Abstract
Treatment-resistant schizophrenia, affecting approximately 20–30% of patients with schizophrenia, has a high burden both for patients and healthcare services. There is a need to identify treatment resistance earlier in the course of the illness, in order that effective treatment, such as clozapine, can be offered promptly. We conducted a systemic literature review of prospective longitudinal studies with the aim of identifying predictors of treatment-resistant schizophrenia from the first episode. From the 545 results screened, we identified 12 published studies where data at the first episode was used to predict treatment resistance. Younger age of onset was the most consistent predictor of treatment resistance. We discuss the gaps in the literature and how future prediction models can identify predictors of treatment response more robustly.
Key words: First episode, longitudinal, prediction, psychosis, schizophrenia, treatment resistant
Predictors of treatment-resistant schizophrenia: A systematic review of prospective observational studies
For approximately a third of patients with schizophrenia, standard antipsychotic medications do not adequately alleviate their psychotic symptoms (Conley and Kelly, 2001). This subgroup is termed treatment-resistant schizophrenia (TRS). The most common clinical and research criteria used for TRS is the failure to respond to two trials of non-clozapine antipsychotics, of adequate dose and duration (Suzuki et al., 2011; Howes et al., 2017).
Patients with TRS have higher rates of unemployment, worse quality of life, and poorer social and occupational functioning than people who respond to treatment (Iasevoli et al., 2016). Researchers have estimated that the direct healthcare costs for TRS in the US is 3–11-fold higher than for the schizophrenia population as a whole, with multiple hospitalisations accounting for a large proportion of this cost (Kennedy et al., 2014). In England, 25–50% of the National Health Service's (NHS) £11.8 billion mental health budget is allocated to schizophrenia services and TRS is thought to contribute a large proportion of these costs (Andrews et al., 2012; Killaspy et al., 2013).
Clozapine is the only antipsychotic recommended for TRS and is more effective than other antipsychotics in alleviating psychotic symptoms in patients with TRS (Kane et al., 1988; Siskind et al., 2016; Taylor, 2017). However, owing to its adverse effects, clozapine is only licenced in the UK (NICE, 2014) and most other developed countries (Warnez and Alessi-Severini, 2014) as a third-line treatment. Nevertheless, evidence suggests that TRS is often not recognised promptly, and that clozapine is offered after a delay of some years or not at all. According to treatment guidelines, the earliest that patients can be diagnosed with TRS, and prescribed clozapine, is 12 weeks after commencing antipsychotic treatment; however, Howes et al. (2012) report an average delay of 3.9 years, suggesting that there is considerable scope to shorten this period of inadequate treatment. Furthermore, patients with a shorter delay before clozapine initiation show a better symptomatic response to clozapine (Yoshimura et al., 2017).
Thus, there is a need to identify patients-who are likely to develop TRS-earlier in the course of their illness and expedite their access to specialist treatment; this may require moving beyond the current definition of TRS towards criteria based upon predictors and biomarkers, which quantify a patient's risk of developing TRS. If predictors of TRS can be identified, they may be useful in three ways: firstly, to identify TRS patients earlier in treatment so that they can be offered effective treatments earlier; secondly, to identify patients for clinical trials of interventions for TRS; and thirdly, to improve our understanding of the aetiology of TRS.
We present a comprehensive systematic review of all prospective observational studies in schizophrenia populations, which report baseline predictors of TRS. We focused solely on prospective observational studies to draw clearer conclusions regarding the causal relationship between predictors and TRS in naturalistic settings over a long follow-up, and because only longitudinal studies can identify risk factors at first episode that might predict TRS.
Method
Inclusion/exclusion criteria
Studies were included if they met the following inclusion criteria: (1) participants were diagnosed with schizophrenia, schizophreniform disorder, schizoaffective disorder, and/or a psychotic disorder; we did not exclude studies that also included affective disorders or substance-induced psychosis, given the diagnostic uncertainly around the first episode of psychosis; (2) participants were followed from the first episode or first treatment with antipsychotics; (3) the majority of participants were aged between 16 and 64 at baseline (we excluded studies that focused exclusively on children or older adults); (4) data were collected prospectively from the first episode; (5) the outcome was a categorical definition of TRS, established using longitudinal prospective medication history; and (6) a non-TRS comparison group was recruited and followed up in the same manner as the TRS group. Studies were excluded if (1) they were clinical trials, or if non-antipsychotic treatments, such as CBT or ECT, were administered as part of the study procedure; (2) the study focussed exclusively on early or late-onset schizophrenia; or (3) inferential statistics measuring the association between baseline variables and TRS were not reported, and our subsequent requests to the authors for unpublished data were unsuccessful.
Defining TRS
Only recently has attention been given to the standardisation of TRS criteria (Farooq et al., 2013; Suzuki et al., 2012; Lee et al., 2015; Howes et al., 2017); therefore, we did not restrict studies to one definition of TRS. We did, however, only include studies with a categorical definition of TRS to capture the key underlying concept-at least two treatment failures – and differentiate TRS from relative measures of response/nonresponse. If patients took clozapine at follow-up, we inferred that they met criteria for TRS. Clozapine prescription is likely to underestimate the true proportion of patients with TRS (Howes et al., 2012), but it is a pragmatic criterion, since clozapine is only used for TRS, except in very rare indications (e.g. psychosis in the context of Parkinson's disease or for people who suffer severe side-effects to other antipsychotics).
Literature search
Studies were identified by searching Pubmed, PsychINFO (up to October 2017), Medline (up to October 2017), Embase (up to October 2017), and OpenGrey on the 1 November 2017. In addition, we examined the first 20 pages of Google Scholar using terms ‘predictor AND treatment-resistant AND schizophrenia’ on 3 January 2018. No restrictions were placed on the publication date, but searches were restricted to the titles and abstracts of papers (and subject headings in Medline, Embase, and PsychINFO), studies published in English, and studies using human participants. Search terms for Pubmed were as follows: ‘((treatment resistant) OR (treatment resistance) OR (treatment refractory)) AND (schizophrenia) AND ((longitudinal) OR (prospective))’. Search strategies for other databases can be found in Appendix 1. We screened the title and abstracts of all identified studies and then performed full-text screening of all potentially eligible studies. Potentially eligible studies were cross-referenced; additional relevant studies were identified by hand-searches of the references, and by screening papers which had previously cited these studies. Each additional paper was also hand-searched until no new studies were identified. When full-text articles were not available, the corresponding author was contacted. Author SES conducted the initial screening, with APK independently screening the studies identified through database searches and all studies identified through cross-referencing.
Quality assessment
We followed the PRISMA guidelines for reporting systematic reviews (Liberati et al., 2009).
Study quality was assessed using the Newcastle-Ottawa Scale (NOS) for cohort studies (http://www.ohri.ca/programs/clinical_epidemiology/oxford.asp). Eight items measure the selection, comparability, and outcome of each study. These items were modified for this review, for example, follow-up needed to have been longer than one year to score on the item concerning adequate duration of follow-up (see Appendix 2). Authors SES and APK independently rated each study on the NOS (Appendix 3), any differences in rating were discussed between authors and final ratings were a consensus.
When available, we report adjusted hazard (HR) or odds ratios (OR) with 95% confidence intervals (95% CI) in parentheses, for predictors measured at baseline.
Results
A total of 12 studies were identified for inclusion in this review. Study screening is depicted in Fig. 1 and a summary of the number of participants recruited into each study is presented in Table 1. Database searches identified 545 records, 293 of which were duplicates and removed. A total of 252 records were screened and 248 were excluded. The main reasons for exclusion were: the study did not follow participants from the first episode or first treatment with antipsychotics (31%), participants recruited after TRS had been identified (29%), and an outcome other than TRS was reported (23%). The remaining four records were examined in more detail, as were the eight records identified by cross-referencing. Only duplicates were identified through Google Scholar. Of the 12 studies, 11 were published in peer-reviewed academic journals. One study was unpublished (Chan et al., 2014), however, after corresponding with the authors, a full report was identified on the funding body's website containing enough information to be included in this review (https://rfs1.fhb.gov.hk/app/fundedsearch/projectdetail.xhtml?id=1363).
Table 1.
Study | Number of participants | Length of follow-up (years) | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Baseline | Follow-up | (%) | TRS | (%) | Non-TRS | (%) | ||
Chan et al. (2014) | 469 | 469 | (100) | 160 | (34) | 309 | (66) | 16 |
Demjaha et al. (2017) | 557 | 274 | (49) | 62 | (23) | 212 | (77) | 10 |
Horsdal et al. (2017a)* | 390 | 390 | (100) | 52 | (13) | 338 | (87) | 2 |
Horsdal et al. (2017b)* | 3252 | 3252 | (100) | 359 | (11) | 2893 | (89) | 2 |
Kim et al. (2017) | 114 749 | NR | NR | NR | NR | |||
Lally et al. (2016) | 283 | 240 | (85) | 81 | (34) | 159 | (66) | 5 |
Meltzer et al. (1997) | 322 | 322 | (100) | 196 | (61) | 126 | (39) | 4 |
Sorensen et al. (2014) | 5968 | 5328 | (89) | 1223 | (23) | 4105 | (77) | 34 |
Üçok et al. (2016) | 187 | 105 | (56) | 28 | (27) | 77 | (73) | 2 |
Wimberley et al. (2016a)* | 13 349 | 13 349 | (100) | 2313 | (17) | 11 036 | (83) | 17 |
Wimberley et al. (2016b) | 9332 | 8044 | (86) | 1703 | (21) | 6341 | (79) | 14 |
Wimberley et al. (2017)* | 862 | 862 | (100) | 181 | (21) | 681 | (79) | 11 |
NB: NR, not reported; *, analysis of data also presented in Wimberley et al. (2016b)
Of the 12 included studies, eight presented original data and four presented data on additional exposures within the same cohort as a previous study, or a subset thereof. Of these eight, three were population cohort studies. Both Sorensen et al. (2014) and Wimberley et al. (2016b) used Danish population registers: data was extracted from multiple national databases and linked using a unique personal identification number. Additional analyses of Wimberley et al. (2016b)'s data tested whether urbanicity (Wimberley et al., 2016a), the polygenic risk score for schizophrenia (PRS-SZ; Wimberley et al., 2017), functioning (Horsdal et al., 2017b), and C-reactive protein levels (Horsdal et al., 2017a) could predict TRS. The third population cohort came from South Korea (Kim et al., 2017). The remaining five studies analysed longitudinal first episode psychosis patient cohorts (Meltzer et al., 1997; Chan et al., 2014; Lally et al., 2016; Üçok et al., 2016; Demjaha et al., 2017).
In population registries, a proxy definition of first-episode psychosis is required. In the Danish studies, the first International Classification of Diseases (ICD; World Health Organization, 1993) diagnosis of schizophrenia was used to define the baseline cohort. The South Korean study used ICD diagnosis of schizophrenia and the first use of antipsychotics to define the baseline cohort. When using diagnoses, the first episode is likely to be later in the disease course, when compared to cohort studies. Additional study characteristics, including information about recruitment, diagnoses, and criteria for TRS and non-TRS can be found in Appendix 4. The variables measured, and tested as predictors of TRS, varied considerably across studies, therefore this information is summarised in Table 2. Appendix 5 contains the unadjusted and adjusted OR/HR, when these were reported.
Table 2.
Chan et al. (2014) | Demjaha et al. (2017) | Kim et al. (2017) | Lally et al. (2016) | Meltzer et al. (1997) | Sorensen et al. (2014) | Üçok et al. (2016) | Wimberley et al. (2016b) | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Younger age of onset | ||||||||
Alcohol misuse during follow-up period | ||||||||
Antipsychotic polypharmacy during follow-up period | ||||||||
C-reactive protein | ||||||||
Comorbid diagnosis of personality disorder | ||||||||
Comorbid diagnosis of suicide attempts | ||||||||
Schizophrenia diagnosis | ||||||||
Paranoid schizophrenia diagnosis | ||||||||
Duration of first episode | ||||||||
Longer DUP | ||||||||
Early parental loss | ||||||||
Lower education qualification | ||||||||
Fewer years in education | ||||||||
Employment status | ||||||||
Black ethnicity | ||||||||
Family history of schizophrenia | ||||||||
Worse functioning | ||||||||
Worse premorbid functioning | ||||||||
Male | ||||||||
Living arrangements | ||||||||
Living arrangements during follow-up period | ||||||||
Marital status | ||||||||
Mode of onset | ||||||||
Paternal age | ||||||||
Polygenic risk score for schizophrenia | ||||||||
Relapse despite adherence | ||||||||
Relapse in first 6 months | ||||||||
More relapses in the first three years | ||||||||
Born in Autumn/winter | ||||||||
Substance misuse | ||||||||
Substance misuse during follow-up period | ||||||||
More symptoms of psychosis | ||||||||
Inpatient | ||||||||
Urbanicity at birth | ||||||||
Living in a more rural area at diagnosis | ||||||||
Violent offence | ||||||||
Chan et al. (2014) | Demjaha et al. (2017) | Kim et al. (2017) | Lally et al. (2016) | Meltzer et al. (1997) | Sorensen et al. (2014) | Üçok et al. (2016) | Wimberley et al. (2016b) |
NB: grey squares, variables not significantly associated with TRS; dark grey squares, variables significantly associated with TRS; all analyses using the Wimberley et al., Danish dataset were grouped under Wimberley et al. (2016b)
Predictors of TRS
Chan et al. (2014) analysed a subsample of a first episode cohort who presented to mental health services over a five-year period and used clozapine prescription as a definition of TRS. As this was a case-control study including all patients with TRS and a ratio of two non-TRS patients for every TRS patient, the prevalence of TRS could not be calculated. The two groups were matched on baseline diagnosis. Chan et al. (2014) included age of onset, duration of untreated psychosis (DUP; days), duration of first episode, years of education, Premorbid Adjustment Scale (PAS) adult (19 + years) subscale score (Cannon-Spoor et al., 1982), substance misuse history, and the number of relapses in the first three years, in a Cox proportional hazard regression. The model significantly predicted TRS (Chi-square = 66.11, df = 7, p = <0.0001). While number of relapses in the first three years significantly predicted TRS, the only baseline predictors significantly associated with TRS were younger age of onset (HR = 0.88, 95% CI = 0.83–0.94) and poorer premorbid functioning (indicated by higher scores) according to the PAS (HR = 3.22, 95% CI = 1.43–7.23).
Demjaha et al. (2017) analysed data from the AESOP study, which recruited first episode patients over a three-year period and followed them up ten years later. The researchers entered gender, diagnosis, age of onset, negative symptoms, mode of onset, DUP (weeks), and ethnicity into a multivariate penalised logistic regression. The model selected five variables that predicted TRS: a diagnosis of schizophrenia at baseline (instead of psychotic depression; OR = 0.41, or psychotic mania; OR = 0.52), younger age of onset (years, OR = 0.97), higher severity of negative symptoms (OR = 1.09), an insidious mode of onset (instead of acute; OR = 1.28), and longer DUP (OR = 1.0013). Goodness-of-fit was measured using McFadden's pseudo R2 and correct classification rates were measured using the Brier score. A McFadden's pseudo R2 between 0.20 and 0.40 is considered a good model fit. The Brier score is used to evaluate predictive models; if the incidence of TRS is 23%, as estimated from Demjaha et al. (2017), a Brier score of 0 would be a perfect model while a score of 0.177 would be a non-informative model (Steyerberg et al., 2010). Demjaha et al. (2017) reported a McFadden's pseudo R2 of 0.10 and a Brier score of 0.146, suggesting that their model is not a good fit of the data nor is it a good classifier of TRS.
Kim et al. (2017), in their South Korean population cohort, estimated the cumulative incidence of clozapine use using the Kaplan–Meier method and log-rank test. They reported that younger age of onset predicted TRS. Unlike Chan et al. (2014) and Demjaha et al. (2017), Kim et al. (2017) examined age of onset categorically: defining younger age of onset as those aged between 15–20 years of age, and comparing them to a middle-onset group (21–44 years of age) and a late-onset group (45–64 years of age). Kim et al. (2017) also found, using the Walter–Elwood method (Walter and Elwood, 1975), a higher incidence of clozapine use, in those born during winter (December to February) when compared to those born in summer (June to August). This pattern remained true when stratifying season of birth by age of onset. Kim et al. (2017) reported no measures of overall model fit.
Lally et al. (2016) recruited first episode patients over a five-year period and used electronic medical records to follow them up five years later. They entered age of onset, Positive and Negative Symptom Scale (PANSS; Kay et al., 1987) scores, Global Assessment of Functioning (GAF; Hall, 1995) disability score, and GAF symptom scores into a penalised logistic regression, controlling for living arrangements, employment status, and alcohol/substance misuse during the follow-up period. Lally et al. (2016) included the PANSS total score, the positive, negative and general psychopathology subscale scores, as well as two individual items: lack of insight and conceptual disorganisation. None of the PANSS or GAF variables predicted TRS. Age at first contact with mental health services was split into four categories: 18–20, 21–25, 26–30, >31 years. Only age of onset between 18 and 20 years, compared to all other age groups, significantly predicted TRS (OR = 2.49, 95% CI = 1.25–4.94). The authors did not report the overall model fit. Age of onset was subsequently stratified by gender and ethnicity. Age of onset, between 18 and 20, only predicted TRS in males (OR = 2.13, 95% CI = 1.35–7.23) or those of black ethnicity (OR = 3.71, 95% CI = 1.44–9.56).
Meltzer et al. (1997) recruited patients at first admission to hospital for schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder and followed them up for approximately four years. The authors examined the age of onset and gender in relation to TRS using a two-way analysis of variance (ANOVA). Gender was not associated with TRS but younger age of onset was. As males had a younger age of onset than females, the researchers examined the associations between age of onset and gender in more depth using simple effects ANOVA. In the non-TRS group, males had a younger age of onset (F = 6.6, df = 1, p < 0.01), however, in the TRS group, there was no difference in age of onset between males and females. Meltzer et al. (1997) calculated the conditional probability of a patient having TRS given their age of onset. For those aged between 15 and 18 years old, the probability of developing TRS was between 32% and 38% for both males and females.
Sorensen et al. (2014), in their Danish population cohort, entered a season of birth into a Cox proportion hazard regression adjusted for birth year and gender. The model did not significantly predict TRS. However, the authors found that being born in autumn (September to November), compared to spring (March to May), predicted TRS (HR = 1.24, 95% CI = 1.06–1.46). Unlike in Kim et al. (2017)'s study, being born in winter (December to February) failed to predict TRS.
Üçok et al. (2016) analysed a subsample of patients recruited into an ongoing first episode schizophrenia study. Üçok et al. (2016) entered the age of onset, DUP (days), first relapse despite adherence to antipsychotic treatment, relapse in the first six months, and antipsychotic polypharmacy during follow-up, into logistic regression. The authors did not report the overall model fit. Only first relapse despite adherence to antipsychotic treatment and antipsychotic polypharmacy predicted TRS. No baseline variables predicted TRS.
Wimberley et al. (2016b), in their Danish population cohort, entered twenty-three variables into a Cox proportion hazard regression. These variables included: gender, age at first schizophrenia diagnosis as a proxy for age of onset, family history of schizophrenia in first-degree relatives, winter birth (December to March), paternal age, parental loss before the age of 18, living alone, conviction for a violent offence before first schizophrenia diagnosis, level of education, employment status, urbanicity at first schizophrenia diagnosis, admission to psychiatric hospital before first schizophrenia diagnosis, schizophrenia subtype (paranoid v. all others), comorbid psychiatric diagnosis before first schizophrenia diagnosis, antipsychotic prescription in the year before first schizophrenia diagnosis, antidepressant prescription in the year before first schizophrenia diagnosis, and benzodiazepine prescription in the year before first schizophrenia diagnosis. Goodness-of-fit was measured using McFadden's pseudo R2 and correct classification rates using Harrell's C statistic; a C statistic of 0.5 would be a non-informative model while a score of 1 would be a perfect model. Wimberley et al. (2016b) report a McFadden's pseudo R2 of 0.027 and a Harrell's C statistic of 0.70, suggesting that this model is a good fit of the data and reasonable classifier of TRS. At baseline, younger age of onset (years, HR = 0.96, 95% CI = 0.95–0.97), living in less urban areas (rural v. capital area, HR = 1.44, 95% CI = 1.25–1.65), higher education (higher v. primary education, HR = 0.88, 95% CI = 0.79–0.98), psychiatric hospital admission at diagnosis (HR = 2.07, 95% CI = 1.87–2.29), having spent more than 30 bed-days in a psychiatric hospital in the year before diagnosis (HR = 1.54, 95% CI = 1.35–1.75), paranoid subtype diagnosis (HR = 1.24, 95% CI = 1.13–1.37), comorbid personality disorder (HR = 1.24, 95% CI = 1.11–1.39), comorbid suicide attempt (HR = 1.21, 95% CI = 1.07–1.39), antipsychotic use (HR = 1.51, 95% CI = 1.35–1.69), antidepressant use (HR = 1.15, 95% CI = 1.03–1.29), and benzodiazepines use (HR = 1.22, 95% CI = 1.10–1.37), all predicted TRS. Data on additional exposures within the same cohort, or a subset thereof, were published separately. Lower levels of urbanicity (Wimberley et al., 2016a) and severely impaired functioning (a GAF functioning score ⩽30; Horsdal et al., 2017b) predicted TRS, but the polygenic risk score for schizophrenia (PRS-SZ; Wimberley et al., 2017) and C-reactive protein levels (Horsdal et al., 2017a) did not predict TRS.
Subcategories of TRS
Some patients have little or no response to antipsychotic treatment from the onset of their illness, while others initially respond to medication and then later develop TRS. Two of the studies in our review reported comparisons between subgroups TRS patients; early-onset TRS was operationalised as meeting criteria for TRS from the onset of schizophrenia and delayed-onset TRS as meeting criteria after a period of symptomatic remission. Chan et al. (2014) found no differences, in demographics, clinical characteristics, or premorbid functioning, between early-onset TRS (N = 17, 11.64%) and delayed-onset TRS (N = 129, 88.36%). Lally et al. (2016) found no differences in demographics between the two groups, but the early-onset TRS group (N = 56, 70%) had a younger mean age of onset than the delayed-onset TRS group (N = 24, 30%).
Discussion
This review identified twelve research papers that examined predictors of TRS. Seven of the studies included in this review tested the age of onset as a predictor, and six reported that younger age of onset predicted TRS. Given that multiple definitions of the age of onset – age of onset of psychotic symptoms, age of first diagnosis of schizophrenia, age of first contact with mental health services – were reported and data was treated both continuously and categorically, this is a robust finding. Other potential risk factors, that have been identified by more than one study, include diagnosis, level of functioning, male gender, and season of birth.
A recent meta-analysis linked younger age of onset to multiple poor outcomes in schizophrenia: more hospitalisations, more negative symptoms, more relapses, poorer social/occupational functioning, and poorer global outcome (Immonen et al., 2017). Many of these poor outcomes have also been associated with TRS. Immonen et al. (2017) found that males had a younger age of onset and, therefore, samples with a higher proportion of males tended to show stronger associations between age of onset and outcomes. In the studies included in this review, the association between age of onset and TRS is unlikely to be wholly confounded by gender, as the proportion of males ranged from 49% (Kim et al., 2017) to 67% (Lally et al., 2016) and the studies which controlled for gender still showed an effect of age of onset (Meltzer et al., 1997; Lally et al., 2016; Wimberley et al., 2016b; Demjaha et al., 2017). In schizophrenia, age of onset has been thought to reflect genetic liability for the disease; younger age of onset has been associated with an increased familial risk of schizophrenia (Hilker et al., 2017; Byrne et al., 2018). Could, therefore, TRS be the result of increased genetic risk? While Wimberley et al. (2017) found no association between PRS-SZ and TRS, other work published by Frank et al. (2014) reports that an increased PRS-SZ is associated with TRS. In addition, rare copy number variations have been associated with both TRS (Martin and Mowry, 2015) and childhood-onset schizophrenia (Addington and Rapoport, 2009). Therefore, patients with TRS, who also have a younger age of onset, may have a more salient genetic influence than later-onset cases, although further work is required to substantiate this claim.
This review complements previous reviews by Gillespie et al. (2017) and Carbon and Correll (2014). Gillespie et al. (2017) examined studies comparing patients with treatment-resistant to patients with treatment-responsive schizophrenia. They included all study methodologies, but excluded studies where treatment-responsiveness was defined solely as not meeting treatment-resistant criteria. Carbon and Correll (2014) examined studies identifying predictors of response and remission. The researchers focused on first-episode psychosis studies where participants were followed up for five years. Some of the predictors of TRS, identified in this review, were found to be associated with less chance of response/remission by Carbon and Correll (2014), e.g. younger age of illness onset, poor premorbid adjustment, being male, lower level of education, living in a rural environment, diagnosis of schizophrenia, longer duration of untreated psychosis, poorer functioning, and worse psychopathology. However, Carbon and Correll (2014) also associated less chance of response/remission with being single, family history of psychosis, greater cognitive dysfunction, more family conflicts, and substance misuse; characteristics not identified as predictors of TRS. There was relatively little overlap between this review and Gillespie et al. (2017)'s review. In terms of studies included, only Meltzer et al. (1997)'s study was included in both reviews. In terms of characteristics associated with TRS, Gillespie et al. (2017) identified five neuroimaging studies, nine gene-association studies, and two studies of neurocognitive function, and these studies were not included in our review. The examination of biological markers, associated with TRS, within longitudinal study designs is rare; this is understandable for genome-wide association studies, which require large sample sizes more easily acquired using a cross-sectional methodology. However, there is a clear gap in the literature investigating biological markers that change over time (for example, proinflammatory cytokines or differently methylated positions within the epigenome) and TRS as an outcome. In terms of neuroimaging research, a review by Nakajima et al. (2015) found only five studies which compared patients with TRS to non-TRS patients, none of which had identified neural correlates of TRS. McGuire and Dazzan (2017) highlight only one study where neuroimaging data predicted a six-year, non-remitting course of illness. Longitudinal imaging studies of TRS are still relatively rare and constitute another gap in the literature.
Of the studies included in this review, few identified characteristics of abnormal neurodevelopment as predictors of TRS, despite neurodevelopment changes being linked with schizophrenia. The neurodevelopmental theory of schizophrenia proposes that disrupted normal development, in utero or early infancy, leads to deficits in psychophysiological and neurological functioning in childhood or early adolescence, and eventually to prodromal or diagnostic symptoms of schizophrenia (Jablensky et al., 2017; Murray et al., 2017). Previous research has linked characteristics of abnormal development with TRS; higher rates of minor physical anomalies (Lin et al., 2015), more neurological soft signs (de Bartolomeis et al., 2018), poor verbal intelligence and fluency (Kravariti et al., 2018), and poor verbal memory (Joober et al., 2002; de Bartolomeis et al., 2013). None of the studies in this review included variables measuring physiology during development or cognition at the first episode. Only Chan et al. (2014) examined premorbid functioning, retrospectively using the PAS. They found no difference, between the TRS and non-TRS groups, in terms of functioning during childhood, early adolescence, or late adolescent. There was a difference in functioning after the age of 19 and subsequently, worse functioning predicted TRS in their final model. If educational attainment can be considered a proxy for development only lower level of education qualification was found to significantly predict TRS (Wimberley et al., 2016b). The number of years in education was not predictive of TRS (Chan et al., 2014). Abnormal neurodevelopment and neuropsychology have not been sufficiently investigated as potential predictors of TRS.
Our review has illuminated some gaps in the existing literature, where potential predictors have not been fully investigated, however, we believe our review has captured all published work and identified predictors that, with further study, may prove to be clinically useful in determining treatment for patients with schizophrenia.
Strengths and limitations
The main strength of this review is that we have focused solely on studies that included temporal forecasting (observations at baseline that are used to predict outcomes at follow-up), and as such eliminated recall bias and established a key component necessary for predictive models. All the studies included in this review are likely to be sufficiently powered to detect predictors of TRS. All the studies reported large sample sizes, and most followed participants for more than one year. Although no studies reported a priori power analysis, and only Meltzer et al. (1997) reported an ad hoc power analysis, we believe lack of power is unlikely to explain these results.
When attrition reduces the sample size at follow-up of longitudinal studies, consequently, statistical power is also reduced. For the studies we have reviewed, that reported on participants lost to follow-up, it is unlikely that the low attrition rates introduced bias. In particular, many studies used Cox proportional hazard regression; an analytic method that not only takes into account that individuals lost to follow-up may develop TRS, but also that individuals may develop TRS after the study endpoint. However, TRS, in particular, may be biased by attrition. There is a case both that TRS patients may be more likely to drop out of research studies due to their higher severity of symptoms and worse social and occupational functioning, and that responders are more likely to drop out as they lose touch with clinical services, but we are not aware of any published studies examining attrition in relation to treatment response.
One limitation to consider, when discussing the findings from these studies, is that some patients may have been misclassified. None of the studies included in this review explicitly accounted for adherence to medication, therefore characteristics may be predicting nonadherence rather than treatment resistance. None of the studies measured antipsychotic plasma levels, therefore characteristics may be predicting sub-therapeutic drug plasma levels, as a consequence of nonadherence, noncompliance, or pharmacokinetics, rather than treatment resistance. McCutcheon et al. (2015) found that 44% of patients referred to an outpatient service for clozapine treatment had sub-therapeutic conventional-antipsychotic plasma levels. On the other hand, it is unlikely that TRS patients have been wrongly classified as responders because the long follow-up periods allow plenty of time for a diagnosis of TRS to be established. Most studies had follow-ups longer than four years; the average delay before being treated for TRS estimated by Howes et al. (2012). The definitions of TRS, used in these studies, are pragmatic criteria: any predictors identified by these naturalistic studies are generalisable to real-world, clinical settings where adherence, compliance, or drug plasma levels influence treatment.
The use of multiple definitions of TRS is a problem across all TRS literature; Suzuki et al. (2011) reviewed 33 studies of prospective studies of pharmacological interventions for TRS and found that all 33 definitions of TRS were different. Howes et al. (2017) reviewed 42 clinical trials and found only two studies which used identical criteria. In addition, some studies use clozapine prescription as a proxy for TRS. When clozapine is under-prescribed, supposed predictors of TRS may, in fact, represent predictors of clozapine initiation (e.g. clinicians' attitudes towards clozapine prescription). All of the studies, identified in this review, used existing data, not designed to examine TRS, and researchers had to established proxy definitions based on the data available to them. When evidence concerning predictors of TRS is not consistent, it can be hard to draw a clear conclusion about the validity of the predictor, yet when the evidence is consistent across studies, with different definitions, the predictor in question is highly likely to generalise to other cohorts and have clinical validity.
Finally, we must consider the statistical methodology used to establish predictors. A common misconception is that predictive accuracy can be inferred from explanatory accuracy. However, the two are different and should be assessed separately (Shmueli, 2010). Only three studies included in this review reported the overall model fit, and only two reported statistics that measure the predictive validity of the model. Additionally, in predictive modelling, variable selection and overfitting must be considered. Lally et al. (2016) and Demjaha et al. (2017) attempted to reduce overfitting by penalising regression coefficients. However, none of the studies used holdout data (training data), cross-validation, or external validation to evaluate the predictive power of models; the latter being the current ‘gold-standard’ approach. In terms of variable selection, the only methods reported were LASSO regression (Demjaha et al., 2017) and step-wise selection using statistical significance (Chan et al., 2014; Üçok et al., 2016). Stepwise methods are no longer considered appropriate for explanatory models, but stepwise-type algorithms are very useful in predictive modelling (Shmueli, 2010), as long as the selection criteria rely on predictive power (e.g. Akaike information criterion) rather than explanatory power (e.g. statistical significance), as was the case in these studies. These methodological limitations must be taken into consideration when evaluating predictive models. The studies included in the review, on the whole, report analyses designed to identify explanatory variables of TRS. Future studies will need to use more robust prediction methods before moving from statistical prediction to clinical prediction.
Conclusion
The aim of this systemic literature review was to identify predictors of treatment-resistant schizophrenia from prospective longitudinal studies. In choosing to focus exclusively on longitudinal studies, we have filled a gap in the existing literature, and hope that consolidating this information will be of use to researchers attempting to identify clinical predictors of TRS and the biological mechanisms causing TRS. We have identified earlier age of schizophrenia-onset as a robust predictor of TRS, with evidence that male gender, autumn/winter birth, poor premorbid functioning and rural upbringing may also contribute. We have also highlighted gaps in the literature namely, studies examining neuroimaging, immune, and genetic markers of TRS. Examination of biological markers, particularly within the framework of a prospective longitudinal study, has the potential to go beyond simple prediction and add to our understanding of the underlying causes of TRS. In conclusion, while early identification of TRS is clinically important, we currently have very limited knowledge of its predictors.
Funding
This work was supported by the Medical Research Council (MRC) (S.E.S., R.M.M., & J.H.M., grant number: MR/L011794/1) and the National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) Collaboration for Leadership in Applied Health Research and Care (CLAHRC) South London at King's College Hospital NHS Foundation Trust South London (S.E.S.). A.P.K. was supported by the NIHR Biomedical Research Centre (BRC) at South London and Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust and King's College London.
The views expressed in this article are those of the authors and not necessarily those of the NHS, the NIHR, the Department of Health and Social Care, the BRC, the MRC, or King's College London.
Conflict of interest
S.E.S. has received travel and accommodation expenses from H Lundbeck A/S. A.P.K. declares no conflicts of interest. R.M.M. has received honoraria for lectures from Janssen, Otsuka, Lundbeck and Sunovian. J.H.M. has received travel and accommodation expenses and research funding from H Lundbeck A/S.
Ethical standards
The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008.
Supplementary material
For supplementary material accompanying this paper visit https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291719002083.
References
- Addington AM and Rapoport JL (2009) The genetics of childhood-onset schizophrenia: when madness strikes the prepubescent. Current Psychiatry Reports 11, 156–161. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andrews A, Knapp M, McCrone P, Parsonage M. and Trachtenberg M (2012) Effective Interventions in Schizophrenia: the Economic Case. A Report Prepared for the Schizophrenia Commission. Rethink Mental Illness: London.
- Byrne M, Agerbo E and Mortensen PB (2018) Family history of psychiatric disorders and age at first contact in schizophrenia: an epidemiological study. The British Journal of Psychiatry 181, s19–s25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cannon-Spoor HE, Potkin SG and Wyatt RJ (1982) Measurement of premorbid adjustment in chronic schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin 8, 470–484. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carbon M and Correll CU (2014) Clinical predictors of therapeutic response to antipsychotics in schizophrenia. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience 16, 505–524. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chan SKW, Chang WC, Lee EHM, Wong GHY, Hui CLM, Chen EYH and Sham PC (2014) Predictors of treatment resistant schizophrenia-spectrum disorder: 10-year retrospective study of first-episode psychosis. Early Intervention in Psychiatry 8, 78. [Google Scholar]
- Conley RR and Kelly DL (2001) Management of treatment resistance in schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry 50, 898–911. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Bartolomeis A, Balletta R, Giordano S, Buonaguro EF, Latte G. and Iasevoli F (2013) Differential cognitive performances between schizophrenic responders and non-responders to antipsychotics: correlation with course of the illness, psychopathology, attitude to the treatment and antipsychotics doses. Psychiatry Research 210, 387–395. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Bartolomeis A, Prinzivalli E, Callovini G, D'Ambrosio L, Altavilla B, Avagliano C and Iasevoli F (2018) Treatment resistant schizophrenia and neurological soft signs may converge on the same pathology: evidence from explanatory analysis on clinical, psychopathological, and cognitive variables. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry 81, 356–366. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Demjaha A, Lappin JM, Stahl D, Patel MX, MacCabe JH, Howes OD, Heslin M, Reininghaus UA, Donoghue K, Lomas B, Charalambides M, Onyejiaka A, Fearon P, Jones P, Doody G, Morgan C, Dazzan P and Murray RM (2017) Antipsychotic treatment resistance in first-episode psychosis: prevalence, subtypes and predictors. Psychological Medicine 47, 1981–1989. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Farooq S, Agid O, Foussias G and Remington G (2013) Using treatment response to subtype schizophrenia: proposal for a new paradigm in classification. Schizophrenia Bulletin 39, 1169–1172. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frank J, Lang M, Witt SH, Strohmaier J, Rujescu D, Cichon S, Degenhardt F, Nöthen MM, Collier DA, Ripke S, Naber D and Rietschel M (2014) Identification of increased genetic risk scores for schizophrenia in treatment-resistant patients. Molecular Psychiatry 20, 150. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gillespie AL, Samanaite R, Mill J, Egerton A and MacCabe JH (2017) Is treatment-resistant schizophrenia categorically distinct from treatment-responsive schizophrenia? A systematic review. BMC Psychiatry 17, 12. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hall RC (1995) Global assessment of functioning: a modified scale. Psychosomatics 36, 267–275. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hilker R, Helenius D, Fagerlund B, Skytthe A, Christensen K, Werge TM, Nordentoft M and Glenthøj B (2017) Is an early Age at illness onset in schizophrenia associated with increased genetic susceptibility? Analysis of data from the nationwide Danish Twin register. EBioMedicine 18, 320–326. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Horsdal HT, Wimberley T, Benros ME and Gasse C (2017a) C-reactive protein levels and treatment resistance in schizophrenia—A Danish population-based cohort study. Human Psychopharmacology: Clinical and Experimental 32, e2632-n/a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Horsdal HT, Wimberley T, Köhler-Forsberg O, Baandrup L and Gasse C (2017b) Association between global functioning at first schizophrenia diagnosis and treatment resistance. Early Intervention in Psychiatry 12, 1198–1202. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Howes OD, Vergunst F, Gee S, McGuire P, Kapur S and Taylor D (2012) Adherence to treatment guidelines in clinical practice: study of antipsychotic treatment prior to clozapine initiation. The British Journal of Psychiatry 201, 481–485. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Howes OD, McCutcheon R, Agid O, de Bartolomeis A, van Beveren NJ, Birnbaum ML, Bloomfield MA, Bressan RA, Buchanan RW, Carpenter WT, Castle DJ, Citrome L, Daskalakis ZJ, Davidson M, Drake RJ, Dursun S, Ebdrup BH, Elkis H, Falkai P, Fleischacker W, Gadelha A, Gaughran F, Glenthoj BY, Graff-Guerrero A, Hallak JE, Honer WG, Kennedy J, Kinon BJ, Lawrie SM, Lee J, Leweke F, MacCabe JH, McNabb CB, Meltzer H, Moller H-J, Nakajima S, Pantelis C, Marques TR, Remington G, Rossell SL, Russell BR, Siu CO, Suzuki T, Sommer IE, Taylor D, Thomas N, Ucok A, Umbricht D, Walters JT, Kane J and Correll CU (2017) Treatment-resistant schizophrenia: treatment response and resistance in psychosis (TRRIP) working group consensus guidelines on diagnosis and terminology. The American Journal of Psychiatry 174, 216–229. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Iasevoli F, Giordano S, Balletta R, Latte G, Formato MV, Prinzivalli E, De Berardis D, Tomasetti C and de Bartolomeis A (2016) Treatment resistant schizophrenia is associated with the worst community functioning among severely-ill highly-disabling psychiatric conditions and is the most relevant predictor of poorer achievements in functional milestones. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry 65, 34–48. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Immonen J, Jääskeläinen E, Korpela H and Miettunen J (2017) Age at onset and the outcomes of schizophrenia: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Early Intervention in Psychiatry 11, 453–460. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jablensky A, McNeil TF and Morgan VA (2017) Barbara fish and a short history of the neurodevelopmental hypothesis of schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin 43, 1158–1163. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Joober R, Rouleau GA, Lal S, Dixon M, O'Driscoll G, Palmour R, Annable L, Bloom D, Lalonde P, Labelle A and Benkelfat C (2002) Neuropsychological impairments in neuroleptic-responder vs. -nonresponder schizophrenic patients and healthy volunteers. Schizophrenia Research 53, 229–238. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kane J, Honigfeld G, Singer J and Meltzer H (1988) Clozapine for the treatment-resistant schizophrenic. A double-blind comparison with chlorpromazine. Archives of General Psychiatry 45, 789–796. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kay SR, Fiszbein A and Opler LA (1987) The positive and negative syndrome scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin 13, 261–276. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kennedy JL, Altar CA, Taylor DL, Degtiar I and Hornberger JC (2014) The social and economic burden of treatment-resistant schizophrenia: a systematic literature review. International Clinical Psychopharmacology 29, 63–76. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Killaspy H, Marston L, Omar RZ, Green N, Harrison I, Lean M, Holloway F, Craig T, Leavey G and King M (2013) Service quality and clinical outcomes: an example from mental health rehabilitation services in England. The British Journal of Psychiatry 202, 28–34. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim JS, Park CM, Choi JA, Park E, Tchoe HJ, Choi M, Suh JK, Kim YH, Won SH, Chung YC, Bae KY, Lee SK, Park SC and Lee SH (2017) The association between season of birth, age at onset, and clozapine use in schizophrenia. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 136, 445–454. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kravariti E, Demjaha A, Zanelli J, Ibrahim F, Wise C, MacCabe JH, Reichenberg A, Pilecka I, Morgan K, Fearon P, Morgan C, Doody GA, Donoghue K, Jones PB, Kacar AS, Dazzan P, Lappin J and Murray RM (2018) Neuropsychological function at first episode in treatment-resistant psychosis: findings from the AESOP-10 study. Psychological Medicine 1–11. 10.1017/S0033291718002957 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lally J, Ajnakina O, Di Forti M, Trotta A, Demjaha A, Kolliakou A, Mondelli V, Marques T, Pariante C, Dazzan P, Shergil S, Howes O, David A, MacCabe J, Gaughran F and Murray R (2016) Two distinct patterns of treatment resistance: clinical predictors of treatment resistance in first-episode schizophrenia spectrum psychoses. Psychological Medicine 46, 3231–3240. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee J, Takeuchi H, Fervaha G, Sin GL, Foussias G, Agid O, Farooq S and Remington G (2015) Subtyping schizophrenia by treatment response: antipsychotic development and the central role of positive symptoms. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry. Revue Canadienne de Psychiatrie 60, 515–522. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liberati A, Altman DG, Tetzlaff J, Mulrow C, Gøtzsche PC, Ioannidis JPA, Clarke M, Devereaux PJ, Kleijnen J and Moher D (2009) The PRISMA statement for reporting systematic reviews and meta-analyses of studies that evaluate health care interventions: explanation and elaboration. PLoS Medicine 6, e1000100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lin AS, Chang SS, Lin SH, Peng YC, Hwu HG and Chen WJ (2015) Minor physical anomalies and craniofacial measures in patients with treatment-resistant schizophrenia. Psychological Medicine 45, 1839–1850. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martin AK and Mowry B (2015) Increased rare duplication burden genomewide in patients with treatment-resistant schizophrenia. Psychological Medicine 46, 469–476. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McCutcheon R, Beck K, Bloomfield MA, Marques TR, Rogdaki M and Howes OD (2015) Treatment resistant or resistant to treatment? Antipsychotic plasma levels in patients with poorly controlled psychotic symptoms. Journal of Psychopharmacology 29, 892–897. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McGuire P and Dazzan P (2017) Does neuroimaging have a role in predicting outcomes in psychosis? World Psychiatry 16, 209–210. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meltzer HY, Rabinowitz J, Lee MA, Cola PA, Ranjan R, Findling RL and Thompson PA (1997) Age at onset and gender of schizophrenic patients in relation to neuroleptic resistance. American Journal of Psychiatry 154, 475–482. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murray RM, Bhavsar V, Tripoli G. and Howes O (2017) 30 years on: how the neurodevelopmental hypothesis of schizophrenia morphed into the developmental risk factor model of psychosis. Schizophrenia Bulletin 43, 1190–1196. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nakajima S, Takeuchi H, Plitman E, Fervaha G, Gerretsen P, Caravaggio F, Chung JK, Iwata Y, Remington G and Graff-Guerrero A (2015) Neuroimaging findings in treatment-resistant schizophrenia: a systematic review: lack of neuroimaging correlates of treatment-resistant schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 164, 164–175. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- NICE (2014) Psychosis and Schizophrenia in Adults: Treatment and Management: Updated Edition 2014. In National Institute for Health and Clinical Excellence: Guidance National Collaborating Centre for Mental Health: London. [PubMed]
- Shmueli G (2010) To explain or to predict? Statistical Science 25, 289–310. [Google Scholar]
- Siskind D, McCartney L, Goldschlager R and Kisely S (2016) Clozapine v. first- and second-generation antipsychotics in treatment-refractory schizophrenia: systematic review and meta-analysis. British Journal of Psychiatry 209, 385–392. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sorensen HJ, Foldager L, Roge R, Pristed SG, Andreasen JT and Nielsen J (2014) An association between autumn birth and clozapine treatment in patients with schizophrenia: a population-based analysis. Nordic Journal of Psychiatry 68, 428–432. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Steyerberg EW, Vickers AJ, Cook NR, Gerds T, Gonen M, Obuchowski N, Pencina MJ and Kattan MW (2010) Assessing the performance of prediction models: a framework for some traditional and novel measures. Epidemiology 21, 128–138. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Suzuki T, Remington G, Mulsant BH, Rajji TK, Uchida H, Graff-Guerrero A and Mamo DC (2011) Treatment resistant schizophrenia and response to antipsychotics: a review. Schizophrenia Research 133, 54–62. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Suzuki T, Remington G, Mulsant BH, Uchida H, Rajji TK, Graff-Guerrero A, Mimura M and Mamo DC (2012) Defining treatment-resistant schizophrenia and response to antipsychotics: a review and recommendation. Psychiatry Research 197, 1–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taylor DM (2017) Clozapine for treatment-resistant schizophrenia: still the gold standard? CNS Drugs 31, 177–180. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Üçok A, Çıkrıkçılı U, Ergül C, Tabak Ö, Salaj A, Karabulut S and Correll CU (2016) Correlates of clozapine use after a first episode of schizophrenia: results from a long-term prospective study. CNS Drugs 30, 997–1006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Walter S and Elwood J (1975) A test for seasonality of events with a variable population at risk. Journal of Epidemiology & Community Health 29, 18–21. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Warnez S. and Alessi-Severini S. (2014) Clozapine: a review of clinical practice guidelines and prescribing trends. BMC Psychiatry 14, 102–102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wimberley T, Pedersen CB, MacCabe JH, Støvring H, Astrup A, Sørensen HJ, Horsdal HT, Mortensen PB and Gasse C (2016a) Inverse association between urbanicity and treatment resistance in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 174, 150–155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wimberley T, Støvring H, Sørensen HJ, Horsdal HT, MacCabe JH and Gasse C (2016b) Predictors of treatment resistance in patients with schizophrenia: a population-based cohort study. The Lancet Psychiatry 3, 358–366. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wimberley T, Gasse C, Meier SM, Agerbo E, MacCabe JH and Horsdal HT (2017) Polygenic risk score for schizophrenia and treatment-resistant schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin 43, 1064–1069. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- World Health Organization (1993) The ICD-10 Classification of Mental and Behavioural Disorders: Diagnostic Criteria for Research. Geneva: World Health Organization. [Google Scholar]
- Yoshimura B, Yada Y, So R, Takaki M and Yamada N (2017) The critical treatment window of clozapine in treatment-resistant schizophrenia: secondary analysis of an observational study. Psychiatry Research 250, 65–70. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
For supplementary material accompanying this paper visit https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291719002083.