Skip to main content
Clinical Psychopharmacology and Neuroscience logoLink to Clinical Psychopharmacology and Neuroscience
. 2021 Feb 28;19(1):26–45. doi: 10.9758/cpn.2021.19.1.26

Second-to-Fourth Digit Ratio (2D:4D) in Psychiatric Disorders: A Systematic Review of Case-control Studies

Laura Fusar-Poli 1,*,, Alessandro Rodolico 1,*, Serena Sturiale 1, Bianca Carotenuto 1, Antimo Natale 1, Davide Arillotta 1, Spyridon Siafis 2, Maria Salvina Signorelli 1, Eugenio Aguglia 1
PMCID: PMC7851454  PMID: 33508786

Abstract

The second-to-fourth digit ratio (2D:4D) is an indirect, retrospective, non-invasive measure that correlates negatively with intrauterine exposure to testosterone. The present meta-analysis aimed to evaluate if 2D:4D differs between patients with psychiatric disorders and controls. In September 2019, we searched in Web of Knowledge, PsycINFO, Embase, and CINHAL, and retrieved 619 papers. We finally included 43 case-control studies which compared the 2D:4D ratio of patients with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) (n = 16), schizophrenia (n = 8), gender non-conformity (n = 7), addictions (n = 5), attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) (n = 4), mood disorders (n = 2), and intellectual disability (n = 1) to non-clinical controls. Meta-analyses showed that, overall, psychiatric patients had lower 2D:4D than healthy controls (n = 43, overall sample = 9,484, mean difference = −0.0056, 95% confidence interval from −0.0093 to −0.002, I2 = 74%), with more pronounced differences in the right hand, males, and children. Considering psychiatric disorders individually, significant differences were found in the ASD, ADHD, and addictions groups, in which 2D:4D was significantly lower than healthy controls. Conversely, the right hand of males with schizophrenia showed higher 2D:4D than healthy controls. No other significant differences were detected. Although our results need to be cautiously interpreted and find limited applications in clinical practice, they may suggest that 2D:4D is altered in some psychopathological conditions, underlining the role of prenatal exposure to sex steroids in the etiology of psychiatric disorders.

Keywords: Meta-analysis, Mental disorders, Testosterone, Autism spectrum disorder, Attention deficit disorder with hyperactivity, Substance addiction.

INTRODUCTION

The second-to-fourth digit ratio (2D:4D) is a biological marker, defined as the ratio of the length of the index (second digit) to the length of the ring finger (fourth digit) of the same hand. 2D:4D is constant throughout life [1,2] and represents an indirect, retrospective, and non-invasive measure that correlates negatively with intrauterine exposure to testosterone, i.e., a lower 2D:4D is the result of increased levels of fetal testosterone [3,4]. Many debates exist around the reasons why 2D:4D could be considered an indirect marker of the prenatal, but not the present, testosterone level [5] and even more strongly, a marker of the ratio between prenatal testosterone and estradiol levels. More evidence is provided by molecular genetic association studies, relating a polymorphism of the androgen receptor gene to individual differences in the 2D:4D ratio [6].

In humans, the 2D:4D ratio has been assumed to reflect the exposure to testosterone during the second trimester of gestation, because of the sex difference detectable in childhood, and because of postulated mechanisms regarding digit development [2]. Prenatal hormone exposure is critical for sexual differentiation and masculinization. In fact, males are exposed to higher levels of testosterone than females, particularly from about week 8 to 24 of gestation and week 2 to 26 of postnatal life [7]. This is confirmed by the observation that the 2D:4D ratio is sexually dimorphic: generally, females have a higher 2D:4D than males and this effect is more pronounced in the right hands, although the reasons still need to be clarified [8].

Digit ratio has been reported to correlate with a wide number of traits and conditions, ranging in almost every field of medicine, with particular regard to sex hormones- dependent conditions, i.e., breast cancer [9], prostate cancer [10,11], obesity [12], and osteoarthritis [13]. It has also been associated with physical characteristics, such as facial shape [14], sperm count [15], age of menarche [16], and penis size [17]. Of interest, scholars have also investigated the association between the 2D:4D ratio and behavioral features, such as aggression [18], stuttering [19], visuo-spatial ability [20], handedness [21], schizotypal personality [22], sporting ability [23], successful financial risk-taking [24], and sexual orientation [25].

Given the findings obtained in the field of behavioral sciences, over the last years, many researchers have sought to examine potential links between the 2D:4D ratio and psychiatric disorders, aiming to find a significant correlation between intrauterine exposure to testosterone and those conditions. Since the amount of available literature has been constantly growing, we aimed to perform a systematic review and meta-analysis to examine if the 2D:4D ratio consistently differed between people with psychiatric disorders and non-clinical controls. Second, we aimed to investigate potential mediators of 2D:4D differences, such as gender, age, and hand.

METHODS

Search Strategy

We followed the PRISMA Statement guidelines to perform a systematic search [26]. The protocol was registered on PROSPERO, an international database of prospectively registered systematic reviews in health and social care managed by the Centre for Reviews and Dissemination, University of York (Registration number: CRD42019124184).

In September 2019, we searched the following databases: Web of KnowledgeSM (including Web of Science, MEDLINE, KCI−Korean Journal Database, Russian Science Citation Index, and SciELO Citation Index), PsycINFO, Embase, and CINAHL. The complete search string can be found in the Supplementary Materials, Appendix 1 (available online) [27]. The search was not restricted to any language, reference type, or year of publication. The electronic search was supplemented by hand-searching of reference lists of the included review articles to identify any additional sources.

Study Selection

We selected all the studies published in English on peer-reviewed journals, which fulfilled the following inclusion criteria:

(1)Participants: Individuals of any age and gender, diagnosed with any psychiatric disorder according to valid international diagnostic criteria (e.g., Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders; International Classification of Diseases), or with validated scales (e.g., Hamilton Rating Scale for depression and Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale for schizophrenia), or followed by clinics or mental health services. Studies with patients recruited through web-surveys or subjects divided into groups according to scores obtained at self-reported questionnaires were excluded.

(2)Controls: Individuals with no psychiatric disorders.

(3)Outcome: Measurement of 2D:4D ratio through direct or indirect tools, and availability of data.

(4)Study design: Case-control studies.

Data Extraction

Couples of researchers (SS, BC, AN, DA) independently reviewed and extracted the information from the included articles. Discrepancies were solved after consultation with a third reviewer (LF). We extracted data using a format which included:

(1)Study characteristics: author, year, country.

(2)Participants’ characteristics: type of diagnosis, diagnostic tool (only for psychiatric patients), sample size, mean age, age range, proportion of males.

(3)2D:4D measurement tool.

(4)Mean and standard deviation (SD) of the 2D:4D ratio. If reported in the studies, data were extracted separately for left and right hand, and for males and females.

We contacted study authors via e-mail to request missing data or for clarification, providing an individualized data table for reporting the requested information.

Appraisal of Quality

Quality of the included studies was assessed by two review authors (AR, DA) using the Newcastle-Ottawa quality assessment scale for case-control studies [28]. Any discrepancy was solved after consultation with a third reviewer (LF). The Newcastle-Ottawa comprises eight items, categorized into three groups: the selection of the study groups; the comparability of the groups; and the ascertainment of either the exposure or outcome of interest for case-control or cohort studies, respectively. Stars awarded for each quality item serve as a quick visual assessment. Stars are awarded such that the highest quality studies are awarded up to nine stars [28].

Statistical Analyses

Meta-analysis calculations

The primary aim of this study was to compare the 2D:4D digit ratio in all psychiatric conditions, regardless of the hand measured and the gender of the included subjects. Most of the studies reported the means of 2D:4D divided by hand and gender. Some studies reported results only for the left or the right hand, while other studies reported aggregated data for left and right hands. Thus, we decided to combine the data to have a common estimate of the digit ratio, regardless of gender and hand. First, in order to combine the measure of right and left hand, we calculated the averaged means for left and right hands digit ratio. To avoid an underestimation of the SD, that might be the case in within-subjects data combination, we used the formula suggested by Borenstein et al. [29], setting as a correlation coefficient 0.8, as proposed in previous literature. Then, we computed a weighted mean for each study, in order to combine males and females digit ratios, while we calculated SD according to the Cochrane Handbook formula for grouping independent samples [30]. In case SD was missing [31], we replaced it with the mean of the SD among the same diagnostic category (i.e., autism spectrum disorder, ASD).

Effect size and heterogeneity

We used mean differences (MD) as effect size, being the 2D:4D always measured in the same unit. Studies were pooled using a random-effects model since a consistent heterogeneity among observational studies was expected. Between-study heterogeneity was assessed using the I² statistic. According to the Cochrane handbook, an I² of 0−40% represents a low heterogeneity, I² of 30−60% is moderate heterogeneity, I² of 50−90% indicates substantial heterogeneity [30]. Small study effects as a proxy of publication bias were explored with contour-enhanced funnel plots for the visual detection of asymmetries. Egger’s regression test was used to detect asymmetry in the funnel plots.

Subgroup analyses

For each psychiatric disorder, we conducted subgroup meta-analyses on males and females and left and right hands, separately. In the main analysis of the overall sample, we conducted a subgroup analysis also on children and adults. Chi-squared (χ2) was used to test differences between subgroups.

Analyses were conducted using meta package (v.4.9-9) within the open-source software environment R (v3.6). α was set at 0.05.

RESULTS

Characteristics of the Included Studies

Our search yielded a total of 619 articles, while four additional papers were retreived from other sources. After duplicates removal, we screened the titles and abstracts of 399 papers and read the full texts of 96 papers. We finally included 43 articles, evaluating the 2D:4D ratio in patients with the following psychiatric diagnoses:

(1)Neurodevelopmental disorders, specifically attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder (ADHD; n = 4), ASD (n = 16), and intellectual disability (ID; n =1)

(2)Schizophrenia (n = 8)

(3)Addictions, specifically alcohol dependence (n = 3) and heroin dependence (n = 2)

(4)Gender nonconformity, such as gender dysphoria, gender identity disorder, transsexualism, or transgenderism (n = 7)

(5)Mood disorders, specifically bipolar disorder (n = 1) and depression (n = 1).

The study selection process and the reasons for exclusion are reported in the PRISMA Flow Diagram (Fig. 1).

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

PRISMA Flow Chart of the study selection process. 2D:4D, Second-to-fourth Digit Ratio.

Neurodevelopmental disorders

Twenty-one studies evaluated the 2D:4D ratio in neurodevelopmental disorders, such as ADHD, ASD, and ID. Thirteen studies were conducted in Europe, specifically in the United Kingdom, The Netherlands, Belgium, Germany, Greece, Slovak Republic, Sweden, and Turkey. Six studies were conducted in Asia, specifically in Japan, Iran, Saudi Arabia, and Thailand. Finally, three studies were conducted in the United States. Participants were children in all the studies involving patients with ADHD and ID. Moreover, five studies evaluated 2D:4D in adults with ASD [32 -36], and two studies included mixed samples of children and adults with ASD [37,38]. Four papers included only males [36,39 -41], and in two articles [31,42], the proportion of males represented almost the entirety of the sample. Five studies evaluated the 2D:4D only in the right hand, twelve studies in both hands (left and right). In one paper the mean between the 2D:4D of left and right hands was calculated, and in two studies it was unclear which hand was measured. Measurement of 2D:4D was mostly direct (14 studies).

Characteristics of the included studies about 2D:4D ratio in patients with neurodevelopmental disorders are reported in Table 1 [31 -51].

Table 1.

Characteristics of studies evaluating 2D:4D in neurodevelopmental disorders, i.e., attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), autism spectrum disorder (ASD), and intellectual disability (ID)

Diagnosis (tool) First author Year Country Patients Controls Hand 2D:4D measurement tool


Number Mean age (range) Male, n (%) Number Mean age (range) Male, n (%)
ADHD (DSM-IV) Buru 2017 Turkey 104 / (7−17) 77 (74.03) 436 / (7−17) 240 (55.04) L, R Digital compass (direct)
ADHD (DSM-IV-TR) Lemiere 2010 Belgium 64 / (7−12) 47 (73.44) 46 / (7−12) 25 (54.35) / Scanned photocopies (indirect)
ADHD (Clinical) Martel 2009 USA 168 12.71 (8−17) 106 (63.10) 144 14.01 (8−17) 72 (50) R Ruler (direct)
ADHD (DSM-IV-TR) McFadden 2005 USA 46 9.83 (7−15) 34 (73.91) 33 10.6 (7−15) 17 (51.51) L, R Photocopy or digital scanner (indirect)
ASD (DSM-IV) Al-Zaid 2015 Saudi Arabia 31 5.59 (3−8) 31 (100) 29 5.59 (3−8) 29 (100) R Digital Caliper (direct)
ASD (DSM-IV-TR, ADOS) Baharara 2014 Iran 48 7.38 (7−8) 38 (79.17) 41 7.46 (7−8) 31 (75.61) L, R Ruler (direct)
ASD (Clinical, ADOS) Bejerot 2012 Sweden 50 30 (20−47) 26 (52) 53 30.3 (20−47) 28 (52.83) L, R Digital Caliper (direct)
ASD (DSM-IV) De Bruin 2006 The Netherlands 24 9 (6−14) 24 (100) 96 9 (6−13) 96 (100) L, R Digital Caliper (direct)
ASD (DSM-IV-TR) Falter 2008 UK 28 12.7 (/) 27 (96.43) 28 12.7 (/) 27 (96.43) L, R Digital Caliper (direct)
ASD (ADI-R) Hauth 2014 The Netherlands 216 11.6 (4−21) 178 (82.40) 174 11 (4−21) 79 (45.40) L, R Tape (indirect)
ASD (DSM-IV) Krajmer 2011 Slovak Republic 56 11.2 (/) 56 (100) 32 12.1 (/) 32 (100) R Scanner (indirect)
ASD (DSM-IV-TR, ICD-10) Lai 2013 UK 60 27.5 (18−49) 0 60 27.8 (18−49) 0 L, R Digital Caliper (direct)
ASD (DSM-5, DISCO) Masuya 2015 Japan 52 28.5 (/) 35 (67.31) 116 27.74 (/) 59 (42.45) L, R Digital Caliper (direct)
ASD (DSM-IV) Milne 2006 UK 23 10.8 (/) 22 (95.65) 23 10.8 (/) 10 (43.48) / Digital Caliper (direct)
ASD (ASQ) Manning 2001 UK 72 / (2−15) 62 (86.11) 72 / (2−15) 62 (86.11) Mean L, R Digital Caliper (direct)
ASD (DSM-IV) Noipayak 2009 Thailand 46 5.25 (1.5−15) 39 (84.78) 46 5.25 (1.5−15) 39 (84.78) L, R Digital Caliper (direct)
ASD (Clinical) Rohde 2018 Germany 26 42.86 (20−55) 14 (53.84) 26 41.44 (20−55) 14 (53.85) R Ruler (direct)
ASD (ADOS, ADI-R) Schieve 2018 USA 599 / (2−5) 487 (81.30) 811 / (2−5) 431 (53.14) L, R Scanner (indirect)
ASD (DSM-IV) Sugie 2010 Japan 98 12.7 (5−31) 82 (85.71) 89 / / R Digital Camera (indirect)
ASD (DSM-IV-TR) Togo 2019 Japan 20 26.75 (/) 20 (100) 14 26 (/) 14 (100) L, R Digital Photos (indirect)
Intellectual disability (WISC-III) Ypsilanti 2008 Greece 100 17.4 (14−18) 47 (47) 85 19.24 (18−23) 37 (43.53) R Digital Caliper (direct)

2D:4D, Second-to-fourth Digit Ratio ADI-R, Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised; ADOS, Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule; ASQ, Autism Screening Questionnaire; DSM, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders; L, left; R, right; WISC-III, Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Third Edition; ICD, International Classification of Diseases; DISCO, Diagnostic Interview for Social and Communication Disorders; /, not available.

Schizophrenia

Eight studies measured 2D:4D in patients with schizophrenia and controls. Half of the studies were conducted in Asia, and particularly two in India [52,53], one in China [54], and one in Singapore [55]. Moreover, two studies were conducted in Turkey [56,57], one in Spain [58], and one in Germany [22]. All participants were adults with mean ages ranging from 22 [57] to 47 [58]. Samples generally included both males and females, apart from Bolu et al. [57] that recruited only males. All papers have evaluated 2D:4D in both hands, apart from Collinson et al. [55] that has measured the ratio solely of the right hand. Measurements were always direct, except in two cases [22,54]. Characteristics of the included studies about 2D:4D ratio in patients with schizophrenia are reported in Table 2.

Table 2.

Characteristics of studies evaluating 2D:4D in schizophrenia

Diagnosis First author Year Country Patients Controls Hand 2D:4D measurement tool


Number Mean age (range) Male, n (%) Number Mean age (range) Male, n (%)
Schizophrenia (DSM-IV) Akgül 2017 Turkey 48 39.85 (18−55) 25 (52.08) 48 39.73 (18−55) 25 (52.08) L, R Digital Caliper (direct)
Schizophrenia (SCID-I) Bolu 2015 Turkey 103 22.73 (/) 103 (100) 100 21.98 (/) 100 (100) L, R Digital Caliper (direct)
Schizophrenia (DSM-IV) Collinson 2010 Singapore 64 30.5 (/) 33 (51.56) 64 27.4 (/) 33 (51.56) R Digital Caliper (direct)
Schizophrenia (DSM-IV) Divakaran 2012 India 200 31.61 (/) 106 (53) 177 33.82 (/) 92 (51.97) L, R Digital Caliper (direct)
Schizophrenia (DSM-IV) Paipa 2018 Spain 51 47 (18−65) 33 (64.71) 50 45.5 (18−65) 31 (62) L, R Digital Caliper (direct)
Schizophrenia (DSM-IV-TR) Qian 2016 China 178 33.8 (15−62) 76 (42.70) 365 33.16 (17−63) 218 (59.73) L, R Photography (indirect)
Schizophrenia (DSM-IV) Venkatasubramanian 2011 India 79 24.4 (/) 41 (51.90) 75 31.1 (/) 37 (49.33) L, R Digital Caliper (direct)
Schizophrenia (SCID-I) Zhu 2014 Germany 51 26.49 (18−45) 24 (47.06) 51 24.98 (18−45) 23 (45.10) L, R Scanner (indirect)

2D:4D, Second-to-fourth Digit Ratio; DSM, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders; L, left; R, right; SCID-I: Structured Clinical Interview for DSM IV Axis I Disorders; /, not available.

Addictions

Two papers recruited individuals with alcohol dependence and were conducted in South Korea [59] and Germany [60]. Moreover, we included three studies involving participants with heroin dependence, which were conducted in Turkey [61,62] and Germany [63]. Participants were all adults, with mean ages ranging from 22.8 [61] to 51.2 [59]. Three studies included only men [59,61,62], while in the remaining two articles samples were mixed. Three papers measured 2D:4D ratio in both hands; one paper considered the average between the left and right hand [60] and one measured only right hand [62]. Measure-ments were indirect in three papers [59,60,63], and direct in two articles [61,62]. See Table 3 for details regarding studies evaluating 2D:4D in addictions.

Table 3.

Characteristics of studies evaluating 2D:4D in addictions

Diagnosis First author Year Country Patients Controls Hand 2D:4D measurement tool


Number Mean age (range) Male, n (%) Number Mean age (range) Male, n (%)
Alcohol dependence (DSM-IV) Han 2016 South Korea 87 51.2 (/) 87 (100) 52 48.32 (/) 52 (100) L, R Scanner (indirect)
Alcohol dependence (DSM-5, ICD-10) Lenz 2017 Germany 200 48 (42−54) 113 (56.50) 240 48 (39−56) 133 (55.42) Mean L, R Scanner (indirect)
Alcohol dependence (ICD-10) Kornhuber 2011 Germany 131 / (24−77) 87 (66.41) 185 / (24−77) 83 (44.86) L, R Scanner (indirect)
Heroin dependence (DSM-5) Canan 2018 Turkey 150 22.8 (/) 150 (100) 266 23 (/) 266 (100) L, R Digital Caliper (direct)
Heroin dependence (DSM-IV) Cicek 2017 Turkey 62 24.09 (18−45) 62 (100) 50 24.42 (18−45) 50 (100) R Digital Caliper (direct)

2D:4D, Second-to-fourth Digit Ratio; DSM, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders; ICD, International Classification of Diseases; L, left; R, right; /, not available.

Gender non-conforming identity

Six papers (including seven studies) measured 2D:4D in people with gender nonconforming identity who had a psychiatric diagnosis or were followed by specialized clinics. Particularly, in four studies participants had a diagnosis of gender identity disorder, in one study a diagnosis of transsexualism, whereas they were defined as transgender in one paper. Studies were conducted mainly in Europe, specifically one in The Netherlands (including two studies) [64], one in Germany [65], one in the Republic of Serbia [66], and one in Switzerland [67]. Moreover, one study was conducted in Japan [68] and one in the United States [69]. One study involved children [64]; all the other studies recruited adults, with the exception of Leinung and Wu [69], where the age of participants was not reported. All studies evaluated 2D:4D in both hands; Leinung and Wu [69] have measured the ratio only in the dominant hand. Measurements were conducted directly in half of the studies, and indirectly in the remaining articles. Study characteristics are reported in Table 4 [66].

Table 4.

Characteristics of studies evaluating 2D:4D in individuals with gender non-conforming identities

Diagnosis First author Year Country Patients Controls Hand 2D:4D measurement tool


Number Mean age (range) M-to-F, n (%) Number Mean age (range) M-to-F, n (%)
Gender identity disorder (DSM-IV) Hisasue 2012 Japan 37 27 (19−45) 0 20
35 (23−60) 0 L, R Photocopies (indirect)
Gender identity disorder (DSM-IV) Kraemer 2009 Switzerland 56 37.1 (18−65) 39 (69.64) 366 37.1 (18−65) 176 (48.08) L, R Digital Caliper (direct)
Transgender (Clinical) Leinung 2017 USA 118 Not reported 68 (57.62) 37 Not reported 19 (51.35) Dominant Digital Caliper (direct)
Transsexualism (Clinical) Schneider 2006 Germany 106
37.24 (/) 63 (59.53) 123 39 (/) 58 (47.15) L, R Photocopies (indirect)
Gender identity disorder (DSM-IV) Vujović 2014 Republic of Serbia 80 31.25 (/) 42 (52.50) 93
29 (/) 45 (48.39) L, R Digital Caliper (direct)
Gender identity disorder (DSM-IV) Wallien (study 1) 2008 The Netherlands 147 40.53 (/) 96 (65.30) 202 41.25 (/) 90 (44.55) L, R Scanner (indirect)
Wallien (study 2) 2008 The Netherlands 101 8.29 (/) 67 (66.33) 146 7.8 (/) 74 (50.68) L, R Scanner (indirect)

2D:4D, Second-to-fourth Digit Ratio; DSM, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders; L, left; M-to-F, male-to-female; R, right; /, not available.

Mood disorders

One paper [70] recruited patients with major depression in Germany, and another study recruited individuals affected by bipolar disorder [71] in the United States. All participants were adults and mainly women. In the two studies regarding mood disorders, 2D:4D was measured in both hands and using indirect measurement tools. Details regarding the study characteristics are reported in Table 5.

Table 5.

Characteristics of studies evaluating 2D:4D in mood disorders

Diagnosis First author Year Country Patients Controls Hand 2D:4D measurement tool


Number Mean age (range) Male, n (%) Number Mean age (range) Male, n (%)
Bipolar Disorder (MINI) Tegin 2019 USA 50 52.9 (/) 21 (42) 50 48.6 (/) 21 (42) L, R Scanner (indirect)
Depression (DSM-IV) Sanwald 2019 Germany 139 39.4 (18−65) 49 (35.3) 137 28.74 (18−63) 49 (35.8) L, R Scanner (indirect)

2D:4D, Second-to-fourth Digit Ratio; DSM, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders; MINI, Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview; L, left; R, right; /, not available.

Meta-analyses of the Included Studies

2D:4D ratio in all psychiatric disorders

To evaluate the global differences of 2D:4D in psychiatric disorders, we conducted four main meta-analyses. In the first one, we pooled the data of all 43 studies, finding that psychiatric patients had significantly lower 2D:4D than healthy controls (n = 43, overall sample = 9,484, MD = −0.0056, 95% confidence interval [CI] from −0.0093 to −0.002, I2 = 74%). The forest plot is presented in Figure 2 and the funnel plot in Figure 3.

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

Meta-analysis of the 2D:4D ratio pooling all psychiatric disorders (n = 43). 2D:4D, Second-to-fourth Digit Ratio; SD, standard deviation; MD, mean difference; CI, confidence interval.

Fig. 3.

Fig. 3

Funnel plot of the included studies (n = 43).

Then, data extracted from included studies were pooled by hand, gender, and age. No differences were found between left and right hand in any psychiatric disorder (χ2 = 0.85, df = 1, p = 0.36). However, considering each subgroup independently, it could be observed that the 2D:4D in the right hand showed significant lower 2D:4D in psychiatric patients than controls; conversely, no differences between psychiatric patients and controls were detected in the left hand. Moreover, no significant differences were detected between males and females (χ2 = 0.44, df = 1, p = 0.51), even if both groups showed significantly lower 2D:4D in psychiatric patients than controls. Finally, dividing the studies by age, it could be observed that both adults and children showed significantly lower 2D:4D ratio in psychiatric patients than controls, with no significant differences between the two groups (χ2 = 0.77, df = 1, p = 0.38). The forest plots are presented in the Supplementary Materials, Appendix 2 (available online) [27].

2D:4D ratio in autism spectrum disorder

The analyses of the 2D:4D ratio in ASD, with data pooled by hand and gender, showed a statistically significant difference between patients and controls (n = 16, overall sample = 2,981, MD = −0.006, 95% CI from −0.0119 to −0.0001). Heterogeneity was moderate (I2 = 53%). The results of the meta-analysis are presented in Figure 4A.

Fig. 4.

Fig. 4

Meta-analyses of the 2D:4D ratio in individual psychiatric disorders. 2D:4D, Second-to-fourth Digit Ratio; SD, standard deviation; MD, mean difference; CI, confidence interval.

Subgroup analyses with studies divided by hand and gender did not detect any significant difference between ASD patients and controls. The forest plots of subgroup analyses are presented in the Supplementary Materials, Appendix 3 (available online) [27].

2D:4D ratio in attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder

The meta-analysis of 2D:4D ratio in ADHD showed a statistically significant difference between patients and controls (n = 4, overall sample = 1,128, MD = −0.0124, 95% CI from −0.0188 to −0.0059). Heterogeneity was low (I2 = 0%). The results of the meta-analysis are presented in Figure 4B.

Subgroup analyses revealed significant differences in the right hand of both males (n = 3, overall sample = 526, MD = −0.0198, 95% CI from −0.036 to −0.0036, I2 = 65%) and females (n = 3, overall sample = 382, MD = −0.0245, 95% CI from −0.0451 to −0.0039, I2 = 66%). No significant differences were found in the left hand, neither in males nor in females. The forest plots of the subgroup analyses plot are presented in the Supplementary Materials, Appendix 4 (available online) [27].

2D:4D ratio in intellectual disability

Only one study [48] evaluated 2D:4D ratio in ID, without detecting any significant difference. Forest plot is presented in Figure 4C [27].

2D:4D ratio in schizophrenia

The meta-analysis of pooled data did not show any significant difference between patients with schizophrenia and other psychotic disorders and non-clinical controls (n = 8, overall sample = 1,700, MD = −0.0012, 95% CI from −0.0129 to 0.0105). Heterogeneity was high (I2 = 86%). The forest plot is presented in Figure 4D.

Subgroup analyses with data divided by gender and hand did not reveal any significant difference except for the right hand in males: in this group, the 2D:4D ratio was significantly higher in patients than controls (n = 6, overall sample = 882, MD = 0.009, 95% CI from 0.0004 to 0.0177, I2 = 64%). The subgroup analyses are presented in the Supplementary Materials, Appendix 5 (available online) [27].

2D:4D ratio in addictions

Pooling data of patients affected by addictions, we did find a significant difference, as patients had a significantly lower 2D:4D than controls (n = 5, overall sample = 1,423, MD = −0.014, 95% CI from −0.0199 to −0.0081). Heterogeneity was moderate (I2 = 50%). The forest plot is presented in Figure 4E.

The subgroup analyses revealed statistically significant differences in both the males right and left hands, while for females the difference between the patients and the control group was limited to the right hand. In all cases, the patients suffering from addictions had a smaller 2D:4D ratio than controls. The analyses are presented in the Supplementary Materials, Appendix 6 (available online) [27].

2D:4D ratio in gender non-conforming people

No significant differences were found between indivi-duals with gender non-conforming identities (n = 7, overall sample = 1,725, MD = −0.0051, 95% CI from −0.0131 to 0.0028). Heterogeneity was substantial (I2 = 65%). Forest plot is presented in Figure 4F.

Subgroup analyses did not detect any significant differences according to hand or biological sex (see Supple-mentary Materials, Appendix 7 [available online] [27]).

2D:4D ratio in mood disorders

We found no significant differences between patients with depression or bipolar disorder and healthy controls (n = 2, overall sample = 342, MD = 0.0027, 95% CI from −0.0071 to 0.0125). Heterogeneity was moderate (I2 = 49%). Forest plot is presented in Figure 4G.

Quality of the Included Studies

The quality of the studies included in the systematic review and meta-analysis has been reported in Table 6. Two studies were judged with a score of 4 out of 9 points [41,42], four studies with 5 out of 9 [49,55,65,66], ten studies with 6 out of 9, four studies with 7 out of 9 and the the remaining papers with 8 or 9 stars. As concerns the study selection, we have found a relatively low risk of bias in diagnostic criteria: in fact, we included only studies in which participants had received a psychiatric diagnosis by a clinician. Only two studies were judged as having high risk: first, participants recruited by Manning et al. [50] were members of an autistic society and the diagnoses were confirmed only using the Autism Screening Questionnaire (ASQ), which is not intended as a diagnostic tool [72]; second, in Schneider et al. [65], transgenders were followed by a specific clinic for transition, thus we assumed that a clinical diagnosis had been performed, even if not explicitly reported in the paper. As for the remaining items of study selection, the most problematic criterion was the representativeness of cases, as in most studies the authors did not report consecutive recruitment of patients, thus raising concerns regarding potential selection biases. Ten studies did not match patients to controls according to socio-demographic variables, while in seven study only one variable was considered (age or gender). Regarding exposure, we did not find major biases, since the 2D:4D ratio was measured in cases and controls using the same methodology with direct or indirect measures. However, in five studies [35,41,42,51,65] concerns were raised because of missing data.

Table 6.

Quality assessment of the included studies using the Newcastle-Ottawa Scale

Diag-nosis Study characteristics Selection Comparability Exposure Total (n)




First author Year Country Diagnostic adequacy Representa-tiveness of the cases Selection of controls Definition of controls Total (n) Comparability Total (n) Ascertain-ment of 2D:4D Same method of ascertainment for cases and controls Missing data rate Total (n)
Attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder
Buru 2017 Turkey * * / / 2 / 0 * * * 3 5
Lemiere 2010 Belgium * / * * 3 / 0 * * * 3 6
Martel 2009 USA * * * * 4 * (age) 1 * * * 3 8
McFadden 2005 USA * * * * 4 ** (age, gender, CEOAEs) 2 * * * 3 9
Autism spectrum disorder
Al-Zaid 2015 Saudi Arabia * / / * 2 ** (age, gender) 2 * * * 3 7
Baharara 2014 Iran * / * * 3 ** (age, gender) 2 * * * 3 8
Bejerot 2012 Sweden * / * * 3 ** (age, gender) 2 * * * 3 8
De Bruin 2006 The Netherlands * * * * 4 ** (age, gender) 2 * * * 3 9
Falter 2008 UK * / * * 3 ** (age, sex, non-verbal reasoning ability) 2 * * * 3 8
Hauth 2014 The Netherlands * * / * 3 / 0 * * * 3 6
Krajmer 2011 Slovak Republic * / / * 2 / 0 * * / 2 4
Lai 2013 UK * / / * 2 ** (age, gender) 2 * * * 3 7
Masuya 2015 Japan * / * * 3 ** (age, gender) 2 * * * 3 8
Milne 2006 UK * / / / 1 * (age) 1 * * / 2 4
Manning 2001 UK / / * * 2 * (age) 1 * * * 3 6
Nojpayak 2009 Thailand * / / / 1 ** (age, gender) 2 * * * 3 6
Rohde 2018 Germany * * / * 3 ** (age, gender) 2 * * / 2 7
Schieve 2018 USA * * * / 3 * (age) 1 * * / 2 6
Sugie 2010 Japan * / / / 1 ** (age, gender, IQ) 2 * * * 3 6
Togo 2019 Japan * * * / 3 ** (age, handedness, full scale IQ) 2 * * * 3 8
Intellectual disability
Ypsilanti 2008 Greece * * * * 4 / 0 * * * 3 7
Schizophrenia
Akgul 2017 Turkey * / * * 3 ** (age, gender, education) 2 * * * 3 8
Bolu 2015 Turkey * / * * 3 * (age) 1 * * * 3 7
Collinson 2010 Singapore * / * / 2 / 0 * * * 3 5
Divakaran 2012 India * * * * 4 ** (age, gender) 2 * * * 3 9
Paipa 2018 Spain * / * * 3 ** (age, gender) 2 * * * 3 8
Qian 2016 China * / * * 3 ** (age, gender) 2 * * * 3 8
Venkatasubramanian 2011 India * * / * 3 ** (age, sex) 2 * * * 3 8
Zhu 2014 China * * * * 4 ** (age, gender) 2 * * * 3 9
Alcohol dependence
Han 2016 South Korea * / * * 3 / 0 * * * 3 6
Lenz 2017 Germany * * * * 4 ** (age, gender) 2 * * * 3 9
Kornhuber 2011 Germany * * * / 3 / 0 * * * 3 6
Heroin dependence
Canan 2018 Turkey * * * * 4 * (age) 1 * * * 3 8
Cicek 2017 Turkey * * / / 2 * (age, education) 1 * * * 3 6
Gender nonconformity
Hisasue 2012 Japan * / / / 1 ** (age, gender) 2 * * * 3 6
Kraemer 2009 Switzerland * * * * 4 ** (age, gender, sexual orientation) 2 * * * 3 9
Leinung 2017 USA * / * * 3 / 0 * * * 3 6
Schneider 2006 Germany / * / / 1 ** (age, gender) 2 * * / 2 5
Vujović 2014 Serbia * * / / 2 / 0 * * * 3 5
Wallien (study 1, 2) 2008 The Netherlands * * * * ** (age, gender, sexual orientation) * * * 3 9
Mood disorders
Sanwald 2019 Germany * / * * 3 ** (age, gender, handedness) 2 * * * 3 8
Tegin 2019 USA * * * * 4 ** (age, gender, race, dominant hand) 2 * * * 3 9

One star (*) indicates that the item is satisfied, while a slash (/) is assigned when the item is not fulfilled by the study. Comparability can be judged with up to two stars according to the number of control parameters reported by the researchers. One star (*) indicates that the control group has been matched with the patients' group according to one parameter; two stars (**) indicate two or more control parameters. The total score for each study is given by the sum of the stars.

2D:4D, Second-to-fourth Digit Ratio; CEOAEs, click-evoked otoacoustic emissions; IQ, intelligence quotient.

DISCUSSION

The present meta-analysis aimed to evaluate the differences in 2D:4D ratio between psychiatric patients and controls, and thus to explore its potential usefulness as a clinical biomarker for psychiatric disorders. The first and main finding of our meta-analysis is that the 2D:4D ratio is significantly lower in patients than controls, indicating exposure to higher levels of prenatal testosterone in individuals with psychiatric disorder. The effect seemed more pronounced in males than females, in the right than the left hand, and in children than adults, even if between-groups differences were not statistically significant. Our finding is important because, on one hand, it may propose 2D:4D as a potential biomarker for psychiatric disorders in general; on the other hand, it highlights the role of prenatal exposure to hormones, specifically testosterone, in the etiopathogenesis of psychiatric disorders. Interestingly, prenatal exposure to androgens may explain the different sex ratios encountered in several conditions (e.g., schizophrenia, ASD, addictions).

As shown by the number of studies included in our meta-analysis, the literature has extensively evaluated the 2D:4D in ASD for which we have included 16 case-control studies. Nevertheless, the effective measurement of sex steroid levels in the amniotic fluid with a prospective follow-up has been performed by a few researchers. Auyeung et al. [73] positively correlated fetal testosterone levels with autism-related behaviors at 18−24 months and at 6−9 years [74]. Elevated fetal steroidogenic activity during the prenatal masculinization window in the amniotic fluid of autistic boys was confirmed by subsequent research [75]. On the contrary, Kung et al. [76] recently found no relationship between prenatal androgen exposure and autistic traits in typically developed children nor in young children with congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Of note, 2D:4D has been negatively associated with empathy [77], which is typically lower in people with ASD [78]; conversely, it seems positively correlated with systemizing traits [79], which are more pronounced in autistic individuals [80]. In 2002, Baron-Cohen developed the so-called theory of the “extreme male brain”. This theory assumes that women tend to have more social intelligence (i.e., empathizing ability), whereas men tend to excel at following rules and recognizing patterns (i.e., systemizing ability). The “male brain” is typical of individuals in whom systemizing is significantly better than empathizing, while the “female brain” defines the opposite cognitive profile. Using these definitions, ASD could be considered as an expression of the “extreme male brain” [81]. The potential role of fetal testosterone in the onset of ASD is further supported by the link found between autism and maternal polycystic ovarian syndrome, a condition associated with androgenic excess [82]. Moreover, both autistic women and their mothers have elevated rates of steroid-related cancers, such as breast and ovarian cancer [83]. In summary, our findings reflect previous literature: subjects with ASD have significantly lower 2D:4D than controls, suggesting higher levels of fetal testosterone exposure.

Contrary to previous studies [84], our results did not support the notion that patients with schizophrenia would be exposed to lower levels of prenatal testosterone. In fact, it has been hypothesized that schizophrenia, in opposition with ASD, could reflect the “extreme female brain”, with higher empathizing and lower systemizing abilities [81]. However, as underlined by other researchers, this theory might be erroneously based on the presump-tion that hyperdeveloped theory-of-mind skills in psychotic patients would be accurate and adaptive, rather than pathological [85]. Indeed, our findings did not confirm the theory of the “extreme female brain”, as 2D:4D did not significantly differ between patients with schizophrenia and controls, thus suggesting no differences in empathizing and/or systemizing traits, neither in prenatal exposure to sex hormones. In fact, a significant difference was found only in the right hand of males with schizophrenia (MD = 0.009).

The evidence on addictions was quite robust, as 2D:4D resulted significantly lower in patients than healthy controls (MD = −0.014), indicating exposure to higher levels of prenatal testosterone. Even if no specific studies have evaluated the levels of sex steroids in the amniotic fluid in people with addictions—due to the obvious difficulties in following up the subjects for many years—some studies have linked several typical features of individuals with substance misuse to the digit ratio. For instance, it has been shown that 2D:4D is negatively correlated with risk-taking [18,86] and sensation-seeking [87,88]. Such features have been in turn linked to a higher vulnerability to addictions [89]. 2D:4D appears also negatively correlated with aggression [90] and impulsivity [91], which are in turn connected to the use of illicit substances, particularly alcohol and heroin [92], such as those used by the groups of patients included in our meta-analysis. Other authors [93] have argued an association between 2D:4D and externalizing behavioral symptoms in young boys: it is well-known that conduct disturbances confer an increased risk for substance abuse later in life [94]. Notably, this negative association between 2D:4D and externalizing behaviors may partially explain also the significant difference found between children with ADHD and non-clinical controls (MD = −0.0124).

Our meta-analyses did not retrieve any significant differences between people with non-conforming gender identities, mood disorders and ID, and non-clinical controls. The case of gender identity might seem surprising since in the imagination sex hormones are strictly connected to gender expression. However, it has been reported that gender identity is not exclusively related to prenatal exposure of androgens [95,96], but appears to be strongly influenced by genetic and social factors, with adoles-cence being a key period for the development of non-conforming identities [97,98]. As far as concern ID and mood disorders, it is worth noting that we have retrieved only one and two studies, respectively, and thus it is too premature to drive to any conclusion.

To our knowledge, this is the most up-to-date and comprehensive meta-analysis examining the 2D:4D ratio in psychiatry. However, some limitations should be discussed. First, we have included only papers in which psychiatric diagnoses were confirmed by clinicians or valid international diagnostic criteria. Therefore, we have excluded papers reporting analyses about 2D:4D in self-diagnosed individuals, or individuals which were classified as having a disorder only according to self-reported questionnaires [99,100]. Also, we excluded papers about new addictions, such as videogames or computer addiction, which have been instead considered in a previous meta-analysis [101]. A second major limitation is related to the different types of measurement used by the authors included in the studies. In fact, some authors have suggested that indirect 2D:4D measurements (such as in many of the studies included in the present review) may overestimate the length of the ring finger thereby distorting the 2D:4D ratio [16]. Another limitation, directly related to the meta-analytic approach, is the presence of clinical heterogeneity which could not be controlled as for statistical heterogeneity. Even if we tried to reduce clinical heterogeneity by selecting patients with standardized diagnoses, we could not account for the presence of individual or genetic differences, as well as for the influence of environmental factors. Finally, the number of studies included in the meta-analyses were generally small.

In conclusion, our results are promising and highlight the importance of prenatal hormonal factors in the etiopathogenesis of some psychiatric disorders. However, they need to be cautiously interpreted as the measurement of 2D:4D ratio cannot prescind from a complex and exhaustive assessment process. It is important to consider, in fact, that a number of other physiological and pathological conditions linked to the prenatal exposure to sex hormones may influence the length of digit ratio, thus representing potential confounders. The absence of a definite cut-off also represents a limitation for the clinical application of 2D:4D. Future research should investigate more in-depth the relationships between 2D:4D and psychiatric disorders, focusing on other conditions characterized by traits that seem to be linked to lower or higher digit ratio, such as borderline and antisocial personality disorders, eating disorders, or disruptive mood dysregulation disorder.

SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIALS

Supplementary data is available online (https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.12220493.v1).

Footnotes

Conflicts of Interest

No potential conflict of interest relevant to this article was reported.

Author Contributions

Laura Fusar-Poli conceptualized the study, supervised data extraction, and wrote the original draft of the manuscript. Alessandro Rodolico performed statistical analyses and participated to write the first draft of the manuscript. Serena Sturiale, Bianca Carotenuto, Antimo Natale, and Davide Arillotta performed data extraction and edited the manuscript. Spyridon Siafis helped in statistical analyses and contributed to write the manuscript. Maria Salvina Signorelli and Eugenio Aguglia supervised the project and edited the draft of the manuscript. All authors have read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

References

  • 1.Manning JT, Trivers RL, Thornhill R, Singh D. The 2nd: 4th digit ratio and asymmetry of hand performance in Jamaican children. Laterality. 2000;5:121–132. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.McIntyre MH, Ellison PT, Lieberman DE, Demerath E, Towne B. The development of sex differences in digital formula from infancy in the Fels Longitudinal Study. Proc Biol Sci. 2005;272:1473–1479. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2005.3100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Manning JT. Digit ratio: a pointer to fertility, behavior, and health. Rutgers University Press; Piscataway: 2002. [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Lutchmaya S, Baron-Cohen S, Raggatt P, Knickmeyer R, Manning JT. 2nd to 4th digit ratios, fetal testosterone and estradiol. Early Hum Dev. 2004;77:23–28. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2003.12.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Hönekopp J, Bartholdt L, Beier L, Liebert A. Second to fourth digit length ratio (2D:4D) and adult sex hormone levels: new data and a meta-analytic review. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2007;32:313–321. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2007.01.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Manning JT, Bundred PE, Newton DJ, Flanagan BF. The second to fourth digit ratio and variation in the androgen receptor gene. Evol Hum Behav. 2003;24:399–405. [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Auyeung B, Lombardo MV, Baron-Cohen S. Prenatal and postnatal hormone effects on the human brain and cognition. Pflugers Arch. 2013;465:557–571. doi: 10.1007/s00424-013-1268-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Hönekopp J, Watson S. Meta-analysis of digit ratio 2D:4D shows greater sex difference in the right hand. Am J Hum Biol. 2010;22:619–630. doi: 10.1002/ajhb.21054. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Manning JT, Leinster SJ. re: The ratio of 2nd to 4th digit length and age at presentation of breast cancer: a link with prenatal oestrogen? Breast. 2001;10:355–357. [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Jung H, Kim KH, Yoon SJ, Kim TB. Second to fourth digit ratio: a predictor of prostate-specific antigen level and the presence of prostate cancer. BJU Int. 2011;107:591–596. doi: 10.1111/j.1464-410X.2010.09490.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Rahman AA, Lophatananon A, Stewart-Brown S, Harriss D, Anderson J, Parker T, et al. Hand pattern indicates prostate cancer risk. Br J Cancer. 2011;104:175–177. doi: 10.1038/sj.bjc.6605986. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Fink B, Manning JT, Neave N. The 2nd-4th digit ratio (2D:4D) and neck circumference: implications for risk factors in coronary heart disease. Int J Obes (Lond) 2006;30:711–714. doi: 10.1038/sj.ijo.0803154. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Zhang W, Robertson J, Doherty S, Liu JJ, Maciewicz RA, Muir KR, Doherty M. Index to ring finger length ratio and the risk of osteoarthritis. Arthritis Rheum. 2008;58:137–144. doi: 10.1002/art.23237. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Fink B, Grammer K, Mitteroecker P, Gunz P, Schaefer K, Bookstein FL, et al. Second to fourth digit ratio and face shape. Proc Biol Sci. 2005;272:1995–2001. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2005.3179. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Bang AK, Carlsen E, Holm M, Petersen JH, Skakkebaek NE, Jørgensen N. A study of finger lengths, semen quality and sex hormones in 360 young men from the general Danish popula-tion. Hum Reprod. 2005;20:3109–3113. doi: 10.1093/humrep/dei170. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Manning JT, Fink B. Is low digit ratio linked with late menarche? Evidence from the BBC internet study. Am J Hum Biol. 2011;23:527–533. doi: 10.1002/ajhb.21186. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Choi IH, Kim KH, Jung H, Yoon SJ, Kim SW, Kim TB. Second to fourth digit ratio: a predictor of adult penile length. Asian J Androl. 2011;13:710–714. doi: 10.1038/aja.2011.75. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Hönekopp J. Relationships between digit ratio 2D:4D and self-reported aggression and risk taking in an online study. Pers Individ Dif. 2011;51:77–80. [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Montag C, Bleek B, Breuer S, Prüss H, Richardt K, Cook S, et al. Prenatal testosterone and stuttering. Early Hum Dev. 2015;91:43–46. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2014.11.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Peters M, Manning JT, Reimers S. The effects of sex, sexual orientation, and digit ratio (2D:4D) on mental rotation perfor-mance. Arch Sex Behav. 2007;36:251–260. doi: 10.1007/s10508-006-9166-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Fink B, Manning JT, Neave N, Tan U. Second to fourth digit ratio and hand skill in Austrian children. Biol Psychol. 2004;67:375–384. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2004.03.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Zhu YK, Li CB, Jin J, Wang JJ, Lachmann B, Sariyska R, et al. The 2D:4D ratio of the hand and schizotypal personality traits in schizophrenia patients and healthy control persons. Asian J Psychiatr. 2014;9:67–72. doi: 10.1016/j.ajp.2014.01.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Hönekopp J, Schuster M. A meta-analysis on 2D:4D and athletic prowess: substantial relationships but neither hand out-predicts the other. Pers Individ Dif. 2010;48:4–10. [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Coates JM, Gurnell M, Rustichini A. Second-to-fourth digit ratio predicts success among high-frequency financial traders. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2009;106:623–628. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0810907106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Grimbos T, Dawood K, Burriss RP, Zucker KJ, Puts DA. Sexual orientation and the second to fourth finger length ratio: a meta-analysis in men and women. Behav Neurosci. 2010;124:278–287. doi: 10.1037/a0018764. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Moher D, Liberati A, Tetzlaff J, Altman DG. Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. Ann Intern Med. 2009;151:264–269. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-151-4-200908180-00135. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Fusar-Poli L, Rodolico A, Sturiale S, Carotenuto B, Natale A, Arillotta D, et al. figshare [Internet] Digital Science. 3.47 MB; London: [2020 Apr 30]. Second-to-fourth digit ratio (2D:4D): a potential biomarker for psychiatric disorders? - Supplementary Materials [dataset] Available from: https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.12220493.v1. [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Stang A. Critical evaluation of the Newcastle-Ottawa scale for the assessment of the quality of nonrandomized studies in meta-analyses. Eur J Epidemiol. 2010;25:603–605. doi: 10.1007/s10654-010-9491-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Borenstein M, Hedges LV, Higgins JP, Rothstein HR. Introduc-tion to meta-analysis. John Wiley & Sons; Borenstein M, Hedges LV, Higgins JP, Rothstein HR. Introduc-tion to meta-analysis. Hoboken: 2011. [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Higgins JPT, Green S. Cochrane handbook for systematic reviews of interventions. Cochrane Collaboration; Higgins JPT, Green S. Cochrane handbook for systematic reviews of interventions. Oxford: 2011. [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Falter CM, Plaisted KC, Davis G. Visuo-spatial processing in autism--testing the predictions of extreme male brain theory. J Autism Dev Disord. 2008;38:507–515. doi: 10.1007/s10803-007-0419-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Bejerot S, Eriksson JM, Bonde S, Carlström K, Humble MB, Eriksson E. The extreme male brain revisited: gender coherence in adults with autism spectrum disorder. Br J Psychiatry. 2012;201:116–123. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.111.097899. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Lai MC, Lombardo MV, Suckling J, Ruigrok AN, Chakrabarti B, Ecker C, et al. Biological sex affects the neurobiology of autism. Brain. 2013;136(Pt 9):2799–2815. doi: 10.1093/brain/awt216. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Masuya Y, Okamoto Y, Inohara K, Matsumura Y, Fujioka T, Wada Y, et al. Sex-different abnormalities in the right second to fourth digit ratio in Japanese individuals with autism spectrum disorders. Mol Autism. 2015;6:34. doi: 10.1186/s13229-015-0028-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Rohde MS, Georgescu AL, Vogeley K, Fimmers R, Falter-Wagner CM. Absence of sex differences in mental rotation performance in autism spectrum disorder. Autism. 2018;22:855–865. doi: 10.1177/1362361317714991. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Togo S, Itahashi T, Hashimoto R, Cai C, Kanai C, Kato N, et al. Fourth finger dependence of high-functioning autism spectrum disorder in multi-digit force coordination. Sci Rep. 2019;9:1737. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-38421-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Hauth I, de Bruijn YG, Staal W, Buitelaar JK, Rommelse NN. Testing the extreme male brain theory of autism spectrum disorder in a familial design. Autism Res. 2014;7:491–500. doi: 10.1002/aur.1384. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Sugie Y, Sugie H, Fukuda T, Osawa J. Study of HOXD genes in autism particularly regarding the ratio of second to fourth digit length. Brain Dev. 2010;32:356–361. doi: 10.1016/j.braindev.2009.05.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Al-Zaid FS, Alhader AA, Al-Ayadhi LY. The second to fourth digit ratio (2D:4D) in Saudi boys with autism: a potential screening tool. Early Hum Dev. 2015;91:413–415. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2015.04.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.De Bruin EI, Verheij F, Wiegman T, Ferdinand RF. Differences in finger length ratio between males with autism, pervasive developmental disorder‐not otherwise specified, ADHD, and anxiety disorders. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2006;48:962–965. doi: 10.1017/S0012162206002118. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Krajmer P, Spajdel M, Kubranska A, Ostatnikova D. 2D:4D finger ratio in Slovak autism spectrum population. Bratisl Lek Listy. 2011;112:377–379. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Milne E, White S, Campbell R, Swettenham J, Hansen P, Ramus F. Motion and form coherence detection in autistic spectrum disorder: relationship to motor control and 2:4 digit ratio. J Autism Dev Disord. 2006;36:225–237. doi: 10.1007/s10803-005-0052-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Lemiere J, Boets B, Danckaerts M. No association between the 2D:4D fetal testosterone marker and multidimensional attentional abilities in children with ADHD. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2010;52:e202–e208. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.2010.03684.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Martel MM. Conscientiousness as a mediator of the association between masculinized finger-length ratios and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2009;50:790–798. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02065.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.McFadden D, Westhafer JG, Pasanen EG, Carlson CL, Tucker DM. Physiological evidence of hypermasculinization in boys with the inattentive type of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) Clin Neurosci Res. 2005;5:233–245. [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Baharara J, Hojjati M, Rasti H, Sarabi Jamab M. The Ratio of Second to Fourth Digit Length (2D:4D) in Children with Autistic Disorder. Int J Pediatr. 2014;2:5–11. [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Noipayak P. The ratio of 2nd and 4th digit length in autistic children. J Med Assoc Thai. 2009;92:1040–1045. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Ypsilanti A, Ganou M, Koidou I, Grouios G. Digit ratio (2D:4D) in individuals with intellectual disability: investigating the role of testosterone in the establishment of cerebral lateralisation. Laterality. 2008;13:527–544. doi: 10.1080/13576500802117164. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Buru E, Gözil R, Bahçelioğlu M, Özkan S, İşeri E. Evaluation of the hand anthropometric measurement in ADHD children and the possible clinical significance of the 2D:4D ratio. East J Med. 2017;22:137–142. [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Manning JT, Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Sanders G. The 2nd to 4th digit ratio and autism. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2001;43:160–164. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Schieve LA, Tian L, Dowling N, Croen L, Hoover-Fong J, Alexander A, et al. Associations between the 2nd to 4th digit ratio and autism spectrum disorder in population-based samples of boys and girls: findings from the study to explore early development. J Autism Dev Disord. 2018;48:2379–2395. doi: 10.1007/s10803-018-3495-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Divakaran A, Narayanaswamy JC, Kalmady SV, Narayan V, Rao NP, Venkatasubramanian G. Family history correlates of digit ratio abnormalities in schizophrenia. Indian J Psychol Med. 2012;34:355–359. doi: 10.4103/0253-7176.108220. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Venkatasubramanian G, Arasappa R, Rao NP, Gangadhar BN. Digit ratio (2D:4D) asymmetry and Schneiderian first rank symptoms: implications for cerebral lateralisation theories of schizophrenia. Laterality. 2011;16:499–512. doi: 10.1080/1357650X.2010.499910. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Qian W, Huo Z, Lu H, Sheng Y, Geng Z, Ma Z. Digit ratio (2D:4D) in a Chinese population with schizophrenia. Early Hum Dev. 2016;98:45–48. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2016.05.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Collinson SL, Lim M, Chaw JH, Verma S, Sim K, Rapisarda A, et al. Increased ratio of 2nd to 4th digit (2D:4D) in schizo-phrenia. Psychiatry Res. 2010;176:8–12. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2009.08.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Akgül Ö, Küçükçoban O, Binbay T, Bora E, Alptekin K, Binnur Akdede B. Do clinical features relate to theory of mind, empathy and 2D:4D in schizophrenia? Psychiatry and Clinical Psychopharmacology. 2017;27:380–385. [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Bolu A, Oznur T, Develi S, Gulsun M, Aydemir E, Alper M, et al. The ratios of 2nd to 4th digit may be a predictor of schizophrenia in male patients. Clin Anat. 2015;28:551–556. doi: 10.1002/ca.22527. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Paipa N, Stephan-Otto C, Cuevas-Esteban J, Núñez-Navarro A, Usall J, Brébion G. Second-to-fourth digit length ratio is associated with negative and affective symptoms in schizophrenia patients. Schizophr Res. 2018;199:297–303. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2018.02.037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Han C, Bae H, Lee YS, Won SD, Kim DJ. The ratio of 2nd to 4th digit length in Korean alcohol-dependent patients. Clin Psychopharmacol Neurosci. 2016;14:148–152. doi: 10.9758/cpn.2016.14.2.148. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Lenz B, Mühle C, Braun B, Weinland C, Bouna-Pyrrou P, Behrens J, et al. Prenatal and adult androgen activities in alcohol dependence. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2017;136:96–107. doi: 10.1111/acps.12725. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Canan F, Sogucak S, Karaca S, Tegin C, Gecici O, Kuloglu M. The second to fourth digit (2D:4D) ratios in patients with heroin use disorder. Heroin Addict Relat Clin Probl. 2018;20:5–12. [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Cicek IE, Cicek E, Demirel B, Ayhan MG, Varsak N, Özbek SY, et al. Digit ratio (2D:4D), impulsiveness and aggression in male heroin addicts: a prospective controlled study. Pers Individ Dif. 2017;117:1–5. [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Kornhuber J, Erhard G, Lenz B, Kraus T, Sperling W, Bayerlein K, et al. Low digit ratio 2D:4D in alcohol dependent patients. PLoS One. 2011;6:e19332. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0019332. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Wallien MS, Zucker KJ, Steensma TD, Cohen-Kettenis PT. 2D:4D finger-length ratios in children and adults with gender identity disorder. Horm Behav. 2008;54:450–454. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2008.05.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Schneider HJ, Pickel J, Stalla GK. Typical female 2nd-4th finger length (2D:4D) ratios in male-to-female transsexuals-possible implications for prenatal androgen exposure. Psychoneuro-endocrinology. 2006;31:265–269. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2005.07.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Vujović S, Popović S, Mrvošević Marojević L, Ivović M, Tančić-Gajić M, Stojanović M, et al. Finger length ratios in Serbian transsexuals. ScientificWorldJournal. 2014;2014:763563. doi: 10.1155/2014/763563. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Kraemer B, Noll T, Delsignore A, Milos G, Schnyder U, Hepp U. Finger length ratio (2D:4D) in adults with gender identity disorder. Arch Sex Behav. 2009;38:359–363. doi: 10.1007/s10508-007-9262-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Hisasue S, Sasaki S, Tsukamoto T, Horie S. The relationship between second-to-fourth digit ratio and female gender identity. J Sex Med. 2012;9:2903–2910. doi: 10.1111/j.1743-6109.2012.02815.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Leinung M, Wu C. The biologic basis of transgender identity: 2D:4D finger length ratios implicate a role for prenatal androgen activity. Endocr Pract. 2017;23:669–671. doi: 10.4158/EP161528.OR. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Sanwald S, Widenhorn-Müller K, Wernicke J, Sindermann C, Kiefer M, Montag C. Depression is associated with the absence of sex differences in the 2D:4D ratio of the right hand. Front Psychiatry. 2019;10:483. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00483. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Tegin C, Canan F, El-Mallakh RS. The 2nd to 4th digit ratios (2D:4D) in patients with bipolar disorder. J Affect Disord. 2019;259:27–30. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2019.08.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Berument SK, Rutter M, Lord C, Pickles A, Bailey A. Autism screening questionnaire: diagnostic validity. Br J Psychiatry. 1999;175:444–451. doi: 10.1192/bjp.175.5.444. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Auyeung B, Taylor K, Hackett G, Baron-Cohen S. Foetal testosterone and autistic traits in 18 to 24-month-old children. Mol Autism. 2010;1:11. doi: 10.1186/2040-2392-1-11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Auyeung B, Baron-Cohen S, Ashwin E, Knickmeyer R, Taylor K, Hackett G. Fetal testosterone and autistic traits. Br J Psychol. 2009;100(Pt 1):1–22. doi: 10.1348/000712608X311731. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Baron-Cohen S, Auyeung B, Nørgaard-Pedersen B, Hougaard DM, Abdallah MW, Melgaard L, et al. Elevated fetal steroidogenic activity in autism. Mol Psychiatry. 2015;20:369–376. doi: 10.1038/mp.2014.48. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Kung KT, Spencer D, Pasterski V, Neufeld S, Glover V, O’Connor TG, et al. No relationship between prenatal androgen exposure and autistic traits: convergent evidence from studies of children with congenital adrenal hyperplasia and of amniotic testosterone concentrations in typically developing children. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2016;57:1455–1462. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12602. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Chapman E, Baron-Cohen S, Auyeung B, Knickmeyer R, Taylor K, Hackett G. Fetal testosterone and empathy: evidence from the empathy quotient (EQ) and the “reading the mind in the eyes” test. Soc Neurosci. 2006;1:135–148. doi: 10.1080/17470910600992239. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S. The empathy quotient: an investigation of adults with Asperger syndrome or high functioning autism, and normal sex differences. J Autism Dev Disord. 2004;34:163–175. doi: 10.1023/b:jadd.0000022607.19833.00. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Manning JT, Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Fink B. Is digit ratio (2D:4D) related to systemizing and empathizing? Evidence from direct finger measurements reported in the BBC internet survey. Pers Individ Dif. 2010;48:767–771. [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Baron-Cohen S, Richler J, Bisarya D, Gurunathan N, Wheelwright S. The systemizing quotient: an investigation of adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism, and normal sex differences. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2003;358:361–374. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2002.1206. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Baron-Cohen S. The extreme male brain theory of autism. Trends Cogn Sci. 2002;6:248–254. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(02)01904-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Katsigianni M, Karageorgiou V, Lambrinoudaki I, Siristatidis C. Maternal polycystic ovarian syndrome in autism spectrum disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Mol Psychiatry. 2019;24:1787–1797. doi: 10.1038/s41380-019-0398-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Ingudomnukul E, Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Knickmeyer R. Elevated rates of testosterone-related disorders in women with autism spectrum conditions. Horm Behav. 2007;51:597–604. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2007.02.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Voracek M. Digit ratio (2D:4D) as a marker for mental disorders: low (masculinized) 2D:4D in autism-spectrum disorders, high (feminized) 2D:4D in schizophrenic-spectrum disorders. Behav Brain Sci. 2008;31:283–284. [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Crespi B, Badcock C. Psychosis and autism as diametrical disorders of the social brain. Behav Brain Sci. 2008;31:241–261. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X08004214. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Stenstrom E, Saad G, Nepomuceno MV, Mendenhall Z. Testosterone and domain-specific risk: digit ratios (2D:4D and rel2) as predictors of recreational, financial, and social risk-taking behaviors. Pers Individ Dif. 2011;51:412–416. [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Roberti JW. A review of behavioral and biological correlates of sensation seeking. J Res Pers. 2004;38:256–279. [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Campbell BC, Dreber A, Apicella CL, Eisenberg DT, Gray PB, Little AC, et al. Testosterone exposure, dopaminergic reward, and sensation-seeking in young men. Physiol Behav. 2010;99:451–456. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2009.12.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Kreek MJ, Nielsen DA, Butelman ER, LaForge KS. Genetic influences on impulsivity, risk taking, stress responsivity and vulnerability to drug abuse and addiction. Nat Neurosci. 2005;8:1450–1457. doi: 10.1038/nn1583. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Hönekopp J, Watson S. Meta-analysis of the relationship between digit-ratio 2D:4D and aggression. Pers Individ Dif. 2011;51:381–386. [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Hanoch Y, Gummerum M, Rolison J. Second-to-fourth digit ratio and impulsivity: a comparison between offenders and nonoffenders. PLoS One. 2012;7:e47140. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0047140. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Tomlinson MF, Brown M, Hoaken PNS. Recreational drug use and human aggressive behavior: a comprehensive review since. 2003 Aggress. [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Eichler A, Heinrich H, Moll GH, Beckmann MW, Goecke TW, Fasching PA, et al. Digit ratio (2D:4D) and behavioral symptoms in primary-school aged boys. Early Hum Dev. 2018;119:1–7. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2018.02.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Hopfer C, Salomonsen-Sautel S, Mikulich-Gilbertson S, Min SJ, McQueen M, Crowley T, et al. Conduct disorder and initiation of substance use: a prospective longitudinal study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2013;52:511–518.e4. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2013.02.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Pasterski V, Zucker KJ, Hindmarsh PC, Hughes IA, Acerini C, Spencer D, et al. Increased cross-gender identification independent of gender role behavior in girls with congenital adrenal hyperplasia: results from a standardized assessment of 4- to 11-year-old children. Arch Sex Behav. 2015;44:1363–1375. doi: 10.1007/s10508-014-0385-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 96.Kleinemeier E, Jürgensen M, Lux A, Steensma TD, Cohen- Kettenis PT, Hiort O, et al. Psychosexual development in adolescents and adults with disorders of sex development--results from the German Clinical Evaluation Study. J Sex Med. 2013;10:2703–2714. doi: 10.1111/j.1743-6109.2012.02751.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 97.Steensma TD, Kreukels BP, de Vries AL, Cohen-Kettenis PT. Gender identity development in adolescence. Horm Behav. 2013;64:288–297. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2013.02.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 98.Surace T, Fusar-Poli L, Vozza L, Cavone V, Arcidiacono C, Mammano R, et al. doi: 10.1007/s00787-020-01508-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 99.Kornhuber J, Zenses EM, Lenz B, Stoessel C, Bouna-Pyrrou P, Rehbein F, et al. Low 2D:4D values are associated with video game addiction. PLoS One. 2013;8:e79539. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0079539. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 100.Quinton SJ, Smith AR, Joiner T. The 2 to 4 digit ratio (2D:4D) and eating disorder diagnosis in women. Pers Individ Dif. 2011;51:402–405. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2010.07.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 101.Siegmann EM, Bouna-Pyrrou P, Lenz B, Kornhuber J. Digit ratio (2D:4D) in relation to substance and computer use: a meta-analysis. J Neural Transm (Vienna) 2019;126:623–636. doi: 10.1007/s00702-019-02002-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Clinical Psychopharmacology and Neuroscience are provided here courtesy of Korean College of Neuropsychopharmacology

RESOURCES