Skip to main content
Brain Communications logoLink to Brain Communications
. 2020 Sep 29;2(2):fcaa158. doi: 10.1093/braincomms/fcaa158

Preoperative transcranial magnetic stimulation for picture naming is reliable in mapping segments of the arcuate fasciculus

Davide Giampiccolo 1,2,#,, Henrietta Howells 3,#, Ina Bährend 4, Heike Schneider 5, Giovanni Raffa 6, Tizian Rosenstock 7, Francesco Vergani 8, Peter Vajkoczy 9, Thomas Picht 10
PMCID: PMC7846168  PMID: 33543136

Abstract

In preoperative planning for neurosurgery, both anatomical (diffusion imaging tractography) and functional tools (MR-navigated transcranial magnetic stimulation) are increasingly used to identify and preserve eloquent language structures specific to individuals. Using these tools in healthy adults shows that speech production errors occur mainly in perisylvian cortical sites that correspond to subject-specific terminations of the major language pathway, the arcuate fasciculus. It is not clear whether this correspondence remains in oncological patients with altered tissue. We studied a heterogeneous cohort of 30 patients (fourteen male, mean age 44), undergoing a first or second surgery for a left hemisphere brain tumour in a language-eloquent region, to test whether speech production errors induced by preoperative transcranial magnetic stimulation had consistent anatomical correspondence to the arcuate fasciculus. We used navigated repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation during picture naming and recorded different perisylvian sites where transient interference to speech production occurred. Spherical deconvolution diffusion imaging tractography was performed to map the direct fronto-temporal and indirect (fronto-parietal and parieto-temporal) segments of the arcuate fasciculus in each patient. Speech production errors were reported in all patients when stimulating the frontal lobe, and in over 90% of patients in the parietal lobe. Errors were less frequent in the temporal lobe (54%). In all patients, at least one error site corresponded to a termination of the arcuate fasciculus, particularly in the frontal and parietal lobes, despite distorted anatomy due to a lesion and/or previous resection. Our results indicate that there is strong correspondence between terminations of the arcuate fasciculus and speech errors. This indicates that white matter anatomy may be a robust marker for identifying functionally eloquent cortex, particularly in the frontal and parietal lobe. This knowledge may improve targets for preoperative mapping of language in the neurosurgical setting.

Keywords: language, TMS, tumour, tractography, arcuate


Navigated transcranial magnetic stimulation for mapping language areas is increasingly used in presurgical planning for personalized brain tumour treatment. Giampiccolo et al. use tractography to show arcuate fasciculus terminations match speech-eloquent cortex, even in patients with distorted anatomy.

Graphical Abstract

Graphical Abstract.

Graphical Abstract

Introduction

Preservation of function is an essential goal in neuro-oncology. Permanent language deficits are a major concern when planning the surgical removal of tumours in eloquent areas, as deficits have a profound effect on patients’ quality of life: affecting ability to return to work, social ability and mood (Ferro and Madureira, 1997). A key challenge is the unpredictability of the location of essential language sites, as evaluated from anatomical landmarks. To overcome this, extensive presurgical examinations and intraoperative mapping using direct electrical stimulation (DES) in the awake neurosurgical setting are advocated to test eloquent areas (Bello et al., 2007). Operating on awake patients requires a complex setting, necessitating good patient compliance, a trained neuropsychologist and tailored anaesthesia protocols. Therefore, reliable preoperative techniques to identify language-eloquent regions would be of great value to the neurosurgical community.

Functional magnetic resonance imaging is commonly used in the preoperative phase, but its reliability has been questioned as the effect of damaged tissue on brain haemodynamics is unknown (Giussani et al., 2010). Transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) offers a causal but non-invasive means by which to test cortical regions involved in a given function, by applying a magnetic field to the scalp, electrically inducing a physiological response in brain tissue (Rossi et al., 2012). Repetitive TMS produces a focal transient virtual lesion that disrupts brain function to cause behavioural changes, that is somewhat comparable to DES (Lioumis et al., 2012). In healthy subjects, TMS using different picture naming paradigms has significantly extended the understanding of the neurobiological underpinnings of different components of speech production (Devlin and Watkins, 2007). Despite this, its reliability for preoperative planning has been heavily debated due to both behavioural and anatomical interindividual variability (Michelucci et al., 1994). One limitation has been that although this disruption is focal, it has traditionally been applied using cranial landmarks, thus it has been unable to evaluate differences in cortical architecture. Recent technological upgrades enable real-time tracking of the induced electrical field alongside the patient’s structural MRI, termed e-field navigated transcranial magnetic stimulation (nTMS) (Ruohonen and Karhu, 2010). This approach provides more anatomically specific stimulation targets for preoperative neurosurgical mapping of language-eloquent regions, and is a rapidly growing field within personalized medicine.

Using repetitive nTMS with an object picture naming paradigm disrupts speech production, in the form of speech arrest (anarthria), articulation errors (dysarthria) and phonological paraphasia (Fridriksson et al., 2018). Recent studies have demonstrated that nTMS may correspond to DES when mapping posterior inferior frontal regions (pars opercularis, pars triangularis, ventral precentral cortex) well known to be essential in articulatory rehearsal, phonological processing and motor aspects of speech production (Dronkers et al., 2007; Picht et al., 2013; Tarapore et al., 2013). Speech production errors are also commonly identified when stimulating the inferior parietal lobule, including the supramarginal gyrus and angular gyrus (Geschwind, 1965; Chang et al., 2015; Igelström et al., 2015; Sarubbo et al., 2020). While errors can be induced across other perisylvian regions, correspondence between DES and nTMS has not been so clear-cut (Picht et al., 2013; Tarapore et al., 2013). Using diffusion tensor imaging in healthy subjects, it has been demonstrated that nTMS mapping using a picture naming paradigm is a reliable technique for identifying cortical terminations of the arcuate fasciculus, the core circuit of the wider language network (Catani et al., 2005; Lin et al., 2017). The arcuate fasciculus is made up of direct fronto-temporal connections (the long segment), and has also indirect connections with the inferior parietal lobule via the anterior segment (fronto-parietal) and posterior segment (parieto-temporal) (Catani et al., 2005). As there is an indication for preoperative mapping and awake surgery when lesions are in proximity of the arcuate fasciculus (Sanai and Berger, 2008), it is relevant to determine whether the result reported in healthy subjects can be reproduced also in patients with heterogeneous brain lesions, particularly when a previous surgery has altered brain tissue in language-relevant regions (Duffau, 2014; Herbet et al., 2016).

Diffusion imaging tractography has enabled indirect non-invasive mapping of white matter tracts involved in language function, using models to estimate the diffusion of water molecules (Basser et al., 2000). Most commonly, nTMS stimulation sites are used as seed region-of-interests (ROIs) to track the arcuate fasciculus in tumour patients (Raffa et al., 2016). However this does not take into account the entire structure of the arcuate fasciculus, nor its interindividual variability, relevant particularly in situations of heavily distorted anatomy due to the presence of a lesion. Furthermore, most studies combine nTMS with tensor-based modelling, which cannot correctly model fibre crossing within a single voxel (Dell’Acqua and Tournier, 2018). High angular resolution diffusion imaging models such as spherical deconvolution can overcome this limitation, even using clinical sequences with relatively low b-values, improving modelling of fibre crossing that is present in over 80% of white matter in the human brain (Dell’Acqua et al., 2010; Farquharson et al., 2013). We used nTMS and whole brain spherical deconvolution tractography in a broad spectrum of patients with brain tumours in perisylvian regions, to evaluate whether there is reliable correspondence between error sites identified with nTMS mapping during picture naming, and cortical terminations of the arcuate fasciculus.

Materials and methods

Participants

The study design was a prospective case collection. Patients scheduled for tumour removal in the vicinity of eloquent language-related areas were screened for enrolment, recruited from the Charité Hospital, Berlin between March 2013 and December 2016. Right-handed patients aged over 18 years were included, for whom there was the presence of a left hemisphere brain tumour compressing/infiltrating perisylvian cortical and subcortical language-related areas. Exclusion criteria were (i) moderate to severe aphasia, assessed on Aachen Aphasia Test, (ii) dementia (DemTect over 2), (iii) no planned surgery (iv) no good quality diffusion imaging acquisition. All patients provided written informed consent. The study proposal is in accordance with ethical standards of the Declaration of Helsinki and was approved by the local Ethics Commission.

Image acquisition for nTMS and diffusion tractography

Datasets were acquired on a 3T Siemens scanner with an 8 channel head coil. T1-weighted 3D MPRAGE images were acquired for nTMS using the following parameters (echo train length: 1, TE: 2.67 ms, TR: 2.000, matrix size: 256 × 246, slice thickness: 1 mm). T2-weighted, FLAIR images were acquired to identify the non-contrast enhancing lesions (i.e. lower grade gliomas), using T2-weighted inversion recovery fast spin echo sequences (TR 6000 ms, TE 150 ms, TI 2000 ms). A diffusion weighted single-shot echo-planar sequence was used along 40 geometric directions with a b-value of 1000 s/mm2. One non-diffusion-weighted volume was collected. A matrix size of 128 × 128 was used, with a field of view of 240 mm × 240mm, and 2 mm isotropic voxels, with a TE of 83 ms and TR of 11 ms. Sixty contiguous 2 mm thick axial sections were acquired resulting in 2460 images.

Navigated transcranial magnetic stimulation

Experimental setup

All patients underwent repetitive nTMS for localization of language-eloquent cortex. Mapping of both hemispheres was performed using the Nexstim NBS 4.3 with a NexSpeech module (Nexstim Oy, Helsinki, Finland) with particular focus around the peritumoural area and the regions surrounding the sylvian fissure (perisylvian) in the left hemisphere. The crus of the helix bilaterally, the nation and nine more scalp regions were used as cranial landmarks so that the system could co-register the patient’s head with the previously performed MRI scan. This enabled MR-guided targeting of specific regions with the coil, using the native anatomy of the patient. The maximum stereotactic error allowed by the co-registration software was 2 mm. Detailed information about the stimulation techniques and parameters have been described in previous studies (Krieg et al., 2017). Briefly, the resting motor threshold (RMT) for the first dorsal interosseous was determined to identify the lowest cortical excitability threshold, which was applied at all stimulation points.

Stimulation was performed using a cooled figure-of-eight coil with an outer diameter of 16.5 cm and inner diameter of 9 cm. The coil was randomly moved in about 10 mm steps over the peritumoural and perisylvian cortex. The coil was oriented to be aligned perpendicular to the nearest sulcus, using real-time optical tracking to navigate it relative to each individual patient’s sulcal morphology, posterior to the stimulated point to achieve maximum field induction.

Speech mapping procedure

An object picture naming paradigm was used during nTMS mapping, approved for clinical use according to Food and Drug Administration regulations, which is composed of black-and-white standardized line drawings of familiar objects, presented on a screen located in front of the patient. Patients underwent a baseline naming task three times, shown up to 150 objects (depending on clinical time constraints). During this, any objects which were unfamiliar, unrecognized or misnamed were eliminated to reduce false positive results as far as possible. The final number of objects presented to patients during the test session, and the number of baseline errors is reported in Table 1. Pictures were presented to the patient for 1 s, with individualized inter-picture interval of 2.5–4 s, depending on their performance in the baseline task. The words in English and German, along with their lexical characteristics are presented in Supplementary Table 1. According to the most recent literature, the repetitive nTMS stimulation was triggered alongside picture presentation, using an onset delay of 0 ms (Krieg et al., 2017). The stimulation protocol comprised a train-of-five pulses with a 5 Hz frequency. The starting intensity was always 100% of RMT, however in certain patients, the intensity was increased in steps of 5% up to 130% of RMT if no language errors could be obtained in perisylvian areas, or the intensity could be decreased in steps of 5 if pain was reported, to avoid discomfort interfering with responses (Table 1). In case of negative mapping (no errors), stimulation intensity was increased and/or frequency increased to 7 pulses at 7 Hz, according to the protocol established by an expert panel for clinical nTMS mapping (Table 1; Krieg et al., 2017). Patients were asked to express discomfort during the mapping through the Visual Analogue Scale from zero (no pain) to 10 (maximum pain). Stimulation intensity was reduced if patients complained about pain during the procedure. During the mapping procedure, 80–120 sites of the frontal, temporal and parietal cortex were stimulated at least three times each on non-consecutive trials (Fig. 1A). An error site was recorded when at least one of the three stimulations produced a speech error. Even if there were more than one errors at a single site this was counted only once. Stimulation was performed over both healthy tissue and the tumour. The software automatically registered the location of each stimulation spot. The coil was not kept at the same site for consecutive stimulation trains, to avoid summation effects.

Table 1.

Demographic information on patient group and individual parameters for transcranial magnetic stimulation language mapping

Patient no. Age Sex Handedness Histology Tumour location Tumour volume (cm3) Cavity volume (cm3) RMT (% of maximum stimulator output) Proportion of RMT used for task (%) Stimulation intensity Total number of pictures Baseline errors Total no. stimulations Total no. naming errors Stimulation frequency
First surgery 1 38 M R Glioma (WHO III) Fr 5.5 36 90 32 78 2/80 221 3 5 Hz/5 Stim
2 49 F L Glioma (WHO III) Fr 9.12 36 90 32 73 7/80 137 4 5 Hz/5 Stim
3 55 F R Metastasis (breast carcinoma) Fr 8.2 32 100 32 60 20/80 170 8 5 Hz/5 Stim
4 29 M R Oligodendroglioma (WHO II) Fr 40 28 100 28 66 14/80 214 17 5 Hz/5 Stim
5 39 F R Glioma (WHO III) TPO 18.63 34 100 34 65 15/80 216 15 5 Hz/5 Stim
6 67 M R Glioma (WHO III) AT 4.7 26 100 26 133 17/150 149 11 5 Hz/5 Stim
7 40 M R Ependymoma (WHO II) TPO 13.4 26 100 26 75 5/80 198 4 7 Hz/7 Stim
8 28 F R Glioma (WHO III) Fr 24.6 39 100 39 90 10/80 192 11 5 Hz/5 Stim
9 51 F R Glioma (WHO III) AT 45.53 35 90 31 128 22/150 189 4 5 Hz/5 Stim
10 36 F R Astrocytoma (WHO II) AT 10.2 49 70 34 77 3/80 231 5 5 Hz/5 Stim
11 66 M R Metastasis (renal cell carcinoma) AT 2.6 30 100 30 64 16/80 222 6 5 Hz/5 Stim
12 69 F R Glioma (WHO IV) AT 20.6 34 80 27 44 36/80 190 15 5 Hz/5 Stim
13 60 M R Glioma (WHO IV) Fr 8 36 100 36 46 50/96 169 6 5 Hz/5 Stim
14 33 F R Glioma (WHO III) Fr 28.1 29 100 29 72 8/80 228 3 5 Hz/5 Stim
15 54 M R Glioma (WHO IV) AT 3.3 43 120 52 128 22/150 207 12 7 Hz/7 Stim
16 41 F R Glioma (WHO III) Fr 0.65 37 90 33 101 17/118 167 3 5 Hz/5 Stim
Second surgery 17 51 F R Glioma (WHO IV) Fr 8.9 8.5 32 100 32 68 12/80 174 4 5 Hz/5 Stim
18 21 M R Glioma (WHO IV) Fr 69.5 3.1 28 115 32 74 6/80 186 9 5 Hz/5 Stim
19 32 M R Glioma (WHO III) AT 59.8 0.9 30 130 39 76 4/80 194 8 5 Hz/5 Stim
20 40 F R Oligoastrocytoma (WHO II) AT 17.3 1.7 50 80 40 97 53/150 169 8 7 Hz/7 Stim
21 34 M L Oligoastrocytoma (WHO III) TPO 4.7 2.2 38 70 27 74 6/80 175 9 5 Hz/5 Stim
22 35 M R Ganglioglioma (WHO I) TPO 1.7 0.5 34 100 34 80 0/80 150 4 5 Hz/5 Stim
23 50 F R Glioma (WHO III) TPO 3.4 5.1 32 100 32 64 16/80 206 3 5 Hz/5 Stim
24 37 M R Glioma (WHO III) TPO 36.2 5.3 34 90 31 73 7/80 162 12 5 Hz/5 Stim
25 77 M R Glioma (WHO IV) AT 8 5.4 27 110 30 49 31/80 153 15 5 Hz/5 Stim
26 31 F R Glioma (WHO III) AT 6.23 4.7 30 100 30 113 37/150 146 6 5 Hz/5 Stim
27 37 F R Oligoastrocytoma (WHO III) TPO 6.4 3.3 24 100 24 58 22/80 177 10 5 Hz/5 Stim
28 35 M R Glioma (WHO III) AT 9.06 0.5 29 110 32 120 30/150 194 15 5 Hz/5 Stim

Sex: M, male; F, female; Handedness: R, right-handed; L, left-handed; Location of tumour: Fr, frontal; TPO, temporo-parieto-occipital; AT, anterior temporal, calculated as percentage of maximum stimulator output.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Approach for combining TMS and tractography. (A) The extent of testing in each left hemisphere region as a proportion of total patients, shown in relevant cortical areas [parcellation based on Corina et al. (2010)]. nTMS error sites are recorded in the Nexstim system (B) and exported. Whole brain spherical deconvolution of the arcuate fasciculus is performed using a two-ROI approach (C) and the error sites and tractography are compared in the native space of each patient using (D) Trackvis software.

Data analysis

The whole procedure was analysed offline by a specialist neurolinguist. Errors included in the analysis were anarthria (a complete lack of response), dysarthria (form-based distortions that are slurred, stuttered or imprecisely articulated) and phonemic paraphasia (unintended phonemic modification of the target word, e.g. /pænts/is replaced with/fants/). We classed all of the above error types as speech articulation errors.

Diffusion imaging processing and tractography dissection

Data were corrected for head motion, eddy current distortion and susceptibility artefacts using ExploreDTI. While tensor-based approaches are commonly recommended for sequences with low b-values (Jones et al., 2013), a number of recent studies have indicated that spherical deconvolution techniques may also be used and provide fewer false negatives (Farquharson et al., 2013; Calamuneri et al., 2018). Spherical deconvolution tractography was hence calculated using StarTrack software (www.natbrainlab.co.uk), using a damped Richardson–Lucy algorithm (Dell’Acqua et al., 2010). A fibre response parameter of alpha = 1.5, 200 algorithm iterations, and n = 0.15 and v = 15 as threshold and geometrical regularization parameters. An absolute and relative threshold was applied to exclude local spurious maxima. Whole brain deterministic tractography was used, using Euler interpolation to reconstruct streamlines (Dell’Acqua et al., 2010). Spherical deconvolution tractography estimates multiple fibre directions within a voxel, constituting an advantage over diffusion tensor methods which cannot resolve multiple orientations within a voxel (Dell’Acqua and Tournier, 2018). When entering a region with crossing white matter bundles, the algorithm followed the orientation vector of least curvature. Streamlines were halted when a voxel without fibre orientation was reached or when the curvature between two steps exceeded a threshold of 45 degrees.

Tractography dissections were performed by the first authors (D.G. and H.H.), using a region-of-interest (ROI) based approach described in a previous study (Forkel et al., 2014). A three-ROI approach was used on anisotropic power maps defined in the patient’s native space (Fig. 1C). The frontal ROI was defined on sagittal slices, anterior to the central sulcus to incorporate the posterior inferior frontal white matter (pars opercularis and pars triangularis) and ventral precentral gyrus. The parietal ROI was defined on sagittal slices to incorporate the white matter of the supramarginal gyrus and angular gyrus, and the temporal ROI was defined on sagittal and axial slices to incorporate the posterior superior and middle temporal gyrus. The direct long segment was defined as all streamlines running between the frontal and temporal ROI, while the indirect segments included those running between frontal and parietal ROIs (anterior segment) or parietal and temporal ROIs (posterior segment). This is in line with previous studies (Catani et al., 2005). In certain cases where the pathology had altered anatomical landmarks used to define brain regions, the ROIs had to modified to reflect this.

Comparison of TMS and tractography

Error sites were saved onto the patient’s T1 image used for navigation by the Nexstim software (Fig. 1B). Each site was anatomically localized on a standard parcellation template (Corina et al., 2010) and checked by an expert anatomist (H.H.). The sites were registered to the anisotropic power maps (Chen et al., 2019) and imported into the tractography software as 6 mm diameter spherical seed ROIs to cover size of the anatomical locus of stimulation (Romero et al., 2019) for each patient using non-linear registration tools from the FMRIB Software Library (FSL, Jenkinson et al., 2012). Stimulation sites where then adjusted at the grey-white matter boundary.

Each spherical ROI was overlaid and compared with the pre-traced segments of the arcuate fasciculus (Fig. 1D). Errors were attributed to four tract categories—anterior segment, long segment, posterior segment, non-arcuate, only if there was at least one overlapping voxel between the TMS ROI and the streamlines belonging to that tract. If error sites were in locations corresponding to streamlines belonging to more than one segment of the arcuate, they were classified in both (e.g. a site in inferior frontal regions could be recorded as corresponding to terminations of both the anterior and long segment). A number of error sites were identified within the tumour, however as tractography is commonly unreliable in tracking through lesioned tissue, we did not include these sites in the analysis comparing error sites with tractography reconstructions of the arcuate fasciculus.

Statistical analysis

All statistical analysis was performed using SPSS (version 26). Linear regressions were performed to identify whether demographics (age, sex or handedness), clinical characteristics (tumour volume, tumour location, resection volume or grade), TMS variables (RMT, number of total stimulations or proportion of baseline errors) or tract measurements were linked to the number of speech errors made. Wilcoxon signed-rank tests were conducted to assess which branches of the arcuate fasciculus were more linked to error sites than others, and how this differed between lobes.

Data availability statement

The spreadsheets that support the findings of this study are available on the Open Science Framework (https://osf.io/xyekp/). The clinical data is available on reasonable request to the first or last author (D.G. or T.P.).

Results

Participants

Thirty patients were considered for the study, of which two patients were excluded as two or fewer speech errors could be induced (14 male, mean age 44 SD 14; Table 1). The majority of patients were undergoing surgery for a glioma with a grade of III or IV. Sixteen patients underwent nTMS mapping for a first surgery, and 12 patients were returning for a second surgery. In the entire sample (Fig. 2C), 10 patients had a frontal lobe lesion, 7 patients had a temporo-parieto-occipital lesion, 11 had a temporal lesion. Mean tumour volume was 16.9 cm3 (SD 17.8). The mean previous resection cavity size in the 12 returning patients was 3.4 cm3 (SD 2.4; Fig. 2D). No patients reported discomfort during the nTMS procedure and no adverse events occurred (e.g. seizures).

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Correspondence between TMS and tractography. (A) The proportion of patients in which speech errors were induced by TMS in different cortical perisylvian sectors. (B) The proportion of patients with an error site that corresponded to a cortical termination of the entire arcuate fasciculus (shown on a 3D standard space template in blue), within each lobe. (C) Percentage overlay map of the tumour locations in left hemisphere perisylvian regions. (D) Percentage overlay map of the previous resections in left hemisphere perisylvian regions.

Cortical localization of nTMS speech errors

The perisylvian cortex was mapped using nTMS in the left hemisphere of all 28 patients (mean number of stimulations 185, SD 26.5; Fig. 1A). The RMT and the proportion of baseline errors for each patient are reported in Table 1. We show all the error sites identified in each patient and their anatomical location in Supplementary Figs 1 and 2. We first performed linear regressions to identify interactions between demographic, clinical or TMS variables with the number of speech errors induced. This showed no significant interactions, including no influence of previous surgery on the result. The number of baseline errors approached significance [F(1,26)=4.1, P = 0.052].

We first examined the prevalence of speech errors in different cortical areas across the entire group (Fig. 2A). The mean number of errors was 8 per patient (SD 4.5). At least one error was recorded in all patients when stimulating the frontal region (100%). Errors were identified in 92% of patients when stimulating the parietal region, and 53% of patients in the posterior temporal region.

We next compared the prevalence of errors within each lobe. Within the frontal lobe, stimulation of the pars opercularis produced errors most frequently (82% of patients). Errors were also identified in around half of patients in the middle frontal gyrus (56%) and ventral precentral gyrus (48%). Errors were also identified to a lesser extent in the pars triangularis (37%) and a more dorsal region of the precentral gyrus closer to the hand representation (11%). In the parietal lobe, stimulation of the supramarginal gyrus resulted in at least one picture naming error in 86% of patients, and occurred less commonly when stimulating the angular gyrus (14%). Stimulation of the postcentral gyrus also produced errors, in 32% of patients when stimulating ventral postcentral regions and 21% of patients in a more dorsal region closer to the hand representation. In the temporal lobe, at least one error was identified in the superior temporal gyrus in 43% of patients, and the middle temporal gyrus in 29% of patients.

Comparison of nTMS speech errors and cortical projections of the arcuate fasciculus

We examined if error sites were significantly linked with a projection of the arcuate fasciculus within each lobe, excluding those sites identified on tumorous tissue (Fig. 2B). Wilcoxon signed-rank tests showed that in the parietal lobe, there were significantly more error sites associated with a cortical projection of the arcuate than not (Z = −4.3, P < 0.001), and this approached significance also in the frontal lobe (Z = −1.9, P = 0.05). This was not significant in the temporal lobe (Z = −4.6, P = 0.6).

We examined whether the individual stimulation sites corresponded to a termination of the patient’s arcuate fasciculus dissected with tractography (the anterior, posterior and long segments). We report tract measurements for all three segments in Table 2 (mean arcuate volume 18 ml, SD 7.7). The anterior segment (mean 5.9 ml SD 3.1) could be reconstructed in all but three patients, and the long segment (7.2 ml SD 3.3) was missing in one patient. The posterior segment (5.9 ml SD 3.5) was also missing in one patient. We first performed regressions to evaluate whether any clinical or demographic variables were associated with tract volume for any of the three arcuate segments, or the entire fascicle. This showed no effect of any of these variables on tract volume, including no influence of the previous surgery.

Table 2.

Tract measurements for the arcuate fasciculus in the left hemisphere

Patient number Group Anterior segment volume (ml) Long segment volume (ml) Posterior segment volume (ml) Entire arcuate volume (ml)
1 No previous surgery 8.18 12.66 9.84 30.7
2 No previous surgery 2.67 8.06 3.89 14.6
3 No previous surgery 2.72 4.37 6.15 13.2
4 No previous surgery 2.37 3.07 5.72 11.2
5 No previous surgery 4.24 7.14 3.28 14.7
6 No previous surgery 9.29 5.47 1.24 16.0
7 No previous surgery 8.14 7.58 2.78 18.5
8 No previous surgery 5.18 5.2
9 No previous surgery 8.73 10.81 8.51 28.1
10 No previous surgery 4.05 8.59 2.92 15.6
11 No previous surgery 4.60 7.37 5.16 17.1
12 No previous surgery 10.09 7.93 9.10 27.1
13 No previous surgery 2.90 13.74 7.70 24.3
14 No previous surgery 2.69 9.15 11.8
15 No previous surgery 3.92 0.56 4.5
16 No previous surgery 0.97 8.65 12.98 22.6
17 Previous surgery 3.38 3.95 7.06 14.4
18 Previous surgery 3.24 10.10 7.18 20.5
19 Previous surgery 3.52 5.71 7.00 16.2
20 Previous surgery 6.74 8.10 2.82 17.7
21 Previous surgery 7.88 2.24 10.1
22 Previous surgery 12.80 12.41 14.47 39.7
23 Previous surgery 9.06 8.41 8.65 26.1
24 Previous surgery 2.81 6.12 6.03 15.0
25 Previous surgery 7.71 8.09 1.20 17.0
26 Previous surgery 7.24 2.45 6.15 15.8
27 Previous surgery 8.43 11.17 3.26 22.9
28 Previous surgery 4.99 2.80 2.16 10.0

Our results clearly showed that at least one error site corresponded to a cortical termination of the arcuate fasciculus in every patient, even in cases where the arcuate had been considerably displaced or infiltrated (Fig. 3; Supplementary Figs 1 and 2). A Wilcoxon signed-rank test showed there were significantly more error sites associated with arcuate projections than those not associated with the arcuate (Z = −3.6, P = 0.001). In particular, error sites corresponded to cortical projections of the anterior segment in 90% of patients, with the long segment in 82% of patients and the posterior segment in 53% of patients (Fig. 3D). Wilcoxon signed-rank tests showed significantly more error sites associated with the anterior segment than the long segment (Z = −3.7, P < 0.001) and posterior segment (Z = −4.2, P < 0.001), and significantly more sites associated with the long segment than the posterior segment (Z = −2.7, P = 0.006).

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Four patient examples of correspondence between TMS and tractography. The distribution of errors sites (blue spheres), the lesion (light blue) or previous surgery (purple) and the individual arcuate fasciculus (red, yellow, green). (A) In Patient 17 the lesion is located distantly from the arcuate fasciculus and the TMS-induced speech error sites. (B) In patient 18 the lesion is bordering the arcuate fasciculus in the frontal lobe. (C) In patient 19, the arcuate fasciculus is displaced by the lesion. (D) Bar graph showing the proportion of patients with a site that corresponds to the entire arcuate fasciculus or one segment. (E) In patient 5, the lesion has infiltrated the arcuate fasciculus in the parietal lobe. Tracts: A, anterior segment; l, long segment; p, posterior segment of the arcuate fasciculus. A dash refers to a site located on the tumour that was not associated with the arcuate fasciculus. Cortical sites: mfg, middle frontal gyrus; poc, postcentral; stg, superior temporal gyrus; smg, supramarginal gyrus.

Discussion

In this study, we evaluated whether nTMS is a reliable tool for mapping the functional architecture of the arcuate fasciculus in a heterogenous cohort of patients with lesions in perisylvian regions. We examined whether, as previously described in healthy subjects (Lin et al., 2017), there was good correspondence between nTMS-induced speech errors and the arcuate fasciculus in patients with altered white matter anatomy due to a lesion, and/or a previous surgery. We showed that nTMS speech errors, including anarthria, dysarthria and phonological paraphasias, occur most commonly when stimulating frontal and parietal regions and corresponded in all cases with at least one anatomical termination of the arcuate fasciculus. This result was evident in both patients undergoing a first surgery in a perisylvian area, and those awaiting a second surgery. Our preliminary results indicate that the underlying white matter anatomy may be a good indicator of the migration of functionally eloquent cortex, which is of great relevance to presurgical planning. As this was consistent in both first and second surgery groups, even with considerable infiltration/displacement of brain tissue, we suggest that integrating nTMS mapping using picture naming and diffusion tractography into the presurgical plan may be a useful tool to evaluate functional architecture in patients with perisylvian brain lesions.

nTMS speech mapping is effective in identifying eloquent frontal and parietal regions

We show that speech error sites induced by nTMS are reliable for mapping the arcuate fasciculus in its entirety, and particularly when targeting the pars opercularis in the frontal lobe and the supramarginal gyrus in the parietal lobe. A previous study (Lin et al., 2017) using a similar approach in healthy controls (nTMS and tractography, a picture naming paradigm and n = 28), also showed that the inferior frontal gyrus could reliably elicit picture naming errors with TMS, although they did not find similar results in the parietal lobe. However, when comparing TMS results with the underlying white matter anatomy (Lin et al., 2017) also showed very good correspondence between error sites in both the frontal and parietal lobe, and cortical terminations of the arcuate fasciculus. While caution needs to be taken in comparing our results with this study as the nTMS and tractography methodologies are not exactly analogous, these results may be clinically useful.

Diffusion tractography of the arcuate may be a robust marker of the location of essential functional cortex, hence integrating a reliable tractography reconstruction into the MR-navigation system prior to language mapping may improve mapping specificity. Stimulation studies using DES as well as nTMS have shown that language function can reorganize in patients awaiting a second operation, with function potentially moving from the operated area (Krieg et al., 2014; Southwell et al., 2016). In our study, all patients showed errors involving the arcuate fasciculus, irrespective of any clinical features. This result was robust, even when the tumour had quite dramatically altered the anatomy. Our results indicate that even when a previous surgery in a perisylvian area has been performed, the arcuate fasciculus appears to be a reliable signature of the migration of functionally eloquent cortex.

Our results indicate that nTMS mapping particularly identified cortical terminations of different segments of the arcuate fasciculus. In fact, while the arcuate fasciculus was traditionally described solely in its fronto-temporal component (Geschwind, 1965), more recently it has been shown to have parietal ‘indirect’ projections which have been incorporated into modern models of language organization (Catani and Bambini, 2014; Tremblay and Dick, 2016; Poeppel and Assaneo, 2020). Interindividual differences in segments of the arcuate fasciculus have been linked to differences in language ability in a number of studies (Forkel et al., 2020a, b). Terminations of the fronto-parietal anterior segment (also termed the superior longitudinal fascicle III) were identified with nTMS almost 90% of patients. This tract plays a direct role in motor programming for articulation, as well as in short-term memory for phoneme articulation, which is supported by intraoperative studies (Papagno et al., 2017). DES of the anterior segment reliably induces dysarthria and speech arrest both at its cortical terminations in the frontal and parietal lobe (Duffau et al., 2003) as well as after subcortical stimulation (van Geemen et al., 2014). A recent study of 256 patients undergoing awake surgery described how cortical and subcortical stimulation of the anterior segment using DES causes interference particularly with speech articulation and speech output over other facets of language, for example reading and non-verbal comprehension (Sarubbo et al., 2020). Lesion studies also show that disconnection of the anterior segment over other arcuate segments is linked to speech production difficulties following stroke (Fridriksson et al., 2013) and damage to this tract is associated with apraxia of speech (Fridriksson et al., 2018). Previous studies have shown that DES applied over the pars opercularis prevents speech production without affecting oro-facial and tongue muscle activity, whereas stimulation over ventral precentral gyrus does not: here there is interference with basic phono-articulatory muscle activity (Cerri et al., 2015; Ferpozzi et al., 2018). Future studies integrating electromyography may be able to dissociate these different aspects also when using TMS. Given this tract is also relevant in sensorimotor integration for goal-directed actions of the upper limb, its identification and preservation in preoperative planning may have wider functional impact than just for speech articulation (Howells et al., 2018).

Speech error sites also corresponded to cortical terminations of the direct fronto-temporal segment of the arcuate fasciculus (the long segment) in around 80% of patients. This tract is the major component of the language circuit, dedicated to mapping verbal input onto articulatory-based representations, including phonological elaboration and encoding (Sarubbo et al., 2015; Tremblay and Dick, 2016) and processing of syntactically complex sentences (Vidorreta et al., 2011; Poeppel and Assaneo, 2020). Intraoperative stimulation of the long segment accurately and reliably induces phonemic paraphasias (Bello et al., 2007; Benzagmout et al., 2007). Furthermore, it mediates fast interaction between auditory and motor areas, thus facilitating phoneme categorization to create motor codes of the new phonological sequences, i.e. word learning (Lopez-Barroso et al., 2013). Our results indicated that the TMS protocol used was reliable in identifying its cortical terminations when in the frontal lobe, as far fewer speech errors could be elicited in the temporal lobe.

Error sites also corresponded to terminations of the posterior ‘indirect’ segment of the arcuate fasciculus in around half of the patient group, connecting the parietal and temporal lobes. This rate may have been lower as speech errors could not be elicited so commonly in the temporal lobe (also identified in around half of patients). As a recent language model has indicated that the temporo-parietal segment of the arcuate fasciculus plays an important role in speech production, specifically in word repetition, we propose that this lower incidence of errors may be due to the paradigm used (Forkel et al., 2020a). To name a picture, one must recognize the object, categorize it semantically, retrieve and select the target word and phonologically encode this, thus converting incoming visual information into articulatory output. However, in models of language, the posterior temporal lobe is classically associated with auditory processing (Indefrey and Levelt, 2004; Hickok and Poeppel, 2007). Thus, the lower number of errors identified in the temporal lobe may be a reflection of the behavioural paradigm used: were the cognitive load on auditory processing increased, nTMS may be more effective in preoperative mapping also in the temporal lobe. This may be achieved by implementing verb or noun generation tasks, repetition, acoustic or reading paradigms into nTMS, which are used in intraoperative mapping for identification temporal sites (Duffau et al., 2014; Sarubbo et al., 2020). Future studies may be able to determine the underlying mechanism limiting the efficiency of nTMS in mapping the temporal lobe.

Another aspect for discussion is the error sites identified that were not associated with the arcuate fasciculus. These were identified in the precentral, postcentral and middle frontal gyri. Direct motor responses for face muscles from both the pre- and post-central gyri are well established (Penfield and Boldrey, 1937). Moreover, errors in ventral regions of the postcentral gyrus are likely related to disruption of facial sensorimotor synergies, as tightly linked interactions between precentral and postcentral cortices are crucial during syllabification (Bouchard et al., 2013). The middle frontal gyrus, and particularly its most posterior part, is involved in semantic aspects of language processing, as well as switching between languages (Binder et al., 2009; Sierpowska et al., 2018): hence it is possible that stimulation here interfered with the broader language network. Alternatively, this result could be explained as a disruption of the attentional network, targeting the frontal eye fields connected to the parietal lobe via the middle branch of the superior longitudinal fasciculus II, which runs in parallel to the anterior segment (or superior longitudinal fascicle III). There were also considerable speech errors identified in tumorous or perilesional areas: in these regions tractography can be limited in tracing white matter tracts due to oedema and partial volume effects (Catani and Dell’Acqua, 2011). New diffusion imaging techniques such as three-tissue constrained spherical deconvolution have been developed that may be better able to track through lesioned tissue. However, mapping of eloquent tissue within and surrounding the tumour is particularly critical, thus DES is evidently still required within the surgical routine and our results indicate that preoperative techniques should still be considered adjunctive to the gold standard rather than a replacement.

nTMS-tractography and intraoperative mapping

nTMS has certain advantages over DES: it is non-invasive therefore is not restricted to the area of the craniotomy, and can support presurgical planning (Picht et al., 2013). It is also safe and well tolerated, with a very low seizure risk (Tarapore et al., 2016). In our study, we show that induced speech errors during nTMS mapping with picture naming corresponded to the arcuate fasciculus in all patients, particularly the fronto-parietal anterior segment (or superior longitudinal fascicle III). Speech errors occurred at both frontal and parietal cortical terminations in patients with the anterior segment, while in patients without no parietal site was identified. The protocol followed in this study used guidelines advised by an expert clinical panel for the presurgical application of nTMS (Krieg et al., 2017). This indicates that nTMS and tractography are highly complementary methods and are sensitive to interindividual variability, which was remarkably evident in our heterogeneous patient group. This is highly relevant when combining structural and functional information for preoperative planning, where tissue is damaged. We did not have access to the intraoperative stimulation sites identified in these patients, however it would be of value for future studies to evaluate whether the positive predictive value of nTMS for DES improves if targeted on the fronto-parietal branch of the arcuate using this paradigm, or can detect errors more widely if changing the paradigm to include auditory feedback.

Limitations and conclusions

There are several limitations to our study. The tractography acquisition used was a common clinical acquisition using a relatively low b-value (1000), and there is some discrepancy in the literature as to whether this can be used for higher quality reconstructions than those made possible with the diffusion tensor (Jones, 2010; Calamuneri et al., 2018). As our diffusion sequence was not up to the standards commonly used in research protocols, it is possible that we were unable to reconstruct streamlines projecting to other positive sites highlighted by TMS (false negatives). Future studies using more advanced sequences may be able to determine whether this is the case. Moreover, our sample size was relatively limited and therefore this should be considered preliminary. Further studies may confirm our results in a larger cohort. As speech errors are relatively rare per subject, we grouped different types of speech articulation error to improve power, however in a larger cohort it would be possible to evaluate whether there were specific pattern at cortical sites. Furthermore, the association between the number of baseline errors and the number of errors during mapping approached significance, which may indicate that the patient’s pre-existing language deficits may contribute to the number of errors identified during stimulation. We also did not perform repeated TMS mappings in the same subject, but it would be important to do this to evaluate individual reproducibility of the results. We cannot also rule out the role of other white matter tracts that have been linked to speech output such as the frontal aslant tract, linking the inferior and superior frontal gyri. Picture naming is also just one of many aspects of language that may be important to test preoperatively, such as spoken word recognition, syntax or sentence understanding (Ohlerth et al., 2020). Furthermore, it is not possible to evaluate the precise relationship between the cortex and the streamline endings using tractography due to partial volume effects at the grey–white matter boundary. However, given the wide field of TMS, we judged it possible to estimate likely white matter streamlines by extending the radius of the identified site (3 mm) into white matter.

To conclude, we show that preoperative nTMS mapping in a heterogeneous cohort of patients with brain tumours is reliable in causing interference in speech production during picture naming, specifically at sites corresponding to both frontal and parietal terminations of the arcuate fasciculus, termed the anterior segment or ventral branch of the superior longitudinal fasciculus III. Furthermore, we show that nTMS mapping is robust in identifying these terminations, even when white matter is displaced or infiltrated by the lesion, or a previous surgery has taken place. As there was lower reliability for mapping the temporal projections of the arcuate fasciculus, and given previous literature indicating a lesser role for these tracts in speech production per se, we propose that altering the behavioural paradigm may improve the positive predictive value of nTMS for DES, and preoperative mapping. Our results indicate that a combined functional–anatomical approach may be a relevant tool in providing personalized targets for DES in mapping speech production, or alternative choices of surgical strategy.

Supplementary material

Supplementary material is available at Brain Communications online.

Supplementary Material

fcaa158_Supplementary_Data

Acknowledgements

We are grateful to Michael Youssef for his assistance in providing additional details for the manuscript.

Funding

This publication was sustained by the Image Knowledge Gestaltung. An Interdisciplinary Laboratory Cluster of Excellence at the Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin (sponsor number EXC 1027/1) with financial support from the German Research Foundation as a part of the Excellence Initiative. D.G. was supported by a grant from Fondazione Cassa Rurale di Trento, Italy.

Competing interests

The authors report no competing interests.

Glossary

DES =

direct electrical stimulation;

nTMS =

navigated transcranial magnetic stimulation;

RMT =

resting motor threshold;

ROI =

region-of-interest;

TMS =

transcranial magnetic stimulation.

Contributor Information

Davide Giampiccolo, Department of Neurosurgery, Verona University Hospital, University of Verona, Verona, Italy; Department of Neurosurgery, Charité University Hospital, Berlin, Germany.

Henrietta Howells, MoCa Laboratory, University of Milan, Milan, Italy.

Ina Bährend, Department of Neurosurgery, Charité University Hospital, Berlin, Germany.

Heike Schneider, Department of Neurosurgery, Charité University Hospital, Berlin, Germany.

Giovanni Raffa, Department of Neurosurgery, Messina University Hospital, Italy.

Tizian Rosenstock, Department of Neurosurgery, Charité University Hospital, Berlin, Germany.

Francesco Vergani, Department of Neurosurgery, King’s College Hospital NHS Foundation Trust, London, UK.

Peter Vajkoczy, Department of Neurosurgery, Charité University Hospital, Berlin, Germany.

Thomas Picht, Department of Neurosurgery, Charité University Hospital, Berlin, Germany.

References

  1. Basser PJ, Pajevic S, Pierpaoli C, Duda J, Aldroubi A.  In vivo fiber tractography. Magn Reson Med  2000; 44: 625–32. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Bello L, Gallucci M, Fava M, Carrabba G, Giussani C, Acerbi F, et al.  Intraoperative subcortical language tract mapping guides surgical removal of gliomas involving speech areas. Neurosurgery  2007; 60: 67–80. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Benzagmout M, Gatignol P, Duffau H.  Resection of World Health Organization Grade II glioma involving Broca’s area: methodological and functional considerations. Neurosurgery  2007; 61: 741–53. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Binder JR, Desai RH, Graves WW, Conant LL.  Where is the semantic system? A critical review and meta-analysis of 120 functional neuroimaging studies. Cereb Cortex  2009; 19: 2767–96. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bouchard KE, Mesgarani N, Johnson K, Chang EF.  Functional organization of human sensorimotor cortex for speech articulation. Nature  2013; 495: 327–32. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Calamuneri A, Arrigo A, Mormina E, Milardi D, Cacciola A, Chillemi G, et al.  White matter tissue quantification at low b-values within constrained spherical deconvolution framework. Front Neurol  2018; 9: 1–14. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Catani M, Bambini V.  A model for social communication and language evolution and development (SCALED). Curr Opin Neurobiol  2014; 28: 165–71. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Catani M, Dell’Acqua F.  Mapping white matter pathways with diffusion imaging tractography: focus on neurosurgical applications. In: Duffau H, editor. Brain mapping. Springer, Vienna; 2011. 61–75. [Google Scholar]
  9. Catani M, Jones DK, Ffytche DH.  Perisylvian language networks of the human brain. Ann Neurol  2005; 57: 8–16. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Cerri G, Cabinio M, Blasi V, Borroni P, Iadanza A, Fava E, et al.  The mirror neuron system and the strange case of Broca’s area. Hum Brain Mapp  2015; 36: 1010–27. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Chang EF, Raygor KP, Berger MS.  Contemporary model of language organization: an overview for neurosurgeons. JNS  2015; 122: 250–61. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Chen DQ, Dell’Acqua F, Rokem A, Garyfallidis E, Hayes DJ, Zhong J, et al. Diffusion weighted image co-registration: investigation of best practices. bioRxiv 2019; 864108.
  13. Corina DP, Loudermilk BC, Detwiler L, Martin RF, Brinkley JF, Ojemann G.  Analysis of naming errors during cortical stimulation mapping: implications for models of language representation. Brain Lang  2010; 115: 101–12. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Dell'Acqua F, Scifo P, Rizzo G, Catani M, Simmons A, Scotti G, et al.  A modified damped Richardson–Lucy algorithm to reduce isotropic background effects in spherical deconvolution. NeuroImage  2010; 49: 1446–58. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Dell’Acqua F, Tournier JD.  Modelling white matter with spherical deconvolution: how and why?  NMR Biomed  2018; 1–18. doi: 10.1002/nbm.3945. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Devlin JT, Watkins KE.  Stimulating language: insights from TMS. Brain  2007; 130: 610–22. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Dronkers NF, Plaisant O, Iba-Zizen MT, Cabanis EA.  Paul Broca’s historic cases: high resolution MR imaging of the brains of Leborgne and Lelong. Brain  2007; 130: 1432–41. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Duffau H.  Diffuse low-grade gliomas and neuroplasticity. Diagn Interv Imaging  2014; 95: 945–55. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Duffau H, Gatignol P, Denvil D, Lopes M, Capelle L.  The articulatory loop: study of the subcortical connectivity by electrostimulation. NeuroReport  2003; 14: 2005–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Duffau H, Moritz-Gasser S, Mandonnet E.  A re-examination of neural basis of language processing: proposal of a dynamic hodotopical model from data provided by brain stimulation mapping during picture naming. Brain Lang  2014; 131: 1–10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Farquharson S, Tournier JD, Calamante F, Fabinyi G, Schneider-Kolsky M, Jackson GD, et al.  White matter fiber tractography: why we need to move beyond DTI. JNS  2013; 118: 1367–77. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Ferpozzi V, Fornia L, Montagna M, Siodambro C, Castellano A, Borroni P, et al.  Broca’s area as a pre-articulatory phonetic encoder: gating the motor program. Front Hum Neurosci  2018; 12: 1–17. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Ferro JM, Madureira S, Aphasia type, age and cerebral infarct localisation. J Neurol 1997; 244: 505–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Forkel SJ, Thiebaut de Schotten M, Dell’Acqua F, Kalra L, Murphy DGM, Williams SCR, et al.  Anatomical predictors of aphasia recovery: a tractography study of bilateral perisylvian language networks. Brain  2014; 137: 2027–39. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Forkel SJ, Friedrich P, Thiebaut de Schotten M, de Howells H. White matter variability, cognition, and disorders: a systematic review. medRxiv 2020. a; 04.22.20075127. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  26. Forkel SJ, Rogalski E, Drossinos Sancho N, D'Anna L, Luque Laguna P, Sridhar J, et al.  Anatomical evidence of an indirect pathway for word repetition. Neurology  2020. b; 94: e594–606. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Fridriksson J, Den Ouden DB, Hillis AE, Hickok G, Rorden C, Basilakos A, et al.  Anatomy of aphasia revisited. Brain  2018; 141: 848–62. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Fridriksson J, Guo D, Fillmore P, Holland A, Rorden C.  Damage to the anterior arcuate fasciculus predicts non-fluent speech production in aphasia. Brain  2013; 136: 3451–60. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Geschwind N.  Disconnexion syndromes in animal and man. Brain  1965; 88: 237–94. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Giussani C, Roux FE, Ojemann J, Sganzerla E, Pietro Pirillo D, Papagno C.  Is preoperative functional magnetic resonance imaging reliable for language areas mapping in brain tumor surgery? Review of language functional magnetic resonance imaging and direct cortical stimulation correlation studies. Neurosurgery  2010; 66: 113–20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Herbet G, Maheu M, Costi E, Lafargue G, Duffau H.  Mapping neuroplastic potential in brain-damaged patients. Brain  2016; 139: 829–44. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Hickok G, Poeppel D.  The cortical organization of speech processing. Nat Rev Neurosci  2007; 8: 393–402. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Howells H, Thiebaut de Schotten M, Dell’Acqua F, Beyh A, Zappalà G, Leslie A, et al.  Frontoparietal tracts linked to lateralized hand preference and manual specialization. Cereb Cortex  2018; 28: 2482–94. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Igelström KM, Webb TW, Graziano MSA.  Neural processes in the human temporoparietal cortex separated by localized independent component analysis. J Neurosci  2015; 35: 9432–45. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Indefrey P, Levelt WJM.  The spatial and temporal signatures of word production components. Cognition  2004; 92: 101–44. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Jenkinson M, Beckmann CF, Behrens TE, Woolrich MW, Smith SM.  FSL. NeuroImage  2012; 62: 782–90. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Jones DK. Diffusion MRI. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2010.
  38. Jones DK, Knösche TR, Turner R.  White matter integrity, fiber count, and other fallacies: the do’s and don’ts of diffusion MRI. NeuroImage  2013; 73: 239–54. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Krieg SM, Lioumis P, Mäkelä JP, Wilenius J, Karhu J, Hannula H, et al.  Protocol for motor and language mapping by navigated TMS in patients and healthy volunteers; workshop report. Acta Neurochir (Wien)  2017; 159: 1187–95. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Krieg SM, Sollmann N, Hauck T, Ille S, Meyer B, Ringel F.  Repeated mapping of cortical language sites by preoperative navigated transcranial magnetic stimulation compared to repeated intraoperative DCS mapping in awake craniotomy. BMC Neurosci  2014; 15: 20. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Lin Y, Zhang K, Li S, Li S, Jin J, Jin F, et al.  Relationship between perisylvian essential language sites and arcuate fasciculus in the left hemisphere of healthy adults. Neurosci Bull  2017; 33: 616–26. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Lioumis P, Zhdanov A, Mäkelä N, Lehtinen H, Wilenius J, Neuvonen T, et al.  A novel approach for documenting naming errors induced by navigated transcranial magnetic stimulation. J Neurosci Methods  2012; 204: 349–54. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Lopez-Barroso D, Catani M, Ripolles P, Dell'Acqua F, Rodriguez-Fornells A, de Diego-Balaguer R.  Word learning is mediated by the left arcuate fasciculus. Proc Natl Acad Sci  2013; 110: 13168–73. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Michelucci R, Valzania F, Passarelli D, Santangelo M, Rizzi R, Buzzi AM, et al.  Rapid-rate transcranial magnetic stimulation and hemispheric language dominance: usefulness and safety in epilepsy. Neurology  1994; 44: 1697. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Ohlerth AK, Valentin A, Vergani F, Ashkan K, Bastiaanse R.  The verb and noun test for peri-operative testing (VAN-POP): standardized language tests for navigated transcranial magnetic stimulation and direct electrical stimulation. Acta Neurochir  2020; 162: 397–406. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Papagno C, Comi A, Riva M, Bizzi A, Vernice M, Casarotti A, et al.  Mapping the brain network of the phonological loop. Hum Brain Mapp  2017; 38: 3011–24. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Penfield W, Boldrey E.  Somatic motor and sensory representation in the cerebral cortex of man as studied by electrical stimulation. Brain  1937; 60: 389–443. [Google Scholar]
  48. Picht T, Krieg SM, Sollmann N, Rösler J, Niraula B, Neuvonen T, et al.  A comparison of language mapping by preoperative navigated transcranial magnetic stimulation and direct cortical stimulation during awake surgery. Neurosurgery  2013; 72: 808–19. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Poeppel D, Assaneo MF, Speech rhythms and their neural foundations. Nat Rev Neurosci 2020; 21: 322–34. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Raffa G, Bährend I, Schneider H, Faust K, Germanò A, Vajkoczy P, et al.  A novel technique for region and linguistic specific nTMS-based DTI fiber tracking of language pathways in brain tumor patients. Front Neurosci  2016; 10: 1–17. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Romero MC, Davare M, Armendariz M, Janssen P, Neural effects of transcranial magnetic stimulation at the single-cell level. Nat Commun 2019; 10: 1–11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Rossi S, Hallett M, Rossini PM, Pascual-Leone A.  Safety, ethical considerations, and application guidelines for the use of transcranial magnetic stimulation in clinical practice and research. Clin Neurophysiol  2012; 120: 323–30. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Ruohonen J, Karhu J.  Navigated transcranial magnetic stimulation. Clin Neurophysiol  2010; 40: 7–17. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Sanai N, Berger MS.  Glioma extent of resection and its impact on patient outcome. Neurosurgery  2008; 62: 753–66. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Sarubbo S, De Benedictis A, Merler S, Mandonnet E, Balbi S, Granieri E, et al.  Towards a functional atlas of human white matter. Hum Brain Mapp  2015; 36: 3117–36. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Sarubbo S, Tate M, De Benedictis A, Merler S, Moritz-Gasser S, Herbet G, et al.  Mapping critical cortical hubs and white matter pathways by direct electrical stimulation: an original functional atlas of the human brain. NeuroImage  2020; 205: 116237. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Sierpowska J, Fernandez-Coello A, Gomez-Andres A, Camins À, Castañer S, Juncadella M, et al.  Involvement of the middle frontal gyrus in language switching as revealed by electrical stimulation mapping and functional magnetic resonance imaging in bilingual brain tumor patients. Cortex  2018; 99: 78–92. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Southwell DG, Hervey-Jumper SL, Perry DW, Berger MS.  Intraoperative mapping during repeat awake craniotomy reveals the functional plasticity of adult cortex. JNS  2016; 124: 1460–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Tarapore PE, Findlay AM, Honma SM, Mizuiri D, Houde JF, Berger MS, et al.  Language mapping with navigated repetitive TMS: proof of technique and validation. NeuroImage  2013; 82: 260–72. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Tarapore PE, Picht T, Bulubas L, Shin Y, Kulchytska N, Meyer B, et al.  Safety and tolerability of navigated TMS for preoperative mapping in neurosurgical patients. Clin Neurophysiol  2016; 127: 1895–900. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Tremblay P, Dick AS.  Broca and Wernicke are dead, or moving past the classic model of language neurobiology. Brain Lang  2016; 162: 60–71. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. van Geemen K, Herbet G, Moritz-Gasser S, Duffau H.  Limited plastic potential of the left ventral premotor cortex in speech articulation: evidence from intraoperative awake mapping in glioma patients. Hum Brain Mapp  2014; 35: 1587–96. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Vidorreta JG, Garcia R, Moritz-Gasser S, Duffau H.  Double dissociation between syntactic gender and picture naming processing: a brain stimulation mapping study. Hum Brain Mapp  2011; 32: 331–40. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

fcaa158_Supplementary_Data

Data Availability Statement

The spreadsheets that support the findings of this study are available on the Open Science Framework (https://osf.io/xyekp/). The clinical data is available on reasonable request to the first or last author (D.G. or T.P.).


Articles from Brain Communications are provided here courtesy of Oxford University Press

RESOURCES