Skip to main content
Frontiers in Neuroscience logoLink to Frontiers in Neuroscience
. 2021 Jan 11;14:612445. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2020.612445

Heart Rate Variability Modulates Interoceptive Accuracy

Alexander Lischke 1,*, Rike Pahnke 2, Anett Mau-Moeller 2, Matthias Weippert 2,*
PMCID: PMC7849500  PMID: 33536870

Abstract

Our emotional experiences depend on our interoceptive ability to perceive and interpret changes in our autonomous nervous system. An inaccurate perception and interpretation of autonomic changes impairs our ability to understand and regulate our emotional reactions. Impairments in emotion understanding and emotion regulation increase our risk for mental disorders, indicating that interoceptive deficits play an important role in the etiology and pathogenesis of mental disorders. We, thus, need measures to identify those of us whose interoceptive deficits impair their emotion understanding and emotion regulation. Here, we used cardiac measures to investigate how our ability to engage prefrontal and (para-)limbic brain region regions affects our ability to perceive and interpret cardiac changes. We administered a heartbeat detection task to a sample of healthy individuals (n = 113) whose prefrontal-(para-) limbic engagement had been determined on basis of a heart rate variability recording. We found a positive association between heartbeat detection and heart rate variability, implying that individuals with higher heart rate variability were more accurate in heartbeat detection than individuals with lower heart rate variability. These findings suggest that our interoceptive accuracy depends on our prefrontal-(para-)limbic engagement during the perception and interpretation of cardiac changes. Our findings also show that cardiac measures may be useful to investigate the association between interoceptive accuracy and prefrontal-(para-)limbic engagement in a time- and cost-efficient manner.

Keywords: heartbeat detection, heart rate variability, interoception, attention, vagal tone, emotion

Introduction

Interoception refers to our ability to perceive and interpret changes in our autonomous nervous system (Craig, 2002). Cardiac and respiratory activity are examples of autonomic processes that frequently change during emotional events (Kreibig, 2010). The perception and interpretation of these autonomic changes forms the basis of our emotional experiences and helps us to understand and to regulate our emotional reactions (Critchley and Garfinkel, 2017). For instance, those of us who are sensitive to cardiac changes have less difficulties in understanding and responding to emotional events than those of us who are insensitive to cardiac changes (Fustos et al., 2013; Shah et al., 2017; Lischke et al., 2020a, b). Given that emotion understanding and emotion regulation is central for our mental health (Gross and Jazaieri, 2014), it is not surprising that alterations in the perception and interpretation of autonomic changes increase our risk for mental disorders (Paulus and Stein, 2010). For instance, those of us whose cardiac sensitivity is in the abnormal range have more mental health problems than those of us whose cardiac sensitivity is in the normal range (Pollatos et al., 2007b, 2009; Herbert et al., 2011; Shah et al., 2016). The way we perceive and interpret autonomic changes, thus, appears to be of utmost importance for our mental health.

To understand how we perceive and interpret autonomic changes, we need measures that differentiate between different aspects of interoception (Critchley and Garfinkel, 2017). Fortunately, these measures have already been developed by researchers. Depending on the research question, these measures assess interoceptive accuracy (i.e., objective accounts of interoception), interoceptive sensibility (i.e., subjective accounts of interoception) or interoceptive awareness (i.e., correspondence between objective and subjective accounts of interoception). The most popular measure is the heartbeat detection task (Schandry, 1981), an interceptive accuracy task that requires the tracking of heartbeats within different time intervals. By employing the heartbeat detection task, researchers were able to identify a network of brain regions that is relevant for the perception and interpretation of cardiac changes (Schulz, 2016). This network comprises several brain regions but prefrontal and (para-)limbic brain regions appear to be the most important ones. Prefrontal and (para-)limbic show the most pronounced activity changes during the perception and interpretation of cardiac changes (Critchley et al., 2004; Pollatos et al., 2005; Kuehn et al., 2016), implying a close association between prefrontal-(para-)limbic activity and interoceptive accuracy. It may, thus, be possible that our ability to perceive and interpret cardiac changes depends on our ability to engage prefrontal and (para-)limbic brain regions for this matter.

To test this possibility, we administered measures of interoceptive accuracy and prefrontal-(para-)limbic engagement to a sample of young adults. Interoceptive accuracy was measured with the heartbeat detection task and prefrontal-(para-)limbic engagement was measured with a heart rate recording. The heart rate recording was used for the determination of parasympathically induced heart rate changes, a measure of vagally mediated heart rate variability (Shaffer and Ginsberg, 2017). Parasympathetically induced heart rate changes are closely associated with activity changes in prefrontal and (para-)limbic brain regions (Thayer et al., 2012; Ruiz Vargas et al., 2016), indicating that vagally mediated heart rate variability reflects prefrontal-(para-)limibic engagement (Thayer and Lane, 2009; Smith et al., 2017). The heart rate recording, thus, allowed us to investigate the association between interoceptive accuracy and prefrontal-(para-)limbic engagement in an unobtrusive manner. In light of previous findings showing that activity changes in prefrontal and (para-)limbic brain regions are positively associated with the perception and interpretation of cardiac changes (Critchley et al., 2004; Pollatos et al., 2005; Kuehn et al., 2016), we expected to find a similar association between vagally mediated heart rate variability and heartbeat detection. Preliminary findings suggest that vagally mediated heart rate variability may be positively associated with heartbeat detection (Owens et al., 2018). We, thus, expected to find a positive rather than negative association between vagally mediated heartrate variability and heartbeat detection.

Materials and Methods

Participants

We based our participant recruitment on an a priori power analysis with GPower 3.1.9.2 (Faul et al., 2009). The power analysis suggested that 82 participants would provide sufficient data to detect meaningful associations between vagally mediated heartrate variability and heartbeat detection in our analyses [correlation analysis (two-tailed): α = 0.05, 1-ß = 0.80, r = 0.30; regression analyses (total number of predictors: 8, number of tested predictors: 1): α = 0.05, 1-ß = 0.80, f2 = 0.15]. Following this suggestion, we recruited 113 participants for the study (see Table 1). In order to be included in the study, the participants had to be native speakers and to be aged between 18 and 35 years. Participants who were currently in psychotherapeutic treatment were excluded from the study. Inclusion and exclusion of participants was determined on basis of an in-house questionnaire assessing sociodemographic (age and sex), anthropometric (height and weight) and medical (physical activity in terms of aerobic fitness, smoking status, medication status, treatment status) information (Lischke et al., 2018a). All participants that were included in the study provided written-informed consent to the study protocol. The study protocol, which had been approved by the local ethics committee, was carried out in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.

TABLE 1.

Participant characteristics.

M (SD) 95% CI
Sex (f/m, n) 40/69
Age 26.30 (3.91) [25.55, 27.04]
Tobacco use (n) 28
Medication use (n) 10
 Anti-allergic medication 3
 Endocrine medication 5
 Psychotropic medication 2
Contraceptive use (n) 22
 Unspecified contraceptives 19
 Androgenic contraceptives 1
 Anti-androgenic contraceptives 2
Body mass index (kg/m2) 22.90 (2.83) [22.40, 23.42]
Physical activity (h/week) 6.19 (3.53) [5.57, 6.86]
Respiratory activity (log pHF, Hz) −0.71 (0.10) [−0.72, −0.69]
Heart rate (bpm) 74.70 (12.20) [72.25, 76.88]
Heart rate variability
 RMSSD (ms) 43.54 (26.19) [38.51, 48.79]
 log RMSSD (ms) 1.57 (0.25) [1.52, 1.62]
 pNN50 (%) 20.72 (17.66) [17.35, 24.20]
 log pNN50 (%) 1.14 (0.48) [1.04, 1.23]
Heartbeat detection
 HBDSC 0.69 (0.18) [0.66, 0.72]
 HBDGA 0.60 (0.26) [0.56, 0.65]

pHF, peak of high frequency band (Shaffer and Ginsberg, 2017); RMSSD, root mean square of successive differences between consecutive heartbeats (Shaffer and Ginsberg, 2017); pNN50, number of successive heartbeat interval pairs that differ more than 50 ms divided by the total number of all heartbeat intervals (Shaffer and Ginsberg, 2017); HBDSC, Heartbeat detection – traditional index (Schandry, 1981); HBDGA, Heartbeat detection – alternative index (Garfinkel et al., 2015).

Procedure

We used a heart rate recording to determine participants’ vagally mediated heart rate variability and heartbeat detection. Each recording session was scheduled during the daytime (at least 2 h after wakening time and 5 h before sleeping time) to control for circadian and diurnal variations in participants’ heart rate (Yamasaki et al., 1996; Bonnemeier et al., 2003). Before we started with the recording session, we asked the participants to use the bathroom. This allowed us to rule out that bladder filling and gastric digestion had an effect on participants’ heart rate (Fagius and Karhuvaara, 1989; Rossi et al., 1998). The participants completed the recording session in a comfortable chair that was located in a dimly lit room. The heart rate recording was performed with a mobile heart rate monitor (RS 800, Polar Electro Oy; Kempele, Finland) that has been shown to record heartbeats as accurate as mobile electrocardiogram systems (Weippert et al., 2010).

Heartbeat Detection

Participants’ heartbeat detection was determined during the first part of the recording session. Following an established procedure (Lischke et al., 2020a, b), we asked the participants to count their heartbeats during 25, 35, and 45 s lasting time intervals. They were not informed about the length of the time intervals and they were not allowed to use any measure that may have facilitated the heartbeat detection. We used the number of counted and recorded heartbeats to compute two different indices of participants’ heartbeat detection, a traditional heartbeat detection index1 (Schandry, 1981) and an alternative heartbeat detection index2 (Hart et al., 2013) that has been shown to be to be less sensitive against outliers than the traditional heartbeat detection index (Garfinkel et al., 2015). We used both heartbeat detection indices in our analyses to test the robustness of our findings.

Heart Rate Variability

Participants’ vagally mediated heart rate variability was determined during the second part of the recording session. Following an established procedure (Lischke et al., 2018b, 2019), we asked the participants to sit still and to stay awake during a 300 s lasting time interval. The heartbeats that were recorded during this time interval were analyzed with Kubios HRV 2.2 (Tarvainen et al., 2014). The analysis followed the guidelines of the Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology (1996): The recordings were detrended (smoothn priors: λ = 500), visually inspected and artifact corrected (adaptive filtering: cubic spline interpolation) before they were subjected to a time-domain and spectral analysis. The time-domain analysis was used for the determination of a heart rate index (meanHR) and for the determination of two vagally mediated heartrate variability indices (the root mean square of successive differences between consecutive heartbeats, RMSSD; the number of successive heartbeat interval pairs that differ more than 50 ms divided by the total number of all heartbeat intervals, pNN50). The values of these indices were in the range of values that have been reported in comparable samples of participants (Dantas et al., 2018). The spectral analysis was used to determine a respiration index (Thayer et al., 2002), the peak frequency of the high frequency band (pHF). We used the respiration index to adjust the vagally mediated heartrate variability indices for respiration-induced alterations (Weippert et al., 2015). Using both vagally mediated heartrate variability indices in our analyses allowed us to test the robustness of our findings.

Statistical Analyses

We performed all analyses with the bootstrapping module of SPSS 27 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, United States). Preliminary analyses revealed that the datasets of four participants were incomplete or invalid due to a recording error. We, thus, used the datasets of the remaining 109 participants for the main analyses. The main analyses comprised regression and correlation analyses. Multiple regressions were run to analyze the association between participants’ vagally mediated heart rate variability and heartbeat detection. The vagally mediated heart rate variability indices constituted the predictor variables and the heartbeat detection indices constituted the criterion variables. Participant characteristics that may distort the association between vagally mediated heartrate variability and heartbeat detection were used as additional predictor variables (age, sex, body mass index, physical activity, respiratory activity, smoking status, and medication status). The regression analyses were complemented by correlation analyses. Partial correlations were run to quantify the association between participants’ vagally mediated heart rate variability and heartbeat detection. The vagally mediated heart rate variability indices constituted the dependent variable, the heartbeat detection indices the independent variables and the aforementioned participant characteristics the control variables. We set the significance level for all analyses at α ≤ 0.05 and determined significance values (p), effect size measures (r, R2, ΔR2, and B) and 95% confidence intervals (CIs).

Results

Associations Between the Vagally Mediated Heart Rate Variability Indices (RMSSD, pNN50) and the Traditional Heartbeat Detection Index (HBDSC)

In the first set of regression models, the traditional heartbeat detection index constituted the criterion variable. Entering the participant characteristics as predictor variables in a first step into the regression models accounted for a significant proportion of the variance in the traditional heartbeat detection index [RMSSD: R2 = 0.15, F(8,100) = 2.13, p = 0.040; pNN50: R2 = 0.15, F(8,100) = 2.13, p = 0.040, see Table 2]. However, sex was the only predictor variable that turned out be significant in the regression models [RMSSD: B = 0.14, SE B = 0.05, 95% CI [0.03, 0.23], t(100) = 2.37, p = 0.015; pNN50: B = 0.13, SE B = 0.05, 95% CI [0.04, 0.23], t(100) = 2.37, p = 0.018; see Table 2]. Entering the vagally mediated heart rate variability indices as predictor variables in a second step into the regression models also accounted for a significant proportion of the variance in the traditional heartbeat detection index [RMSSD: ΔR2 = 0.04, ΔF(1,99) = 4.46, p = 0.037; pNN50: ΔR2 = 0.05, F(1,99) = 6.13, p = 0.015, see Table 2]. The vagally mediated heart rate variability indices were, besides sex [RMSSD: B = 0.13, SE B = 0.05, 95% CI [0.03, 0.23], t(99) = 2.33, p = 0.013; pNN50: B = 0.12, SE B = 0.05, 95% CI [0.02, 0.22], t(99) = 2.27, p = 0.020; see Table 2], the only significant predictors in the regression models [RMSSD: B = 0.15, SE B = 0.06, 95% CI [0.02, 0.28], t(99) = 2.11, p = 0.024; pNN50: B = 0.09, SE B = 0.04, 95% CI [0.02, 0.16], t(99) = 2.48, p = 0.009; see Table 2]. Taken together, the regression models suggested that there was a small to medium sized association between the vagally mediated heart rate variability indices and the traditional heartbeat detection index [RMSSD: r(99) = 0.21, 95% CI [0.05, 0.37], p = 0.037; pNN50: r(99) = 24, 95% CI [0.05, 0.41], p = 0.015; see Figure 1].

TABLE 2.

Associations between the vagally mediated heart rate variability indices (RMSSD and pNN50) and the traditional heartbeat detection index (HBDSC).

Heartbeat detection (HBDSC)
Heartbeat detection (HBDSC)
Model One B SE B 95% CI t p Model two B SE B 95% CI t p
Step one Step one
Sex 0.13 0.05 [0.03, 0.23] 2.37 0.015* Sex 0.13 0.05 [0.04, 0.23] 2.37 0.018*
Age 0.00 0.01 [−0.01, 0.01] –0.09 0.943 Age 0.00 0.01 [−0.01, 0.01] –0.09 0.915
Tobacco use –0.03 0.04 [−0.11, 0.06] –0.64 0.552 Tobacco use –0.03 0.04 [−0.11, 0.06] –0.64 0.545
Medication use 0.04 0.08 [−0.10, 0.21] 0.70 0.610 Medication use 0.04 0.08 [−0.10, 0.20] 0.70 0.574
Contraceptive use 0.03 0.05 [−0.08, 0.14] 0.54 0.545 Contraceptive use 0.03 0.05 [−0.07, 0.14] 0.54 0.559
Body mass index –0.01 0.01 [−0.02, 0.01] –0.97 0.342 Body mass index –0.01 0.01 [−0.02, 0.01] –0.97 0.312
Physical activity 0.01 0.01 [0.00, 0.02] 1.79 0.066 Physical activity 0.01 0.01 [0.00, 0.02] 1.79 0.068
Respiratory activity (log pHF) –0.03 0.16 [−0.32, 0.30] –0.15 0.891 Respiratory activity (log pHF) –0.03 0.16 [−0.35, 0.30] –0.15 0.870
Step two Step two
Sex 0.13 0.05 [0.03, 0.23] 2.33 0.013** Sex 0.12 0.05 [0.03, 0.22] 2.27 0.020*
Age 0.00 0.01 [−0.01, 0.01] 0.00 1.000 Age 0.00 0.00 [−0.01, 0.01] –0.07 0.946
Tobacco use –0.02 0.04 [−0.10, 0.07] –0.44 0.661 Tobacco use –0.02 0.04 [−0.1, 0.07] –0.41 0.694
Medication use 0.04 0.08 [−0.1, 0.20] 0.66 0.610 Medication use 0.04 0.07 [−0.08, 0.19] 0.72 0.551
Contraceptive use 0.05 0.05 [−0.06, 0.15] 0.81 0.374 Contraceptive use 0.04 0.05 [−0.06, 0.15] 0.69 0.452
Body mass index 0.00 0.01 [−0.02, 0.01] –0.50 0.669 Body mass index 0.00 0.01 [−0.02, 0.01] –0.41 0.687
Physical activity 0.01 0.00 [0.00, 0.02] 1.41 0.120 Physical activity 0.01 0.01 [0.00, 0.02] 1.24 0.185
Respiratory activity (log pHF) –0.11 0.16 [−0.40, 0.22] –0.59 0.486 Respiratory activity (log pHF) –0.10 0.16 [−0.41, 0.25] –0.53 0.558
Heart rate variability (log RMSSD) 0.15 0.06 [0.02, 0.28] 2.11 0.024* Heart rate variability (log pNN50) 0.09 0.04 [0.02, 0.16] 2.48 0.014**

Model one: step one: R2 = 0.15, F(8,100) = 2.13, p = 0.040*, step two: ΔR2 = 0.04, ΔF(1,99) = 4.46, p = 0.037*; model two: step one: R2 = 0.15, F(8,100) = 2.13, p = 0.040*, step two: ΔR2 = 0.05, ΔF(1,99) = 6.13, p = 0.015*. HBDSC, Heartbeat detection – traditional index (Schandry, 1981); pHF, peak of high frequency band (Shaffer and Ginsberg, 2017); RMSSD, root mean square of successive differences between consecutive heartbeats (Shaffer and Ginsberg, 2017); pNN50, number of successive heartbeat interval pairs that differ more than 50 ms divided by the total number of all heartbeat intervals (Shaffer and Ginsberg, 2017). *p ≤ 0.05, **p ≤ 0.01, ***p ≤ 0.001.

FIGURE 1.

FIGURE 1

Scatterplots with lines of best fit and 95% confidence intervals demonstrating raw associations between the vagally mediated heart rate variability indices (RMSSD and pNN50) and the traditional heartbeat detection index (HBDSC).

Associations Between the Vagally Mediated Heart Rate Variability Indices (RMSSD, pNN50) and the Alternative Heartbeat Detection Index (HBDGA)

In the second set of regression models, the alternative heartbeat detection index constituted the criterion variable. Entering the participant characteristics as predictor variables in a first step into the regression models accounted for a significant proportion of the variance in the alternative heartbeat detection index [RMSSD: R2 = 0.14, F(8,100) = 2.12, p = 0.041; pNN50: R2 = 0.14, F(8,100) = 2.12, p = 0.041; see Table 3]. However, sex was the only predictor variable that turned out be significant in the regression models [RMSSD: B = 0.18, SE B = 0.08, 95% CI [0.03, 0.23], t(100) = 2.36, p = 0.017; pNN50: B = 0.18, SE B = 0.07, 95% CI [0.04, 0.33], t(100) = 2.36, p = 0.009; see Table 3]. Entering the vagally mediated heart rate variability indices as predictor variables in a second step into the regression models also accounted for a significant proportion of the variance in the alternative heartbeat detection index [RMSSD: ΔR2 = 0.04, ΔF(1,99) = 4.79, p = 0.031; pNN50: ΔR2 = 0.05, F(1,99) = 6.70, p = 0.011; see Table 3]. The vagally mediated heart rate variability indices were, besides sex [RMSSD: B = 0.18, SE B = 0.08, 95% CI [0.03, 0.34], t(99) = 2.32, p = 0.022; pNN50: B = 0.17, SE B = 0.07, 95% CI [0.04, 0.32], t(99) = 2.25, p = 0.015; see Table 3], the only significant predictors in the regression models [RMSSD: B = 0.23, SE B = 0.09, 95% CI [0.04, 0.40], t(99) = 2.19, p = 0.020; pNN50: B = 0.13, SE B = 0.05, 95% CI [0.03, 0.22], t(100) = 2.59, p = 0.007; see Table 3]. Taken together, the regression models suggested that there was a small to medium sized association between the vagally mediated heart rate variability indices and the alternative heartbeat detection index [RMSSD: r(99) = 0.21, 95% CI [0.05, 0.37], p = 0.031; pNN50: r(99) = 0.25, 95% CI [0.06, 0.42], p = 0.011; see Figure 2].

TABLE 3.

Associations between the vagally mediated heart rate variability indices (RMSSD and pNN50) and the alternative heartbeat detection index (HBDGA).

Heartbeat detection (HBDGA)
Heartbeat detection (HBDGA)
Model One B SE B 95% CI t p Model two B SE B 95% CI t p
Step one Step one
Sex 0.18 0.08 [0.03, 0.34] 2.36 0.017* Sex 0.18 0.07 [0.04, 0.33] 2.36 0.009**
Age 0.00 0.01 [−0.02, 0.01] –0.49 0.668 Age 0.00 0.01 [−0.02, 0.01] –0.49 0.656
Tobacco use –0.03 0.06 [−0.14, 0.09] –0.45 0.690 Tobacco use –0.03 0.06 [−0.13, 0.09] –0.45 0.696
Medication use 0.06 0.11 [−0.15, 0.29] 0.66 0.555 Medication use 0.06 0.11 [−0.14, 0.28] 0.66 0.583
Contraceptive use 0.04 0.08 [−0.12, 0.18] 0.53 0.579 Contraceptive use 0.04 0.08 [−0.1, 0.20] 0.53 0.565
Body mass index –0.01 0.01 [−0.03, 0.01] –1.05 0.290 Body mass index –0.01 0.01 [−0.03, 0.01] –1.05 0.318
Physical activity 0.01 0.01 [0.00, 0.03] 1.86 0.063 Physical activity 0.01 0.01 [0.00, 0.03] 1.86 0.064
Respiratory activity (log pHF) 0.02 0.23 [−0.42, 0.49] 0.07 0.930 Respiratory activity (log pHF) 0.02 0.23 [−0.42, 0.45] 0.07 0.933
Step two Step two
Sex 0.18 0.08 [0.03, 0.34] 2.32 0.022* Sex 0.17 0.07 [0.04, 0.32] 2.25 0.015*
Age 0.00 0.01 [−0.02, 0.01] –0.40 0.726 Age 0.00 0.01 [−0.02, 0.01] –0.47 0.688
Tobacco use –0.01 0.06 [−0.13, 0.10] –0.23 0.825 Tobacco use –0.01 0.06 [−0.12, 0.1] –0.20 0.842
Medication use 0.05 0.10 [−0.15, 0.27] 0.61 0.568 Medication use 0.06 0.10 [−0.13, 0.27] 0.68 0.552
Contraceptive use 0.07 0.08 [−0.09, 0.21] 0.82 0.384 Contraceptive use 0.06 0.08 [−0.09, 0.2] 0.70 0.468
Body mass index –0.01 0.01 [−0.03, 0.01] –0.56 0.553 Body mass index 0.00 0.01 [−0.03, 0.02] –0.47 0.665
Physical activity 0.01 0.01 [0.00, 0.03] 1.47 0.123 Physical activity 0.01 0.01 [0.00, 0.02] 1.29 0.162
Respiratory activity (log pHF) –0.10 0.24 [−0.57, 0.4] –0.40 0.647 Respiratory activity (log pHF) –0.08 0.23 [−0.54, 0.35] –0.32 0.734
Heart rate variability (log RMSSD) 0.23 0.09 [0.04, 0.4] 2.19 0.020* Heart rate variability (log pNN50) 0.13 0.05 [0.03, 0.22] 2.59 0.007**

Model one: step one: R2 = 0.14, F(8,100) = 2.12, p = 0.031*, step two: ΔR2 = 0.04, ΔF(1,99) = 4.79, p = 0.031*; model two: step one: R2 = 0.14, F(8,101) = 2.12, p = 0.041*, step two: ΔR2 = 0.05, ΔF(1,99) = 6.70, p = 0.011*. HBDGA, Heartbeat detection – alternative index (Garfinkel et al., 2015); pHF, peak of high frequency band (Shaffer and Ginsberg, 2017); RMSSD, root mean square of successive differences between consecutive heartbeats (Shaffer and Ginsberg, 2017); pNN50, number of successive heartbeat interval pairs that differ more than 50 ms divided by the total number of all heartbeat intervals (Shaffer and Ginsberg, 2017). *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

FIGURE 2.

FIGURE 2

Scatterplots with lines of best fit and 95% confidence intervals demonstrating raw associations between the vagally mediated heart rate variability indices (RMSSD and pNN50) and the alternative heartbeat detection index (HBDGA).

Discussion

To explore the possibility that our interoceptive accuracy depends on our ability to engage prefrontal and (para-)limbic brain regions for this matter (Critchley et al., 2004; Pollatos et al., 2005, 2007a; Kuehn et al., 2016), we administered cardiac measures of interoceptive accuracy and prefontal-(para-)limbic engagement to a sample of young adults. Interoceptive accuracy was measured with a heartbeat detection task and prefrontal-(para-)limbic engagement with a heart rate recording. We used the heartbeat detection task to determine two different heartbeat detection indices and the heart rate recording to determine two different vagally mediated heart rate variability indices (Shaffer and Ginsberg, 2017). The vagally mediated heart rate indices were both positively associated with the heartbeat detection indices, regardless whether the traditional or alternative heartbeat detection index were considered in the regression and correlation analyses. The regression and correlation analyses were well-powered and well-controlled, ruling out that the association between the vagally mediated heart rate variability index and the heartbeat detection indices was spurious. We, thus, found the expected association between vagally mediated heart rate variability and heartbeat detection. The association between vagally mediated heart rate variability and heartbeat detection supports the idea that our interoceptive accuracy depends on our ability to engage prefrontal and (para-)limbic brain regions for the perception and interpretation of cardiac changes.

To understand the association between vagally mediated heart rate variability and heartbeat detection, we have to delineate the processes that determine the performance on the heartbeat detection task. Heartbeat detection relies on executive control processes (Critchley and Garfinkel, 2017), in particular on those that are related to attention. For an accurate perception and interpretation of cardiac changes, attention has to be shifted from the outside to the inside of the body, to be shielded against sensations from the outside of the body and to be focused on sensations inside the body. Attention shifting, attention shielding and attention focusing are executive control processes that are driven by activity changes in prefrontal brain and (para-)limbic regions (Petersen and Posner, 2012). Activity changes in prefrontal and (para-)limbic brain regions are associated with vagally mediated heart rate variability (Thayer et al., 2012; Ruiz Vargas et al., 2016), indicating that vagally mediated heart rate variability may also be associated with these executive control processes. Vagally mediated heart rate variability is, in fact, associated with executive control processes (Zahn et al., 2016; Holzman and Bridgett, 2017), including attention shifting, attention shielding and attention focusing (Hansen et al., 2003; Williams et al., 2016; Siennicka et al., 2019; Sorensen et al., 2019). The association between vagally mediated heart rate variability and heartbeat detection may, thus, be mediated by executive control processes that are driven by activity changes in prefrontal and (para-)limbic brain regions.

There are several prefrontal and (para-)limbic brain regions that may mediate the association between vagally mediated heart rate variability and heartbeat detection through executive control processes. These brain regions are organized in networks that are implicated in the up- and downregulation of cardiac changes (Thayer and Lane, 2009), the perception and interpretation of cardiac changes (Schulz, 2016) and the execution of externally and internally oriented attention changes (Petersen and Posner, 2012). Some brain regions are part of more than one network. These brain regions provide functional and structural connections between the networks (Bullmore and Sporns, 2012). Most connections are provided by the anterior cingulate cortex and the insula (Medford and Critchley, 2010). These connections make the anterior cingulate cortex and the insula to central network hubs that coordinate the interplay between the networks (Dosenbach et al., 2007; Sridharan et al., 2008). The anterior cingulate cortex and the insula monitor and regulate the activity of all brain regions in the networks (Barrett and Simmons, 2015), which explains why activity changes in the anterior cingulate cortex and insula are closely associated with vagally mediated heart rate variability (Chang et al., 2013; Allen et al., 2015; Jennings et al., 2016), heartbeat detection (Critchley et al., 2004; Pollatos et al., 2005; Kuehn et al., 2016) and attention (Seeley et al., 2007; Eckert et al., 2009; Shulman et al., 2009). Activity changes in the anterior cingulate cortex and the insula may, thus, trigger executive control processes that mediate the association between vagally mediated heart rate variability and heartbeat detection.

To illustrate the importance of the anterior cingulate cortex and the insula for mediating the association between vagally mediated heart rate variability and heartbeat detection through executive control processes, we only have to take a look at some of the most common mental disorders (Paulus and Stein, 2010). Mood and anxiety disorders are characterized by alterations in vagally mediated heart rate variability (Licht et al., 2008, 2009), heartbeat detection (Pollatos et al., 2009; Dunn et al., 2010) and executive control processes (Mogg et al., 1992, 1995; Lim and Kim, 2005). The alterations in vagally mediated heart rate variability and heartbeat detection are related to alterations in anterior cingulate cortex and insula activity during the perception and interpretation of cardiac changes (Caseras et al., 2013; Avery et al., 2014; Wiebking et al., 2015; Cui et al., 2020; DeVille et al., 2020), presumably via alterations in executive control processes (Mitterschiffthaler et al., 2008; Etkin et al., 2010). The alterations in anterior cingulate cortex and insula activity account for severe alterations in emotion, cognition and behavior (Caseras et al., 2013; Avery et al., 2014; Wiebking et al., 2015; Cui et al., 2020; DeVille et al., 2020), indicating that interoceptive deficits play an important role in the etiology and pathogenesis of mood and anxiety disorders (Paulus and Stein, 2010).

Considering the importance of interoceptive deficits for the etiology and pathogenesis of mood and anxiety disorders (Paulus and Stein, 2010), we need measures that allow us to identify those of us whose interoceptive deficits put them at risk for these disorders. As we have shown, cardiac measures may be useful for this purpose. We combined a cardiac measure of interoceptive accuracy, the heartbeat detection index, with a cardiac measure of prefrontal-(para-)limbic engagement, the vagally mediated heart rate variability index. Combining these measures allowed us to demonstrate that our interoceptive accuracy depends on our prefrontal and (para-)limbic engagement during the perception and interpretation of cardiac changes. It should be noted, however, that we only employed cardiac but not neural measures in our investigation. We, thus, can only assume that vagally mediated heart rate variability reflected prefrontal-(para-)limbic engagement during the perception and interpretation of cardiac changes. Future investigations that supplement cardiac measures with neural measures may help to test this assumption with more rigor (e.g., measuring vagally mediated heart rate variability and heartbeat detection during functional or structural imaging). Future investigations should also employ a more rigorous control of participant characteristics that affect cardiac measures than we did in our investigation (e.g., excluding participants with medication use or caffeine use). We hope that our investigation opens an avenue for these types of investigations because we believe that cardiac measures are a promising tool for researchers in the field of interoception. Cardiac measures can be obtained from unobtrusive heart rate recordings that do not require dedicated staff or equipment. These measures may, thus, be interesting for researchers who need to investigate the association between interoception and prefrontal-(para-)limbic engagement in a time- and cost-efficient manner.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets presented in this article are not readily available because of ethical restrictions. Requests to access the datasets should be directed to AL, alexander.lischke@uni-greifswald.

Ethics Statement

This study was reviewed and approved by the Ethics Committee of the University of Rostock. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in the study.

Author Contributions

AL, RP, and MW designed the study. AM-M and MW collected the data. AL and MW analyzed the data. AL and RP wrote the manuscript. AM-M, MW, and RP contributed to writing, reviewing, and editing of the manuscript. All authors approved the final version of the manuscript.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Funding. Funding for this study was supported by an Open Access Publishing grant that was provided by the German Research Foundation (DFG) and the University of Rostock. AL was supported by a grant provided by the DFG (LI 2517/2-1). The funding source had no further role in the design of the study, in the collection, analysis and interpretation of the data; in the writing of the manuscript; and in the decision to submit the manuscript for publication.

1

1HBDSC=13(1-(|nheartbeatsreal-nheartbeatscounted|)nheartbeatsreal)

2

2HBDGF=13(1-(|nheartbeatsreal-nheartbeatscounted|)(nheartbeatsreal+nheartbeatscounted)/2)

References

  1. Allen B., Jennings J. R., Gianaros P. J., Thayer J. F., Manuck S. B. (2015). Resting high-frequency heart rate variability is related to resting brain perfusion. Psychophysiology 52 277–287. 10.1111/psyp.12321 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Avery J. A., Drevets W. C., Moseman S. E., Bodurka J., Barcalow J. C., Simmons W. K. (2014). Major depressive disorder is associated with abnormal interoceptive activity and functional connectivity in the insula. Biol. Psychiatry 76 258–266. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.11.027 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Barrett L. F., Simmons W. K. (2015). Interoceptive predictions in the brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 16 419–429. 10.1038/nrn3950 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bonnemeier H., Richardt G., Potratz J., Wiegand U. K., Brandes A., Kluge N., et al. (2003). Circadian profile of cardiac autonomic nervous modulation in healthy subjects: differing effects of aging and gender on heart rate variability. J. Cardiovasc. Electrophysiol. 14 791–799. 10.1046/j.1540-8167.2003.03078.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bullmore E., Sporns O. (2012). The economy of brain network organization. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 13 336–349. 10.1038/nrn3214 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Caseras X., Murphy K., Mataix-Cols D., Lopez-Sola M., Soriano-Mas C., Ortriz H., et al. (2013). Anatomical and functional overlap within the insula and anterior cingulate cortex during interoception and phobic symptom provocation. Hum. Brain Mapp. 34 1220–1229. 10.1002/hbm.21503 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Chang C., Metzger C. D., Glover G. H., Duyn J. H., Heinze H. J., Walter M. (2013). Association between heart rate variability and fluctuations in resting-state functional connectivity. Neuroimage 68 93–104. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.11.038 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Craig A. D. (2002). How do you feel? Interoception: the sense of the physiological condition of the body. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 3 655–666. 10.1038/nrn894 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Critchley H. D., Garfinkel S. N. (2017). Interoception and emotion. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 17 7–14. 10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.04.020 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Critchley H. D., Wiens S., Rotshtein P., Ohman A., Dolan R. J. (2004). Neural systems supporting interoceptive awareness. Nat. Neurosci. 7 189–195. 10.1038/nn1176 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Cui H., Zhang B., Li W., Li H., Pang J., Hu Q., et al. (2020). Insula shows abnormal task-evoked and resting-state activity in first-episode drug-naive generalized anxiety disorder. Depress Anxiety 37 632–644. 10.1002/da.23009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Dantas E. M., Kemp A. H., Andreao R. V., da Silva V. J. D., Brunoni A. R., Hoshi R. A., et al. (2018). Reference values for short-term resting-state heart rate variability in healthy adults: Results from the Brazilian Longitudinal Study of Adult Health-ELSA-Brasil study. Psychophysiology 55:e13052. 10.1111/psyp.13052 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. DeVille D. C., Kuplicki R., Stewart J. L., Tulsa I., Aupperle R. L., Bodurka J., et al. (2020). Diminished responses to bodily threat and blunted interoception in suicide attempters. Elife 9:51593. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Dosenbach N. U., Fair D. A., Miezin F. M., Cohen A. L., Wenger K. K., Dosenbach R. A., et al. (2007). Distinct brain networks for adaptive and stable task control in humans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A. 104 11073–11078. 10.1073/pnas.0704320104 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Dunn B. D., Stefanovitch I., Evans D., Oliver C., Hawkins A., Dalgleish T. (2010). Can you feel the beat? Interoceptive awareness is an interactive function of anxiety- and depression-specific symptom dimensions. Behav. Res. Ther. 48 1133–1138. 10.1016/j.brat.2010.07.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Eckert M. A., Menon V., Walczak A., Ahlstrom J., Denslow S., Horwitz A., et al. (2009). At the heart of the ventral attention system: the right anterior insula. Hum. Brain Mapp. 30 2530–2541. 10.1002/hbm.20688 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Etkin A., Prater K. E., Hoeft F., Menon V., Schatzberg A. F. (2010). Failure of anterior cingulate activation and connectivity with the amygdala during implicit regulation of emotional processing in generalized anxiety disorder. Am. J. Psychiatry 167 545–554. 10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.09070931 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Fagius J., Karhuvaara S. (1989). Sympathetic activity and blood pressure increases with bladder distension in humans. Hypertension 14 511–517. 10.1161/01.hyp.14.5.511 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Faul F., Erdfelder E., Buchner A., Lang A. G. (2009). Statistical power analyses using GPower 3.1: tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behav. Res. Methods 41 1149–1160. 10.3758/brm.41.4.1149 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Fustos J., Gramann K., Herbert B. M., Pollatos O. (2013). On the embodiment of emotion regulation: interoceptive awareness facilitates reappraisal. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 8 911–917. 10.1093/scan/nss089 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Garfinkel S. N., Seth A. K., Barrett A. B., Suzuki K., Critchley H. D. (2015). Knowing your own heart: distinguishing interoceptive accuracy from interoceptive awareness. Biol. Psychol. 104 65–74. 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2014.11.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Gross J. J., Jazaieri H. (2014). Emotion, emotion regulation, and psychopathology: An affective science perspective. Clin. Psychol. Sci. 2 387–401. 10.1177/2167702614536164 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  23. Hansen A. L., Johnsen B. H., Thayer J. F. (2003). Vagal influence on working memory and attention. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 48 263–274. 10.1016/s0167-8760(03)00073-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Hart N., McGowan J., Minati L., Critchley H. D. (2013). Emotional regulation and bodily sensation: interoceptive awareness is intact in borderline personality disorder. J. Pers. Disord. 27 506–518. 10.1521/pedi_2012_26_049 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Herbert B. M., Herbert C., Pollatos O. (2011). On the relationship between interoceptive awareness and alexithymia: is interoceptive awareness related to emotional awareness? J. Pers. 79 1149–1175. 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2011.00717.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Holzman J. B., Bridgett D. J. (2017). Heart rate variability indices as bio-markers of top-down self-regulatory mechanisms: a meta-analytic review. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 74(Pt A), 233–255. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.12.032 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Jennings J. R., Sheu L. K., Kuan D. C., Manuck S. B., Gianaros P. J. (2016). Resting state connectivity of the medial prefrontal cortex covaries with individual differences in high-frequency heart rate variability. Psychophysiology 53 444–454. 10.1111/psyp.12586 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Kreibig S. D. (2010). Autonomic nervous system activity in emotion: A review. Biol. Psychol. 84 394–421. 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.03.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Kuehn E., Mueller K., Lohmann G., Schuetz-Bosbach S. (2016). Interoceptive awareness changes the posterior insula functional connectivity profile. Brain Struct. Funct. 221 1555–1571. 10.1007/s00429-015-0989-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Licht C. M., de Geus E. J., van Dyck R., Penninx B. W. (2009). Association between anxiety disorders and heart rate variability in The Netherlands Study of Depression and Anxiety (NESDA). Psychosom. Med. 71 508–518. 10.1097/psy.0b013e3181a292a6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Licht C. M., de Geus E. J., Zitman F. G., Hoogendijk W. J., van Dyck R., Penninx B. W. (2008). Association between major depressive disorder and heart rate variability in the Netherlands Study of Depression and Anxiety (NESDA). Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 65 1358–1367. 10.1001/archpsyc.65.12.1358 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Lim S. L., Kim J. H. (2005). Cognitive processing of emotional information in depression, panic, and somatoform disorder. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 114 50–61. 10.1037/0021-843x.114.1.50 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Lischke A., Jacksteit R., Mau-Moeller A., Pahnke R., Hamm A. O., Weippert M. (2018a). Heart rate variability is associated with psychosocial stress in distinct social domains. J. Psychosom. Res. 106 56–61. 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2018.01.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Lischke A., Mau-Moeller A., Jacksteit R., Pahnke R., Hamm A. O., Weippert M. (2018b). Heart rate variability is associated with social value orientation in males but not females. Sci. Rep. 8:7336. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Lischke A., Pahnke R., Mau-Moeller A., Jacksteit R., Weippert M. (2020a). Sex-specific relationships between interoceptive accuracy and emotion regulation. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 14:67. 10.3389/fnbeh.2020.00067 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Lischke A., Weippert M., Mau-Moeller A., Jacksteit R., Pahnke R. (2020b). Interoceptive accuracy is associated with emotional contagion in a valence- and sex-dependent manner. Soc. Neurosci. 15 227–233. 10.1080/17470919.2019.1690573 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Lischke A., Weippert M., Mau-Moeller A., Paschke S., Jacksteit R., Hamm A. O., et al. (2019). Sex-specific associations between inter-individual differences in heart rate variability and inter-individual differences in emotion regulation. Front. Neurosci. 12:1040. 10.3389/fnins.2018.01040 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Medford N., Critchley H. D. (2010). Conjoint activity of anterior insular and anterior cingulate cortex: awareness and response. Brain Struct. Funct. 214 535–549. 10.1007/s00429-010-0265-x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Mitterschiffthaler M. T., Williams S. C., Walsh N. D., Cleare A. J., Donaldson C., Scott J., et al. (2008). Neural basis of the emotional Stroop interference effect in major depression. Psychol. Med. 38 247–256. 10.1017/s0033291707001523 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Mogg K., Bradley B. P., Williams R. (1995). Attentional bias in anxiety and depression: the role of awareness. Br. J. Clin. Psychol. 34 17–36. 10.1111/j.2044-8260.1995.tb01434.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Mogg K., Mathews A., Eysenck M. (1992). Attentional bias to threat in clinical anxiety states. Cogn. Emot. 6 149–159. 10.1080/02699939208411064 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  42. Owens A. P., Friston K. J., Low D. A., Mathias C. J., Critchley H. D. (2018). Investigating the relationship between cardiac interoception and autonomic cardiac control using a predictive coding framework. Auton. Neurosci. 210 65–71. 10.1016/j.autneu.2018.01.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Paulus M. P., Stein M. B. (2010). Interoception in anxiety and depression. Brain Struct. Funct. 214 451–463. 10.1007/s00429-010-0258-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Petersen S. E., Posner M. I. (2012). The attention system of the human brain: 20 years after. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 35 73–89. 10.1146/annurev-neuro-062111-150525 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Pollatos O., Kirsch W., Schandry R. (2005). Brain structures involved in interoceptive awareness and cardioafferent signal processing: a dipole source localization study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 26 54–64. 10.1002/hbm.20121 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Pollatos O., Schandry R., Auer D. P., Kaufmann C. (2007a). Brain structures mediating cardiovascular arousal and interoceptive awareness. Brain Res. 1141 178–187. 10.1016/j.brainres.2007.01.026 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Pollatos O., Traut-Mattausch E., Schandry R. (2009). Differential effects of anxiety and depression on interoceptive accuracy. Depress Anxiety 26 167–173. 10.1002/da.20504 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Pollatos O., Traut-Mattausch E., Schroeder H., Schandry R. (2007b). Interoceptive awareness mediates the relationship between anxiety and the intensity of unpleasant feelings. J. Anxiety Disord. 21 931–943. 10.1016/j.janxdis.2006.12.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Rossi P., Andriesse G. I., Oey P. L., Wieneke G. H., Roelofs J. M., Akkermans L. M. (1998). Stomach distension increases efferent muscle sympathetic nerve activity and blood pressure in healthy humans. J. Neurol. Sci. 161 148–155. 10.1016/s0022-510x(98)00276-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Ruiz Vargas E., Soros P., Shoemaker J. K., Hachinski V. (2016). Human cerebral circuitry related to cardiac control: A neuroimaging meta-analysis. Ann. Neurol. 79 709–716. 10.1002/ana.24642 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Schandry R. (1981). Heart beat perception and emotional experience. Psychophysiology 18 483–488. 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1981.tb02486.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Schulz S. M. (2016). Neural correlates of heart-focused interoception: a functional magnetic resonance imaging meta-analysis. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 371:20160018. 10.1098/rstb.2016.0018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Seeley W. W., Menon V., Schatzberg A. F., Keller J., Glover G. H., Kenna H., et al. (2007). Dissociable intrinsic connectivity networks for salience processing and executive control. J. Neurosci. 27 2349–2356. 10.1523/jneurosci.5587-06.2007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Shaffer F., Ginsberg J. P. (2017). An overview of heart rate variability metrics and norms. Front. Public Health 5:258. 10.3389/fpubh.2017.00258 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Shah P., Catmur C., Bird G. (2017). From heart to mind: Linking interoception, emotion, and theory of mind. Cortex 93 220–223. 10.1016/j.cortex.2017.02.010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Shah P., Hall R., Catmur C., Bird G. (2016). Alexithymia, not autism, is associated with impaired interoception. Cortex 81 215–220. 10.1016/j.cortex.2016.03.021 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Shulman G. L., Astafiev S. V., Franke D., Pope D. L., Snyder A. Z., McAvoy M. P., et al. (2009). Interaction of stimulus-driven reorienting and expectation in ventral and dorsal frontoparietal and basal ganglia-cortical networks. J. Neurosci. 29 4392–4407. 10.1523/jneurosci.5609-08.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Siennicka A., Quintana D. S., Fedurek P., Wijata A., Paleczny B., Ponikowska B., et al. (2019). Resting heart rate variability, attention and attention maintenance in young adults. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 143 126–131. 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2019.06.017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Smith R., Thayer J. F., Khalsa S. S., Lane R. D. (2017). The hierarchical basis of neurovisceral integration. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 75 274–296. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.02.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Sorensen L., Wass S., Osnes B., Schanche E., Adolfsdottir S., Svendsen J. L., et al. (2019). A psychophysiological investigation of the interplay between orienting and executive control during stimulus conflict: A heart rate variability study. Physiol. Behav. 211:112657. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2019.112657 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Sridharan D., Levitin D. J., Menon V. (2008). A critical role for the right fronto-insular cortex in switching between central-executive and default-mode networks. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A. 105 12569–12574. 10.1073/pnas.0800005105 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Tarvainen M. P., Niskanen J. P., Lipponen J. A., Ranta-Aho P. O., Karjalainen P. A. (2014). Kubios HRV–heart rate variability analysis software. Comput. Methods Programs Biomed. 113 210–220. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology (1996). Heart rate variability standards of measurement, physiological interpretation, and clinical use. Eur. Heart J. 17 354–381. 10.1093/oxfordjournals.eurheartj.a014868 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Thayer J. F., Ahs F., Fredrikson M., Sollers J. J., III, Wager T. D. (2012). A meta-analysis of heart rate variability and neuroimaging studies: implications for heart rate variability as a marker of stress and health. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 36 747–756. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.11.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Thayer J. F., Lane R. D. (2009). Claude Bernard and the heart-brain connection: further elaboration of a model of neurovisceral integration. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 33 81–88. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.08.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Thayer J. F., Sollers J. J., III, Ruiz-Padial E., Vila J. (2002). Estimating respiratory frequency from autoregressive spectral analysis of heart period. IEEE Eng. Med. Biol. Mag. 21 41–45. 10.1109/memb.2002.1032638 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Weippert M., Behrens K., Rieger A., Kumar M., Behrens M. (2015). Effects of breathing patterns and light exercise on linear and nonlinear heart rate variability. Appl. Physiol. Nutr. Metab. 40 762–768. 10.1139/apnm-2014-0493 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Weippert M., Kumar M., Kreuzfeld S., Arndt D., Rieger A., Stoll R. (2010). Comparison of three mobile devices for measuring R-R intervals and heart rate variability: Polar S810i. Suunto t6 and an ambulatory ECG system. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. 109 779–786. 10.1007/s00421-010-1415-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Wiebking C., de Greck M., Duncan N. W., Tempelmann C., Bajbouj M., Northoff G. (2015). Interoception in insula subregions as a possible state marker for depression-an exploratory fMRI study investigating healthy, depressed and remitted participants. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 9:82. 10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00082 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Williams D. P., Thayer J. F., Koenig J. (2016). Resting cardiac vagal tone predicts intraindividual reaction time variability during an attention task in a sample of young and healthy adults. Psychophysiology 53 1843–1851. 10.1111/psyp.12739 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Yamasaki Y., Kodama M., Matsuhisa M., Kishimoto M., Ozaki H., Tani A., et al. (1996). Diurnal heart rate variability in healthy subjects: effects of aging and sex difference. Am. J. Physiol. 271(1 Pt 2), H303–H310. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Zahn D., Adams J., Krohn J., Wenzel M., Mann C. G., Gomille L. K., et al. (2016). Heart rate variability and self-control–A meta-analysis. Biol. Psychol. 115 9–26. 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2015.12.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets presented in this article are not readily available because of ethical restrictions. Requests to access the datasets should be directed to AL, alexander.lischke@uni-greifswald.


Articles from Frontiers in Neuroscience are provided here courtesy of Frontiers Media SA

RESOURCES