected), African American individuals present more markers of stress than other ethnicities (77), likely due to economic and systematic factors (75). However, results in this study suggest that stress does not mediate the relationship between ethnicity and VMS, with the relationship persisting after the inclusion of the stress index and stress dose within the model (S1 Text Table C). Additionally, attributing ethnic differences to stress does not account for why women from an East Asian background in this sample report fewer symptoms, as – due to being an ethnic minority – it would be predicted that due to higher levels of stress (75) they would report more symptoms than white women. Therefore, it is unlikely that stress explains the ethnic differences in menopause symptoms, and further research is still required to understand this variation.”

• Finally, they mention that a limitation of their analyses is that they are correlative. However, they have multiple waves of data and could easily have conducted lagged analyses to determine whether causation would be supported.

Thank you for bringing this up. We do not necessarily think a lagged analysis would be theoretically appropriate: as we explain in the introduction, we are suggesting that current stress is driving the relationship between stress and VMS, and therefore we would likely need data on a shorter timescale to establish a definitive causal relationship. Further, by conducting a lagged analysis, we are still only really showing a correlation between stress at t-1 and VMS, and so a significant effect could be found by chance. However, we have added in a post hoc analysis into the results section and discussion, were we find that current stress better predicts VMS than historic stress, which does support our notion that there is a causal relationship.

• In the first paragraph, first sentence, the authors say “The menopause is often a significant event for women…” I find the use of “The” to be awkward – I think it should just say “Menopause is often a significant event for women…”

This has been changed.

• In Table 2, I think there is a typo – half the sample is Chinese American and only 7% white but that does not match with the text. Also, Caucasian is no longer used (it is actually attached to white supremacists: https://www.latimes.com/opinion/story/2019-09-10/race-caucasian-myth-racism) – instead, it should be listed as “White”.

Thanks for pulling these issues up – they have been changed accordingly.

Attachment

Submitted filename: R2_Arnot et al. 2020_Reviewer reply.docx

Decision Letter 2

Martha Asuncion Sánchez-Rodríguez

2 Jan 2021

The relationship between social support, stressful events, and menopause symptoms

PONE-D-20-28399R2

Dear Dr. Arnot,

We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication.

An invoice for payment will follow shortly after the formal acceptance. To ensure an efficient process, please log into Editorial Manager at http://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the 'Update My Information' link at the top of the page, and double check that your user information is up-to-date. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

Kind regards,

Martha Asuncion Sánchez-Rodríguez, PhD

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments (optional):

The reviews and I agree that it is an interesting paper, congratulations and happy new year.

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #1: All comments have been addressed

Reviewer #2: All comments have been addressed

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

6. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: This paper is focused in a very interesting study field, relationship between social support with stressful events and menopause symptoms.

The authors have corrected the manuscript considering all the comments.

Reviewer #2: I feel that the authors have addressed all of my concerns. I especially think it is interesting that the lagged analyses provided more support for their argument about current stress vs. historic stress. I still think it would be interesting to include race/ethnicity as a moderator but I understand that it was not a main aim of their study. Nice job and interesting paper.

**********

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: Yes: Víctor Manuel Mendoza-Núñez PhD

Reviewer #2: No

Acceptance letter

Martha Asuncion Sánchez-Rodríguez

6 Jan 2021

PONE-D-20-28399R2

The relationship between social support, stressful events, and menopause symptoms

Dear Dr. Arnot:

I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact onepress@plos.org.

If we can help with anything else, please email us at plosone@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access.

Kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Dr. Martha Asuncion Sánchez-Rodríguez

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    S1 Text. Supporting text.

    Supporting information containing information on the selection of the social support variable (Figure A), and the derivation of the stress index (Table A) and stress dose (Table B). Figure B demonstrates the justification for how missing data in the stress dose variable was dealt with, and Figure C shows the distribution of the stress dose variable by wave and age. Complete model results are presented in Table C.

    (DOCX)

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: MR_Arnot et al. 2020_Reviewer reply.docx

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: R2_Arnot et al. 2020_Reviewer reply.docx

    Data Availability Statement

    All data are publicly available at https://www.icpsr.umich.edu/web/ICPSR/series/00253 with no restrictions. Data were cleaned for use within this study.


    Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES