Skip to main content
Brain Communications logoLink to Brain Communications
. 2020 Dec 15;3(1):fcaa202. doi: 10.1093/braincomms/fcaa202

Biomarkers of memory variability in traumatic brain injury

Richard Adamovich-Zeitlin 1, Paul A Wanda 1, Ethan Solomon 2, Tung Phan 1, Bradley Lega 3, Barbara C Jobst 4, Robert E Gross 5, Kan Ding 6, Ramon Diaz-Arrastia 7, Michael J Kahana 1,
PMCID: PMC7850041  PMID: 33543140

Abstract

Traumatic brain injury is a leading cause of cognitive disability and is often associated with significant impairment in episodic memory. In traumatic brain injury survivors, as in healthy controls, there is marked variability between individuals in memory ability. Using recordings from indwelling electrodes, we characterized and compared the oscillatory biomarkers of mnemonic variability in two cohorts of epilepsy patients: a group with a history of moderate-to-severe traumatic brain injury (n = 37) and a group of controls without traumatic brain injury (n = 111) closely matched for demographics and electrode coverage. Analysis of these recordings demonstrated that increased high-frequency power and decreased theta power across a broad set of brain regions mark periods of successful memory formation in both groups. As features in a logistic-regression classifier, spectral power biomarkers effectively predicted recall probability, with little difference between traumatic brain injury patients and controls. The two groups also displayed similar patterns of theta-frequency connectivity during successful encoding periods. These biomarkers of successful memory, highly conserved between traumatic brain injury patients and controls, could serve as the basis for novel therapies that target disordered memory across diverse forms of neurological disease.

Keywords: traumatic brain injury, episodic memory, recall, theta, intracranial EEG


Traumatic brain injury (TBI) is a leading cause of cognitive disability and often associated with episodic memory impairment. Adamovich-Zeitlin et al. reveal that neural biomarkers of variable mnemonic function (see figure) appear highly conserved in epileptic patients with and without significant TBI history, which may guide future neuromodulatory therapies.

Graphical Abstract

Graphical Abstract.

Graphical Abstract

Introduction

Traumatic brain injury (TBI) produces lasting impairments in episodic memory. Episodic memory refers to our ability to associate information within a spatio-temporal context and to use contextual cues to retrieve information from memory. Episodic memory is also autobiographical; it places ‘us’ in our memories, giving us the sense that something happened to us at a particular time and in a particular place (Tulving, 1985). Neuropsychological tests reveal some of the most profound functional impairments in measures of delayed recall, a particularly sensitive index of episodic memory function (Dikmen et al., 2009; Vakil, 2020). Cognitive rehabilitation therapy has been the primary treatment strategy for patients suffering from memory and executive dysfunction, but therapeutic evidence has been mixed, leading to continued uncertainty over clinical best practices (Shoulson et al., 2012), and no medication constitutes a clinical standard to address cognitive impairment in the TBI population (Cicerone et al., 2006). To develop effective therapies for treating such memory loss, we must first understand whether and how the physiology of memory function differs between TBI-affected individuals and their non-TBI counterparts.

TBI is a heterogeneous injury, resulting in both focal and diffuse pathologies, and the mechanisms of memory dysfunction may be distinct from those resulting from developmental or neurodegenerative conditions. Post-traumatic axonal degeneration may contribute to hippocampal atrophy, reduced volume of white matter and passive ventricular expansion (Bigler and Maxwell, 2011). Structural and functional neuroimaging studies in patients with moderate-to-severe TBI have highlighted white matter lesions (Smith et al., 2003) and reduced connectivity among the frontal, temporal and parietal lobes (Wang et al., 2011; Pandit et al., 2013). This pattern of damage, known as diffuse axonal injury, may underlie slowed cognitive processing (Spitz et al., 2013).

Although neuroimaging studies provide an important first step towards understanding the changes in brain structure and networks in TBI, they do not provide critical information as to whether or how TBI alters the electrical activity underlying affected cognitive processes such as episodic memory. Indeed, research remains ongoing regarding the effectiveness of electroencephalography (EEG) as a diagnostic tool for brain injury (Nuwer et al., 2005; Slobounov et al., 2012; Edlow et al., 2017). Given the critical importance of electrophysiology for tracking the temporal dynamics of memory function, and the potential of using such biomarkers for neuromodulatory therapies such as direct brain stimulation to alter brain activity and behaviour (Ezzyat et al., 2018; Hanslmayr et al., 2019), we sought to address this gap in our knowledge by investigating the neural correlates of episodic memory using direct brain recordings in epileptic patients with and without a history of moderate-to-severe TBI.

Previous work by multiple research groups has revealed a clear set of physiological biomarkers of intraindividual variability in human memory function. Specifically, it is now well established that high-frequency activity (generally >30 Hz) increases across a broad network of brain regions during periods of successful memory encoding and retrieval (Burke et al., 2015; Greenberg et al., 2015; Kragel et al., 2017). Similarly, increases in theta band (3–8 Hz) functional connectivity, accompanied by decreases in theta band power, mark periods of successful encoding and retrieval in both the medial temporal lobe (Solomon et al., 2019; Hanslmayr et al., 2020) and neocortex (Burke et al., 2013; Solomon et al., 2017).

The present study aims to determine whether these biomarkers of variability in memory function are conserved across TBI and non-TBI cohorts. As direct brain recordings may only be ethically obtained in human volunteers undergoing intracranial EEG for indications such as intractable epilepsy, and because these patients occasionally have significant prior history of TBI [a known risk factor in developing epilepsy (Verellen and Cavazos, 2010; Ding et al., 2016)], we conducted a careful chart review of more than 300 neurosurgical patients who performed memory testing as part of a multi-centre memory study. We identified 37 patients with a history of moderate-to-severe TBI and 111 matched controls. Although findings from this public dataset have been reported in prior publications, none have considered the potential relation between TBI history and biomarkers of successful memory.

Materials and methods

We analysed direct neural recordings from cortical and deep brain structures in patient-subjects while they performed verbal episodic memory tasks. These patients were surgically implanted with intra-parenchymal recording electrodes to directly measure intracranial EEG activity. We quantified each subject’s neural biomarkers of successful memory encoding by analysing two distinct features of the EEG time-series across all implanted electrodes during memory encoding: spectral power across a broad range of frequencies and phase-locking connectivity in the theta (3–8 Hz) band. We compared these features between the groups to discern if a history of moderate-to-severe TBI was associated with a difference in the electrophysiology of successful memory encoding. We then asked if these features could be used to reliably predict successful memory encoding in these TBI patients.

Human subjects

We examined data from 148 neurosurgical patients (105 male, mean age = 41) with medication-resistant epilepsy undergoing invasive monitoring for seizure localization. Neurosurgeons implanted patients with intra-parenchymal depth electrodes and/or subdural grids and strips placed on the cortical surface. The location of these electrodes was determined based solely upon clinical considerations. As such, there are no two patients with the same electrode configuration, but when aggregated, the coverage is expansive across the entire brain (Fig. 1B). The included data were collected as part of a larger project in collaboration with Columbia University Medical Center (New York, NY), Dartmouth-Hitchcock Medical Center (Hanover, NH), Emory University Hospital (Atlanta, GA), Hospital of the University of Pennsylvania (Philadelphia, PA), the Mayo Clinic (Rochester, MN), Thomas Jefferson University Hospital (Philadelphia, PA), the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (Washington, DC) and University of Texas Southwestern Medical Center (Dallas, TX). Institutional review boards at the respective hospitals approved the research protocol, and we obtained informed consent from each patient.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Memory task overview and performance. (A) Memory task schematic. In the delayed free-recall task, subjects encoded a list of 12 items (words) presented sequentially, followed by an arithmetic distractor and verbal free recall, for up to 26 lists in a session. In a given session, all study lists contained either unrelated items or items belonging to one of three semantic categories. (B) Localized intracranial electrodes overlaid on an average brain surface. Electrodes are coloured by patient group (orange: TBI group, blue: non-TBI group). (C) Recall performance. Serial position curves plot the average probability of recall at each list position for the two groups. The TBI group trended towards lower recall performance compared to the non-TBI group [χ2(1) = 3.2, P =0.07]. The TBI group has significantly decreased recall performance in the categorized task {group × task interaction: [χ2(1) = 5.87, P =0.015]}. (D) Temporal clustering in recall. For each subject, we plot temporal clustering scores, with the group mean and standard error. The dotted line at 0.5 represents random chance. Both groups show temporal organization of their recall sequence, with no statistical difference between groups [t(217) = −0.84, P =0.40]. (E) Semantic clustering in recall. For each subject performing the categorized task, we plot the adjusted ratio of clustering score with group mean and standard error. Maximum clustering at 1 and no clustering at 0. Both groups exhibit semantic clustering of their recall sequences, with no significant difference between groups [t(103) = 0.04, P =0.96]

Through careful review of medical records, a subset (n =37) of patients (TBI group) were identified as having a history of moderate-to-severe TBI based on criteria developed by neurologists at the University of Pennsylvania (Dr R.D-A.) and University of Texas Southwestern (Dr K.D.), as follows: a reported history of significant head injury with either loss of consciousness of at least 30 min or post-traumatic amnesia of 24 h or more, or a neuroradiology report describing lesions characteristic of TBI (Gupta et al., 2014). In this dataset, we identified 37 patients whose history indicated that they likely experienced a moderate-to-severe TBI. For 21 of these patients, we obtained hospital records confirming all of the above criteria, for the remaining 17 patients, a careful review of hospital records conducted by two independent neurologists specializing in the treatment of TBI indicated a likely history of moderate-to-severe TBI although the hospital records lacked documentation confirming that the full set of criteria had been met. In Table 1, we provide demographics for the TBI group.

Table 1.

TBI group demographics

Age Sex Handedness Age of seizure onset
TBI1 31 F R 1
TBI2 39 M R 16
TBI3 48 M R 33
TBI4 34 M R 1
TBI5 24 M R 16
TBI6 35 M R 15
TBI7 25 M R 17
TBI8 57 M R 6
TBI9 53 M R 29
TBI10 28 M R 12
TBI11 31 F R 1
TBI12 38 M R 33
TBI13 43 F R 12
TBI14 44 M R 17
TBI15 37 M R 34
TBI16 47 M R 44
TBI17 57 M R 48
TBI18 33 M R 13
TBI19 49 M L 1
TBI20 36 M R 2
TBI21 55 M R 37
TBI22 29 M L 4
TBI23 48 M A 19
TBI24 60 M R 50
TBI25 50 M R 39
TBI26 55 M L 20
TBI27 58 F A 9
TBI28 45 M R 18
TBI29 66 F L 30
TBI30 51 F R 11
TBI31 35 M R 28
TBI32 54 M A 39
TBI33 51 M R 35
TBI34 47 M R 22
TBI35 30 M R 20
TBI36 32 M R 27
TBI37 32 F R 9

We identified the best matched control patients (non-TBI group) from the database (e.g. with no reported history of TBI) for each of the TBI subjects, by implementing a propensity score matching procedure (Rosenbaum and Rubin, 1983). First, we used a generalized linear model to identify a set of relevant demographic and descriptive factors predictive of whether a patient was included in the TBI group: sex, handedness, prior resection, years of education, age at seizure onset, electrode coverage by brain region and seizure onset zone by brain region. We constructed propensity score models (generalized linear models) of the above factors, with the regression estimating a similarity score for each non-TBI subject. We inputted the scores to the Matching package in R, ‘Multivariate and propensity score matching with balance optimization’ (Sekhon, 2011), to determine the three best matches for each TBI subject, with exact matching required for handedness as an indicator of hemispheric dominance. This algorithm resulted in a 111 subject non-TBI group with characteristics that did not significantly differ from the TBI group, as highlighted in Table 2.

Table 2.

Significant model covariates predictive of TBI

Age (mean ± SD) Male (%) Right frontal coverage (%) Left temporal SOZ (%) Right temporal SOZ (%)
TBI (n = 37) 42.9 ± 11.0 73.0 75.0 2.8 5.6
Match group (n = 111) 40.0 ± 11.5 70.3 73.3 2.9 5.7
All non-TBI (n = 309) 36.4 ± 11.3 65.4 65.4 15.1 11.0

Recall tasks and behavioural analyses

Subjects completed between one and eight sessions (30–60 min duration) of a delayed free-recall task (Kahana, 2020). On each of up to 26 lists (per session), subjects studied 12 items, with each item appearing individually for 1.6 s on a laptop display (a 0.75–1-s inter-stimulus interval followed each item presentation). After the appearance of the last item, subjects performed a 20-s distractor task in which they solved a series of self-paced simple arithmetic problems. Next, a row of asterisks appeared, accompanied by a 1-s tone, to signal the start of a 30-s recall period, in which subjects freely recalled as many items as they could remember without regard to order of presentation.

Subjects participated in either, or both, of two variants of the free-recall task described above in which the studied items were either unrelated words or semantically related (categorized) words. See Fig. 1A for a schematic of one typical list from each variant of the task. In the unrelated task, lists composed of 12 common nouns drawn randomly and without replacement from a pool of 300 words chosen to have moderate levels of memorability. For details on list construction, please see Ezzyat et al. (2018). In categorized sessions, each 12-word list comprised four noun exemplars drawn from each of three taxonomic categories, with same category exemplars appearing in successively presented pairs such that the structure of a given list might be A1, A2, B1, B2, C1, C2, B3, B4, A3, A4, C3, and C4. Pairs appeared in a random order constrained such that successive pairs always came from distinct categories. For details on category list composition, see Weidemann et al. (2019). As is customary in list memory experiments, we did not include data from the first (practice) list of each session in our subsequent analyses (Solway et al., 2012).

To assess the effect of prior injury history on recall performance, we used a linear mixed-effects model with group (TBI versus non-TBI), task and serial position as fixed effects and random intercepts across subjects and performed a likelihood ratio test, comparing the log-likelihood of the full model to a reduced model that did not include the main effect of group. To determine the significance of the interaction between group and task, we fit a second model that included interactions for all of the main effects and compared it to a reduced model (without the group-task interaction) using the same likelihood ratio test described above. These analyses were programmed using the statsmodels library in python (Seabold and Perktold, 2010).

When freely recalling lists of items, the order of recalls reveals the dynamic, cue-dependent nature of memory retrieval. Sequentially recalled items exhibit several forms of ‘organization’, clustering according to their temporal and semantic relations (Kahana, 1996; Howard and Kahana, 2002). To quantify temporal organization (Polyn et al., 2009), the temporal distance of each recall transition is compared to the distribution of temporal distances for all words that have not yet been recalled, in which 1.0 represents perfect temporal clustering and 0.5 represents chance. The average percentile score of all recalls for each subject is their temporal recall clustering score. For semantic clustering, we calculated an adjusted ratio of clustering statistic (Roenker et al., 1971; Weidemann et al., 2019), in which a score of 1 represents maximum category clustering, and 0 represents clustering based on chance. For this analysis, we only included subjects from each group that completed the categorized variant of the task. For both metrics of recall, clustering scores for the two groups were statistically compared using a Welch’s independent t-test, which does not assume equal variance.

Intracranial recordings

We recorded EEG signals using varied clinical systems (Nihon-Kohden Neurofax EEG-1200, Grass Aura LTM64, XLTek EMU128FS, Natus Quantum) and research recording systems (Blackrock NeuroPort, Medtronic External Neural Stimulator) at sampling rates ranging from 250 to 2000 Hz. These data were either recorded at individual electrodes referenced to a common contact placed intracranially, on the scalp, or mastoid process and re-referenced to a bipolar referencing scheme or recorded directly using a bipolar referencing montage. In the bipolar scheme, neighbouring contacts were referenced within each electrode. EEG data were aligned with behavioural data via transistor–transistor logic pulses sent from the behavioural computer to the EEG system, or through network packets passed between the behavioural computer and recording computer. The data were filtered using a fourth order Butterworth notch filter to remove 60 Hz line noise. The resulting signals were convolved with Morlet wavelets (wave number = 5) to obtain spectral power and phase measurements. As determined by a clinician, any contacts placed in epileptogenic tissue or exhibiting frequent inter-ictal spiking were excluded from all subsequent analyses.

To compare the activity in brain regions of interest, each electrode was localized to the patient’s specific anatomy by alignment and co-registration of pre-surgical T1 and T2 MRIs and post-implantation CT scans, including an automated segmentation of medial temporal lobe substructures (Yushkevich et al., 2015). Depth electrodes that were visible on CT scans were then localized within all the brain regions defined by the Desikan–Killiany Atlas (Desikan et al., 2006). Exposed recording contacts were ∼1–2 mm in diameter and 1–2.5 mm in length; the smallest recording contacts used were 0.8 mm in diameter and 1.4 mm in length.

Analysis of spectral power and functional connectivity

To determine the relation between power and successful encoding in TBI and non-TBI subjects, we calculated the subsequent memory effect (SME) as the difference in spectral power (i.e. the power SME) for later recalled and not-recalled items. First, we calculated spectral power by convolving the raw EEG signal recorded during item presentation with 20 Morlet wavelets logarithmically spaced between 3 and 170 Hz. The resulting power values were log-transformed and Z-scored within each session and averaged over the 400–1100 ms interval following item onset, resulting in a single power value for each item/frequency/electrode. We used a Welch’s t-test to quantify, for each electrode and frequency, the difference between later recalled and not-recalled items. Here, a positive t-statistic indicates an increase in power during successful memory encoding; a negative t-statistic indicates a decrease. Next, we averaged these SME t-statistics within four anatomical brain regions (frontal, medial temporal, lateral temporal and parietal) and two frequency bands: theta (4–8 Hz) and high-frequency activity (45–170 Hz). To assess differences in the average SME between the TBI and the non-TBI group, we used a Welch’s t-test with Benjamini–Hochberg false discovery rate correction for multiple comparisons.

To assess differences in inter-regional connectivity in TBI and non-TBI subjects, we constructed a theta connectivity network for each subject by calculating the phase-locking value, or PLV (Lachaux et al., 1999) for every pair of electrodes. To compute the PLV, we extracted phase information during the 400–1100 ms interval following item onset using five wavelets in the theta band (4, 5, 6, 7 and 8 Hz), calculated the average phase difference between each recording pair for every item and measured the resultant vector (i.e. PLV) of phase differences for each item class (i.e. recalled versus not recalled). We calculated the connectivity SME as the difference in PLV between these item classes, for each pair of electrodes and frequency in the theta band. A positive connectivity SME indicates greater synchronization at a given frequency between the pair of electrodes during successful memory encoding, and a negative SME represents greater synchronization during unsuccessful memory encoding. For each subject, we averaged the connectivity SME across all five frequencies to measure the overall theta-band connectivity SME and pooled across all electrode pairs that spanned each unique region of interest (ROI) pair, yielding a theta connectivity SME for every ROI pair in the network.

To account for the imbalanced number of recalled and not-recalled items, we performed a non-parametric permutation test to compare the theta connectivity SME to a chance distribution. To generate a null (or chance) distribution of theta connectivity SME values for each electrode pair and subject, we randomly shuffled the item labels (i.e. recalled, not recalled) 500 times and repeated the PLV computation for each shuffle. For each electrode pair, a Z-score was derived by comparing the true theta connectivity SME value to its corresponding shuffled distribution. This Z-score was taken to represent an edge weight of the connectivity network for each electrode pair in every subject, which were then averaged across electrode pairs that spanned ROI pairs. A positive Z-score indicates an increase in synchronization between a pair of brain regions during successful encoding; a negative Z-score indicates increased synchronization during unsuccessful encoding. To assess whether each group exhibited increased synchronization during successful memory encoding, we calculated an average edge weight across all ROI pairs for each subject, and compared the distribution of weights for each group to zero with a one-sample t-test. We used an independent t-test to compare the distributions of edge weights between the TBI and non-TBI groups.

Multivariate classification methods

To discriminate patterns of spectral power predictive of successful memory encoding during the free-recall task, we trained a logistic-regression classifier using methods described in Ezzyat et al. (2018). The input features were average spectral power for each item encoding epoch (0–1366 ms relative to item onset) calculated at each of eight frequencies log-spaced between 3 and 180 Hz and each electrode (frequency x electrode). The logistic-regression model used L2-regularization with a penalty parameter (C) to 2.4 × 104 that was selected by a cross-validation procedure on historical subjects via a grid search to correct for over-fitting. Given the possible class imbalance between later recalled and not-recalled items depending upon subject performance, we weighted observations from the minority class in inverse proportion to the class frequency when training the classifier.

We quantified classifier performance by computing the average area under the curve (AUC), which measures the model’s ability to identify true positives while minimizing false positives, with chance AUC = 0.50. To mitigate the effects of over-fitting on our evaluation of classifier performance, we used leave-one-out cross-validation when calculating AUC. For subjects performing multiple sessions of free recall, we used a leave-one-session-out method; for subjects with a single session, a leave-one-list-out method was used. To assess the significance of each subject’s individual classifier, we randomly permuted the recalled/not-recalled labels in the training data and computed the AUC 1000 times to generate a null distribution. Classification analyses were programmed using the scikit-learn library in python (Pedregosa et al., 2011).

Data availability

De-identified data and analysis code is made available at: http://memory.psych.upenn.edu/Electrophysiological_Data.

Results

To support our analysis of the behavioural and physiological effects of TBI on memory biomarkers, we generated a matched group of controls over a set of relevant variables including age, sex, hemispheric dominance, seizure onset zone, and electrode coverage (see Materials and Methods). This yielded a non-TBI group of 111 subjects whose characteristics did not reliably differ from those of the 37 subject TBI group (see Table 2). Figure 1A illustrates our two memory paradigms: delayed free recall of (i) categorized and (ii) unrelated lists of nouns. In both paradigms, subjects studied lists composed of 12 words for later recall, with each list followed by a brief arithmetic distractor task. Categorized lists comprised four exemplars drawn from each of three categories presented as successive pairs of categorized words. Unrelated lists were composed of words drawn randomly and without replacement from a pool of common nouns (see Materials and Methods). As seen in Fig. 1B, electrodes across both TBI and non-TBI subjects were distributed across similar regions, with the greatest density of electrodes sampling the temporal, medial temporal and frontal regions.

We first asked whether the TBI and non-TBI groups differed in terms of their behavioural performance on our two memory tasks. Both groups exhibited typical serial position effects, recalling a higher proportion of early list items (the primacy effect) and a lower proportion of late-list items, which are attenuated in memory by the post-list arithmetic distractor task (see Fig. 1C and Materials and Methods). Overall, TBI patients recalled fewer items than their non-TBI counterparts during both tasks, though this effect fell short of the significance threshold [χ2(1) = 3.2, P =0.07]. Using a linear mixed-effects model, we found a significant interaction between task (categorized versus unrelated) and group (TBI versus non-TBI), in which the non-TBI group outperformed the TBI group to a greater extent during the categorized list paradigm [χ2(1) = 5.87, P =0.015]. This finding is consistent with prior evidence of TBI-related impairment of semantic organization in verbal delayed free recall (Vakil et al., 2019). Further, analysis of both groups’ recall dynamics revealed significant temporal and semantic clustering, recapitulating well-characterized episodic memory dynamics. List items were more likely to be successively recalled if they were studied in nearby list positions [temporal: TBI, t(51) = 10.6, P < 0.01; non-TBI, t(166) = 18.2, P < 0.01] (Fig. 1D) and came from the same category [semantic: TBI t(28) = 31.6, P < 0.01; non-TBI t(75) = 66.2, P < 0.01]. There was no significant difference between the two groups for these measures of recall organization [temporal: t(217) = −0.84, P =0.40; semantic: t(103) = 0.04, P =0.96] (Fig. 1E).

To determine whether the neural activity underlying successful memory encoding differed between TBI and non-TBI cohorts, we employed the SME paradigm. The SME compares neural measures during encoding of items that were subsequently recalled with items that were subsequently not recalled, reflecting activity that correlates with memory success. As prior work has established that increased high-frequency activity (45–170 Hz) and increased inter-regional theta synchrony mark periods of successful memory encoding (Burke et al., 2014; Solomon et al., 2017), we conducted this subsequent memory contrast for two key biomarkers of successful memory formation: spectral power and theta (4–8 Hz) phase connectivity, asking whether these particular biomarkers serve as indices of successful memory encoding in subjects with a history of moderate-to-severe TBI.

We first compared patterns of whole-brain spectral power SMEs between the TBI and non-TBI groups. To do this, we computed spectral power at a broad range of frequencies (3–170 Hz) during the item encoding period, for each electrode. We then statistically compared the distribution of powers for items that were later recalled to powers for items that were not recalled, generating a spectral power SME t-statistic (see Materials and Methods). Figure 2A illustrates the average power SME t-statistic at 20 different frequencies and 4 brain regions associated with episodic memory (frontal lobe, medial temporal lobe, lateral temporal cortex and parietal lobe) in the two patient subgroups. As in prior studies, we found a significant increase in high-frequency activity (P < 0.05; Benjamini–Hochberg corrected) in both the TBI and non-TBI groups in all regions except the lateral temporal cortex. We further noted a significant decrease in low frequency activity (P < 0.05; Benjamini–Hochberg corrected) in all four brain regions in both groups. There were no significant differences in SMEs between the two groups at any brain region in the theta and high-frequency activity frequency bands (P > 0.1) as seen in Fig. 2B.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Biomarkers of successful memory encoding. (A) Average spectral power SME t-statistics (recalled versus non-recalled encoding periods) at 20 frequencies spanning 3–170 Hz and four distinct brain regions [frontal lobe, medial temporal lobe (MTL), lateral temporal cortex (LTC) and parietal lobe]. Both groups exhibit a significant increase in high-frequency activity (HFA), a significant decrease in low frequency activity during successful encoding [P < 0.05] (B) and statistically similar power SME in every ROI (P > 0.1). (C) Group average theta encoding networks overlaid on an average brain surface. Each node represents a distinct ROI. Connections (edges) between nodes are coloured red when theta synchronization increases during successful memory encoding and blue where synchronization decreases during encoding. (D) Average synchronization Z-score (‘edge weight’) distributions, with positive values reflecting overall memory-related synchronization. Across subjects, we found a significant increase in theta synchronization with successful memory encoding for the non-TBI group [t(110) = 2.20, P =0.03] but not for the TBI group [t(36) = 0.66, P =0.51]; there was no evidence of a difference between the two groups [t(146) = 0.65, P =0.52]

To assess for differences in functional networks underlying successful memory encoding between TBI and non-TBI patients, we analysed patterns of inter-regional phase connectivity in the theta band. To construct networks of connectivity between electrodes in each subject’s brain, we calculated the PLV, which quantifies the phase synchronization in the theta band (4–8 Hz) for every ROI pair. We calculated a connectivity SME Z-score, reflecting the difference in PLV between successful and unsuccessful item encoding events; a positive value indicates increased phase synchrony between ROIs during successful encoding (see Materials and Methods). Connections with Z-scores greater than 1.96 are displayed in Fig. 2C, where red lines represent memory-related increases in connectivity, while blue lines represent memory-related decreases in connectivity. Qualitatively, these maps show strong fronto-temporal synchrony during successful memory encoding in both TBI and non-TBI groups.

To determine if these theta connectivity networks reliably differed between TBI and non-TBI groups, we took the average PLV Z-score across all connections (or ‘edge weights’) in each subject as a measure of overall synchronization during good memory states. Congruent with previous findings, both the TBI and non-TBI subject groups showed a general increase in theta synchronization during successful encoding; average Z-scores were above 0 in both groups, indicating an overall propensity for memory-related synchronization. As seen in Fig. 2D, the non-TBI group had Z-score averages that were significantly higher than zero [t(110) = 2.20, P =0.03], though the TBI group did not meet significance [t(36) = 0.66, P =0.51]. However, we did not find a significant difference in the average Z-score between the two groups [t(146) = 0.65, P =0.52].

The ability to reliably predict subsequent memory in real-time is potentially a critical tool for designing new therapies and interventions. Here, we trained an individual multivariate logistic-regression classifier for each subject to discriminate patterns of spectral power during memory encoding, effectively forecasting whether each item would be later recalled or forgotten. In Fig. 3, we plot the average true positive and false positive rate for probability thresholds ranging from 0 to 1 as receiver operating characteristic curves. To quantify classifier performance, we computed the average AUC for each subject’s classifier and averaged within each group. Overall, the TBI group had an average AUC of 0.624 [standard error of the mean (SEM) = 0.011], and the non-TBI group had an average AUC of 0.620 (SEM = 0.008) with statistically equivalent distributions [t(142) = 0.29, P =0.77]. This result suggests that spectral power features of recorded intracranial EEG can effectively predict memory performance in both TBI and non-TBI patients and could be used to responsively trigger closed-loop brain stimulation.

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Classifier performance. For each unique subject, receiver operating characteristic curves show the performance of a logistic-regression classifier tested on held-out sessions of the delayed free-recall task (grey lines), with the group average performance shown in bold (TBI group, left: mean AUC = 0.624, n =37, SEM = 0.011; non-TBI group, right: mean AUC = 0.620, n =111, SEM = 0.008). The distributions of the two groups were statistically equivalent [t(142) = 0.29, P =0.77]

Discussion

Our mnemonic ability exhibits marked variability from moment to moment and from day to day, independent of item characteristics and task variables (Kahana et al., 2018). Measures of brain activity similarly reflect this behavioural variation, constituting neural biomarkers of mnemonic success. Direct brain recordings from neurosurgical patients have revealed two striking biomarkers of effective memory encoding: First, the power spectrum of intracranial EEG activity tilts upward during periods of successful encoding. Second, theta-frequency neural activity (3–10 Hz) becomes coherent and synchronous during periods of successful encoding (Burke et al., 2013; Solomon et al., 2017). These two patterns emerge across a broad network of brain regions, described as the ‘core memory network’ (Wagner, 1998; Paller and Wagner, 2002; Kim, 2011; Burke et al., 2014; Long and Kahana, 2015). Here, we examined these two biomarkers of successful memory function in matched groups of neurosurgical patients with and without a significant prior history of TBI.

TBI produces diverse and heterogeneous damage to neural networks including diffuse axonal injury, diffuse microvascular injury and neuroinflammation, and ultimately, impaired memory ability in many patients. As such, we wondered whether patients who experienced moderate-to-severe TBIs would exhibit differences in their biomarkers of successful memory function.

To answer this question, we recorded from indwelling electrodes in cohorts of TBI-affected and non-TBI neurosurgical patients as they performed verbal free-recall tasks. Although our TBI group exhibited marginally worse recall performance, both groups showed nearly identical serial position effects, temporal and semantic clustering (see Fig. 1C–E). Both groups also exhibited highly similar electrophysiological correlates of successful memory (Fig. 2), suggesting that biomarkers of local memory processing were unaffected by TBI neuropathology. Finally, we examined the utility of spectral power biomarkers of successful memory in reliably predicting recall performance. We found that this approach yielded statistically equivalent classification ability for both the TBI and non-TBI groups, with both groups showing statistically reliable classification (AUCs of 0.0624 and 0.0620 for TBI and non-TBI groups, respectively).

The surgical treatment of refractory epilepsy occasionally requires long-term implantation of electrodes for seizure localization. By generously volunteering to take part in cognitive studies, patients undergoing such treatment have afforded scientists an unprecedented view of the neural basis of human cognition. Whereas only a handful of epilepsy centres conducted this type of cognitive electrophysiological research at the dawn of the 21st century, we are now seeing scientific reports from most of the major epilepsy centres worldwide. With this advancing research program characterizing the neural correlates of diverse cognitive processes, the neuroscientific community must consider whether these findings generalize beyond the population of epilepsy patients. This question of generalization has obvious scientific and therapeutic implications.

Here, we sought to determine whether spectral correlates of successful memory, studied and extensively documented in epilepsy patients (Ezzyat et al., 2017; Herweg et al., 2020), generalize to those patients with an additional history of TBI, or if TBI-related pathology produces distinct biomarkers of variability in memory function. In conducting this study, we did not know what to expect. Given that a history of TBI suggests an underlying pathology, which differs from that seen in non-TBI epilepsy patients (Bigler and Maxwell, 2011), we did not expect to find such striking similarities among the two groups.

The striking similarity between biomarkers of successful memory in TBI and non-TBI groups helps to address a fundamental question in human neuroscience; namely, do findings in patients with epilepsy-related pathology generalize to other clinical subgroups? The invariance of biomarkers across clinical subgroups bolsters arguments that task-manipulation contrasts identify neural processes that more likely reflect normal brain function (Kahana et al., 1999, 2001). That is, although TBI and epilepsy-related pathology both disrupt neural mechanisms that support memory, these mechanisms still function to some extent, and comparisons of successful and unsuccessful items help to identify these residual functions. Of course, this hypothesis need not be true, and future work across more diverse patient populations may well temper the claims laid out in this paper.

Although our study can only speak to the similarity of a particular set of biomarkers of memory across two distinct epilepsy subgroups, this particular inter-group comparison has important clinical implications. Recent studies have shown that closed-loop neuromodulatory therapy, such as direct brain stimulation, in refractory epilepsy patients can reliably boost memory, albeit to a modest degree (Ezzyat et al., 2018), and closed-loop DBS promises to improve existing therapies in other patient populations, such as Parkinson’s (Swann et al., 2018). Such findings raise hope for the use of closed-loop electrical stimulation as a therapy for memory loss in patients with brain injury or other types of neurological or neurogenerative diseases. Our findings suggest that biomarkers used to guide such therapy appear conserved in at least some epilepsy patients with an additional history of TBI, paving the way for future tests of closed-loop neuromodulatory therapies in TBI patients.

Acknowledgements

We thank Blackrock Microsystems and Medtronic for providing neural recording equipment. We owe a special thanks to the patients and their families for their selfless participation and support of the study. We thank Nora Herweg for her valuable feedback on this work.

Funding

This work was supported by the Defense Advanced Research Projects Agency (DARPA) Restoring Active Memory (RAM) program (Cooperative Agreement N66001-14-2-4032). The views, opinions and/or findings contained in this material are those of the authors and should not be interpreted as representing the official views of the Department of Defense or the United States Government.

Competing interests

M.J.K. holds a greater than 5% equity interest in Nia Therapeutics Inc., a company intended to develop and commercialize brain stimulation therapies for memory restoration. R.E.G. and R.D. each hold a less than 5% equity interest in Nia Therapeutics, Inc. B.C.J. receives research funding from NeuroPace, Medtronic, and Hardvard Pilgrim, Inc. not relating to this research. R.E.G. serves as a consultant to Medtronic, which was a subcontractor on the DARPA RAM project, and receives compensation for these services. The terms of this arrangement have been reviewed and approved by Emory University in accordance with its conflict of interest policies. All other authors declare no competing financial interests.

Glossary

AUC =

area under the curve

PLV =

phase-locking value

ROI =

region of interest

SEM =

standard error of the mean

SME =

subsequent memory effect

TBI =

traumatic brain injury

References

  1. Bigler ED, Maxwell WL.. Neuroimaging and neuropathology of TBI. NeuroRehabilitation 2011; 28: 63–74. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Burke JF, Long NM, Zaghloul KA, Sharan AD, Sperling MR, Kahana MJ.. Human intracranial high-frequency activity maps episodic memory formation in space and time. NeuroImage 2014; 85: 834–43. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Burke JF, Ramayya AG, Kahana MJ.. Human intracranial high-frequency activity during memory processing: neural oscillations or stochastic volatility? Curr Opin Neurobiol 2015; 31: 104–10. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Burke JF, Zaghloul KA, Jacobs J, Williams RB, Sperling MR, Sharan AD, et al. Synchronous and asynchronous theta and gamma activity during episodic memory formation. J Neurosci 2013; 33: 292–304. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Cicerone K, Levin H, Malec J, Stuss D, Whyte J.. Cognitive rehabilitation interventions for executive function: moving from bench to bedside in patients with traumatic brain injury. J Cogn Neurosci 2006; 18: 1212–22. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Desikan RS, Ségonne F, Fischl B, Quinn BT, Dickerson BC, Blacker D, et al. An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest. NeuroImage 2006; 31: 968–80. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Dikmen SS, Corrigan JD, Levin HS, Machamer J, Stiers W, Weisskopf MG.. Cognitive outcome following traumatic brain injury. J Head Trauma Rehabil 2009; 24: 430–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Ding K, Gupta P, Diaz-Arrastia R.. Epilepsy after traumatic brain injury In: Laskowitz D, Grant G, editors. Translational research in traumatic brain injury. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press/Taylor and Francis Group; 2016. Chap. 14. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Edlow BL, Chatelle C, Spencer CA, Chu CJ, Bodien YG, O’Connor KL, et al. Early detection of consciousness in patients with acute severe traumatic brain injury. Brain 2017; 140: 2399–414. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Ezzyat Y, Kragel JE, Burke JF, Levy DF, Lyalenko A, Wanda P, et al. Direct brain stimulation modulates encoding states and memory performance in humans. Curr Biol 2017; 27: 1251–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Ezzyat Y, Wanda PA, Levy DF, Kadel A, Aka A, Pedisich I, et al. Closed-loop stimulation of temporal cortex rescues functional networks and improves memory. Nat Commun 2018; 9: 365. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Greenberg JA, Burke JF, Haque R, Kahana MJ, Zaghloul KA.. Decreases in theta and increases in high frequency activity underlie associative memory encoding. NeuroImage 2015; 114: 257–63. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Gupta PK, Sayed N, Ding K, Agostini MA, Van Ness PC, Yablon S, et al. Subtypes of post-traumatic epilepsy: clinical, electrophysiological, and imaging features. J Neurotrauma 2014; 31: 1439–43. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Hanslmayr S, Axmacher N, Inman CS.. Modulating human memory via entrainment of brain oscillations. Trends Neurosci 2019; 42: 485–99. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Hanslmayr S, Staresina B, Jensen O.. Oscillatory brain mechanism for memory formation In: Kahana M, Wagner A, editors. Oxford handbook of human memory. Oxford, United Kingdom: Oxford University Press; 2020. Chap. 4.6. [Google Scholar]
  16. Herweg NA, Solomon EA, Kahana MJ.. Theta oscillations in human memory. Trends Cogn Sci 2020; 24: 208–27. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Howard MW, Kahana MJ.. When does semantic similarity help episodic retrieval? J Mem Lang 2002; 46: 85–98. [Google Scholar]
  18. Kahana MJ. Associative retrieval processes in free recall. Mem Cogn 1996; 24: 103–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Kahana MJ. Computational models of memory search. Annu Rev Psychol 2020; 71: 107–38. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Kahana MJ, Aggarwal EV, Phan TD.. The variability puzzle in human memory. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 2018; 44: 1857–63. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Kahana MJ, Seelig D, Madsen JR.. Theta returns. Curr Opin Neurobiol 2001; 11: 739–44. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Kahana MJ, Sekuler R, Caplan JB, Kirschen M, Madsen JR.. Human theta oscillations exhibit task dependence during virtual maze navigation. Nature 1999; 399: 781–4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Kim H. Neural activity that predicts subsequent memory and forgetting: a meta-analysis of 74 fMRI studies. NeuroImage 2011; 54: 2446–61. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Kragel JE, Ezzyat Y, Sperling MR, Gorniak R, Worrell GA, Berry BM, et al. Similar patterns of neural activity predict memory function during encoding and retrieval. NeuroImage 2017; 155: 60–71. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Lachaux J-P, Rodriguez E, Martinerie J, Varela FJ.. Measuring phase synchrony in brain signals. Hum Brain Mapp 1999; 8: 194–208. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Long NM, Kahana MJ.. Successful memory formation is driven by contextual encoding in the core memory network. NeuroImage 2015; 119: 332–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Nuwer MR, Hovda DA, Schrader LM, Vespa PM.. Routine and quantitative EEG in mild traumatic brain injury. Clin Neurophysiol 2005; 116: 2001–25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Paller KA, Wagner AD.. Observing the transformation of experience into memory. Trends Cogn Sci 2002; 6: 93–102. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Pandit AS, Expert P, Lambiotte R, Bonnelle V, Leech R, Turkheimer FE, et al. Traumatic brain injury impairs small-world topology. Neurology 2013; 80: 1826–33. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Pedregosa F, Varoquaux G, Gramfort A, Michel V, Thirion B, Grisel O, et al. Scikit-learn: machine learning in python. J Mach Learn Res 2011; 12: 2825–30. [Google Scholar]
  31. Polyn SM, Norman KA, Kahana MJ.. A context maintenance and retrieval model of organizational processes in free recall. Psychol Rev 2009; 116: 129–56. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Roenker DL, Thompson CP, Brown SC.. Comparison of measures for the estimation of clustering in free recall. Psychol Bull 1971; 76: 45–8. [Google Scholar]
  33. Rosenbaum PR, Rubin DB.. The central role of the propensity score in observational studies for causal effects. Biometrika 1983; 70: 41–55. [Google Scholar]
  34. Seabold S., Perktold J.. statsmodels: econometric and statistical modeling with python. In: 9th Python in Science Conference. 2010.
  35. Sekhon JS. Multivariate and propensity score matching software with automated balance optimization: the matching package for R. J Stat Softw 2011; 42: 1–52. [Google Scholar]
  36. Shoulson IWilhelm EE, Koehler R Cognitive rehabilitation therapy for traumatic brain injury: evaluating the evidence. Washington, D.C.: National Academies Press; 2012. [Google Scholar]
  37. Slobounov S, Sebastianelli W, Hallett M.. Residual brain dysfunction observed one year post-mild traumatic brain injury: combined EEG and balance study. Clin Neurophysiol 2012; 123: 1755–61. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Smith DH, Meaney DF, Shull WH.. Diffuse axonal injury in head trauma. J Head Trauma Rehabil 2003; 18: 307–16. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Solomon EA, Kragel JE, Sperling MR, Sharan A, Worrell G, Kucewicz M, et al. Widespread theta synchrony and high-frequency desynchronization underlies enhanced cognition. Nat Commun 2017; 8: 1704. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Solomon EA, Stein JM, Das S, Gorniak R, Sperling MR, Worrell G, et al. Dynamic theta networks in the human medial temporal lobe support episodic memory. Curr Biol 2019; 29: 1100–11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Solway A, Murdock BB, Kahana MJ.. Positional and temporal clustering in serial order memory. Mem Cogn 2012; 40: 177–90. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Spitz G, Maller JJ, O’Sullivan R, Ponsford JL.. White matter integrity following traumatic brain injury: the association with severity of injury and cognitive functioning. Brain Topogr 2013; 26: 648–60. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Swann NC, de Hemptinne C, Thompson MC, Miocinovic S, Miller AM, Ostrem JL, et al. Adaptive deep brain stimulation for Parkinson’s disease using motor cortex sensing. J Neural Eng 2018; 15: 046006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Tulving E. How many memory systems are there? Am Psychol 1985; 40: 385–98. [Google Scholar]
  45. Vakil E. The mnemonic consequences of moderate-to-severe brain injury In: Kahana M, Wagner A, editors. Oxford handbook of human memory. Oxford, United Kingdom: Oxford University Press; 2020. Chap. 9.6. [Google Scholar]
  46. Vakil E, Greenstein Y, Weiss I, Shtein S.. The effects of moderate-to-severe traumatic brain injury on episodic memory: a meta-analysis. Neuropsychol Rev 2019; 1–18. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Verellen R, Cavazos J.. Post-traumatic epilepsy: an overview. Therapy 2010; 7: 527–31. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Wagner AD. Building memories: remembering and forgetting of verbal experiences as predicted by brain activity. Science 1998; 281: 1188–91. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Wang J, Bakhadirov K, Abdi H, Devous M, De La Plata CM, Moore C, et al. Longitudinal changes of structural connectivity in traumatic axonal injury. Neurology 2011; 77: 818–26. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Weidemann CT, Kragel JE, Lega BC, Worrell GA, Sperling MR, Sharan AD, et al. Neural activity reveals interactions between episodic and semantic memory systems during retrieval. J Exp Psychol Gen 2019; 148: 1–12. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Yushkevich P, Pluta J, Wang H, Ding S, Xie L, Gertje E, et al. Automated volumetry and regional thickness analysis of hippocampal subfields and medial temporal cortical structures in mild cognitive impairment. Hum Brain Mapp 2015; 36: 258–87. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Data Availability Statement

De-identified data and analysis code is made available at: http://memory.psych.upenn.edu/Electrophysiological_Data.


Articles from Brain Communications are provided here courtesy of Oxford University Press

RESOURCES