Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2022 Jan 15.
Published in final edited form as: J Neurosci Methods. 2020 Nov 20;348:109005. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2020.109005

Can transsynaptic viral strategies be used to reveal functional aspects of neural circuitry?

Alexandra Rogers 2, Kevin T Beier 1,2,3,4,5,6
PMCID: PMC7856199  NIHMSID: NIHMS1650295  PMID: 33227339

Abstract

Viruses have proved instrumental to elucidating neuronal connectivity relationships in a variety of organisms. Recent advances in genetic technologies have facilitated analysis of neurons directly connected to a defined starter population. These advances have also made viral transneuronal mapping available to the broader neuroscience community, where one-step rabies virus mapping has become routine. This method is commonly used to identify inputs onto defined cell populations, to demonstrate the quantitative proportion of inputs coming from specific brain regions, or to compare input patterns between two or more cell populations. Furthermore, the number of inputs labeled is often assumed to reflect the number of synaptic connections, and these viruses are commonly believed to label strong synapses more efficiently than weak synapses. While these maps are often interpreted to provide a quantitative estimate of the synaptic landscape onto starter cell populations, in fact very little is known about how transneuronal transmission takes place. We do not know how these viruses transmit between neurons, if they display biases in the cell types labeled, or even if transmission is synapse-specific. In this review, we discuss the experimental evidence against or in support of key concepts in viral tracing, focusing mostly on the use of one-step rabies input mapping and related methods. Does spread of these viruses occur specifically through synaptic connections, preferentially through synapses, or non-specifically? How efficient is viral transneuronal transmission, and is this efficiency equal in all cell types? And lastly, to what extent does viral labeling reflect functional connectivity?

Keywords: Rabies, input mapping, transsynaptic, transneuronal, functional connectivity, one step, monosynaptic

Brief history of neural circuit mapping and development of one-step viral methods

The brain consists of a highly complex network of neurons which transmit information largely via synaptic connections. The understanding of how neurons are connected is thus critical to understanding both the basic and emergent properties of neural circuits. Though the advent of the microscope enabled an appreciation of the cellular structure of most tissues, the brain remained recalcitrant to study, in large part because thin sections yielded gray blobs of tissue that showed little about the organization of the brain. It was only through the work of Camillo Golgi and Ramón y Cajal that the Neuron Doctrine was established, which put forth the idea that the nervous system was made of discrete cells. This was facilitated by the advent of the Golgi stain, which enabled Ramón y Cajal and others to detail the exquisite morphological details of i ndividual neurons for the first time1. Though this technique was groundbreaking, it requires staining whole blocks of tissue at once, stains neurons non-selectively, and requires thin sections of stained tissue to be imaged. It is thus not suitable for tracing long-distance connectivity between brain regions, nor mapping connections in a targeted fashion.

These barriers were first overcome in 1948 by Paul Weiss and Helen Hiscoe2, who demonstrated the transport of cellular components from the cell body into axonal processes. Later, axonal transport was used to label axon terminals with radioactive amino acids taken up by the cell body 35. For the first time, outputs of cells located in a defined brain region could be labeled without first significantly damaging the tissue. Kristensson and colleagues demonstrated retrograde transport of the horseradish peroxidase (HRP) protein from the axon to the cell body68. This method was more sensitive than previous approaches, as the catalytic properties of HRP enabled amplification of signals in labeled cells. Also, since HRP forms a visible reaction product with benzidine, several different histochemical methods can be used to label HRP-containing neurons, including those using benzidine hydrochloride or tetramethyl benzidine as a substrate9. Further improvements were made by conjugating HRP with plant lectins such as wheat germ agglutinin (WGA) which increased axonal uptake and retrograde transport10,11. Though the lack of toxicity and increased sensitivity were both improvements on previous methods, HRP-WGA labeled both inputs (retrograde) and outputs (anterograde) of neurons near where the tracer was injected. In addition, spread could occasionally be observed to secondary neurons, which could either be a benefit or a drawback. Since this time, a number of retrograde tracing strategies have been developed, including several fluorescent molecules such as propidium iodide, Nuclear Yellow, and Fluoro-Gold, which are efficiently retrogradely transported from axons to cell bodies1215. Multiple strategies are available for anterograde tracing as well, such as the plant lectin Phaseolus vulgaris PHA-L16 and biotinylated dextran amine (BDA)17. Additionally, the lipophilic DiI1820 has been used widely to trace axonal tract projections. While these approaches enable identification of neuronal populations projecting to or from a targeted brain region, they all lack cell type-specificity, in some cases lack directional specificity, and those that can transmit transneuronally such as WGA do not amplify, leading to diminution of signal with distance. Though here we lump molecularly distinct methods into common categories for simplicity, each method has its unique pros and cons, which have been reviewed more extensively elsewhere21,22.

Viruses have in large part solved many of these shortcomings. Herpes simplex virus (HSV) and related family members were observed almost a century ago to transmit along nerve tracts23 and were the first viruses to be used to trace neural circuits. Unlike small molecular dyes, viruses replicate in second-order neurons and can therefore self-amplify. This enables robust labeling across multiple neuronal connections. Kristensson showed that HSV can spread along chains of neurons in both the retrograde and anterograde directions24. Viral spread proceeds quickly from the initial inoculation site to higher order neurons. Despite its strengths, this approach has two key limitations: one, the virus can label inputs and outputs, and it can be difficult to discern if a given labeled cell is an input or output of the primary cell(s). Two, the virus can transmit along chains of connected neurons, and thus the presence of viral labeling cannot unambiguously delineate the order of connectivity. Early studies which traced the inputs to various muscles could mitigate this problem as the initial transfer could only be retrograde (muscle to motor neuron to motor input), and by quickly sacrificing the animal, uninterpretable multi-step viral transmission can be largely avoided. An example of this approach was illustrated by Rouiller and colleagues through combining HRP and pseudorabies (PRV) labeling of motor neurons and their inputs. In these experiments, HRP and PRV-labeled neurons were defined as first-order neurons, and neurons labeled by PRV but not HRP were defined as second-order neurons. To limit the extent of PRV spread, rats were sacrificed as soon as they started to show somatic signs of infection25. In order to conduct circuit mapping using these polysynaptic viruses, careful titration of infection timing is essential26.

HSV and similar viruses such as PRV were considered to spread via synaptic connections, largely based on the observation that the majority of viral spread is consistent with known anatomical connectivity. However, it is important to note that for these viruses, the synaptic specificity of transmission, relative to transmission to nearby non-connected neurons, has not been rigorously tested, or at least has not been publicly reported, and non-specific spread to surrounding non-neuronal cells such as glia is also observed2729. In addition, some viral strains exhibit biases in their direction of transport. For example, the Bartha strain of PRV appears to exclusively transmit between neurons in the retrograde direction30,31, while other viruses such as the HSV H129 prefer anterograde transneuronal transmission32,33. In the latter case, retrograde transmission has also been observed in multiple studies34,35. In addition, most HSV/PRV recombinants are highly cytotoxic (with a few exceptions36), and their large genomes make genetic engineering relatively slow and laborious.

Due in part to these limitations, the rabies virus (RABV) has emerged as the most widely used vector for transneuronal labeling. Wild-type RABV was originally used to map circuits, using a careful titration of timing to construct circuit diagrams37,38. Although comparing cells labeled at different time points following viral injection can be used as an indicator of the degree of connectivity between labeled neurons in a circuit, it cannot unambiguously define connectivity relationships nor can it differentiate strong indirect inputs from weak direct inputs39. RABV spread appears to be retrograde-specific, with a few exceptions in the dorsal root ganglia4042; however, the rapid transneuronal transmission and its ability to infect humans prevent the widespread use of wild-type rabies43. A major breakthrough occurred in 2007 when a genetically engineered version lacking the viral glycoprotein44 was used to enable single step-restricted viral spread45. Rather than relying on a temporal analysis of the neurons labeled a given number of days after infection, by having an essential gene removed from the viral genome and having it provided only in select cell types the virus could only label neurons projecting directly to the “starter” neurons expressing the glycoprotein, delivered via transfection or viral delivery. This approach also allows for cell type-specificity, as RABV can be targeted to specific cell types by virtue of expression of an exogeneous viral receptor such as TVA on the cells of interest and the use of RABV pseudotyped with exogeneous glycoproteins, such as ASLV-A. Importantly, electrophysiological results in cortical slice cultures showed that 9 out of 11 virally-labeled cells were synaptically connected to labeled starter cells, suggesting that the virus transmits predominantly to synaptically-connected neurons45. The fact that this virus cannot spread on its own has enabled its widespread use in neuroscience labs to map neuronal connectivity. In this review we will critically evaluate the use of transneuronal viral technologies for mapping neural circuits. We will focus principally on the use of one-step RABV input mapping, highlighting other viral technologies to emphasize critical or common features.

What is the evidence that viruses label synaptically-connected populations?

RABV and HSV/PRV are commonly assumed to spread through synaptic connections, and this assumption is rarely questioned. The original assertions about the transsynaptic preference of viral transmission can be traced back to an observational study by Ernest Goodpasture and Oscar Teague in 1923, where HSV was transferred from the lip of a human patient onto a rab bit’s cornea, where it transmitted via the optic nerve to the brain and produced encephalitis23. RABV and HSV/PRV have been noted to label both direct, weak connections and indirect, strong connections39. A commonly held assumption is that these viruses can label second-order neurons in a relatively unbiased fashion. However, it is worth revisiting and critically assessing the data in support of or against each of these assumptions.

Viral spread along known anatomical pathways forms the basis for the assumption that viral transneuronal transfer has a transsynaptic preference. This premise is inherently problematic; is viral spread to unexpected populations indicative of previously unknown synaptic connections, or of non-synaptic spread of virus? In some cases this has led to the discovery of new circuits, while in others the results have not been explained. In one example, the specificity of retrograde transmission was used to explain an enigmatic result whereby injection of a supposedly retrograde-specific PRV Bartha into one eye resulted in retinal ganglion cell labeling in the opposite eye46. This was first believed to have occurred through anterograde transmission from retinal ganglion cells along the optic nerve in the injected eye to retinorecipient reg ions, and subsequent retrograde transmission to the contralateral eye. However, this was inconsistent with the supposed retrograde specificity of this virus. A more thorough investigation found that the PRV Bartha traveled through autonomic circuits innervating the eye to retinorecipient regions, whereby it transmitted to the contralateral eye. These results are thus a classical example of the virus providing new information about unexpected connectivity. In a second example, using RABV in mice we identified unexpected inputs from lateral habenula (LHb) neurons onto ventral tegmental area dopamine neurons (VTA-DA) projecting to the nucleus accumbens lateral shell (NAcLat), a projection not hypothesized by previous work47,48. We will discuss the implications of this work in the following section. We also identified novel connections from starburst amacrine cells onto alpha ganglion cells in mice using the vesicular stomatitis virus (VSV), and Viney and colleagues identified novel inhibitory connections from monostratified amacrine cells onto intrinsically photosensitive retinal ganglion cells using PRV15249,50. In these cases, the presence of direct synaptic connections was confirmed using slice electrophysiology. However, other observations are more difficult to reconcile with synaptic transmission. For example, after injecting VSV into the nostril of young mice, while the pattern of transmission in the olfactory bulb was consistent with anterograde transsynaptic spread, we observed viral transmission into and along the rostral migratory stream (RMS)51. The RMS is composed of neuronal progenitor cells migrating from the subventricular zone to the olfactory bulb. These cells are thought to make transient contacts, though whether synaptic-like contacts occur among these cells is not clear.

In general, the extent of spread to synaptically-connected neurons vs. nearby neurons based on proximity alone is not clear. The principal evidence for a synaptic mechanism of spread is that most viral labeling along defined neuroanatomical tracts is consistent with known connectivity. In support of this mechanism, simultaneous pre-synaptic uptake was observed of RABV virions budding from postsynaptic surfaces52. However, in vivo transmission of virions through a synapse-only route to mediate infection has not been observed. The relatively low level of glia labeling, especially at early time points post -viral injection, has been used as evidence in support of spread through synapses. Preferential viral spread to neurons could also be due to selective tropism or reduced infectivity of glial cells. However, transmission to glial cells has been noted to occur for all neurotropic viruses, including HSV2729,53, PRV54,55, VSV.5658, and RABV5961. This labeling undermines the absolute synaptic specificity of viral spread. The question then is whether the spread we observe is preferentially through synapses, or simply to cells with nearby processes.

How efficient is viral transmission? Do viruses label input cells equally?

An important consideration in any viral transneuronal experiment is the percentage of total input cells to the cell population of interest that get labeled by the virus. This number is difficult to discern when tracing inputs onto populations of starter neurons but is feasible when examining inputs onto single cells. The first study to provide this estimate was from Marshel and colleagues, who performed RABV one-step input tracing experiments from single layer 2/3 cortical neurons62. In this work, each starter cell gave rise to approximately 48 input neurons. As a layer 2/3 cortical pyramidal neuron receives input from ~1000 neurons at the age tested, the authors estimated that RABV was labeling ~5% of input neurons. Two newer studies performing a similar experiment in vivo reported a higher efficiency, with ~10% and 40% reported in the Rossi and Wertz studies, respectively63,64. Given that the experiments are fundamentally similar, the reasons for this discrepancy are not clear, though it may reflect the longer incubation time used in the Wertz and Rossi studies (up to 11 days or two weeks post-RABV injection, versus up to 5 days in the Marshel study). Of note, we regularly terminate RABV input mapping experiments after 5 days post -RABV injection because while we indeed see more putative input neurons labeled with longer incubation times, the number of labeled glia cells increases rapidly with time (unpublished observations). In a third study sampling inputs from populations of layer 2/3 neurons instead of single cells, the ratio of total inputs/starter cells was approximately 50:1, also suggesting an input labeling efficiency near 5%65. A fourth, unrelated study with a different virus in a different circuit used VSV mapping in the retina to estimate the efficiency of transneuronal labeling. We used a modified VSV tracing strategy to label starburst amacrine inputs onto direction-selective retinal ganglion cells50. Given that each direction-selective ganglion cell is thought to receive input from ~200 starburst amacrine cells and we observed 4–5 labeled starburst amacrine cells labeled near each direction-selective ganglion cell after 2 days post-infection, we estimated a transmission efficiency of roughly 2–3%. This number may be slightly lower due to results being quantified after 2 days post-infection, rather than the 5 or more days used in the RABV studies. Therefore, while a range of values have been reported, most studies suggest that RABV and other transneuronal viruses label a minority of the total inputs to a given cell. However, it is important to note that there is a large number of variables in any such experiment, including the expression of viral helper genes, number of cells expressing the helper genes, age of animal, incubation time, etc. Therefore, while the reported numbers provide a frame of reference, how various factors contribute to the efficiency of viral spread, individually or in combination, is not known, but is beginning to be explored 66.

Another unanswered question is whether the virus can transmit via all synapses equally well. If only a small portion of the input population is being sampled in any given experiment, it is important to know whether labeling is an accurate representation of the actual synaptic connectivity or if it is biased towards particular cell types. Given that viruses use cellular receptors to gain access to cells and that these receptors are not all expressed equally in all cells, whether or not the viral receptor(s) is expressed on presynaptic cells may influence input labeling efficiency. For example, RABV has affinity for three known receptors: the nicotinic acetylcholine receptor, NCAM, and p75NTR6769. However, each is sufficient but not necessary for viral infection. Thus which, if any, of these receptors are engaged in vivo is not known. Additionally, the topology of synapses may influence the efficiency of RABV transmission. For example, many GABAergic inputs are located proximally to the cell body, while excitatory inputs connect onto spines located more distally on the dendrites. However, identifying potential biases requires a detailed quantification of the numbers of synapses from excitatory and inhibitory neurons onto defined starter cell populations in order to compare synapse numbers to RABV input. While a handful of such quantifications have been completed, they were not done on the same cell types that have been used as starter populations for RABV input mapping. Therefore, this remains an open question. Also, cells receive neuromodulatory inputs, many of which do not form classic synaptic contacts. Therefore, RABV may not transmit well to neuromodulatory connections such as from the dopaminergic nigrostriatal pathway. Indeed, one study compared RABV spread to midbrain DA inputs either from two different populations of medium spiny neurons in the dorsal striatum (when EnvA-pseudotyped RABV was injected into the striatum, infected medium spiny neurons, and spread to their inputs), or from G-deleted RABV containing its own envelope protein, RABV-G, injected directly injected into the striatum. The authors found that RABV one-step transmission to DA neurons from medium spiny neurons occurred at a much lower frequency than when the RABV-G-enveloped virus was injected into the striatum70. These results suggest that transmission to neuromodulatory inputs may occur less frequently than to neurons making more conventional contacts, principally synapses. Note that reduced spread to neuromodulatory inputs does not distinguish synaptic vs. non-synaptic spread, it only suggests that spread to input cells that make non-canonical contacts onto starter cells may occur at a lower efficiency.

Limitations in interpreting inputs to cell populations

One-step RABV tracing is typically conducted from populations of cells defined by location in the brain, expression of a recombinase protein (e.g., Cre or Flp), and/or projection site. Therefore, the inputs labeled represent a composite of inputs to individual cells within the starter population. It is not known if the inputs labeled equally innervate all starter neurons, if they innervate only a subset of neurons, or somewhere in between. One study from Schwarz and colleagues attempted to use results from brains with sparse RABV input labeling to perform a simulation of the input distribution to each norepinephrine cell in the locus coeruleus71. They estimated that each norepinephrine neuron receives a minimum of one input from at least 9 brain regions, but that the regions from which innervation is received are likely heterogenous. However, this estimation has two key limitations; one, RABV labels only a small fraction of the total inputs to starter cells, a caveat discussed above and acknowledged by the authors. Another limitation is that this simulation requires a fixed number of starter cells, which were quantified by visible expression of an mCherry fluorophore tagged to TVA, the receptor for EnvA-pseudotyped, G-deleted RABV. However, only a small amount of TVA protein is required for RABV entry72. Therefore, it is possible that there may be starter cells that were not visibly expressing the mCherry protein. This limitation applies to most RABV studies where starter cells are quantified by expression of a fluorescent tag. The best way to ensure an accurate count of starter cells is to introduce TVA/RABV-G in a targeted fashion to each cell, for example through single cell electroporation 62. Though this has been done for single cells, it is a highly laborious and low-throughput procedure not suited for rapid whole-brain quantification of inputs to targeted cells.

The fact that most experiments using one-step RABV are conducted on a population rather than single, defined cells makes it difficult to interpret the functional consequence of observed labeling patterns. For example, we and others demonstrated that RABV tracing shows that the LHb provides a quantitatively equal input to ventral tegmental area dopamine (VTA-DA) and midbrain GABA neurons48,73,74. The LHb input comprised 5% of the total labeled inputs onto these cells. We also showed that the LHb provides ~5% of total inputs onto four different output-defined VTA-DA cell populations, including those that project to the nucleus accumbens lateral shell (NAcLat) that are involved in reward behaviors 47. This connection was thought to be weak to non-existent based on a previous study, as tested through electrophysiological recording of 4 neurons47. Stimulation of the LHb is also highly aversive, likely through strong innervation of GABA neurons in the ventral midbrain7577. However, by conducting electrophysiological recordings from 27 neurons we were able to detect a weak but direct input from the LHb onto VTA-DA-NAcLat neurons48. Thus, though RABV tracing data suggests that 5% of the inputs onto VTA-DA-NAcLat neurons arise from the LHb, electrophysiological and behavioral analyses suggest that this connection is functionally weak. In fact, RABV labeling in our study suggested that the LHb provides a ~3x larger input to VTA-DA-NAcLat neurons than the laterodorsal tegmentum (LDT), which is thought to provide a much stronger synaptic and functional input to these neurons47. Although the observation that LHb neurons are heterogeneous in their projections to the midbrain provides a potential explanation7880, population mapping using one-step RABV, even to defined subsets of neurons in midbrain, cannot alone be used to infer functional relationships between different circuit elements.

To what extent does viral mapping reflect functional connectivity?

The premise of neuroanatomy is that an understanding of neural connectivity is necessary for understanding brain function. However, it is clear that even a detailed understanding of connectivity is not sufficient. The goal of viral approaches to neuroanatomical mapping is that through labeling specific neural circuits or pathways, precise mapping of these connections can be used to infer function of these pathways. However, the extent to which viral labeling reflects functional aspects of neuronal connectivity has not been thoroughly explored. One significant barrier is that for most circuits in the brain, we lack a rigorously validated connectivity map of the inputs and outputs of cell types. Without a “ground truth” of what the connectivity actually is, it is difficult to assess the performance of transneuronal viruses, and if the maps generated in this way can be used to infer circuit function. In fact, there are multiple pieces of evidence that RABV one-step input maps are insufficient to predict circuit function, several of which we will present here.

Using the somatosensory cortex as a model circuit

A precise and detailed understanding of the connectivity between neurons in the brain is required to assess factors that influence RABV spread. In terms of input-output connectivity, cortical microcircuits represent perhaps the best-studied circuit in the rodent brain. Though RABV spread is typically assumed to be proportional to the number of synaptic connections, this has never been shown. Karel Svoboda provided a discussion of this topic and the potential for non-synapse-specific RABV spread81. For example, DeNardo and colleagues examined inputs to excitatory neurons in different cortical layers in the somatosensory barrel cortex, and conducted electrophysiological recordings to validate a few of these connections65. However, the proportion of RABV-labeled inputs to these different cell types are not in quantitative agreement with those measured in a separate electrophysiological study, which performed paired recordings between excitatory neurons in different cortical layers82. For example, DeNardo and colleagues reported a relatively high proportion of layer 3 inputs to layer 6 starter cells, though that connection has been shown to be functionally weak65,82,83. Additionally, RABV tracing in the DeNardo study identified few or no inputs from layer 1 inhibitory neurons to any deeper layer excitatory cells, despite the fact that layer 1 has been shown to provide direct inhibition onto layer 2/3 pyramidal cells84,85. This same input from layer 1 inhibitory neurons onto layer 2/3 excitatory pyramidal cells was also not captured in a more recent study86, suggesting this false negative may be a limitation of RABV one-step labeling.

In addition to the question of whether RABV labeling reflects functional connectivity, the extent of synaptic specificity vs. transmission to passing neurites is not clear. Electron microscopy studies have demonstrated that projections from nearby neurons are highly intertwined without regard to their functional connectivity, resulting in many more non-synaptic than synaptic contacts between neurons, and that there is not a significant difference in axon-synapse distance between axons that make connections with particular spines and ones that do not87. We previously provided evidence, discussed in the following section, against non-synapse-specific spread in the VTA, though it remains a possibility. It is also possible that synaptic specificity may occur in some cells/synapse types but not others. It is also important to note the limitation of electrophysiological analyses in assessing neuronal connectivity. Because slice electrophysiological recordings require thin sections of tissue, it is possible that the preparation may sever connections that exist in the brain. It is also possible that connections atrophy as a consequence of tissue slicing. Thus, the preparation used may significantly influence results, particularly in experiments that require paired recordings.

Another way to assess the functional interaction between RABV-labeled inputs and starter neurons is to measure correlated activity between these cells. Connected populations should either show correlated, or anti-correlated, activity, depending on if the connections are excitatory or inhibitory, respectively. One study using a combination of RABV tracing and two-photon GCaMP imaging in the mouse visual cortex found that a large fraction of RABV-labeled inputs to a defined pyramidal neuron did not show a common visually-tuned response. In the minority of labeled cells that did show a common tuning, the correlation was weak63. However, a more recent study separated the contributions from excitatory and inhibitory neurons in this microcircuit and determined that the combined excitatory and inhibitory activity in labeled presynaptic cells correlated strongly (R=0.67) with the starter cell’s activity64. These results together provide functional data to support the possibility that RABV spreads preferentially between synaptically-connected neurons, though it stops short of demonstrating that RABV spread occurs specifically through synaptic connections.

While the above analysis is focused on cortical microcircuits, similar discrepancies can be observed for long-distance inputs onto different cortical neuron types. For example, Wall and colleagues88 used RABV tracing to characterize long-range inputs to three different cortical GABAergic interneuron types expressing either parvalbumin (PV), somatostatin (SST+), or vasoactive intestinal peptide (VIP). RABV labeling suggested that the ventral posteromedial nucleus (VPM) of the thalamus provides similar levels of input to cortical PV+ and SST+ interneurons. This observation is not consistent with previous results using optogenetic stimulation of thalamic inputs to the cortex, which suggested that PV+ interneurons receive a strong thalamic input while the input to putative SST+ interneurons is very weak89. Moreover, a more recent study90 of touch-evoked responses in four different neuron types in the barrel cortex – excitatory pyramidal neurons, PV+, SST+, and VIP+ interneurons – showed that while pyramidal and PV+ interneurons were strongly driven by thalamic input (likely from the VPM), VIP+ neurons did not respond, and SST+ neurons were activated but only after a long (>10 ms) latency indicative of a multi-synaptic response. An optimistic interpretation of these data would be that post-hoc electrophysiological analysis of connections suggested by RABV labeling in most cases is able to detect functional connections, providing support for the hypothesis that at least on a population level, RABV labels synaptically-connected cells, an observation supported by results in other brain regions 48,91. However, a more pessimistic outlook is that RABV one-step labeling neither provides an accurate picture of the synaptic weights between different cells nor labels all input populations onto starter neurons. RABV labeling appears to have a non-zero false negative rate (it does not label all inputs onto starter neurons, discussed above), a low to zero false positive rate (most labeled neurons are synaptically connected), and the percentage of inputs onto given cell type does not always scale with functional strength of connectivity.

Observations from other circuits

The general observation that RABV labeling does not comprehensively reflect functional connectivity extends to neural circuits in other brain regions. For example, in our study of the inputs onto different types of dopaminergic, GABAergic, and glutamatergic neurons in the VTA, biases in RABV labeling could be predicted by the density of input innervation onto each of these cell populations92. Furthermore, the location of these cells in the midbrain was related to the number of RABV-labeled input neurons in different input regions, but the cell type, as defined by neurotransmitter, was not correlated with whole-brain input patterns. While this could reflect that connections in the VTA are roughly equally distributed onto each cell population, it has been shown that a number of input sites more robustly innervate either DA or GABA neurons in the ventral midbrain47,9197. Another potential explanation for our results is that the spread of RABV is not synapse-specific, and thus the virus is released and gets taken up by any neuronal process that passes by star ter neurons. However, we performed a control experiment that argues against this possibility. We injected a G-deleted RABV directly into the VTA, where the virus could get taken up by passing inputs, and we observed a roughly ten-fold difference in the percentage of inputs from the medial habenula (MHb) and the striatum. The presence of synaptic inputs from the MHb onto VTA neurons is controversial, since a dense fiber bundle, the fasciculus retroflexus, passes through the VTA en route to terminating in the interpeduncular nucleus (IPN). Though a direct connection from MHb neurons onto VTA-DA neurons has yet to be demonstrated, activation of the dorsal aspect of the MHb is reinforcing while inhibition is aversive, which is consistent with direct activation of VTA-DA neurons, not inactivation as would be expected if the projection was solely or preferentially to local GABAergic neurons98. Consistent with this observation, most studies conducting RABV tracing from VTA cells report a small input from the MHb48,73,74, but the fraction is <3% of total inputs. The substantially larger fraction labeled when the G-deleted RABV was injected directly into the VTA was thus likely due to labeling of axons in passage.

In a second study, investigators examined inputs onto different cell types in the dorsal striatum99. This question had been investigated previously, and both studies found that, by and large, direct and indirect pathway medium spiny neurons share a common set of inputs and receive a quantitatively similar prop ortion of inputs from each brain region70,100. Note that in these studies the location of starter neurons was not carefully controlled, and thus may have played a role in the differences that were observed. In addition, statistical corrections for multiple comparisons were not conducted, which has historically been the case for nearly all papers employing the one-step RABV input mapping method or its variants. Choi and colleagues examined inputs onto direct pathway medium spiny neurons as well as two types of interneurons intermingled in the same region, PV+ and SST+ cells99. They noted a significant discrepancy between anatomical and physiological connectivity. Specifically, despite the fact that RABV tracing data suggested that SST+ interneurons and direct pathway D1-positive neurons received similar fractions of inputs from the anterior cingulate cortex and parafascicular thalamic nucleus, the SST+ neurons showed significantly reduced synaptic strength due to multiple factors including reduced probability of release, fewer release sites, and reduced postsynaptic sensitivity. Therefore, while RABV labeling suggested an equal number of inputs from two excitatory inputs onto these cells, the functional aspects of those connections were quite different.

Conclusion:

In consideration of over a decade of conducting RABV one-step tracing experiments from multiple cell types over many different circuits, there is no clear evidence that the extent of viral labeling can be used on its own to reproduce key aspects of functional connectivity. However, the majority of the data suggest that RABV input mapping can be used to detect synaptically-connected neurons, as where RABV labeling has been reported and tested, synaptic connections have also been observed, at least on a population level. Therefore, strictly from an anatomical standpoint, RABV labeling can be used to detect the presence of synaptic connections. In addition, it may be used as a predictive identifier for behaviorally relevant cell populations by detecting differences in connectivity induced by experience97,101.

The lack of clarity regarding synaptic transfer of virus gives rise to a significant chicken and egg confound. When the virus labels unexpected connections, is this because spread of the virus is non-synaptic or non-specific, or is it because this connection has been missed using other methods? This question could be important for precise anatomical mapping of neural connectivity and perhaps development/pruning of synaptic connections, though if the connections are all weak or functionally not relevant for behavioral outputs, the impact of discovering these connections may be minimal. On the other hand, elucidation of previously unknown direct connections may be relevant to explain unexpected behavioral results. We discussed earlier that RABV tracing detects a small but reliable input from the MHb to VTA-DA neurons. Whether this connection could explain the reinforcing properties of dorsal MHb stimulation remains to be tested.

If RABV labeling does not reflect the strength of connectivity, it must be influenced by factors that do not strictly relate to functional connectivity. However, the nature of these factors has largely been unexplored. Part of this is due to an incomplete understanding of the anatomical architecture and functional properties of circuits throughout the brain. We recently showed that RABV transmission can be influenced by neuronal activity97. In animals that had received a single dose of cocaine, we observed increased RABV labeling of inputs from the globus pallidus external segment (GPe) onto VTA-DA neurons. We then tested if this increase in inputs was due to a change in the number of synapses, strength of synapses, or activity of connections. We found no evidence of a change in the numbers or strength of synaptic connections from the GPe onto midbrain neurons but did find that activity in PV-positive GPe neurons was elevated 24 hours following the cocaine exposure97. Thus, RABV labeling can be influenced by other factors besides the presence or absence of synaptic connections. Interestingly, changes in neuronal activity were not observed to alter spread of RABV in primary neuronal cultures102. We also failed to observe differences in RABV or VSV transmission in organotypic hippocampal cultures (data not shown), suggesting that this phenomenon may not be universal, and may not occur in all circuits and cell types. However, evidence from our ongoing studies suggests that RABV labeling can detect changes in multiple cell types/circuits in vivo, and that differences can be detected with stimuli other than drugs of abuse. However, the potential contribution of changes in the numbers or strength of connections remains to be tested. Furthermore, just because we did not detect a change in synapse number or strength does not mean one did not occur. We prefer the use of RABV as a comparative tool to assess input changes in response to an experience 97, or to generate hypotheses to be tested using functional methods48, rather than to provide a quantitative map of the input landscape to targeted cell types. This is because so little is known about the mechanisms of RABV transmission that we should interpret as little as possible about the meaning of differences in viral labeling. Rather, identification of differences in viral labeling should be interpreted as a change in something, which then should be interrogated using more widely validated approaches. Questions about viral transneuronal labeling could only be answered through an increase in funding and research effort geared towards elucidating the basic cellular mechanisms of viral transmission in neuronal systems. Such efforts would likely give rise to improved viral tracers as well as a more sophisticated understanding of how data from these tracers should be interpreted.

Svoboda81 distinguishes four possibilities for synaptic specificity of RABV spread, summarized below:

  1. Strong synapse-specificity: Viral spread strongly reflects functional connectivity - spread is dictated by the number or strength of synapses. Viral transmission is not biased to and from specific cell types. Virus transmits primarily through synaptic contacts.

  2. Medium synapse-specificity: Viral spread reflects functional connectivity - spread is largely dictated by the number or strength of synapses, but some biases may influence proportions of inputs labeled. Virus transmits primarily through synaptic contacts.

  3. Weak synapse-specificity: Viral spread no longer reflects functional connectivity. Biases appear in transneuronal spread. Virus transmits primarily through synaptic contacts.

  4. No synapse-specificity: Viral spread no longer reflects functional connectivity. Biases appear in transneuronal spread. A significant amount of viral transmission occurs via local contacts, not synapse-specific.

He concludes that, at best, RABV transmission exhibits weak synaptic specificity and suggests that there is no compelling evidence that RABV spreads predominantly through synapses and does not simply spread to nearby processes. Our conclusions are largely in agreement with this assessment, though we are more optimistic that RABV transneuronal transmission occurs predominantly through synaptic connections, as direct injection of RABV into the brain labels inputs with a different quantitative distribution than one-step RABV labeling from neurons located at the same brain site70,92, and simple proximity labeling would be likely to label more glial cells from starter neurons. However, the distribution of neurites in the extracellular space that could be infected by injected RABV may be different than neurites in close proximity to starter cells. Also, RABV may have preferential tropism for neurons. It appears that, even though spread of RABV between neurons is not entirely synapse-specific, at least on a population level the inputs that are detected reflect the presence of synaptic connections. One key consideration is that the inputs quantified in RABV labeling experiments are those that were successfully infected by virions that completed the entire replication/infection cycle. This includes: replication in starter cells, assembly in starter cells, release from starter cells, entry into secondary cells, translocation of the intact virion to the cell body, uncoating of the virion, and successful replication/expression of fluorophore. If any of these steps fails, it would result in aborted infection events, which would not be scored. More likely than not, one of these steps is limiting and dictates specificity of transmission. However, without a clearer understanding of the basic mechanisms of viral transmission between neurons, we do not know which if any of these steps may disproportionately influence which cells become infected. One hypothesis is that one or more of these steps successfully occurs more frequently in synaptically-connected neurons. It is also possible that the mechanisms that dictate specificity for short-distance and long-distance connections are different. Successful infection of long-distance connections requires efficient long-distance retrograde transport from the axon terminal to the cell body, whereas infection of neurons whose soma is nearby to the starter neuron would not require efficient retrograde transport in secondary neurons, as the virion would already be close to the soma upon entry. Thus, it is possible that experiments using RABV to examine microcircuits near starter cells may be more susceptible to non-synaptic transmission events that occur through proximity labeling. However, without rigorous studies of the mechanisms of transneuronal viral transmission, this is strictly speculation. All these conclusions would be greatly strengthened by a more rigorous assessment of the synaptic specificity of RABV transneuronal labeling, as the electrophysiological evidence to date has fallen short of providing a clear answer to these questions.

Transneuronal viral tracing has emerged as a useful anatomical tool to label connected cells, and improved versions have the potential to interrogate functional properties of these connections. These advantages ensure that viral labeling methods, in particular RABV one-step input labeling, will remain a staple technique to investigate neural circuit structure/function. However, if one-step RABV truly has only weak synaptic specificity, this substantially limits our interpretations of RABV tracing data, and significant questions remain. These include, but are not limited to: Do viruses have cell type biases? If so, what are they? Do biases differ between viruses? Do viruses transmit through conventional synapses/neuromodulatory synapses/other contacts, or all the above? To what extent is spread synapse-specific? To what extent is viral transmission reflective of synapse number/strength/activity? Do the properties of viral spread chang e with the time of incubation or the multiplicity of infection (MOI) of the initial starter population? What fraction of inputs per cell does RABV label? Are these values consistent across cell types/brain regions or is there substantial variation?

To date, we have accumulated a large amount of indirect evidence that is consistent with synaptic specificity of viral transneuronal transmission. However, we still lack direct evidence of specificity (e.g., through electron microscopy). Though the majority of studies report results consistent with retrograde transsynaptic spread, in cases where different results are observed (glial transmission, anterograde spread, etc.), it is not clear why. Clearly, there are many questions that remain to be answered, which can only occur through more detailed research. Until we understand more about the basic mechanisms underlying viral transneuronal transmission, a cautious interpretation of viral mapping results is warranted. The more we know about how the virus works, the more we can take advantage of its unique properties to interrogate and decode the complex information carried by neural circuits.

Highlights.

  • One-step RABV mapping does not recapitulate key aspects of functional connectivity

  • Viruses label only a small fraction of the inputs onto first-order neurons

  • The majority of RABV spread is likely synaptic, though non-synaptic spread occurs

  • Users of transsynaptic technologies should be aware of limits in data interpretation

  • A deeper understanding of how viruses transmit in the brain is required

Acknowledgements:

We would like to thank Michael Toledano and other members of the Beier lab for a critical review of this manuscript. This review was supported by the NIH (R00 DA041445, DP2 AG067666), Tobacco Related Disease Research Program (T31KT1437, T31IP1426), American Parkinson Disease Association (APDA-5589562), Alzheimer’s Association (AARG-NTF-20–685694), New Vision Research (CCAD2020–002), and the Brain and Behavior Research Foundation (NARSAD 26845).

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

Conflicts of Interest: We have no competing interests to declare.

References:

  • 1.Cajal Ramón y, Santiago, and Azoulay Léon. Histologie Du Système Nerveux De L’homme & Des Vertébrés Paris: A. Maloine, 1909. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Weiss P & Hiscoe HB Experiments on the mechanism of nerve growth. J. Exp. Zool 107, 315–395 (1948). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Ochs S, Dalrymple D & Richards G Axoplasmic flow in ventral root nerve fibers of the cat. Exp. Neurol 5, 349–363 (1962). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Taylor AC & Weiss P Demonstration of axonal flow by the movement of tritium-labeled protein in mature optic nerve fibers. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A 54, 1521–1527 (1965). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Lasek R, Joseph BS & Whitlock DG Evaluation of a radioautographic neuroanatomical tracing method. Brain Res 8, 319–336 (1968). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Graham RC & Karnovsky MJ The early stages of absorption of injected horseradish peroxidase in the proximal tubules of mouse kidney: ultrastructural cytochemistry by a new technique. J. Histochem. Cytochem 14, 291–302 (1966). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Kristensson K & Olsson Y Retrograde axonal transport of protein. Brain Res 29, 363–365 (1971). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Kristensson K, Olsson Y & Sjöstrand J Axonal uptake and retrograde transport of exogenous proteins in the hypoglossal nerve. Brain Res 32, 399–406 (1971). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Mesulam MM & Rosene DL Sensitivity in horseradish peroxidase neurohistochemistry: a comparative and quantitative study of nine methods. J. Histochem. Cytochem 27, 763–773 (1979). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Gonatas NK, Harper C, Mizutani T & Gonatas J Superior Sensitivity of Conjugates of Horseradish Peroxidase with Wheat Germ Agglutinin for Studies of Retrograde Axonal Transport. J. Histochem. Cytochem 27, 728–734 (1979). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Gonatas NK & Avrameas S Detection of Carbohydrates with Lectin—Peroxidase Conjugates. Methods Cell Biol 15, 387–406 (1977). [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Kuypers HGJM, Bentivoglio M, Van Der Kooy D & Catsman-Berrevoets CE Retrograde transport of bisbenzimide and propidium iodide through axons to their parent cell bodies. Neurosci. Lett 12, 1–7 (1979). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Kuypers HGJM, Catsman-Berrevoets CE & Padt RE Retrograde anoxal transport of fluorescent substances in the rat’s forebrain. Neurosci. Lett 6, 127–133 (1977). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Bentivoglio M, Kuypers HGJM, Catsman-Berrevoets CE, Loewe H & Dann O Two new fluorescent retrograde neuronal tracers which are transported over long distances. Neurosci. Lett 18, 25–30 (1980). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Schmued LC, Heimer L Iontophoretic injection of fluoro-gold and other fluorescent tracers. J. Histochem. Cytochem 38, 721–723 (1990). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Gerfen CR & Sawchenko PE An anterograde neuroanatomical tracing method that shows the detailed morphology of neurons, their axons and terminals: Immunohistochemical localization of an axonally transported plant lectin, Phaseolus vulgaris leucoagglutinin (PHA-L). Brain Res 290, 219–238 (1984). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Veenman CL, Reiner A & Honig MG Biotinylated dextran amine as an anterograde tracer for single- and double-labeling studies. J. Neurosci. Methods 41, 239–254 (1992). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Honig MG & Hume RI Fluorescent carbocyanine dyes allow living neurons of identified origin to be studied in long-term cultures. J. Cell Biol 103, 171–187 (1986). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Honig MG & Hume RI Dil and DiO: versatile fluorescent dyes for neuronal labelling and pathway tracing. Trends Neurosci 12, 333–341 (1989). [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Sims PJ, Waggoner AS, Wang CH & Hoffman JF Mechanism by which cyanine dyes measure membrane potential in red blood cells and phosphatidylcholine vesicles. Biochemistry 13, 3315–3330 (1974). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Lanciego JL & Wouterlood FG A half century of experimental neuroanatomical tracing. J. Chem. Neuroanat 42, 157–183 (2011). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Wouterlood FG, Bloem B, Mansvelder HD, Luchicchi A, Deisseroth K The fourth generation of neuroanatomical tracing techniques: exploiting the offspring of optogenetics, J. Neurosci Meth 236, 331–348 (2014). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Goodpasture EW & Teague O Transmission of the Virus of Herpes Febrilis along Nerves in experimentally infected Rabbits. J. Med. Res 44, 139–184.7 (1923). [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Kristensson K Morphological studies of the neural spread of herpes simplex virus to the central nervous system. Acta Neuropathol 16, 54–63 (1970). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Rouiller EM, Capt M, Dolivo M & De Ribaupierre F Neuronal organization of the stapedius reflex pathways in the rat: a retrograde HRP and viral transneuronal tracing study. Brain Research 476, 21–28 (1989). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Kuypers HGJM & Ugolini G Viruses as transneuronal tracers. Trends Neurosci 13, 71–75 (1990). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Fournier JG, Privat A & Bouteille M Infection of Cultivated CNS Tissue with Herpes Virus, HSVII A Reappraisal. Acta neuropath 39, 177–180 (1977). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Vahlne A, Nystrom B, Sandberg M, Hamberger A & Lycke E Attachment of Herpes Simplex Virus to Neurons and Glial Cells. J. Gen. Virol 40, 359–371 (1978). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Kristensson K, Thormar H & Wisniewski HM Myelin lesions in the rabbit eye model as a bystander effect of herpes simplex and visna virus sensitization. Acta Neuropathol 48, 215–217 (1979). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Lomniczi B, Watanabe S, Ben-Porat T & Kaplan AS Genome Location and Identification of Functions Defective in the Bartha Vaccine Strain of Pseudorabies Virus. J. Virol 61, 796–801 (1987). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Strack AM & Loewy AD Pseudorabies virus: a highly specific transneuronal cell body marker in the sympathetic nervous system. J. Neurosci 10, 2139–2147 (1990). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Zemanick MC, Strick PL & Dixt RD Direction of transneuronal transport of herpes simplex virus 1 in the primate motor system is strain-dependent. Neurobiology 88, 8048–8051 (1991). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Lo L & Anderson DJ A Cre-dependent, anterograde transsynaptic viral tracer for mapping output pathways of genetically marked neurons. Neuron 72, 938–950 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Archin NM & Atherton SS Rapid spread of a neurovirulent strain of HSV-1 through the CNS of BALB/c mice following anterior chamber inoculation. J. Neurovirol 8, 122–135 (2002). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Archin NM & Atherton SS Infiltration of T-lymphocytes in the brain after anterior chamber inoculation of a neurovirulent and neuroinvasive strain of HSV-1. J. Neuroimmunol 130, 117–127 (2002). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Oyibo HK, Znamenskiy P, Oviedo HV, Enquist LW & Zador AM Long-term Cre-mediated retrograde tagging of neurons using a novel recombinant pseudorabies virus. Front. Neuroanat 8, 1–11 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Ugolini G Specificity of rabies virus as a transneuronal tracer of motor networks: transfer from hypoglossal motoneurons to connected second-order and higher order central nervous system cell groups. J. Comp. Neurol 356, 457–480 (1995). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Kelly RM & Strick PL Rabies as a transneuronal tracer of circuits in the central nervous system. J. Neurosci. Methods 103, 63–71 (2000). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Ugolini G Specificity of rabies virus as a transneuronal tracer of motor networks: Transfer from hypoglossal motoneurons to connected second-order and higher order central nervous system cell groups. J. Comp. Neurol 356, 457–480 (1995). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Tsiang H, Lycke E, Ceccaldi PE, Ermine A & Hirardot X The anterograde transport of rabies virus in rat sensory dorsal root ganglia neurons. J. Gen. Virol 70, 2075–2085 (1989). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Zampieri N, Jessell TM & Murray AJ Mapping sensory circuits by anterograde transsynaptic transfer of recombinant rabies virus. Neuron 81, 766–778 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Bauer A et al. Anterograde Glycoprotein-Dependent Transport of Newly Generated Rabies Virus in Dorsal Root Ganglion Neurons. J. Virol 88, 14172–14183 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Callaway EM Transneuronal circuit tracing with neurotropic viruses. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol 18, 617–623 (2008). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Mebatsion T, Konig M & Conzelmann KK Budding of rabies virus particles in the absence of the spike glycoprotein. Cell 84, 941–951 (1996). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Wickersham IR et al. Monosynaptic restriction of transsynaptic tracing from single, genetically targeted neurons. Neuron 53, 639–647 (2007). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Pickard GE et al. Intravitreal injection of the attenuated pseudorabies virus PRV Bartha results in infection of the hamster suprachiasmatic nucleus only by retrograde transsynaptic transport via autonomic circuits. J. Neurosci 22, 2701–2710 (2002). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Lammel S et al. Input-specific control of reward and aversion in the ventral tegmental area. Nature 491, 212–217 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Beier KT et al. Circuit Architecture of VTA Dopamine Neurons Revealed by Systematic Input-Output Mapping. Cell 162, 622–634 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Viney TJ et al. Local retinal circuits of melanopsin-containing ganglion cells identified by transsynaptic viral tracing. Curr. Biol. CB 17, 981–988 (2007). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Beier KT et al. Transsynaptic Tracing with Vesicular Stomatitis Virus Reveals Novel Retinal Circuitry. J. Neurosci 33, 35–51 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Beier KT et al. Anterograde or retrograde transsynaptic labeling of CNS neurons with vesicular stomatitis virus vectors. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A 108, 15414–15419 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Charlton KM & Casey GA Experimental rabies in skunks: immunofluorescence light and electron microscopic studies. Lab. Invest 41, 36–44 (1979). [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Ugolini G, Kuypers HGJM & Simmons A Retrograde transneuronal transfer of Herpes simplex virus type 1 (HSV 1) from motoneurones. Brain Res 422, 242–256 (1987). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Card JP et al. Neurotropic Properties of Pseudorabies Virus: Uptake and Transneuronal Passage in the Rat Central Nervous System. J. Neurosci 10, 1974–1994 (1990). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Rinaman L, Card JP & Enquist LW Spatiotemporal responses of astrocytes, ramified microglia, and brain macrophages to central neuronal infection with pseudorabies virus. J. Neurosci 13, 685–702 (1993). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Plakhov IV, Arlund EE, Aoki C & Reiss CS The Earliest Events in Vesicular Stomatitis Virus Infection of the Murine Olfactory Neuroepithelium and Entry of the Central Nervous System. Virology 209, 257–262 (1995). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Chauhan VS et al. Vesicular stomatitis virus infects resident cells of the central nervous system and induces replication-dependent inflammatory responses. Virology 400, 187–196 (2010). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.van den Pol AN, Dalton KP & Rose JK Relative neurotropism of a recombinant rhabdovirus expressing a green fluorescent envelope glycoprotein. J. Virol 76, 1309–1327 (2002). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Ray NB, Power C, Lynch WP, Ewalt LC & Lodmell DL Rabies viruses infect primary cultures of murine, feline, and human microglia and astrocytes. Arch. Virol 142, 1011–1019 (1997). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Potratz M et al. Astrocyte Infection during Rabies Encephalitis Depends on the Virus Strain and Infection Route as Demonstrated by Novel Quantitative 3D Analysis of Cell Tropism. Cells 9, 412 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Pfefferkorn C et al. Abortively Infected Astrocytes Appear To Represent the Main Source of Interferon Beta in the Virus-Infected Brain. J. Virol 90, 2031–2038 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Marshel JH, Mori T, Nielsen KJ & Callaway EM Targeting single neuronal networks for gene expression and cell labeling in vivo. Neuron 67, 562–574 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Wertz A et al. Single-cell-initiated monosynaptic tracing reveals layer-specific cortical network modules. Science 349, 70–74 (2015). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Rossi LF, Harris K & Carandini M Excitatory and inhibitory intracortical circuits for orientation and direction selectivity. bioRxiv 556795 (2019) doi: 10.1101/556795. [DOI]
  • 65.DeNardo LA, Berns DS, DeLoach K & Luo L Connectivity of mouse somatosensory and prefrontal cortex examined with trans-synaptic tracing. Nat. Neurosci 84, 778–789 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Lavin TK, Jin L, Lea NE & Wickersham IR Monosynaptic Tracing Success Depends Critically on Helper Virus Concentrations. Front. Synaptic Neurosci 12, 1–12 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Lentz TL, Burrage TG, Smith AL, Crick J & Tignor GH Is the acetylcholine receptor a rabies virus receptor? Science 215, 182–184 (1982). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Thoulouze M-I et al. The Neural Cell Adhesion Molecule Is a Receptor for Rabies Virus. J Virol 72, 7181–7190 (1998). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Tuffereau C, Bénéjean J, Blondel D, Kieffer B & Flamand A Low-affinity nerve-growth factor receptor (P75NTR) can serve as a receptor for rabies virus. EMBO J 17, 7250–7259 (1998). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Wall NR, De La Parra M, Callaway EM & Kreitzer AC Differential innervation of direct - and indirect-pathway striatal projection neurons. Neuron 79, 347–360 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Schwarz LA et al. Viral-genetic tracing of the input-output organization of a central noradrenaline circuit. Nature 524, 88–92 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Gray ER, Illingworth CJR, Coffin JM & Stoye JP Binding of more than one Tva800 molecule is required for ASLV-A entry. Retrovirology 8, 96 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Watabe-Uchida M, Zhu L, Ogawa SK, Vamanrao A & Uchida N Whole-Brain Mapping of Direct Inputs to Midbrain Dopamine Neurons. Neuron 74, 858–873 (2012). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Faget L et al. Afferent Inputs to Neurotransmitter-Defined Cell Types in the Ventral Tegmental Area. Cell Rep 15, 2796–2808 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Matsumoto M & Hikosaka O Lateral habenula as a source of negative reward signals in dopamine neurons. Nature 447, 1111–1115 (2007). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Ji H & Shepard PD Lateral habenula stimulation inhibits rat midbrain dopamine neur ons through a GABAA receptor-mediated mechanism. J. Neurosci 27, 6923–6930 (2007). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Christoph GR, Leonzio RJ & Wilcox KS Stimulation of the lateral habenula inhibits dopamine - containing neurons in the substantia nigra and ventral tegmental area of the rabbit. J. Neurosci 6, 613–619 (1986). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Wallace ML et al. Anatomical and single-cell transcriptional profiling of the murine habenular complex. Elife 9, 1–22 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Maroteaux M & Mameli M Cocaine Evokes Projection-Specific Synaptic Plasticity of Lateral Habenula Neurons. J. Neurosci 32, 12641–12646 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Li B et al. Synaptic potentiation onto habenula neurons in the learned helplessness model of depression. Nature 470, 535–541 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Svoboda K Using rabies virus for tracing neural connections: caveats and limitations exposed by studies of barrel cortex circuits [blog] http://web.archive.org/web/20201007174439/https://spikesphotons.blog/ (2019).
  • 82.Lefort S, Tomm C, Floyd Sarria JC & Petersen CCH The Excitatory Neuronal Network of the C2 Barrel Column in Mouse Primary Somatosensory Cortex. Neuron 61, 301–316 (2009). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Hooks BM et al. Laminar analysis of excitatory local circuits in vibrissal motor and sensory cortical areas. PLoS Biol 9, e1000572 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Cruikshank SJ et al. Thalamic control of layer 1 circuits in prefrontal cortex. J. Neurosci 32, 17813–17823 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Schuman B et al. Systems/Circuits Four Unique Interneuron Populations Reside in Neocortical Layer 1. J. Neurosci 39, 125–139 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Yetman MJ et al. Intersectional monosynaptic tracing for dissecting subtype-specific organization of GABAergic interneuron inputs. Nat. Neurosci 22, 492–502 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Kasthuri N et al. Saturated Reconstruction of a Volume of Neocortex. Cell 162, 648–661 (2015). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Wall NR et al. Brain-wide maps of synaptic input to cortical interneurons. J. Neurosci 36, 4000–4009 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Cruikshank SJ, Urabe H, Nurmikko AV & Connors BW Pathway-Specific Feedforward Circuits between Thalamus and Neocortex Revealed by Selective Optical Stimulation of Axons. Neuron 65, 230–245 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Yu J, Hu H, Agmon A & Svoboda K Recruitment of GABAergic Interneurons in the Barrel Cortex during Active Tactile Behavior. Neuron 104, 412–427.e4 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Weissbourd B et al. Presynaptic Partners of Dorsal Raphe Serotonergic and GABAergic Neurons. Neuron 83, 645–662 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Beier KT et al. Topological Organization of Ventral Tegmental Area Connectivity Revealed by Vir al-Genetic Dissection of Input-Output Relations. Cell Rep 26, 159–167.e6 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Jennings JH et al. Distinct extended amygdala circuits for divergent motivational states. Nature 496, 224–228 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Yang H et al. Nucleus Accumbens Subnuclei Regulate Motivated Behavior via Direct Inhibition and Disinhibition of VTA Dopamine Subpopulations. Neuron 97, 434–449.e4 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95.de Jong JW et al. A Neural Circuit Mechanism for Encoding Aversive Stimuli in the Mesolimbic Dopamine System. Neuron 101, 133–151.e7 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 96.Edwards NJ et al. Circuit specificity in the inhibitory architecture of the VTA regulates cocaine-induced behavior. Nat. Neurosci 20, 438–448 (2017). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 97.Beier KT et al. Rabies screen reveals GPe control of cocaine-triggered plasticity. Nature 549, 345–350 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 98.Hsu YWA et al. Role of the dorsal medial habenula in the regulation of voluntary activity, motor function, hedonic state, and primary reinforcement. J. Neurosci 34, 11366–11384 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 99.Choi K, Holly EN, Davatolhagh MF, Beier KT & Fuccillo MV Integrated anatomical and physiological mapping of striatal afferent projections. Eur. J. Neurosci 49, 623–636 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 100.Guo Q et al. Whole-brain mapping of inputs to projection neurons and cholinergic interneurons in the dorsal striatum. PLoS One 10, 1–11 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 101.Wall NR et al. Complementary Genetic Targeting and Monosynaptic Input Mapping Reveal Recruitment and Refinement of Distributed Corticostriatal Ensembles by Cocaine. Neuron 104, 916–930.e5 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 102.Bergami M et al. A Critical Period for Experience-Dependent Remodeling of Adult-Born Neuron Connectivity. Neuron 85, 710–717 (2015). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES