Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2021 Jan 27;16(1):e0242546. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0242546

United on Sunday: The effects of secular rituals on social bonding and affect

Sarah J Charles 1,*, Valerie van Mulukom 1, Jennifer E Brown 1, Fraser Watts 2, Robin I M Dunbar 3, Miguel Farias 1
Editor: Amy Michelle DeBaets4
PMCID: PMC7840012  PMID: 33503054

Abstract

Religious rituals are associated with health benefits, potentially produced via social bonding. It is unknown whether secular rituals similarly increase social bonding. We conducted a field study with individuals who celebrate secular rituals at Sunday Assemblies and compared them with participants attending Christian rituals. We assessed levels of social bonding and affect before and after the rituals. Results showed the increase in social bonding taking place in secular rituals is comparable to religious rituals. We also found that both sets of rituals increased positive affect and decreased negative affect, and that the change in positive affect predicted the change in social bonding observed. Together these results suggest that secular rituals might play a similar role to religious ones in fostering feelings of social connection and boosting positive affect.

Introduction

There is a substantial body of research demonstrating beneficial health and wellbeing effects that stem from the attendance of religious rituals (for an overview, see [1]). The current evidence suggests that the benefits of religious ritual attendance include improved wellbeing [2], as well as protection against all-cause mortality [36], depression [79], suicidality [10] and immune dysfunction [11]. Moreover, VanderWeele [1] notes that many of these positive effects appear to be best maintained when ritual attendance is at least once per month. Much of this literature was conducted in western, democratic nations, and within Christian settings, though there are some notable exceptions to this [1214].

A classical assumption of sociological and anthropological sciences is that that one of the primary functions of religion is to promote group solidarity (e.g. [15]). Dunbar [16] has suggested that religious rituals developed as a mechanism to help form and maintain social bonds in groups of humans in particularly effective. A wealth of evidence supports this theory, showing that many of the behaviours that are incorporated into religious rituals lead to feelings of social bonding, such as joint attention [1719], shared goals [20], synchronised movement [2123], music making [2426], eating [27], and moderate alcohol consumption [28, 29]. Accordingly, religious rituals that incorporate a multitude of these behaviours should foster bonds efficiently [16].

Recently, Charles and colleagues [30] provided the first evidence that religious rituals (Christian and Afro-Brazilian) directly increase feelings of social bonding with other attendees. They collected data in over 20 religious rituals in the UK and Brazil, assessing self-reported levels of social bonding before and then after the rituals. Their results showed that taking part in these rituals significantly increased social bonding towards the group. They also reported that increased social bonding was significantly predicted by an increase in positive affect, a decrease in negative affect, a purported increase in endorphins (as reflected by increases in pain threshold) and by feelings of connection to God during the ritual, but not by age, gender, or religiosity.

This not only demonstrated that rituals lead to social bonding, but also provided support for the hypothesis that emotional state–and more specifically positive affect–is related to feelings of social connectedness [19, 31, 32]. This ‘Broaden and Build’ hypothesis [17, 19, 32, 33] suggests that positive affect leads to wellbeing improvements via the broadening of social relationships which then builds an individual’s social support network. The broadening is said to occur as positive affect allows for an increasing scope of attention and behaviours to facilitate bonding with others. This hypothesis also serves as the basis for the proposal that the link between religion and wellbeing stems from changes in positive affect [34, 35], which in turn can lead to improved social bonding, and wellbeing [36]. Therefore, the mechanism by which religious rituals lead to social bonding could be by incorporating behaviours that lead to positive affect, which then contributes to social bonding.

There is increasing evidence for the link between social bonding and health: having strong social bonds has been shown to lead to improved health outcomes compared to those who lack them [37, 38], in the form of reduced mortality [39, 40] and lowering levels of depression, suicidality, and immune dysfunction [41, 42]. These health benefits overlap with those that religious rituals are purported to provide [1]. Consequently, the Broaden and Build hypothesis links religious ritual to wellbeing via positive emotions’ role in broadening social bonding [34, 35].

Charles and colleagues [30] also showed that the feeling of connection to God during the ritual positively predicted changes in social bonding, albeit to a lower level than affect. How this connection to a higher power leads to increased social bonding is unclear, however. It could be that connection to a higher power directly leads to further feelings of connectedness, or it could lead to affect changes, which in turn would promote bonding with others. It may be that attachment to God leads to some of the improved wellbeing effects that stem from religiosity [43, 44]. However, Pirutinsky et al. [44] also showed that, in addition to attachment to God, social support significantly predicted the protective effects of religion against mental health problems. This raises the following questions: which aspects of rituals are particularly apt at providing wellbeing effects, and what role a connection to God plays? What of rituals that are not religious?

Secular rituals

As we progress further into the 21st century, growing numbers of individuals are identifying as non-religious. In 2012, 16% of the world population reported no religious affiliation [45] and these numbers are growing in several countries, such as the UK [46, 47], the USA [48], and other Western countries [49]. Given the positive health effects associated with religious attendance [1], leaving religion or not having a religion, and thus not attending religious rituals, means the positive effects of such rituals can no longer be reaped.

There have, however, been efforts to counteract the loss of religious rituals. For over two centuries there have been attempts at creating secular rituals that mimic religious rituals, such as Comte’s Church of Positivism, established in the 1800s [50]. The Church of Positivism, which had the ambitious aim of becoming the ‘Religion of Humanity’, held regular services at their own temples in the UK, France and Brazil, though with time they failed to recruit new members and have mostly closed down (the last remaining active church is located in the south of Brazil; for a recent documentary, see [51]).

A more recent, and thus far successful, attempt to develop a secular ritual is that of the Sunday Assembly. This movement was initiated in London, England in 2013 by Sanderson Jones and Pippa Evans, as a way of “doing church without god” [52]. This regular Sunday ritual intentionally mimics Christian Evangelical services with traditional hymns being replaced by popular songs with positive messages, such as U2’s ‘It’s a Beautiful Day’. Instead of a sermon, a TED-style informative talk is given on various topics, from the importance of local flora on the community’s sense of identity to talks on gender equality in modern society. Were one to see a video of a Sunday Assembly taking place without sound, it would be largely indistinguishable from many evangelical churches. Since its inception, the Sunday Assembly has grown to become an international brand of secular ritual, with 22 Assemblies in Europe, another 22 in North America, and five across Australia and New Zealand at the time of writing (see https://sundayassembly.online/find-an-assembly/ for a full list).

Price and Launay [53] recently conducted a longitudinal study with the flagship Sunday Assembly in London to better understand some of the impacts on wellbeing that being part of such a group can provide. Their results suggest that attending the Sunday Assembly services significantly improved a composite measure of wellbeing. In the discussion of their paper, Price and Launay [53] hypothesise that the improved wellbeing they found may stem from the social connections made at Sunday Assembly, not unlike connections found in religion and health research (e.g. [54]).

Some researchers believe that religion provides health benefits in ways that secular means cannot such as through the sense of meaning that religion provides (e.g. [55]). However, Galen [56] has suggested that the link between religiosity/spirituality and factors that lead to well-being stems from a congruence fallacy, where religiosity/spirituality is not adequately compared to secular equivalents. In this study we sought to test for the first time whether a secular ritual increased the feelings of social bonding with others in a similar way to a religious ritual. To do so, we recruited participants from Sunday Assembly services, as well as matched Christian churches, and measured levels of social bonding before and after the rituals. Our hypotheses were that: (1) as the Sunday Assembly ritual mimics Sunday church ritual, social bonding will significantly increase at ritual follow-up; (2) The change in social bonding would not be significantly different between participants attending a church service from those attending Sunday Assembly services. As there is research suggesting that emotional state/affect is related to feelings of social connectedness [19, 3032], (3) we expected that positive changes in affect would predict an increase in social bonding, where increased positive affect and decreased negative affect would lead to an increase in social bonding.

Method

Participants

Adult (>18 years) participants were recruited from four Sunday Assemblies in the UK; (a) Central London, (b) Reading, (c) Bristol and (d) London, East End. Four churches from a large dataset [30] were chosen as close matches for total congregation size and level of education.

49 (Mage = 39.6, SDage = 12.24) participants were recruited from Sunday Assemblies, of which 16 identified as male, 32 as female and one identified as non-binary, and these were matched with 50 (Mage = 57.8, SDage = 18.08) participants from churches, of which 34 identified as female and the remainder as male. A total of 99 participants (Mage = 48.2, SDage = 18.21) were included in the study.

Materials

Social bonding

This scale consists of six questions. Five of these were measured on a 7-point Likert scale: “1, not at all”, “2, Very slightly”, “3, A little”, “4, Moderately”, “5, Quite a bit”, “6, Very much”, and “7, extremely”. For the Sunday Assembly participants, these five questions were: (one) “At this moment, how connected do you feel to the people in this Sunday Assembly?” (adapted from [57]); (two) “At this moment, how emotionally close do you feel to the other members of this Sunday Assembly as a whole?”; (three) “Thinking about everyone in this Sunday Assembly now, how much do you trust the others in this group?”; (four) “How much do you like the people in this Sunday Assembly overall?” (adapted from [58]); and (five) “Thinking about everyone in this Sunday Assembly now, do you feel you have a lot in common with others in this congregation?” (adapted from [59]). The last question was the pictorial Inclusions of Others in Self scale (IOS; [60]), which is a series of seven pairs of circles that go from not overlapping (one) to heavily overlapping (seven) to picture how much one’s self-identity overlaps with others/the group (see Fig 1). Answers to these questions were averaged into a single social bonding score. For the church populations, we used the same phrasing but references to “this Sunday Assembly” were replaced with “your congregation” e.g. “At this moment, how connected do you feel to the people in your congregation?”

Fig 1. The inclusion of others in self (adapted from 60) question used as part of the social bonding scale.

Fig 1

Raykov and Marcoulides [61] and Savalei & Reise [62] suggest conducting a factor analysis on any data that is being used to create an average or summed score to check for which measure of reliability should be used. We conducted such a factor analysis (see analysis script) and found that all items loaded onto a single factor, with mean factor loadings were above 0.7, suggesting that the assumption of essential tau-equivalence was not violated. As such Cronbach’s alpha and McDonald’s omega should be equal. Here we present only the alpha values, but the analysis script also provides omega values for those sceptical of alpha due to recent critical articles (e.g. [6365]). Internal reliability for the social bonding measure in the Sunday Assembly participants was α = .93, 95% CI [.90, .96] before the ritual and α = .93, 95% CI [.90, .96] for the post-ritual measure, providing evidence of high scale reliability. For the church participants, these scores were α = .94, 95% CI [.91, .96] and α = .89, 95% CI [.84, .93] for the pre- and post-ritual measures, respectively. The internal reliability for all participants is α = .93, 95% CI [.91, .95] and α = .91, 95% CI [.88, .94] for the pre- and post-ritual measures, respectively, also providing evidence of high internal reliability.

Affect

We used the Positive And Negative Affect Scale (PANAS; [66]) to collect data on the emotional state of participants both before and after ritual participation. The PANAS asks participants to say how much they are feeling 20 emotions (10 positive, 10 negative) “at this moment” measured on a 6-point Likert scale, from zero to five, where zero is “not at all” and five is “very much”. Examples of positive emotions include ‘Interested’, ‘Proud’, and ‘Inspired’, examples of negative emotions include ‘Nervous’, ‘Upset’ and ‘Irritable’. Watson and colleagues [66] sum the values for the 10 emotions for both positive and negative to provide two scores: the sum of the scores for positive emotions, PANAS+, and the sum of scores for the negative emotions, PANAS–.

Other measures

Religion and Spirituality. Sunday Assembly participants were asked to rate how religious and spiritual they considered themselves to be, each on a scale of one to seven, where one meant “Not at all” and seven was “Extremely so”. Church participants had only been asked to self-rate how religious they considered themselves to be.

Demographic information (age, gender identity, level of education) was recorded.

Connectedness to God/Something Bigger. Sunday Assembly participants were asked after their ritual: “During today’s Sunday Assembly, did you feel connected to something bigger than yourself, like the universe, and/or feel a sense of awe or wonder?” This was considered analogous to a sense of awe or wonder that may be felt when connected to the divine in a religious ritual, and church participants were asked after their service “During today’s service, did you feel connected to God, Jesus, and/or the Holy Spirit?”

Procedure

Assembly recruitment. Initial contact was made at the flagship London Sunday Assembly. Once the flagship Assembly confirmed their participation, contact was made with other Assemblies. A total of four assemblies, including Central London, agreed to take part.

Participant recruitment. Ahead of our attending an Assembly, the Assembly alerted attendees that we would be there via newsletter communications, Facebook posts, and announcements at the previous month’s Assembly. On the day of the Assembly, researchers arrived 1 hour before the start of the Assembly. Attendees to the Assembly who arrived anytime between 45 minutes to 5 minutes before the Assembly began were provided with information sheets. Those who opted to take part and met the inclusion criteria were provided with a consent form before taking part.

Participants who took part were provided with a questionnaire, which had an ID code on the front unique to each participant, to ensure anonymity. The pre-Assembly section of the questionnaire contained the PANAS, social bonding questions, and their self-reported level of religiosity and level of spirituality. Halfway through the questionnaire, there was a two-page break which alerted participants that this was the end of the pre-Assembly section. Participants’ names were then noted on a post-it note and attached to the questionnaire to match it with the same participant after the Assembly.

After the Assembly, participants returned to the researchers and filled in the post-Assembly half of the questionnaire, which re-measured both PANAS and social bonding and demographic information. The post-it notes were collected and destroyed to ensure anonymity of the data. After this, the data collection was completed. The same procedure was used for church participants.

Data analysis

A power analysis was conducted to determine the appropriate number of participants. Based on the data reported by Charles et al. [30], the effect size of change in social bonding from before to after a religious ritual was given by both the conservative rR, [67, 68] and the ‘simple difference’ effect size rK [69]. Charles and colleague’s [30] UK religious ritual effects had an effect size of rR = .34 and rK = .62,which converts to a d of 0.72 or 1.58, respectively Using G*Power [70], and a one-tailed Wilcoxon-Signed-Rank test (non-parametric, within-participant design) in the t-test family with the distribution assumed to be the minimum asymptotic relative efficiency (the most conservative distribution assumption; [71]), α = 0.05, Power (1-β) = 0.80, and an effect size of d = .72, we calculated that 16 participants would be needed to have an appropriately powered study for testing the first hypothesis, suggesting this study is appropriately powered.

A similar power analysis for hypothesis two (the comparison between churches and Sunday Assemblies) was also conducted. To detect an interaction effect (before/after ritual interacting with ritual type) using an ANOVA, an effect size of f = .36 (converted from r = .34), α = 0.05, Power (1-β) = 0.80, 2 groups, 2 measurements, nonsphericity correction of 1 and a correlation among repeated measures of 0.75 (calculated from the data collected in this study), a total of 10 participants would be needed to find a within-factors and/or an interaction effect and 56 total participants to find a between factors effect, suggesting this study is appropriately powered.

Results

The full analysis script and anonymised dataset have been made available in the supplementary materials.

Effect of Sunday Assembly attendance on social bonding

A Shapiro-Wilk test was conducted to check whether data met the assumptions for parametric testing. The pre-meeting SB6 score (W = .977, p = .433) was not significantly different from normal, but the post-meeting SB6 score (W = .952, p = .047) was. Therefore, non-parametric tests were used.

To test the hypothesis that there would be an increase in self-reported social bonding, from before to after the Sunday Assembly ritual, a one-tailed, non-parametric Wilcoxon signed-rank test was conducted. As predicted, post-Assembly scores (M = 4.96, SD = 1.16, Mdn = 5.17) were significantly higher than at pre-ritual (M = 4.27, SD = 1.26, Mdn = 4.33, Z = 5.02, p < .001, rR = .51, rK = .84). In this case these results suggest a ‘moderate-to-high’ effect size according to Fergusson’s criteria for social science (Fergusson, 2009). These results remain significant when using a two-tailed test (p < .001), and when using the Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons to account for the follow-up analyses, below (p < .001).

Comparing bonding between church and Sunday Assembly

Both pre- and post-service social bonding scores for church participants were significantly different from normally distributed (W = .927, p = .004 and W = .939, p = .011, respectively). Therefore, a non-parametric form of ANOVA was used for the analysis.

Using the nparLD package of the R coding language, a non-parametric within-between ANOVA was run via the f1.ld.f1 function [72]. The nparLD package’s functions provide an ANOVA-like statistic with the denominator degrees of freedom listed as infinite. The f1.ld.f1 function found that there were significant main effects of both time (F (1, ∞) = 52.06, p < .001) and ritual group (F (1, ∞) = 4.38, p = .036), but there was no significant interaction effect (F (1, ∞) = 3.77, p = .052). As seen in Fig 2, there was a significant increase in social bonding from before to after both ritual types, and there was a significantly higher level of social bonding at both time points for participants at church than at Sunday Assembly, but there was no significant interaction. In other words, the increase from before to after the ritual was not significantly different between groups.

Fig 2.

Fig 2

Notched boxplot showing pre-ritual and post-ritual social bonding in Sunday Assembly (blue) and Church (red) participants. The notch (indent) around the median shows the 95% confidence interval. The whiskers are +/- 1.5*IQR from the upper and lower quartiles. The diagonal lines show the mean change for the two sets of participants.

Note that effect sizes cannot be directly calculated using the non-parametric within-between ANOVA. However, Feys [73] notes that for non-parametric within-between ANOVA tests involving only two time-points (pre-post), there are several alternative ways that effect sizes might be estimated. For completeness, we carried out four alternatives suggested by Feys [73], results of which can be found in the analysis script in the supplemental materials.

We then tested whether affect, as measured by the PANAS, changed from before to after the Sunday Assembly. The pre-meeting PANAS+ (W = .976, p = .445) was not significantly different from normally distributed, however the post-meeting PANAS+ measure (W = .939, p = .017) and both pre- (W = .861, p < .001) and post-meeting (W = .738, p < .001) PANAS- measures were significantly different from normal. Consequently, non-parametric Wilcoxon signed-ranks tests were conducted.

A two-tailed Wilcoxon signed-ranks test showed that there was a significant change in positive affect, with an increase in positive affect from before (M = 26.21, SD = 9.23, Mdn = 24.0) to after the Sunday Assembly ritual (M = 31.68, SD = 11.57, Mdn = 32.0, Z = 3.90, Bonferroni-corrected p < .001, rR = .40, rK = .60), as well as a significant decrease in negative affect (PANAS-) from before (M = 5.00, SD = 4.10, Mdn = 4.0) to after the ritual (M = 3.30, SD = 4.23, Mdn = 2.0, Z = 2.41, Bonferroni-corrected p = .047, rR = .25, rK = .44). These results suggest that there was a moderate effect of taking part in secular ritual on positive affect and a small effect on negative affect. Fig 3 shows these results and includes the matched church data for comparison.

Fig 3. Notched boxplots showing pre-ritual and post-ritual (a) PANAS+ and (b) PANAS- in Sunday Assembly (blue) and Church (red) participants.

Fig 3

The notch (indent) around the median shows the 95% confidence interval. The whiskers are +/- 1.5*IQR from the upper and lower quartiles. The diagonal lines show the mean change for the two sets of participants.

The role of affect on social bonding

We then examined whether this change in affect predicted the significant change in social bonding we observed in Sunday Assembly participants. First, we visualised the Sunday Assembly data using a correlation plot (See S1A Fig). To test our hypothesis that affect change predicted social bonding change, we conducted a multiple regression for Sunday Assembly participants with PANAS+ and PANAS- as predictors. Both social bonding and the PANAS subscales were standardised for the regression analysis. A multiple regression showed that there was a significant model (F(2,44) = 16.76, Bonferroni-corrected p < .001, R2 = .432, Radj2 = .407), with change in PANAS+ being a significant positive predictor of change in social bonding score, but not PANAS- (see Table 1).

Table 1. Multiple linear regression predicting change in social bonding.

Variable B 95% CI β t Sig. (p)
(Constant) - .00 [-.23, .23] 0.00 >.999
ΔPANAS+ .70 [.45, .95] .70 5.66 < .001
ΔPANAS- .14 [-.11, .39] .14 1.15 .257

Note: R2 = .432, 95% CI = [.19, .58]

Exploratory analyses

In their work on religious ritual, Charles, van Mulukom and colleagues [30] found that the feeling of connection to something bigger was also a significant predictor of social bonding. Further, Price and Launay [53] suggested that research should account for the length of time one had been attending Sunday Assembly to determine if this plays a role in some of the effects seen. As such, we conducted an exploratory correlation analysis and created a correlation plot to visualise the analysis. As shown in S1A Fig, both the length of how long one had been attending the Sunday Assembly and the feeling of connection to something bigger were positively correlated with social bonding change. Moreover, baseline social bonding score may play a role in the level of change in bonding, i.e. the lower the starting score, the larger the change. Consequently, we conducted stepwise multiple linear regressions to include the control variable of baseline social bonding score, the level of connection to something bigger and how long (in months) participants had been attending Sunday Assemblies. Control variables of age, education, self-rated spirituality, and self-rated religiosity were also added to the stepwise process to determine if they played a role.

The stepwise regression analysis was conducted using the stepAIC function in the MASS package in R. This conducts a stepwise regression using the Akaike Information Criterion (AIC; [74]). An AIC provides a numerical indicator of the ratio between the goodness of fit of the model and the simplicity of the model. The lower the AIC value, the lower the level of information loss, meaning it is a better, more parsimonious model (or more likely to be indicative of the true model). The absolute AIC values are not important, but the relative values are compared. The model with the lowest AIC value is the most parsimonious model that best fits the data. This can be run in one of three ways: (one) backwards (aka backwards elimination), starting from a full model with all predictor variables and iteratively removing the predictors that contribute the least to the model; (two) forwards, starting with a null model and adding predictor variables iteratively until the model no longer improves, or (three) both, where a combination of the two methods is used to determine the best model. The both-direction method starts with no predictors and add predictors that contribute the most value (like the forward method) then removes any variables that no longer provide an improvement to model fit (like the backward method). The default method in the stepAIC function is ‘both’, as this allows predictors to be added and removed at each step, and is best used if conducting exploratory analyses [75, 76].

A both-direction stepwise regression converged on a final model where four variables were included: baseline social bonding, PANAS+ change, connectedness to something bigger and age. The final model was significant (F(4,42) = 23.67, p < .001, R2 = .693, R2Adj = .663), and showed that change in social bonding was significantly predicted by baseline social bonding, PANAS+ change, and connectedness to something bigger. Including age improved the model, but it was not a significant predictor (see Table 2).

Table 2. Final model after both-direction stepwise multiple linear regression predicting change in social bonding in Sunday Assembly participants.

Variable B 95% CI β t Sig. (p)
(Constant) - .00 [-.17, .17] 0.00 >.999
Baseline social bonding - .48 [-.65, -.30] -.48 -5.47 < .001
ΔPANAS+ .50 [.30, .70] .50 5.09 < .001
Connected to Something Bigger .31 [.11, .51] .31 3.10 .003
Age .01 [-.00, .03] .16 1.88 .068

Note: R2 = .693, 95% CI = [.48, .77]

Church exploratory analyses

First data were plotted on a correlation plot to visualise the data (see S1B Fig). As with the Sunday Assembly data, a stepwise regression was conducted on the matching church participants to see if similar results were found in the matched participants. The full model included the same predictors and control variables the Sunday Assembly participants apart from spirituality, as self-rated spirituality was not measured in the survey for church participants. The both-direction stepwise regression converged on a final model where five variables were included, baseline social bonding, PANAS+ change, PANAS- change, connectedness to God, and length of attendance. The final model was significant (F(5,41) = 6.90, p < .001, R2 = .457, R2Adj = .391), and showed that change in social bonding was significantly predicted by baseline social bonding, PANAS+ change, and length of time one had been attending. Connectedness to God, and PANAS- change improved the model, but were not significant predictors (see Table 3).

Table 3. Final model after both-direction stepwise multiple linear regression predicting change in social bonding in church participants.

Variable B 95% CI β t Sig. (p)
(Constant) - .00 [-.23, .23] -0.00 >.999
Baseline social bonding - .42 [-.69, -.15] -.42 -3.13 .003
ΔPANAS+ .40 [.12, .68] .40 2.85 .007
ΔPANAS- - .17 [-.41, .07] - .17 - 1.43 .161
Connected to Something Bigger .27 [-.02, .56] .27 1.88 .067
Months Attended .27 [-.00, .03] .27 2.20 .033

Note: R2 = .457, 95% CI = [.16, .57]

Discussion

Religious rituals occur in all human societies [77], and they seem to confer various benefits to those who take part [1]. It has been suggested that rituals are evolutionarily adaptive by helping foster social bonds [16]. This hypothesis has received some support from field research on religious rituals [30] and from a large body of research showing social bonds provide health benefits [3941]. It has also been proposed that attending secular rituals, such as Sunday Assembly meetings, may lead to improved wellbeing (e.g. [53]). However, whether the social bonding effect reported from religious rituals is also seen in secular rituals that mimic the behaviours of religious rituals had not been tested before. This study of participants from Sunday Assemblies provides the first evidence that the fostering of social bonds occurs in a secular ritual setting. We compared this to a matched group of individuals from four Christian churches. The results showed that social bonding improvements are of a similar level at both Sunday Assembly and religious ritual settings.

Follow-up analyses found that the increase in social bonding from before to after the Sunday Assemblies was positively predicted by the change in positive affect, as has been found for churches across the UK [30], but not negative affect. These findings are in line with the ‘broaden and build’ hypothesis, which suggests that positive emotions increase the scope of one’s attention to others to allow for the formation of social connections, which themselves lead to improved mental wellbeing [19, 34, 35]. This hypothesis has also been used to suggest that link between religion and wellbeing stems from changes in positive affect [34, 35], which in turn leads to protective social benefits, such as social support [36].

Stepwise regression analysis found that neither level of spirituality nor level of religiosity played a significant role in social bonding change, despite both variables having been related to wellbeing in the past [78]. This could be the result of the methodology of previous studies, which have often used attendance of religious services as a measure for religiosity itself [1, 79], which could conflate the effect of ritual attendance with religiosity and/or spirituality. Diener and colleagues [36] have noted that the reported relationship between religiosity and wellbeing is conditional on social support and social structure. This may explain why religiosity did not directly predict social bonding change in either Sunday Assembly or church participants. Price and Launay [53] have specifically suggested that future research should account for the length of time one had been attending Sunday Assembly, to see if this could explain the wellbeing effects they reported. In the stepwise regression model, the length of attendance did not add predictive value for the change in social bonding in the Sunday Assembly participants. If, as Price and Launay [53] suggest, the improved wellbeing stems from social bonding, this may suggest that protective effects of participating in secular ritual could occur quickly. We must note, though, that we likely failed to detect this effect for Sunday Assembly participants because there were a number of people attending the ritual for the first time in the Sunday Assembly population, which was not the case with the Christian church participants, for whom we found that length of attendance predicted strength of social bonding. Future research should attempt to account for the effect of newcomers on social bonding during group rituals.

This work is the first to demonstrate that secular rituals, much like religious rituals, promote feelings of social bonding. However, we acknowledge that there are limitations to this study. Firstly, this study was not pre-registered. Given the changes suggested by those promoting Open Science methodologies since the advent of the replication crisis [8082], the methods and analysis plans could have been registered in advance of conducting the study. Though pre-registration was not done in this case, the full anonymised dataset and the research materials are provided in supplementary materials in accordance with other Open Science practices, and a power analysis was provided to support the sample size used in this study.

Another limitation is that we only conducted research with one type of secular ritual, the Sunday Assembly meetings. Sunday Assembly meetings are not the only secular ritual that mimic religious ritual, with other examples including the Church of Positivism [50]–still active in Brazil–and the Religious Humanism movement in the United States.

One avenue for future research is to conduct studies investigating whether the positive health effects found in those who regularly attend religious rituals can also be seen in those who regularly attend Sunday Assemblies or other similar non-religious rituals that mimic the behaviours of religious rituals, compared to those who do not attend such rituals. Examples of such positive health effects are better immune function and lowering levels of all-cause mortality [36], depression [79, 83, 84] and suicidality [1]. Here, we have examined the role of ritual on social bonding. However, to better understand the mechanisms underlying the protective factors that have previously been related only to religious participation, future research could compare health outcomes from those who attend secular rituals to those who do not, while taking affect and social bonding into account. We also recommend that, much like in our research, social bonding factors be explicitly measured in future ritual and health research, as this may provide a more comprehensive understanding of the mechanism by which ritual attendance appears to improve wellbeing.

Future research can also look more widely at gatherings of secular groups, which are not intentionally ‘rituals’ but nonetheless may create a sense of connection to something bigger than oneself. A variety of gatherings may function as a form of ‘implicit religion’ [8587], such as sporting events where one feels connected to a team spirit [88, 89], thus creating social bonds in ways similar to religious rituals. Conducting research in such settings would allow us to better understand the nature and effects of ritual-like social bonding in secular contexts.

Conclusion

Throughout the first two decades of the 21st century, levels of religiosity have been on the decline in many Western countries [4549]. This may be worrying to some, as there is a wealth of evidence from the religion and health field demonstrating that participation in religious activities provides a variety of health benefits [1]. However, some research suggests that secular rituals, such as the Sunday Assembly, may provide some boost wellbeing [53]. It is thus possible that the mechanism by which health benefits arise from religious participation are not exclusive to religious rituals. Dunbar [16] provides one possible explanatory mechanism underlying the positive health benefits that seems to stem from rituals: the formation of social bonds. Previous research has demonstrated that religious rituals lead to social bonding [30] and that social bonding leads to better health outcomes [3941]. In this study we have demonstrated that secular rituals, in the form of Sunday Assembly meetings, also lead to increases in feelings of social bonding, in part via an increase in positive affect. This shows that, for some individuals, secular rituals may serve as an alternative to religious ones.

Supporting information

S1 Fig

Two supplementary figures showing the correlation plots Sunday Assembly participants (A) and one for church participants (B).

(DOCX)

S1 File. Sunday Assembly questionnaire.

(DOCX)

S2 File. Sunday Assembly R script.

(R)

S1 Data. Social bonding factor analysis.

(CSV)

S2 Data. SA data only–WIDE.

(CSV)

S3 Data. Church and SA data–LONG.

(CSV)

S4 Data. Church and SA data–WIDE.

(CSV)

S5 Data. Church data only–WIDE.

(CSV)

Data Availability

All data and supplementary materials can be found on the related OSF project: 10.17605/OSF.IO/NPDZM.

Funding Statement

YES MF, RIMD and FW were recipients of a Templeton Religion Trust grant (Grant number 0153). The Trust's website can be found here: https://templetonreligiontrust.org/ The funders did not play a role in the design of the study nor in data collection, analysis, decision to publish or preparation of the manuscript.

References

  • 1.VanderWeele T.J., Religion and Health. Vol. 1 2017: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Koenig H., King D., and Carson V.B., Handbook of Religion and Health. 2012: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Chida Y., Steptoe A., and Powell L.H., Religiosity/Spirituality and Mortality. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 2009. 78(2): p. 81–90. 10.1159/000190791 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Gillum R.F., et al. , Frequency of Attendance at Religious Services and Mortality in a U.S. National Cohort. Annals of epidemiology, 2008. 18(2): p. 124–129. 10.1016/j.annepidem.2007.10.015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Hummer R.A., et al. , Religious Involvement and U.S. Adult Mortality. Demography, 1999. 36(2): p. 273 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Musick M.A., House J.S., and Williams D.R., Attendance at Religious Services and Mortality in a National Sample. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 2004. 45(2): p. 198–213. 10.1177/002214650404500206 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Barton Y.A., et al. , Religious attendance and social adjustment as protective against depression: A 10-year prospective study. Journal of affective disorders, 2013. 146(1): p. 53–57. 10.1016/j.jad.2012.08.037 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Balbuena L., Baetz M., and Bowen R., Religious Attendance, Spirituality, and Major Depression in Canada: A 14-Year Follow-up Study. The Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 2013. 58(4): p. 225–232. 10.1177/070674371305800408 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Li S., et al. , Religious Service Attendance and Lower Depression Among Women—a Prospective Cohort Study. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 2016. 50(6): p. 876–884. 10.1007/s12160-016-9813-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Rasic D., et al. , Longitudinal relationships of religious worship attendance and spirituality with major depression, anxiety disorders, and suicidal ideation and attempts: Findings from the Baltimore epidemiologic catchment area study. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 2011. 45(6): p. 848–854. 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2010.11.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Suh H., Hill T.D., and Koenig H.G., Religious Attendance and Biological Risk: A National Longitudinal Study of Older Adults. Journal of Religion and Health, 2019. 58(4): p. 1188–1202. 10.1007/s10943-018-0721-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Loewenthal K.M. and Dein S., Religious Ritual and Wellbeing, in Applied Jewish values in social sciences and psychology. 2016, Springer; p. 151–163. [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Chang W.-C., Religious attendance and subjective well-being in an Eastern-culture country: Empirical evidence from Taiwan. Marburg Journal of Religion, 2009. 14(1). [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Roemer M.K., Religion and subjective well-being in Japan. Review of Religious Research, 2010: p. 411–427. [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Sosis R. and Bradley J. Ruffle, Religious Ritual and Cooperation: Testing for a Relationship on Israeli Religious and Secular Kibbutzim. Current Anthropology, 2003. 44(5): p. 713–722. [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Dunbar R.I.M., The Origin of Religion as a Small-Scale Phenomenon, in Religion Intolerance, and Conflict, Clarke S., Powell R., and Savulescu J., Editors. 2013, Oxford University Press; p. 48–66. [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Fredrickson B.L. and Branigan C., Positive emotions broaden the scope of attention and thought‐action repertoires. Cognition & Emotion, 2005. 19(3): p. 313–332. 10.1080/02699930441000238 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Wolf W., Launay J., and Dunbar R.I.M., Joint attention, shared goals, and social bonding. British Journal of Psychology, 2016. 107(2): p. 322–337. 10.1111/bjop.12144 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Fredrickson B.L., Positive Emotions Broaden and Build, in Advances in Experimental Social Psychology. 2013, Elsevier; p. 1–53. [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Reddish P., Fischer R., and Bulbulia J., Let’s Dance Together: Synchrony, Shared Intentionality and Cooperation. PLoS ONE, 2013. 8(8): p. e71182 10.1371/journal.pone.0071182 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Bamford, J., B. Burger, and P. Toiviainen. Are we dancing to the same beat? Empathy and interpersonal synchronisation in the silent disco. in 9th International Conference of Students of Systematic Musicology (SysMus 16), Jyväskylä, Finland. 2016.
  • 22.Cross L., Turgeon M., and Atherton G., Moving with the in-crowd: Cooperation and interpersonal entrainment in in- vs. out- groups. Current Psychology, 2019. [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Freeman W.J. III, A neurobiological role of music in social bonding. 1998. [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Kreutz G., Does singing facilitate social bonding. Music Med, 2014. 6(2): p. 51–60. [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Pearce E., Launay J., and Dunbar R.I.M., The ice-breaker effect: singing mediates fast social bonding. Royal Society Open Science, 2015. 2(10): p. 150221 10.1098/rsos.150221 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Pearce E., et al. , Singing together or apart: The effect of competitive and cooperative singing on social bonding within and between sub-groups of a university Fraternity. Psychology of Music, 2016. 44(6): p. 1255–1273. 10.1177/0305735616636208 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Jaremka L.M., Sunami N., and Nadzan M.A., Eating moderates the link between body mass index and perceived social connection. Appetite, 2017. 112: p. 124–132. 10.1016/j.appet.2017.01.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Kirchner T.R., et al. , Effects of Alcohol on Group Formation Among Male Social Drinkers. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 2006. 67(5): p. 785–793. 10.15288/jsa.2006.67.785 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Sayette M.A., et al. , Alcohol and Group Formation: A Multimodal Investigation of the Effects of Alcohol on Emotion and Social Bonding. Psychological Science, 2012. 23(8): p. 869–878. 10.1177/0956797611435134 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Charles S.J., et al. , Religious Rituals Increase Social Bonding and Pain Threshold. 2020, PsyArXiv. [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Mauss I.B., et al. , Don't hide your happiness! Positive emotion dissociation, social connectedness, and psychological functioning. Journal of personality and social psychology, 2011. 100(4): p. 738 10.1037/a0022410 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Waugh C.E. and Fredrickson B.L., Nice to know you: Positive emotions, self–other overlap, and complex understanding in the formation of a new relationship. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 2006. 1(2): p. 93–106. 10.1080/17439760500510569 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Fredrickson B.L., The broaden–and–build theory of positive emotions. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 2004. 359(1449): p. 1367–1377. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Fredrickson B.L., How does religion benefit health and well-being? Are positive emotions active ingredients? Psychological Inquiry, 2002. 13(3): p. 209–213. [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Van Cappellen P., et al. , Religion and well-being: The mediating role of positive emotions. Journal of Happiness Studies, 2016. 17(2): p. 485–505. [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Diener E., Tay L., and Myers D.G., The religion paradox: If religion makes people happy, why are so many dropping out? Journal of personality and social psychology, 2011. 101(6): p. 1278 10.1037/a0024402 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Lakey B. and Orehek E., Relational regulation theory: A new approach to explain the link between perceived social support and mental health. Psychological Review, 2011. 118(3): p. 482–495. 10.1037/a0023477 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Zaki J. and Williams W.C., Interpersonal emotion regulation. Emotion, 2013. 13(5): p. 803–810. 10.1037/a0033839 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Holt-Lunstad J., Smith T.B., and Layton J.B., Social Relationships and Mortality Risk: A Meta-analytic Review. PLoS Medicine, 2010. 7(7): p. e1000316 10.1371/journal.pmed.1000316 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Holt-Lunstad J. and Smith T.B., Social Relationships and Mortality: Social Relationships and Mortality. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 2012. 6(1): p. 41–53. [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Charles S.J., Farias M., and Dunbar R.I.M., The aetiology of social deficits within mental health disorders: The role of the immune system and endogenous opioids. Brain, Behavior, & Immunity—Health, 2020. 1: p. 100003 10.1179/1743132814Y.0000000392 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Lutz P.E., Courtet P., and Calati R., The opioid system and the social brain: implications for depression and suicide. Journal of Neuroscience Research, 2020. 98(4): p. 588–600. 10.1002/jnr.24269 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Tung E.S., et al. , Attachment to God as mediator of the relationship between God Representations and mental health. Journal of Spirituality in Mental Health, 2018. 20(2): p. 95–113. [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Pirutinsky S., Rosmarin D.H., and Kirkpatrick L.A., Is Attachment to God a Unique Predictor of Mental Health? Test in a Jewish Sample. The International Journal for the Psychology of Religion, 2019. 29(3): p. 161–171. [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Pew Research Forum. The Global Religious Landscape. Pew Research Center's Religion & Public Life Project 2012 [cited 2020; Available from: https://www.pewforum.org/2012/12/18/global-religious-landscape-exec/.
  • 46.Bullivant S., Catholic Disaffiliation in Britain: A Quantitative Overview. Journal of Contemporary Religion, 2016. 31(2): p. 181–197. [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Clements B., Religious Change and Secularisation in Scotland: An Analysis of Affiliation and Attendance. Scottish Affairs, 2017. 26(2): p. 133–162. [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Twenge J.M., et al. , Declines in American Adults’ Religious Participation and Beliefs, 1972–2014. SAGE Open, 2016. 6(1): p. 215824401663813. [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Brenner P.S., Cross-National Trends in Religious Service Attendance: Table 1. Public Opinion Quarterly, 2016. 80(2): p. 563–583. 10.1093/poq/nfw016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Mill J.S., Auguste Comte and Positivism. 2015, Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Porto, A., Understanding Unbelief in Brazil—Ateus e Agnósticos no Brasil, PrumoPro, Editor. 2019: YouTube. p. https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=9pDKyVDgJvA.
  • 52.Pigott R., Doing church without God, in BBC News. 2013. [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Price M.E. and Launay J., Increased Wellbeing from Social Interaction in a Secular Congregation. Secularism and Nonreligion, 2018. 7(1). [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Holt C.L., et al. , Role of religious social support in longitudinal relationships between religiosity and health-related outcomes in African Americans. Journal of behavioral medicine, 2018. 41(1): p. 62–73. 10.1007/s10865-017-9877-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Hood R.W. Jr, Hill P.C., and Spilka B., The psychology of religion: An empirical approach. 2018: Guilford Publications. [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Galen L.W., Focusing on the nonreligious reveals secular mechanisms underlying well-being and prosociality. Psychology of Religion and Spirituality, 2018. 10(3): p. 296. [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Wiltermuth S.S. and Heath C., Synchrony and cooperation. Psychological science, 2009. 20(1): p. 1–5. 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02253.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Hove M.J. and Risen J.L., It's All in the Timing: Interpersonal Synchrony Increases Affiliation. Social Cognition, 2009. 27(6): p. 949–960. [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Valdesolo P. and DeSteno D., Synchrony and the social tuning of compassion. Emotion, 2011. 11(2): p. 262 10.1037/a0021302 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Aron A., Aron E.N., and Smollan D., Inclusion of Other in the Self Scale and the structure of interpersonal closeness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1992. 63(4): p. 596–612. [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Raykov T. and Marcoulides G.A., Thanks coefficient alpha, we still need you! Educational and Psychological Measurement, 2019. 79(1): p. 200–210. 10.1177/0013164417725127 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Savalei V. and Reise S.P., Don’t Forget the Model in Your Model-based Reliability Coefficients: A Reply to McNeish (2018). Collabra: Psychology, 2019. 5(1). [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Dunn T.J., Baguley T., and Brunsden V., From alpha to omega: A practical solution to the pervasive problem of internal consistency estimation. British journal of psychology, 2014. 105(3): p. 399–412. 10.1111/bjop.12046 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.McNeish D., Thanks coefficient alpha, we’ll take it from here. Psychological Methods, 2018. 23(3): p. 412 10.1037/met0000144 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Peters G., The Alpha and the Omega of Scale Reliability and Validity: why and how to Abandon Cronbach’s Alpha. European Health Psychologist, 2014. 16(S): p. 576. [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Watson D., Clark L.A., and Tellegen A., Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. Journal of personality and social psychology, 1988. 54(6): p. 1063 10.1037//0022-3514.54.6.1063 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Rosenthal R., Meta-Analytic Procedures for Social Science Research Sage Publications: Beverly Hills, 1984, 148 pp. Educational Researcher, 1986. 15(8): p. 18–20. [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Pallant J. and Manual S.S., A step by step guide to data analysis using SPSS for windows. SPSS Survival Manual. Open University Press, New York, 2007. [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Kerby D.S., The Simple Difference Formula: An Approach to Teaching Nonparametric Correlation. Comprehensive Psychology, 2014. 3: p. 11.IT.3.1. [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Faul F., et al. , G* Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior research methods, 2007. 39(2): p. 175–191. 10.3758/bf03193146 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Buchner A., et al. , G* Power 3.1 manual. Düsseldorf, Germany: Heinrich-Heine-Universitat Dusseldorf, 2017. [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Noguchi K., et al. , nparLD: an R software package for the nonparametric analysis of longitudinal data in factorial experiments. Journal of Statistical Software, 2012. 50(12). [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Feys J., Nonparametric tests for the interaction in two-way factorial designs using R. The R Journal, 2016. 8(1): p. 367–378. [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Akaike H., Maximum likelihood identification of Gaussian autoregressive moving average models. Biometrika, 1973. 60(2): p. 255–265. [Google Scholar]
  • 75.James G., et al. , An introduction to statistical learning. Vol. 112 2013: Springer. [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Bruce P. and Bruce A., Practical statistics for data scientists: 50 essential concepts. 2017: " O'Reilly Media, Inc.". [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Brown D.E., Human universals and their implications. Being humans: Anthropological universality and particularity in transdisciplinary perspectives, 2000: p. 156–174. [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Kim-Prieto C. and Miller L., Intersection of religion and subjective well-being. Handbook of well-being. Salt Lake City, UT: DEF Publishers, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Koenig H.G. and Büssing A., The Duke University Religion Index (DUREL): a five-item measure for use in epidemological studies. Religions, 2010. 1(1): p. 78–85. [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Open Science Collaboration, Estimating the reproducibility of psychological science. Science, 2015. 349(6251): p. aac4716 10.1126/science.aac4716 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.van't Veer A.E. and Giner-Sorolla R., Pre-registration in social psychology—A discussion and suggested template. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 2016. 67: p. 2–12. [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Charles S.J., et al. , Researcher Degrees of Freedom in the Psychology of Religion. The International Journal for the Psychology of Religion, 2019. 29(4): p. 230–245. [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Strawbridge W.J., et al. , Religious attendance increases survival by improving and maintaining good health behaviors, mental health, and social relationships. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 2001. 23(1): p. 68–74. 10.1207/s15324796abm2301_10 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Norton M.C., et al. , Church Attendance and New Episodes of Major Depression in a Community Study of Older Adults: The Cache County Study. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 2008. 63(3): p. P129–P137. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Aicinena S., Implicit religion and the use of prayer in sport. American Journal of Sociological Research, 2017. 7(1): p. 56–65. [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Lord K., Implicit Religion: Definition and Application. Implicit Religion, 2006. 9(2). [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Bailey E., Introduction. The Notion of Implicit Religion: What it Means, and Does Not Mean. The Secular Quest for Meaning in Life. Denton Papers in Implicit Religions, 2002: p. 1–11. [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Sullivan G.B., Collective emotions: A case study of South African pride, euphoria and unity in the context of the 2010 FIFA World Cup. Frontiers in psychology, 2018. 9: p. 1252 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01252 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Halldorsson V., National sport success and the emergent social atmosphere: The case of Iceland. International Review for the Sociology of Sport, 2020: p. 1012690220912415. [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

Amy Michelle DeBaets

6 Oct 2020

PONE-D-20-19769

United on Sunday: The effects of secular rituals on social bonding and affect

PLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Charles,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

==============================

Please make the minor changes suggested by the reviewers and respond to each comment with the changes made. We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

==============================

Please submit your revised manuscript by Nov 20 2020 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Amy Michelle DeBaets, PhD

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

2.We noted in your submission details that a portion of your manuscript may have been presented or published elsewhere.

[Yes,

Four churches were chosen from the Charles, van Mulukom et al (2020) paper, currently under review/]

Please clarify whether this [conference proceeding or publication] was peer-reviewed and formally published. If this work was previously peer-reviewed and published, in the cover letter please provide the reason that this work does not constitute dual publication and should be included in the current manuscript.

3.Please upload a copy of Figure 3, to which you refer in your text on page 11. If the figure is no longer to be included as part of the submission please remove all reference to it within the text.

4. Please include captions for your Supporting Information files at the end of your manuscript, and update any in-text citations to match accordingly. Please see our Supporting Information guidelines for more information: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/supporting-information.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: I Don't Know

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: The study described in this manuscript investigates an important topic that will be of interest to many behavioural science researchers, and seems to have been conducted carefully and competently. The lit review is appropriate and sufficiently thorough, and the statistical analysis and conclusion seem rigorous and reasonable. Therefore I think this manuscript is already in pretty good shape, and in my opinion nearly ready for publication, and so this will be a light-touch review.

Issues that should be addressed:

1. The first reference listed is numbered ‘2’, which is confusing; what happened to reference 1?

2. I am confused by this statement on p. 12: “Both were correlated with bonding change, seen in supplementary figure A.” Apparently this statement refers to an analysis that was done which showed that ‘connection to something bigger’ and ‘length of time’ correlated with bonding change, but it is unclear whose analysis this is. If it is using data from the current study, why is the figure presented before any analysis procedure is actually described? This point of confusion is made worse by the fact that Supplementary Figures A and B are both very low resolution and hard to read, at least in the copy of the manuscript that I received. Please clarify the meaning of this statement, and also include higher-quality image files for these supplementary figures.

3. There are some grammatical problems throughout the manuscript, such as typos and convoluted and confusing sentence structures. Please proofread and correct these as best you can. A few selected examples are below.

P. 2: “Consequently, the Broaden and Build hypothesis linking religious ritual to wellbeing via positive emotions’ role in broadening social bonding (32, 33) is unsurprising”; this sentence is worded in a confusing way, I’ve read it several times and am still not sure I understand it.

P. 3: Please check the meaning of the phrase “begs the question”, as it is used incorrectly here. I think you mean “raises the question”. Also please re-word the sentence that starts with this phrase, as it is confusing; for one thing, it refers to “the question”, but then states not one but three questions in quick succession.

P. 13: “…and showed that in social bonding was…”, please correct.

P. 17: Another confusingly-worded sentence: “However, comparing health outcomes from those who attend secular rituals to those who do not on health effects, while taking affect and social bonding into account may help further understand the mechanisms underlying the protective factors that have previously been related only to religious participation”. Please simplify and re-word so that the meaning is clearer. Also, a word like ‘illuminate’ would be more appropriate here than ‘understand’.

Reviewer #2: This is a very strong paper that presents innovative data on a topic that should be of academic and practical interest to all. Through carefully testing of their hypothesis linking ritual experience to positive affect and social bonding, the authors convincingly argue that secular groups can be an equally powerful setting for receiving the benefits to welfare of religious participation. The methodology is rigorous and clearly explained, and the results raise fascinating questions for future research.

As well as deepening our understanding of the link between ritual and well being, the paper raises a future path for research that drills down into exactly what it is about ritual that produces positive outcomes. To what extent does one have to participate? Are some ritual actions more powerful than others in eliciting social bonding? Will any ritual do?

I have some minor thoughts and queries for the authors.

The first is regarding the use of Christian religious services as a proxy for religious ritual in the control group. In the literature section, I recommend that the authors make clear what kinds of religion have been studied by scholars when theorising the positive benefits of church attendance, and what forms of religion this excludes.

The study rests on a comparison between the Sunday Assembly and Christian Church services, which is understandable given the historical/contextual analogy between the groups and the challenges of the congruence fallacy. However, I would caution against over-generalising the literature in suggesting that religious ritual is equally-powerful in generating social bonding. I wonder, for example, how positive affect and social bonding are generated in atheistic Buddhist communities, or liberal Quaker communities, which commonly hold services without the elements on communal singing and preaching etc. The authors hint at this limitation toward the end of the paper, but a word of caution against generalisation from Christianity to religion/ritual might help guide the reader toward the start of the paper.

Secondly, why were the attendees of the Christian churches asked specifically if they felt connected to God/Jesus/ Holy Spirit, and not asked if they felt connected to something bigger than oneself/ the universe/ a sense of awe, as the participants attending the Sunday Assembly were? By wording the question in this way, the survey appears to preclude those people attending Christian church services who have less doctrine-driven experiences of connection, but nevertheless attend church regularly. In some senses, the question might predetermine a distinction that for some attendees does not exist.

Both of these queries are to say that exactly what makes a ritual efficacious (leading to social bonding and potential health benefits), if it is not religiosity, appears increasingly unclear as a result of this paper. A useful further venture might be to take this study into the realm of implicit religion studies, and consider how the attendance of sporting matches, to give one example, might have similar affects.

Overall, this is an impressively researched, clearly articulated, and engaging peer that is sure to generate excitement from religious scholars around the world.

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: Yes: Hannah R H Gould

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2021 Jan 27;16(1):e0242546. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0242546.r002

Author response to Decision Letter 0


2 Nov 2020

To Amy Michelle DeBaets, PhD, Academic Editor of PLOS ONE

We are hereby resubmitting the article “United on Sunday: The effects of secular rituals on social bonding and affect”, which has been amended to address your comments, and those made by the two reviewers. We would first like to say that we thank you and the reviewers for their insightful comments and suggestions. We believe that the article is stronger for having taken on the suggestions and addressing the criticisms raised.

In your direct comments to us, you asked that we amend the formatting to conform to the PLOS ONE style guide. We have now done this by amending heading font sizes, adding a supplementary information section below the references and updated in-text citations accordingly. Further to your concern of dual publication: The four churches used in this article are a sub-sample from the Charles, van Mulukom et al. (2020) article, which is currently under peer review. They are used as a matched comparison for the data we collected in Sunday Assemblies, and has been approached with a completely different set of analyses and hypotheses. We believe that this way, this does not constitute dual publication. Finally, we would like to apologise for previously having mislabelled figures. We have now updated the figure numbers to be correct and have reuploaded all figures to meet appropriate resolution requirements.

Regarding the comments made by Reviewer 1: We have made the changes they have recommended, including clarifying the reasons for conducting correlation plots, reuploading the plots in the supplementary figures S1 file as 300dpi images instead of 70dpi images, and have since gone through the article to reword some other sections that may have previously contained typos, or sections that were more difficult to read. For a full list of how we have addressed the comments, please see the next page.

Regarding the comments made by Reviewer 2: Reviewer 2’s comments required more substantial additions to the article, such as an extra paragraph to the discussion regarding implicit religion. These additions have improved the article and have allowed us to ensure that we appropriately describe the scope of our research. Based on their comments, we now emphasise that much of the previous research has been conducted in Western, Christian settings so as to situate our research appropriately and to ensure that we do not claim that the scope of the work we have conducted goes beyond such settings.

Thanks again for the feedback on the article and the opportunity to improve the article based on the feedback.

On behalf of myself and my co-authors,

Yours Sincerely,

Sarah Charles, MBPsS

Direct Responses to Reviewer points

Reviewer #1:

The study described in this manuscript investigates an important topic that will be of interest to many behavioural science researchers, and seems to have been conducted carefully and competently. The lit review is appropriate and sufficiently thorough, and the statistical analysis and conclusion seem rigorous and reasonable. Therefore I think this manuscript is already in pretty good shape, and in my opinion nearly ready for publication, and so this will be a light-touch review.

Issues that should be addressed:

1. The first reference listed is numbered ‘2’, which is confusing; what happened to reference 1?

a. ACTION: We have amended an error being caused by EndNote, where the reference in figure 1 was being listed as reference 1. This has been corrected and the reference list order amended accordingly

2. I am confused by this statement on p. 12: “Both were correlated with bonding change, seen in supplementary figure A.” Apparently this statement refers to an analysis that was done which showed that ‘connection to something bigger’ and ‘length of time’ correlated with bonding change, but it is unclear whose analysis this is. If it is using data from the current study, why is the figure presented before any analysis procedure is actually described? This point of confusion is made worse by the fact that Supplementary Figures A and B are both very low resolution and hard to read, at least in the copy of the manuscript that I received. Please clarify the meaning of this statement, and also include higher-quality image files for these supplementary figures.

a. ACTION: We have clarified that we conducted the correlation analysis and provide a correlation plot of this analysis in the supplementary figure file (in the file named S1 Figures).

b. ACTION: Attempts have been made to upscale Supplementary Figures A and B. They should now be at 300dpi, instead of R’s default 70dpi. We have upscaled all figures in the article using the same method.

3. There are some grammatical problems throughout the manuscript, such as typos and convoluted and confusing sentence structures. Please proofread and correct these as best you can. A few selected examples are below.

a. P. 2: “Consequently, the Broaden and Build hypothesis linking religious ritual to wellbeing via positive emotions’ role in broadening social bonding (32, 33) is unsurprising”; this sentence is worded in a confusing way, I’ve read it several times and am still not sure I understand it.

i. ACTION: We have amended the wording of this sentence to help clarify what was meant. It now reads: “Consequently, the Broaden and Build hypothesis linking religious ritual to wellbeing via positive emotions’ role in broadening social bonding (34, 35).”

b. P. 3: Please check the meaning of the phrase “begs the question”, as it is used incorrectly here. I think you mean “raises the question”. Also please re-word the sentence that starts with this phrase, as it is confusing; for one thing, it refers to “the question”, but then states not one but three questions in quick succession.

i. ACTION: We have amended the wording of this sentence to: “This raises the following questions: which aspects of rituals are particularly apt at providing wellbeing effects, and what role a connection to God plays?; what of rituals that are not religious?” We believe this is better phrased and addresses the issue of misusing the term “begs the question”.

c. P. 13: “…and showed that in social bonding was…”, please correct.

i. ACTION: We have moved the tables, which stopped the sentence from running over the page and stopped a premature paragraph break. We also added the missing word “change”, so the sentence now reads: “…and showed that change in social bonding was significantly predicted by baseline social bonding, PANAS+ change, and connectedness to something bigger.”

d. P. 17: Another confusingly-worded sentence: “However, comparing health outcomes from those who attend secular rituals to those who do not on health effects, while taking affect and social bonding into account may help further understand the mechanisms underlying the protective factors that have previously been related only to religious participation”. Please simplify and re-word so that the meaning is clearer. Also, a word like ‘illuminate’ would be more appropriate here than ‘understand’.

i. ACTION: We have reworded this section, so the sentence now reads: “However, to better understand the mechanisms underlying the protective factors that have previously been related only to religious participation, future research could compare health outcomes from those who attend secular rituals to those who do not, while taking affect and social bonding into account.”

Reviewer #2:

This is a very strong paper that presents innovative data on a topic that should be of academic and practical interest to all. Through carefully testing of their hypothesis linking ritual experience to positive affect and social bonding, the authors convincingly argue that secular groups can be an equally powerful setting for receiving the benefits to welfare of religious participation. The methodology is rigorous and clearly explained, and the results raise fascinating questions for future research.

As well as deepening our understanding of the link between ritual and wellbeing, the paper raises a future path for research that drills down into exactly what it is about ritual that produces positive outcomes. To what extent does one have to participate? Are some ritual actions more powerful than others in eliciting social bonding? Will any ritual do?

I have some minor thoughts and queries for the authors.

1. The first is regarding the use of Christian religious services as a proxy for religious ritual in the control group. In the literature section, I recommend that the authors make clear what kinds of religion have been studied by scholars when theorising the positive benefits of church attendance, and what forms of religion this excludes.

a. ACTION: We fully agree, and thank the reviewer for pointing out this oversight. The majority of studies on health/wellbeing and religion have looked at Western religions. We have noted this in the introduction: “Much of this literature was conducted in western, democratic nations, and within Christian settings, though there are some notable exceptions to this (1-3).”

2. The study rests on a comparison between the Sunday Assembly and Christian Church services, which is understandable given the historical/contextual analogy between the groups and the challenges of the congruence fallacy. However, I would caution against over-generalising the literature in suggesting that religious ritual is equally-powerful in generating social bonding. I wonder, for example, how positive affect and social bonding are generated in atheistic Buddhist communities, or liberal Quaker communities, which commonly hold services without the elements on communal singing and preaching etc. The authors hint at this limitation toward the end of the paper, but a word of caution against generalisation from Christianity to religion/ritual might help guide the reader toward the start of the paper.

a. ACTION: We agree that there may be differences across rituals, even the groups mentioned above have some kind of group activities (whether more meditative or contemplative), which we suggest may create similar social bonding effects. We highlight in the discussion ideas for future research in this area.

3. Secondly, why were the attendees of the Christian churches asked specifically if they felt connected to God/Jesus/ Holy Spirit, and not asked if they felt connected to something bigger than oneself/ the universe/ a sense of awe, as the participants attending the Sunday Assembly were? By wording the question in this way, the survey appears to preclude those people attending Christian church services who have less doctrine-driven experiences of connection, but nevertheless attend church regularly. In some senses, the question might predetermine a distinction that for some attendees does not exist.

a. RESPONSE: We understand where the reviewer is coming from and we agree that many people sensing a connection to God will experience this with awe and as a connection to something bigger than themselves (e.g. the literature on religion and spirituality shows that for many religious people these two experiences overlap and are not separate). However, we argue that the goal of religious ritual is largely focused on a connection with a specific higher power (e.g. 4), and therefore we adapted the specific survey question phrasing to more exactly tap into the religious or secular context.

4. Both of these queries are to say that exactly what makes a ritual efficacious (leading to social bonding and potential health benefits), if it is not religiosity, appears increasingly unclear as a result of this paper. A useful further venture might be to take this study into the realm of implicit religion studies, and consider how the attendance of sporting matches, to give one example, might have similar affects.

a. ACTION: Thank you for this insight. We have added, in the discussion section, suggestions of further research that include implicit religion: “Future research can also look more widely at gatherings of secular groups, which are not intentionally ‘rituals’ but nonetheless may create a sense of connection to something bigger than oneself. A variety of gatherings may function as a form of ‘implicit religion’ (5-7), such as sporting events where one feels connected to a team spirit (8, 9), thus creating social bonds in ways similar to religious rituals. Conducting research in such settings would allow us to better understand the nature and effects of ritual-like social bonding in secular contexts.”

References

1. Loewenthal KM, Dein S. Religious Ritual and Wellbeing. Applied Jewish values in social sciences and psychology: Springer; 2016. p. 151-63.

2. Chang W-C. Religious attendance and subjective well-being in an Eastern-culture country: Empirical evidence from Taiwan. Marburg Journal of Religion. 2009;14(1).

3. Roemer MK. Religion and subjective well-being in Japan. Review of Religious Research. 2010:411-27.

4. Van Cappellen P. Rethinking self-transcendent positive emotions and religion: Insights from psychological and biblical research. Psychology of Religion and Spirituality. 2017;9(3):254.

5. Aicinena S. Implicit religion and the use of prayer in sport. American Journal of Sociological Research. 2017;7(1):56-65.

6. Lord K. Implicit Religion: Definition and Application. Implicit Religion. 2006;9(2).

7. Bailey E. Introduction. The Notion of Implicit Religion: What it Means, and Does Not Mean. The Secular Quest for Meaning in Life Denton Papers in Implicit Religions. 2002:1-11.

8. Sullivan GB. Collective emotions: A case study of South African pride, euphoria and unity in the context of the 2010 FIFA World Cup. Frontiers in psychology. 2018;9:1252.

9. Halldorsson V. National sport success and the emergent social atmosphere: The case of Iceland. International Review for the Sociology of Sport. 2020:1012690220912415.

Attachment

Submitted filename: United On Sunday - Response to Reviewers.docx

Decision Letter 1

Amy Michelle DeBaets

5 Nov 2020

United on Sunday: The effects of secular rituals on social bonding and affect

PONE-D-20-19769R1

Dear Dr. Charles,

We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication.

An invoice for payment will follow shortly after the formal acceptance. To ensure an efficient process, please log into Editorial Manager at http://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the 'Update My Information' link at the top of the page, and double check that your user information is up-to-date. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

Kind regards,

Amy Michelle DeBaets, PhD

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments (optional):

Reviewers' comments:

Acceptance letter

Amy Michelle DeBaets

17 Nov 2020

PONE-D-20-19769R1

United on Sunday: The effects of secular rituals on social bonding and affect

Dear Dr. Charles:

I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact onepress@plos.org.

If we can help with anything else, please email us at plosone@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access.

Kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Dr. Amy Michelle DeBaets

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    S1 Fig

    Two supplementary figures showing the correlation plots Sunday Assembly participants (A) and one for church participants (B).

    (DOCX)

    S1 File. Sunday Assembly questionnaire.

    (DOCX)

    S2 File. Sunday Assembly R script.

    (R)

    S1 Data. Social bonding factor analysis.

    (CSV)

    S2 Data. SA data only–WIDE.

    (CSV)

    S3 Data. Church and SA data–LONG.

    (CSV)

    S4 Data. Church and SA data–WIDE.

    (CSV)

    S5 Data. Church data only–WIDE.

    (CSV)

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: United On Sunday - Response to Reviewers.docx

    Data Availability Statement

    All data and supplementary materials can be found on the related OSF project: 10.17605/OSF.IO/NPDZM.


    Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES