Skip to main content
Paediatrics & Child Health logoLink to Paediatrics & Child Health
. 2019 Oct 21;26(1):e17–e24. doi: 10.1093/pch/pxz124

Supporting caregivers of children born prematurely in the development of language: A scoping review

Roxanne Belanger 1,, Dominique Leroux 1, Pascal Lefebvre 1
PMCID: PMC7850271  PMID: 33542775

Abstract

Background

Infants born prematurely can display impairments that negatively impact the early years of their development. Compared to their peers born at term, preterm children have higher risks of cerebral palsy, sensory deficits, learning disabilities, cognitive and language deficits, as well as difficulties related to attention and behaviour. Following discharge, parents of preterm children are often supported through neonatal follow-up programs or by community health care practitioners. Through assessment and consultation, professionals foster parental resilience by teaching them about their child’s development. Research shows a large volume of literature on improving outcomes for preterm infants, but less attention has been given to the impact and potential importance of education of parents regarding the care they provide from the home.

Objective

A scoping review was completed to determine the best practices for early intervention in premature children regarding the development of language skills during the preschool years.

Methods

The review followed the guidelines for the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA).

Results

Four general themes emerged from the review and included the importance of providing (1) parental training in the care of an infant born prematurely during neonatal intensive care unit stay; (2) education on the development of language and the importance of parental responsiveness; (3) provision of activities to support child language learning; and (4) overall and ongoing monitoring and support by qualified health professionals.

Conclusions

The conclusions drawn will provide guidance to health care professionals regarding the education of parents on best practices for stimulating language development in their child.

Keywords: Language development, Neonatal follow-up program, Parental education, Prematurity, Scoping review


Infants born prematurely can display impairments that negatively impact their development (1–3). When compared to their peers born at term, preterm children have higher risks of delays in all developmental domains (4,5). Consequently, monitoring of premature infants and provision of early intervention is vital to public health (6,7). Regarding delays in language, timely detection may also prevent social and learning deficits, thus improving outcomes and reducing the need for special education (8–10). This is especially important given that intervention prior to kindergarten has academic, social, and economic benefits (11). There is a large volume of published literature on improving outcomes for preterm infants, and some attention has been given to the impact and potential importance of education of parents regarding the care they provide from the home (12). However, regarding child language development surveillance, knowledge and referral practices vary among health care practitioners (13,14). Education in this domain is therefore important given that health professionals play an important role by improving parents’ knowledge and skills regarding their child’s development.

DEVELOPMENTAL SURVEILLANCE OF INFANTS BORN PREMATURELY

In Canada, many preterm infants are followed by a neonatal follow-up program (NFUP) after discharge from the neonatal intensive care unit (NICU) (15). These programs serve several purposes, one of which is to provide anticipatory guidance and teaching to parents regarding their child’s developmental pattern, thereby fostering parental resilience (16). Paediatricians and family practitioners also provide developmental follow-up for children born prematurely. This is especially true for infants born moderately (32 to 33 weeks’ gestation) or late preterm (34 to 36 week’s gestation) and who do not qualify NFUP care (17). These infants have been historically perceived to have similar risks of delays as neonates born at term (17). However, more recently, it has been shown that they are more likely to have developmental delays at preschool age (18–20). This information is important given that these infants account for the majority of preterm births (20).

LANGUAGE DEVELOPMENT AND STIMULATION IN CHILDREN BORN PRETERM

It has been well established that early brain development is a strong foundation for long-term success in education (21). Equally important is early language exposure, as it strongly influences literacy skills, educational achievement, and health outcomes (22). As the quality of language stimulation has been shown to overcome many risk factors, caregivers remain a powerful and vital support to change (23–27). Early interactions and regular conversations between caregiver and infant are key (28,29). In fact, one of the major barriers to early language acquisition is insufficient early language exposure (30–34).

Health professionals have an obligation to increase parents’ understanding of the developmental course of language function in children born prematurely (8,10,34). Through regular contact with families, these professionals are able to develop relationships with parents and can better help them recognize their infant’s cues and respond in ways that nurture development (35–37). A scoping review was completed to determine the best practices for early intervention in premature children regarding the development of language skills during the preschool years.

METHODS

Protocol and registration

The current study followed the guidelines for the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) (38). The method used for the collection of articles and analysis were specified in advance and documented in a protocol. The protocol used in the context of this systematic review was not registered in any library.

Eligibility criteria

For inclusion, the articles had to meet the following criteria: (1) include children born prematurely (gestational age <37 weeks or with low birth weight); (2) be published between 1987 and 2018; (3) be published in English or in French; (4) report on outcomes of early interventions with parents on their child’s language development; (5) be primarily a research article, secondary data analysis, meta-analysis, or a systematic review; and (6) be published in a peer-reviewed journal. Studies were excluded if they did not meet all of these criteria.

Information sources

Studies were identified by searching through electronic databases and scanning reference lists of articles. English and French were applied when searching. This search was applied to PubMed and CINAHL databases. In addition, references of published articles were hand-searched to find additional studies applicable to this review. The literature search was performed from September 9, 2017, to January 2, 2018. See Appendices A and B for full electronic search strategies.

Study selection, data collection, analyses, and risk of bias

Eligibility assessment was performed independently in a standardized manner by two reviewers; studies that did not meet the inclusion criteria were excluded. Where there was disagreement, the two reviewers met to agree upon the articles included for full-text screening. This process was repeated for the selection of full-text articles. The articles were also hand-searched for words like: recommendation(s), suggestion(s), guide (guidance), advice, support, and future direction.

A data extraction sheet was developed and pilot-tested on ten randomly selected studies and refined accordingly. The following information was extracted: (1) study type; (2) participant characteristics (including number and age); (3) study objectives/goals (4) results; (5) recommendations; and (6) article summary. Thematic analysis and extraction of core themes was completed by two independent reviewers. Again, where there was disagreement, the two reviewers met. Risk of bias was then informally evaluated by both reviewers; in all cases, assessment tools and key elements of interventions were judged to be adequate and developmentally appropriate for the population served.

RESULTS

Study selection

PubMed and CINAHL database searches provided a total of 209 citations; these were organized through the reference manager Zotero (39). Once 95 duplicates were extracted, 114 articles remained for the initial review. Titles and abstracts were checked for inclusion or exclusion by the primary reviewer using the above-mentioned criteria. Eighty-five studies were excluded. After the title and abstract reviewing, 29 articles were recognized as relevant and were included for full-text review. Following this review, 23 articles were recognized as meeting all of the desired criteria (Table 1). A flow diagram of the study selection is shown in Figure 1.

Table 1.

Study characteristics

Study Dates Country Setting Support/Funding
Benzies et al., 2013 2008–2011 Canada Home visit Alberta Centre for Child, Family, and Community Research and the Preterm Birth and Healthy Outcomes Team (PreHOT)
Bühler et al., 2009 2005–2008 Brazil Outpatient Clinic, Day Care Centre or both (at the University Hospital) Not stated
Caskey et al., 2014 2008–2009 USA NICU Department of Pediatrics, Women and Infants Hospital, Providence, Rhode Island
Censullo, 1994 Not stated USA Home visit National Centre for Nursing Research Grant
Colditz et al., 2015 Not stated Australia Hospital-based and Community-based National Health and Medical Research Council and for a Career Development Grant; a Royal Brisbane and Woman’s Hospital Foundation Grant
Flierman et al., 2016 2013–2014 Netherlands Home visit Zorg Onderzoek Nederland
Foster-Cohen et al., 2016 Not stated New Zealand Not stated Neurological Foundation of New Zealand, Health Research Council of New Zealand, Canterbury Medical Research Foundation and the Lottery Grants Board of New Zealand
Gabis et al., 2015 Not stated Israel NICU and Home visit None
Guarini et al., 2016 2007–2012 Italy Day Hospital of the Neonatology Unit and School-based Italian Ministry of Education, University and Research
Magill-Evans et al., 2002 Not stated Canada Home visit Alberta Foundation for Nursing Research, the Alberta Association of Registered Nurses, and the Medical Research Council/National Health and Research Development Program, Health Scholar Award to M. Harrison
Mahoney et al., 2017 2006–2016 USA Review Not stated
Milgrom et al., 2013 2007–2009 Australia NICU and home visit (after discharge) National Health and Medical Research Council
Nepomnyaschy et al., 2012 2001-? USA Not stated Based on the Early Childhood Longitudinal Survey-Birth Cohort
Putnick et al., 2017 2014–2016 Germany NICU and home visit National Institutes of Health, Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development; German Federal Ministry of Education and Science
Rechia et al., 2016 2011–2015 Brazil Review Not stated
Ross et al., 2016 2006–2012 USA Neurodevelopmental Follow-Up Clinic Not stated
Sansavini et al., 2015 2011–2014 Italy Unit of Neonatology of Bologna University Italian Ministry of Education, University and Research, University of Bologna, Association in Bologna of parents of preterm children
Smith et al., 1996 1990–1992 USA Home visit Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development
Spittle et al., 2010 2005–2009 Australia Home visit and occasional hospital-based National Health and Medical Council, the Cerebral Palsy Foundation, the Murdoch Children’s Research Institute, the Myer Foundation, Allens Arthur Robinson, and the Thyne Reid Foundation
Suttora et al., 2011 Not stated Italy Laboratory for infant observation at the Macedonio Melloni Maternity Hospital of Milan Not stated
Vohr et al., 2012 2006–2011 USA Not stated National Institutes of Health and the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development
Welch et al., 2015 Not stated USA NICU The Einhorn Family Charitable Trust, The Fleur Fairman Family and Mary Dexter Stephenson; Columbia University’s CTSA
Wu et al., 2016 2006–2008 Taiwan Hospital-based interventions and neonatal clinic visits National Health Research Institute and National Science Council

NICU Neonatal intensive care unit.

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Flow of information through the different phases of the systematic review. Adapted from ref. (59). Copyright 2009 by the Moher et al. Adapted with permission.

Characteristics of included studies

All 23 studies selected described early intervention practices provided to parents regarding their premature child’s language development. These studies were published in English (some were translated) and carried out between 1990 and 2016 in various countries using hospital-, home-, and centre-based interventions (Supplementary Tables 2–24).

After data extraction and analysis, four general themes emerged from the review: (1) parental training in the care of an infant born prematurely during NICU stay; (2) education on the development of language and the importance of parental responsiveness; (3) provision of activities to support child language learning; and (4) the importance of overall and ongoing monitoring and support by qualified health professionals. It should be noted that often, multiple themes were reported in one article.

Theme 1: Caregiver training in infant care during NICU stay

Five studies reported evidence on the use of caregiver training during NICU stay and their impact on language outcomes at a later age (40–44). All studies advocated for family nurturing, parental guidance and infant stimulation. Interventions varied widely but the reported goals were the same: education regarding (1) stimulus overload and distress in the infant; (2) appropriate and timely interaction and responsiveness; and (3) the importance of more regular, close contact and interaction with their infant.

Improved outcomes in a variety of domains were reported and authors hypothesized that targeting the emotional interactions between parents and infants in the NICU was essential to the developmental course of the participants. For example, Caskey et al. (2014) reported that increasing the quantity of parent talk was associated with higher 7- and 18-months corrected age language and cognitive scores on the Bayley-III. In a randomized controlled trial (RCT), Milgrom et al. (2013) used the Mother-Infant Transaction Program (MIPT) to provide mothers with education regarding infant cues. Results at 6 months corrected age showed increased sensitivity in providing care, greater awareness of, and more appropriate responses to negative infant cues, which in turn significantly lowered infant stress.

Taken together, the results of these studies demonstrate that parent-training interventions provided during NICU stay that aimed to reduce stress exposure in the very early newborn period and stimulate social interactions can, in the short-term, improve the negative outcomes reported in preterm children.

Theme 2: Parental education on the development of language

Fourteen studies including two RCT reported evidence on the use of parent education following NICU discharge and aimed at increasing parent responsiveness and knowledge of their child’s development (22,29,33,35–37,40,42,45–50). In these studies, caregiver education was provided regarding the characteristics of positive interactions and the importance of language stimulation, and led to improved caregiver sensitivity as well as the use of attention-maintaining strategies with their child. Parents also consistently demonstrated increased quantity and quality of talking, interacting and reading from 0 to 3 years; these were later strongly associated with improved language and cognitive development at 6 and 12 months, decreased externalizing and dysregulation behaviours at 2 years, and decreased parental anxiety and depression at 2 years.

Theme 3: Providing parents with activities to support cognitive/language learning

Three studies reported evidence on the importance of providing parents with activities to promote cognitive and language learning in preterm infants (22,46,51). In these studies, caregivers were shown how to scaffold language by providing their child with information about objects and actions, how to include age-appropriate problem-solving strategies during play and how to engage in shared reading experiences. Results showed that even with healthy preterm infants, parenting behaviour that encouraged language development was a key factor in improving short-term developmental outcomes.

Theme 4: Ongoing monitoring and early intervention by health professionals

Nine studies reported evidence on the importance of ongoing monitoring and evaluation of preterm infants, as well as the provision of early intervention by health professionals (29,46,49,52–57). It was reported that professionals who specialize in preterm child development are more aware that parenting babies born prematurely can be more difficult than those born full-term and consequently, better prepare parents for the challenges ahead (35,37). Given the importance of communication skills in social and educational setting, it was broadly recommended that infants be offered access to regular monitoring and early intervention services (46,52,53,55). When possible and where appropriate, this included comprehensive developmental evaluations involving standardized tests, and direct observation, as well as family psychosocial assessments.

DISCUSSION AND RECOMMENDATIONS

As parents are poised to be their child’s first and best teachers, it is not surprising that all studies highlighted the importance of involving caregivers and providing education on language milestones, as well as ways in which language can be stimulated within the home. In this review, interventions were provided to families during NICU stay, at surveillance visits or through home visits. Though long-term studies are required in order to determine if these gains were maintained, they provide evidence for the importance of supporting parents regarding their child’s language development. Unfortunately, not all families have access to health professionals that specialize in the development of children born preterm or to NFUPs.

Most health professionals have adequate knowledge regarding major milestones in children’s language development and can refer parents to the appropriate services when these milestones are not being met. However, health professionals are less likely to provide information to caregivers regarding stress and readiness cues. Given that these cues are the basis of all caregiver–child interactions, health professionals must become familiar with the characteristics of stimulus overload and distress in preterm infants and teach parents to recognize their child’s cues with the goal of providing appropriate responses and tailoring interactions to their child’s tolerance.

Similarly, health professionals may not regularly provide caregivers with language stimulation techniques. It is important to educate caregivers regarding the critical role they play in shaping their child’s brain development (58). They must understand that regular, close interactions are crucial to language development and that being a responsive caregiver leads to better outcomes. More specifically, caregivers must be coached to follow their child’s lead, to consistently and positively respond to their child’s cues, to provide words that build on their child’s focus of attention and finally, to scaffold verbal behaviours. Activities to support learning in their child, such as social games, singing, and reading, should also be provided. It is recommended that health professionals model these behaviours to parents by talking to every child during surveillance visits.

This information is especially important given that preterm infants require more specialized support to help them organize responses; this is due to the fact that, during the first year of life, their difficulty in shifting attention is often reported. Interactive strategies that maintains interest infants’ in objects and conversations will help them maintain active involvement with toys, thereby decreasing the demands placed on the attentional system and thus facilitating toy exploration.

Many studies reported on the importance of long-term follow-up after NICU discharge and the support of qualified health professionals because these professionals specialize in preterm child development and are more aware of the short- and long-term consequences. Consequently, they can better monitor these children and prepare parents for the challenges ahead. However, the need for systematic follow-up evaluation protocols that provide qualitative and quantitative information was highlighted; these would help define the true needs of preterm infants and identify the ideal moment for speech-language intervention.

In communities where monitoring and early intervention is not routinely provided to caregivers, some of these strategies could be provided during NICU stay, or during surveillance visits. This information would be most beneficial to families when a child is identified with delays and is being referred to speech language therapy. As families are waiting longer before receiving most early intervention services, health professionals have an increasing role to play in providing caregivers with evidence-based early intervention practices.

LIMITATIONS

Limitations of this review include the lack of rigorous evaluation of bias. Also, data analysis was performed by two reviewers on the same research team.

CONCLUSIONS

Given that preterm children have higher risks of delays are all domains, developmental monitoring is vital to support their development. Regarding language, timely detection of delays in language will help prevent social and learning deficits. Though NFUPs specialize in preterm child development and are best positioned to prepare parents for the challenges ahead, many preterm children do not qualify for these services. Consequently, paediatricians and family practitioners fulfill this role in many cases. Education regarding best practices for this population is important given that these professionals play an important role by improving parents’ knowledge and skills regarding their child’s development.

A scoping review was completed to determine the best practices for early intervention in premature children regarding the development of language skills during the preschool years. Themes that emerged included the importance of providing parental training in the care of an infant born prematurely during NICU stay, as well as education following discharge and monitoring and support by qualified health professionals. The conclusions drawn will provide guidance to health care professionals regarding the education of parents on best practices for stimulating language development in their child.

SUPPLEMENTARY DATA

Supplementary data are available at Paediatrics & Child HealthOnline.

Funding: There are no funders to report for this submission.

Potential Conflicts of Interest: All authors: No reported conflicts of interest. All authors have submitted the ICMJE Form for Disclosure of Potential Conflicts of Interest. Conflicts that the editors consider relevant to the content of the manuscript have been disclosed.

References

  • 1. Burns YR, Danks M, O’Callaghan MJ, et al. Motor coordination difficulties and physical fitness of extremely-low-birthweight children. Dev Med Child Neurol 2009;51(2):136–42. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Burns Y, O’Callaghan M, McDonell B, Rogers Y. Movement and motor development in ELBW infants at 1 year is related to cognitive and motor abilities at 4 years. Early Hum Dev 2004;80(1):19–29. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Hutchinson EA, De Luca CR, Doyle LW, Roberts G, Anderson PJ; Victorian Infant Collaborative Study Group School-age outcomes of extremely preterm or extremely low birth weight children. Pediatrics 2013;131(4):e1053–61. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Dey AC, Mannan MA, Saha L, Hossain MI, Shahidullah M. Magnitude of problems of prematurity-national and global perspective: A review. Bangladesh J Child Health 2013;36(3):146–52. [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Zelkowitz P, Feeley N, Shrier I, et al. The Cues and Care Trial: A randomized controlled trial of an intervention to reduce maternal anxiety and improve developmental outcomes in very low birthweight infants. BMC Pediatr 2008;8:38. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Huddy CLJ, Johnson A, Hope PL. Educational and behavioural problems in babies of 32–35 weeks’ gestation. Archives of Disease in Childhood. Fetal and Neonatal Edition 2001;85(1):F23–F28. doi:10.1136/fn.85.1.F23 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7. World Health Organization. The ICD-10 Classification of Mental and Behavioural Disorders: Diagnostic Criteria for Research, vol. 2, 2nd ed Switzerland: World Health Organization, 2003. <http://www.who.int/iris/handle/10665/37108> Accessed 2 November 2017. [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Beitchman JH, Wilson B, Brownlie EB, Walters H, Inglis A, Lancee W. Long-term consistency in speech/language profiles: II. Behavioral, emotional, and social outcomes. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1996;35(6):815–25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9. van Agt HM, van der Stege HA, de Ridder-Sluiter H, Verhoeven LT, de Koning HJ. A cluster-randomized trial of screening for language delay in toddlers: Effects on school performance and language development at age 8. Pediatrics 2007;120(6):1317–25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10. Young AR, Beitchman JH, Johnson C, et al. Young adult academic outcomes in a longitudinal sample of early identified language impaired and control children. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2002;43(5):635–45. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Pinto-Martin JA, Dunkle M, Earls M, Fliedner D, Landes C. Developmental stages of developmental screening: Steps to implementation of a successful program. Am J Public Health 2005;95(11):1928–32. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Smith VC, Young S, Pursley DM, McCormick MC, Zupancic JA. Are families prepared for discharge from the NICU? J Perinatol 2009;29(9):623–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Pizolato RA, Fonseca LMM, da Silva Bastos R, Fernandes AY, Lefebre F, Maximino LP. Child’s language development surveillance: Knowledge and practices vary among health care professionals. Revista Cefac 2016;18(5):1109–19. [Google Scholar]
  • 14. Sices L, Feudtner C, McLaughlin J, Drotar D, Williams M. How do primary care physicians identify young children with developmental delays? A national survey. J Dev Behav Pediatr 2003;24(6):409–17. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Dosman C, Andrews D. Anticipatory guidance for cognitive and social-emotional development: Birth to five years. Paediatr Child Health 2012;17(2):75–80. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16. Provincial Council for Maternal and Child Health. Final report of the neonatal follow-up clinics working group 2015. <http://www.pcmch.on.ca/wp-content/uploads/2015/09/NFUC-Final-Report_15-06-02.pdf> Accessed 2 November 2017.
  • 17. Loftin RW, Habli M, Snyder CC, Cormier CM, Lewis DF, Defranco EA. Late preterm birth. Rev Obstet Gynecol 2010;3(1):10–9. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Kerstjens JM, Bocca-Tjeertes IF, de Winter AF, Reijneveld SA, Bos AF. Neonatal morbidities and developmental delay in moderately preterm-born children. Pediatrics 2012;130(2):e265–72. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19. Kerstjens JM, de Winter AF, Bocca-Tjeertes IF, ten Vergert EM, Reijneveld SA, Bos AF. Developmental delay in moderately preterm-born children at school entry. J Pediatr 2011;159(1):92–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20. McIntire DD, Leveno KJ. Neonatal mortality and morbidity rates in late preterm births compared with births at term. Obstet Gynecol 2008;111(1):35–41. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21. National Research Council. From Neurons to Neighborhoods: The Science of Early Childhood Development. Washington, DC: National Academies Press, 2000. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22. Darcy Mahoney A, Zauche LH, Hallowell S, Weldon A, Stapel-Wax J. Leveraging the skills of nurses and the power of language nutrition to ensure a better future for children. Adv Neonatal Care 2017;17(1):45–52. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23. Burchinal M, Vernon-Feagans L, Cox M; Key Family Life Project Investigators Cumulative social risk, parenting, and infant development in rural low-income communities. Parent Sci Pract 2008;8(1):41–69. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24. Huttenlocher J, Waterfall H, Vasilyeva M, Vevea J, Hedges LV. Sources of variability in children’s language growth. Cogn Psychol 2010;61(4):343–65. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25. Murray L, Kempton C, Woolgar M, Hooper R. Depressed mothers’ speech to their infants and its relation to infant gender and cognitive development. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1993;34(7):1083–101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26. Poehlmann J, Fiese BH. Parent-infant interaction as a mediator of the relation between neonatal risk status and 12-month cognitive development. Infant Behavior and Development 2001;24(2):171–88. doi:10.1016/S0163-6383(01)00073-X [Google Scholar]
  • 27. Rowe ML Child-directed speech: Relation to socioeconomic status, knowledge of child development and child vocabulary skill. J Child Lang 2008;35(1):185–205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28. Zauche LH, Thul TA, Mahoney AED, Stapel-Wax JL. Influence of language nutrition on children’s language and cognitive development: An integrated review. Early Child Res Q 2016;36:318–33. doi:10.1016/j.ecresq.2016.01.015 [Google Scholar]
  • 29. Spittle AJ, Anderson PJ, Lee KJ, et al. Preventive care at home for very preterm infants improves infant and caregiver outcomes at 2 years. Pediatrics 2010;126(1):e171–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30. Forget-Dubois N, Dionne G, Lemelin JP, Pérusse D, Tremblay RE, Boivin M. Early child language mediates the relation between home environment and school readiness. Child Dev 2009;80(3):736–49. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31. Rowe ML A longitudinal investigation of the role of quantity and quality of child-directed speech in vocabulary development. Child Dev 2012;83(5):1762–74. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32. Weisleder A, Fernald A. Talking to children matters: Early language experience strengthens processing and builds vocabulary. Psychol Sci 2013;24(11):2143–52. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33. Colditz P, Sanders MR, Boyd R, et al. Prem Baby Triple P: A randomised controlled trial of enhanced parenting capacity to improve developmental outcomes in preterm infants. BMC Pediatr 2015;15:15. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34. van Noort-van der Spek IL, Franken MC, Weisglas-Kuperus N. Language functions in preterm-born children: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Pediatrics 2012;129(4):745–54. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35. Benzies KM, Magill-Evans J, Kurilova J, Nettel-Aguirre A, Blahitka L, Lacaze-Masmonteil T. Effects of video-modeling on the interaction skills of first-time fathers of late preterm infants. Infants & Young Children, 2013;26(4):333–48. doi:10.1097/IYC.0b013e3182a4ed5e [Google Scholar]
  • 36. Censullo M Developmental delay in healthy premature infants at age two years: Implications for early intervention. J Dev Behav Pediatr 1994;15(2):99–104. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37. Smith KE, Landry SH, Swank PR, Baldwin CD, Denson SE, Wildin S. The relation of medical risk and maternal stimulation with preterm infants’ development of cognitive, language and daily living skills. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1996;37(7):855–64. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38. Liberati A, Altman DG, Tetzlaff J, et al. The PRISMA statement for reporting systematic reviews and meta-analyses of studies that evaluate health care interventions: Explanation and elaboration. Ann Intern Med 2009;151(4):W65–94. doi:10.1371/journal.pmed.1000100 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39. Zotero. 2016. <www.zotero.org/download> Accessed 2 November 2017.
  • 40. Caskey M, Stephens B, Tucker R, Vohr B. Adult talk in the NICU with preterm infants and developmental outcomes. Pediatrics 2014;133(3):e578–84. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41. Gabis LV, Hacham-Pilosof K, Yosef OB, Rabinovitz G, Leshem G, Shilon-Hadass A, … Bart O. The influence of a multisensory intervention for preterm infants provided by parents, on developmental abilities and on parental stress levels. Journal of Child Neurology, 2015;30(7):896–903. doi:10.1177/0883073814549242 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42. Milgrom J, Newnham C, Anderson PJ, et al. Early sensitivity training for parents of preterm infants: Impact on the developing brain. Pediatr Res 2010;67(3):330–5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43. Ross GS, Foran LM, Barbot B, Sossin KM, Perlman JM. Using cluster analysis to provide new insights into development of very low birthweight (VLBW) premature infants. Early Hum Dev 2016;92:45–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44. Welch MG, Firestein MR, Austin J, et al. Family nurture intervention in the neonatal intensive care unit improves social-relatedness, attention, and neurodevelopment of preterm infants at 18 months in a randomized controlled trial. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2015;56(11):1202–11. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45. Bühler KEB, Limongi SCO, Diniz EMdeA. Language and cognition in very low birth weight preterm infants with PELCDO application. Arquivos De Neuro-Psiquiatria, 2009;67(2A):242–9. doi:10.1590/S0004-282X2009000200013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46. Foster-Cohen SH, Friesen MD, Champion PR, Woodward LJ. High prevalence/low severity language delay in preschool children born very preterm. J Dev Behav Pediatr 2010;31(8):658–67. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47. Sansavini A, Zavagli V, Guarini A, Savini S, Alessandroni R, Faldella G. Dyadic co-regulation, affective intensity and infant’s development at 12 months: A comparison among extremely preterm and full-term dyads. Infant Behav Dev 2015;40:29–40. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48. Suttora C, Salerni N. Maternal speech to preterm infants during the first 2 years of life: Stability and change. Int J Lang Commun Disord 2011;46(4):464–72. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49. Wu YC, Hsieh WS, Hsu CH, et al. Intervention effects on emotion regulation in preterm infants with very low birth weight: A randomized controlled trial. Res Dev Disabil 2016;48:1–12. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50. Flierman M, Koldewijn K, Meijssen D, et al. Feasibility of a preventive parenting intervention for very preterm children at 18 months corrected age: A randomized pilot trial. J Pediatr 2016;176:79–85.e1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51. Lowe J, Erickson SJ, MacLean P, Duvall SW, Ohls RK, Duncan AF. Associations between maternal scaffolding and executive functioning in 3 and 4 year olds born very low birth weight and normal birth weight. Early Hum Dev 2014;90(10):587–93. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52. Nepomnyaschy L, Hegyi T, Ostfeld BM, Reichman NE. Developmental outcomes of late-preterm infants at 2 and 4 years. Matern Child Health J 2012;16(8):1612–24. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53. Putnick DL, Bornstein MH, Eryigit-Madzwamuse S, Wolke D. Long-term stability of language performance in very preterm, moderate-late preterm, and term children. J Pediatr 2017;181:74–79.e3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54. Rechia IC, Oliveira LD, Crestani AH, Biaggio EP, Souza AP. Effects of prematurity on language acquisition and auditory maturation: A systematic review. Codas 2016;28(6):843–54. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55. Guarini A, Marini A, Savini S, Alessandroni R, Faldella G, Sansavini A. Linguistic features in children born very preterm at preschool age. Dev Med Child Neurol 2016;58(9):949–56. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56. Vohr BR, Stephens BE, Higgins RD, et al. ; Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Neonatal Research Network Are outcomes of extremely preterm infants improving? Impact of Bayley assessment on outcomes. J Pediatr 2012;161(2):222–8.e3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57. Magill-Evans J, Harrison MJ, Van der Zalm J, Holdgrafer G. Cognitive and language development of healthy preterm infants at 10 years of age. Phys Occup Ther Pediatr 2002;22(1):41–56. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58. Talge NM, Holzman C, Wang J, Lucia V, Gardiner J, Breslau N. Late-preterm birth and its association with cognitive and socioemotional outcomes at 6 years of age. Pediatrics 2010;126(6):1124–31. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59. Moher D, Liberati A, Tetzlaff J, Altman DG; PRISMA Group Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: The PRISMA statement. Int J Surg 2010;8(5):1009. doi:10.1371/journal.pmed.1000097.g001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Paediatrics & Child Health are provided here courtesy of Oxford University Press

RESOURCES