Skip to main content
Journal of Sport and Health Science logoLink to Journal of Sport and Health Science
. 2018 Oct 27;10(1):73–81. doi: 10.1016/j.jshs.2018.10.001

A person-centered approach to achievement goal orientations in competitive tennis players: Associations with motivation and mental toughness

Richard Gregory Cowden a,b,, Nicolas Mascret c, Timothy Ryan Duckett d
PMCID: PMC7856560  PMID: 33518017

Highlights

  • Latent profile analysis revealed 3 distinct configurations of achievement goal orientations that varied primarily in valence of competence (i.e., approach vs. avoidance).

  • Athletes oriented by approach types of goals (irrespective of their orientation toward avoidance goals) reported higher levels of autonomous motivation and mental toughness.

  • Competitive tennis players tend to pursue a number of achievement goals in combination rather than in isolation.

  • Avoidance goals may also be associated with desirable psychological characteristics, provided they are pursued in conjunction with approach goals.

Keywords: Achievement goals, Latent profile analysis, Mental toughness, Motivation, Sport

Abstract

Background

Research on achievement goal orientations in sport has typically relied on the use of variable-centered approaches that tend to overlook population heterogeneity. In this study, we used a person-centered approach to identify subgroups of competitive tennis players according to unique combinations of achievement goal orientations and tested for subgroup differences in motivation and mental toughness.

Methods

A sample of 323 competitive tennis athletes (69.35% male) between 15 and 25 years of age (17.60 ± 2.40 years, mean ± SD) completed the 3 × 2 Achievement Goal Questionnaire for Sport, Sport Motivation Scale II, and Mental Toughness Index. Latent profile analysis was used to identify unique combinations of achievement goal orientations. Comparisons between latent subgroups on autonomous motivation, controlled motivation, and mental toughness were performed using analysis of variance.

Results

Latent profile analysis supported 3 distinct patterns of achievement goal profiles that were primarily distinguishable based on valence of competence (i.e., approach vs. avoidance). Analyses of variance indicated that athletes who were classified into subgroups that endorsed approach types of goals (regardless of the types of avoidance goals they endorsed) reported higher levels of autonomous motivation and mental toughness.

Conclusion

Results indicated that athletes tend to pursue a number of achievement goals collectively rather than in isolation. Although approach goals are more commonly linked to adaptive psychological functioning and positive outcomes, avoidance goals may also be associated with desirable psychological characteristics if they are pursued in conjunction with approach types of achievement goals.

1. Introduction

Competitive sports offer athletes the opportunity to demonstrate competence by influencing, controlling, and mastering the athletic environment.1 Athletes’ need for competence is typically fulfilled by attaining an objective or subjective standard of performance that they set and evaluate themselves against.2 Scholars commonly refer to internalized performance standards as achievement goals,3 the orientation of which may vary based on athletes’ underlying motives for achievement. Contemporary achievement goal models have developed from the dichotomous mastery (i.e., emphasis on the development of skills and self-improvement) and performance (i.e., emphasis on outperforming others) framework that distinguished between self- and other-dependent achievement goals.4 Subsequent research further differentiated mastery and performance goals into avoidance and approach dimensions.5 This 2 × 2 framework classified mastery and performance goals according to whether they are positively toned (i.e., attaining success) or negatively toned (i.e., avoiding failure).6

Recent perspectives suggest that using a broad mastery dimension may confound self-dependent (e.g., expected self-referenced improvements in performance) and task-dependent (e.g., expected task-referenced improvements in performance) goal orientations.3 Offering a framework for discerning between each type, Elliot et al.7 proposed a 3 × 2 model that designates competence (self, task, other (akin to performance goals)) and valence of competence (positive (approach), negative (avoidance)) into one of 6 classifications of achievement goals (see Mascret et al.3 for detailed descriptions and examples of each type). Several studies have found preliminary support for distinctions between self-orientations and task-orientations among athletes. For example, Mascret et al.3 found task-approach (TAP) goals, but not self-approach (SAP) goals, were positively associated with self-perceived competence. These findings highlight the progress that has been made, yet the existing body of research on achievement goal orientations in sport has disproportionately focused on interindividual (as opposed to intraindividual) differences.

1.1. A person-centered approach to achievement goals

An important limitation of most studies on achievement goals in sport has been the use of variable-centered analyses that assume population homogeneity with regard to the relations among variables.8 Notwithstanding the value of variable-level analyses, such approaches tend to overlook population diversity and intraindividual variability.9 Whereas variable-centered analyses clarify associations among variables and focus on generalizing conclusions across the population, person-centered approaches consider population heterogeneity and identify configurations of variable relations at the within-person level.10 Thus, person-centered analyses are useful for identifying subpopulations of individuals based on within-group similarities and between-group differences in the patterns of association among variables.11

Applying a person-centered approach to achievement goals in sport is warranted for several reasons. First, prior studies have reported considerable variability in the covariance among achievement goal orientations,3 as well as fluctuations in the goal types that dominate athletes’ achievement goal pursuits.12 Second, even though achievement goal pursuits may be dominated by a single preferred type, people typically pursue a combination of achievement goals.13 Third, interventions usually focus on fostering mastery approach (i.e., SAP and TAP) achievement goal pursuits.3 By identifying within-person differences in achievement goal orientations, a more nuanced perspective may be developed about the types of achievement goals that should form the basis of targeted interventions. Fourth, in contrast with studying achievement goal types in isolation, person-centered approaches allow unique achievement goal arrangements to be examined in relation to predictors or outcomes.14 In this study, we examine associations between configurations of achievement goals and auxiliary variables of motivation and mental toughness.

1.2. Achievement goal correlates: motivation and mental toughness

Directed by the motives that give energy to human behavior,15 athletes may be motivated to participate in competitive sport for a variety of reasons. Motivation is often classified on a continuum of motivational subtypes from most self-determined (i.e., intrinsic regulation) to least self-determined (i.e., amotivation).16, 17 Internalization of self-determined motives (e.g., participation out of pleasure, interest, or meaning derived from sport participation) reflects autonomous motivation, whereas adoption of non-self-determined motives (e.g., participation out of intrapersonal and interpersonal pressures, prospects of external gains, or avoidance of negative consequences) corresponds with controlled motivation.17 Although autonomous and controlled forms of motivation represent opposing ends of the motivational spectrum, athletes’ participation in competitive sport may be regulated by combinations of each type.18

Research on motivational orientations in sport has consistently distinguished between the adaptive outcomes associated with autonomous motivation and the maladaptive outcomes linked to controlled motivation.19 Specifically, whereas controlled motivation has yielded positive associations with unfavorable outcomes including burnout, moral disengagement, and perceived injury susceptibility,20, 21, 22 positive relationships have been found between autonomous motivation and desirable outcomes such as coping with adversity, confidence, and athletic performance.23, 24 Considering that mastery-oriented achievement goals are more closely aligned with self-determined motives, it is not surprising that athletes who pursue mastery (i.e., self-based and task-based) types of goals tend to report higher levels of autonomous types of motivation.12 However, findings with regard to the approach–avoidance valence of competence have generally been mixed. Some studies have exclusively linked mastery approach goals to autonomous motivational orientations,25 whereas others have found positive associations between mastery avoidance goals and motivational types that are more autonomous.26 Applying a person-centered approach to achievement goals may clarify the types of motivational tendencies that correspond with different configurations of achievement goals that athletes pursue.

Another construct associated with adaptive outcomes in sport is mental toughness, a psychological resource that promotes goal-directed efforts and success in sport.27, 28, 29 Although debate continues on the dimensionality and traitness of mental toughness,30, 31 evidence indicates that athletes with higher levels of mental toughness are self-confident and believe in their ability to control the environment, approach obstacles as opportunities for self-growth, and strive to attain the challenging goals they set for themselves.32, 33 Mentally tough athletes tend to emphasize and pursue self-referenced (as opposed to norm-referenced) competencies, thrive on competitive environments and opportunities to demonstrate competence, and remain committed to their goals despite adversity.34, 35, 36 Taken together, these findings suggest that mental toughness is more closely tied to establishing and pursuing approach types of achievement goals. This finding has been supported in previous research, with higher levels of mastery and performance approach-oriented achievement goals reported among athletes classified as high in mental toughness.37 However, previous research on achievement goal orientations and mental toughness have examined achievement goal types in isolation rather than collectively, and it is unclear how different combinations of achievement goals relate to mental toughness.

1.3. The current study

The purposes of this study were to (a) identify unique profiles of athletes based on different combinations of achievement goal orientations and (b) compare the subgroups identified on auxiliary variables linked to favorable sport performance outcomes (i.e., motivation and mental toughness).15, 28 Because the types of achievement goals athletes pursue have consequences for athletic performance,2 identifying differences between these subgroups on motivation and mental toughness might highlight the need for performance-enhancing psychological interventions that target achievement goal orientations in combination rather than in isolation. We expected that autonomous motivation and mental toughness would be higher, and controlled motivation lower, among athletes with achievement goal profiles who emphasized approach types of goals.

2. Methods

2.1. Participants and procedure

The sample (n = 323) consisted of male (n = 224) and female (n = 99) tennis players who were actively partaking in national-level singles tennis tournaments at the time of the study. Participants were between 15 and 25 years of age (17.60 ± 2.40 years, mean ± SD) and were competing in U16 (n = 119), U18 (n = 91), and Open (i.e., ≥18 years) (n = 109; unspecified = 4) age brackets of participation. Institutional ethical approval was granted by the University of KwaZulu-Natal Humanities and Social Sciences Research Ethics Committee. The directors of national tournaments were contacted to obtain permission to access relevant tennis players. A team of experienced research assistants trained in standardized survey administration procedures recruited and administered the survey items to tennis players while attending selected national tournaments. Written informed consent was obtained before administering the survey to participants, and all participants were provided an opportunity to inquire about the nature of the study and their participation in it. When eligible minors (i.e., <18 years of age) agreed to partake in the study, we acquired parental consent and child assent. The survey items were administered individually to participants in a standardized format, and items were administered in the same order to all athletes.

2.2. Measures

2.2.1. Achievement goals

The 3 × 2 Achievement Goal Questionnaire for Sport3 measured athletes’ achievement goal orientations. Oriented by an opening phrase (i.e., “In tennis, my goal is…”), participants used a 7-point response format (1 = strongly disagree; 7 = strongly agree) to rate the 18 items included on the 6 subscales (3 items for each subscale) of TAP (e.g., “…to obtain good results”); SAP (e.g., “…to be more effective than before”); other approach ((OAP), e.g., “…to do better than others”); task-avoidance ((TAV), e.g., “…to avoid bad results”); self-avoidance ((SAV), e.g., “…to avoid doing worse than I usually do”); and other avoidance ((OAV), e.g., “…to avoid doing worse than others”). The 3 × 2 factor structure of the Achievement Goal Questionnaire for Sport has been supported over other achievement goal orientation models,3 and evidence of construct validity has been provided on a number of occasions.38 Past research has reported acceptable levels of internal consistency (0.76–0.95) for each of the subscales.3, 38 In this study, omega point estimates for the 6 subscales were between 0.79 and 0.90.

2.2.2. Motivation

A total of 12 items from four of the Sport Motivation Scale II17 subscales (3 items for each subscale) were used to assess motivational regulation. Consistent with previous research,1 the items (each of which contained the same orienting statement, “I participate in tennis”) included on the intrinsic regulation subscales (e.g., “…Because it is very interesting to learn how I can improve”) and integrated regulation subscales (e.g., “…Because through tennis, I am living in line with my deepest principles”) were aggregated for an index of autonomous motivation, whereas the introjected regulation items (e.g., “…Because I would not feel worthwhile if I did not”) and external regulation items (e.g., “…Because people I care about would be upset with me if I did not”) were summed for a measure of controlled motivation. Items are rated using a 7-point response format (1 = does not correspond at all; 7 = corresponds completely). Research has confirmed the factor structure and provided evidence supporting the construct validity of the Sport Motivation Scale II,17, 39 and estimates of test–retest reliability (1-week interval) and internal consistency have been ≥0.70.17, 39 For the subscales included in this study (i.e., intrinsic, integrated, introjected, and extrinsic regulation), omega point estimates ranged from 0.77 to 0.84. Internal consistency reliability for the autonomous and controlled motivation indices used in this study were ω = 0.91 and ω = 0.89, respectively.

2.2.3. Mental toughness

Participants completed the Mental Toughness Index,30 an 8-item measure of mental toughness. Items (e.g., “I strive for continued success in tennis”) are rated on a 7-point response scale from 1 (false, 100% of the time) to 7 (true, 100% of the time). Evidence from several studies supports the factorial, construct, and cross-cultural validity of the Mental Toughness Index.30, 40 Internal consistency estimates reported in prior studies have been within appropriate limits (ω >0.80).40, 41 For the sample included in the current study, internal consistency was estimated at ω = 0.91.

2.3. Data analyses

2.3.1. Preliminary analyses

Statistical analyses were performed using R (R Foundation for Statistical Computing 2017, Vienna, Austria).42 Item-level responses were screened for missing values. Each was replaced using an iterative random forest approach with 10,000 replications.43 Standardized values at the univariate level (|critical value| = 3.29, p < 0.001), and Mahalonobis distance at the multivariate level (p < 0.001), were used to detect gross outliers.44 Skewness and kurtosis estimates provided an indication of univariate normality, with |values| ≥ 2 for skewness and ≥ 7 for kurtosis signaling variables non-normal in distribution.45 Internal consistency was estimated using omega (ω), an approach that assumes a congeneric model and one that has a lower likelihood of underestimating or overestimating reliability.46

2.3.2. Primary analyses

Latent profile analysis was performed to identify subgroups based on the underlying achievement goals consistent with the 3 × 2 framework. After specifying a single-profile baseline model, additional profiles were successively added to identify the model that offered the greatest parsimony and optimal level of fit.47 Model fit was evaluated using the bootstrap (10,000 repetitions) likelihood ratio test and a combination of relative fit indices (the uncorrected and corrected Aikake information criterion and the unadjusted and sample-size adjusted Bayesian information criterion. Entropy values provided an indication of the quality of fit across models. Values that approach 1 reflect clearer delineation of classes.48 Models with statistically significant bootstrap likelihood ratio test p values (p < 0.05), lower relative fit index values, and higher entropy values were prioritized in model selection.49 In addition, we inspected the mean posterior probabilities of the profiles for each model when deciding on the optimal class structure. Values of >0.70 indicate that there is a strong likelihood that a participant belongs to the assigned class and not any of the other classes.50 We also examined the proportion of participants assigned to each profile, as there is an increased likelihood of superfluous profile extraction when small subsamples (i.e., <5%) are assigned to profiles.51

The subgroups that emerged from the latent profile analysis formed the achievement goal orientation variable. The χ2 tests of independence were used to determine whether age group and sex differences existed based on achievement goal orientation subgroups. Analysis of variance (ANOVA) tests were performed to identify mean differences between achievement goal orientation groups on the criterion variables of autonomous motivation, controlled motivation, and mental toughness. To statistically control for potential effects of age and sex, both variables were included in each model. Visual inspection of Wally plots52 for each model indicated that homogeneity of variance could not be assumed. As such, ANOVAs were performed using the HC4 heteroscedasticity-consistent covariance matrix estimator.53 ANOVAs that yielded significant effects for achievement goal orientation were followed by post hoc pairwise comparisons using the Games–Howell procedure.

3. Results

3.1. Preliminary analyses

Missing data diagnostics identified a small percentage of missing values (0.85%), which were replaced (proportion falsely classified = 0.32). One univariate outlier (z = −3.42) was removed. A total of 10 cases were flagged as multivariate outliers, χ2 (9) = 27.88, p < 0.001, which were omitted before proceeding. Univariate skewness (maximum = −0.43) and kurtosis (maximum = −1.23) statistics revealed that the measures were approximately normal in distribution.45 Descriptive statistics and bivariate associations between the measures are reported in Table 1.

Table 1.

Descriptive statistics, internal consistency estimates, and bivariate analyses among study variables.

(1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7) (8) (9)
(1) Task-approach
(2) Self-approach (95%CI) 0.83 (0.79–0.86)
(3) Other approach (95%CI) 0.75 (0.70–0.79) 0.79 (0.74–0.83)
(4) Task-avoidance (95%CI) 0.68 (0.62–0.74) 0.76 (0.71–0.80) 0.79 (0.75–0.83)
(5) Self-avoidance (95%CI) 0.70 (0.64–0.75) 0.77 (0.72–0.81) 0.75 (0.70–0.79) 0.80 (0.76–0.84)
(6) Other avoidance (95%CI) 0.55 (0.47–0.62) 0.64 (0.57–0.70) 0.64 (0.57–0.70) 0.70 (0.64–0.75) 0.73 (0.67–0.78)
(7) Autonomous motivation (95%CI) 0.47 (0.38–0.55) 0.45 (0.36–0.53) 0.42 (0.32–0.51) 0.40 (0.30–0.49) 0.43 (0.33–0.52) 0.41 (0.31–0.50)
(8) Controlled motivation (95%CI) 0.21 (0.10–0.31) 0.19 (0.08–0.29) 0.27 (0.16–0.37) 0.26 (0.15–0.36) 0.23 (0.12–0.33) 0.31 (0.21–0.41) 0.29 (0.18–0.39)
(9) Mental toughness (95%CI) 0.38 (0.28–0.47) 0.42 (0.32–0.51) 0.39 (0.29–0.48) 0.39 (0.29–0.48) 0.33 (0.23–0.43) 0.35 (0.25–0.44) 0.47 (0.39–0.55) 0.15* (0.04–0.26)
Mean ± SD 15.95 ± 3.80 15.87 ± 3.92 16.57 ± 3.78 16.41 ± 3.78 16.18 ± 3.62 15.79 ± 3.84 29.84 ± 7.16 28.51 ± 7.37 39.65 ± 8.44
Skewness −0.40 −0.43 −0.30 −0.20 −0.07 −0.16 0.20 −0.39 0.31
Kurtosis −1.15 −1.10 −1.09 −1.12 −1.23 −0.88 −0.91 0.37 −0.71
ω 0.86 0.90 0.85 0.84 0.79 0.81 0.91 0.89 0.91

Abbreviation: CI = confidence interval.

p < 0.05. All other Pearson correlations statistically significant at p < 0.001.

3.2. Primary analyses

The latent profile analysis model fit indices are displayed in Table 2. The bootstrap likelihood ratio test p values reached statistical significance for models with 2, 3, and 5 profiles. The corrected Aikake information criterion and Bayesian information criterion values indicated a superior level of fit for the 3-profile solution, whereas Aikake information criterion and adjusted Bayesian information criterion fit indices were lower for the 5-profile model. However, one of the classes associated with the 5-profile solution was not assigned any cases, a scenario characteristic of spurious profile extraction.51 A comparison of entropy values and mean posterior probabilities suggested that class allocation was clearer for the 3-profile structure than for the model in which 5 profiles were specified. Overall, the 3-profile solution provided the best fit for the data.

Table 2.

Fit indices for latent profile analysis models.

Model AIC cAIC BIC aBIC BLRT Entropy nMPAP ≤ 0.70 nP < 5%
1-Profile 8655.42 8783.49 8756.49 8670.85 1.000
2-Profile 8537.39 8698.65 8664.65 8556.82 <0.001 0.978 0 0
3-Profile 8484.83 8679.29 8638.29 8508.25 <0.001 0.965 0 0
4-Profile 8499.14 8726.80 8678.80 8526.56 0.999 0.818 2 0
5-Profile 8444.26 8705.13 8650.13 8475.69 <0.001 0.803 2 1
6-Profile 8424.74 8718.81 8656.81 8460.16 0.071 0.776 2 0

Note: Entries in boldface reflect selected model.

Abbreviations: aBIC = sample size adjusted Bayesian information criterion; AIC = Akaike information criterion; BIC = Bayesian information criterion; BLRT = p value for bootstrap likelihood ratio test; cAIC = corrected Akaike information criterion; nMPAP ≤ 0.70 = number of profiles with mean posterior assignment probabilities at or below 0.70; nP < 5% = number of profiles assigned <5% of the cases.

Fig. 1 displays the levels of each achievement goal subtype (mean centered) as a function of subpopulation. Profile 1 (48.08%) comprised participants with low approach (TAP = 12.70 ± 2.47, SAP = 12.33 ± 2.37, OAP = 13.34 ± 2.30; mean ± SD) and low avoidance goals (TAV = 13.46 ± 2.32, SAV = 13.33 ± 2.14, OAV = 13.39 ± 2.69), which represented athletes with a low achievement goal orientation. Participants classified into Profile 2 (8.33%) reported high approach (TAP = 19.31 ± 1.38, SAP = 19.00 ± 1.30, OAP = 19.00 ± 2.12) and low avoidance (TAV = 16.00 ± 3.64, SAV = 14.23 ± 1.82, OAV = 13.77 ± 3.66) goals. This configuration reflected tennis players with an approach-dominant achievement goal orientation. Conversely, the remaining participants (43.59%), who fit Profile 3, reported high approach (TAP = 18.88 ± 1.92, SAP = 19.17 ± 1.46, OAP = 19.66 ± 1.95) and high avoidance (TAV = 19.74 ± 1.89, SAV = 19.69 ± 1.56, OAV = 18.82 ± 2.66) goals. This pattern represented athletes with a high achievement goal orientation. These 3 profiles were used to categorize participants on the achievement goal orientation variable.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1.

Achievement goal orientation profiles.

The χ2 tests of independence revealed age group differences, χ2 (4) = 36.11, p < 0.001, but not sex differences, χ2 (2) = 4.87, p = 0.088, in achievement goal orientation subgroups. Although the majority of tennis players competing in the Open age group (65.69%) were in the high achievement goal orientation group, the largest proportion of players in the U16 (55.93%) and U18 (62.50%) age groups were in the low achievement goal orientation group. The proportion of males and females was similar across the low achievement goal orientation (male, 64.67%), approach-dominant achievement goal orientation (male, 73.08%), and high achievement goal orientation (male, 76.47%) subgroups.

ANOVA model summary statistics and descriptive statistics for the criterion variables used to test for differences in achievement goal orientation are reported in Table 3. Significant differences in means between achievement goal orientation subgroups were found on autonomous motivation, controlled motivation, and mental toughness. Post hoc pairwise comparisons indicated the low achievement goal orientation group scored lower in autonomous motivation than did the approach-dominant achievement goal orientation group (p = 0.038) and the high achievement goal orientation group (p < 0.001). Controlled motivation was higher among the high achievement goal orientation group than the low achievement goal orientation group (p < 0.001). Compared with the low achievement goal orientation group, mental toughness scores were higher for the approach-dominant achievement goal orientation group (p < 0.001) and the high achievement goal orientation group (p < 0.001). The high and approach-dominant groups did not differ with regard to autonomous motivation (p = 0.252), controlled motivation (p = 0.125), or mental toughness (p = 0.910). Controlled motivation scores were similar among the low and approach-dominant achievement goal orientation groups (p = 0.983).

Table 3.

Summary statistics for univariate analyses and post hoc pairwise comparisons by achievement goal orientation.

Predictor Autonomous motivation
Controlled motivation
Mental toughness
F (df) Mean ± SD F (df) Mean ± SD F (df) Mean ± SD
Age group 0.08 (2, 302) 2.07 (2, 302) 2.01 (2, 302)
U16 29.29 ± 7.13 27.64 ± 7.77 39.88 ± 8.15
U18 29.02 ± 6.18 29.03 ± 5.86 39.12 ± 8.37
Open 31.38 ± 7.86 29.18 ± 8.11 40.03 ± 8.96
Sex 1.43 (1, 302) 0.18 (1, 302) 0.81 (1, 302)
Male 29.92 ± 7.25 28.79 ± 7.45 39.69 ± 8.47
Female 29.88 ± 7.05 27.97 ± 7.32 39.77 ± 8.50
Achievement goal orientation 38.81 (2, 302)* 7.53 (2, 302)* 38.40 (2, 302)*
Low 26.59 ± 4.79 26.96 ± 5.50 35.92 ± 6.15
Approach-dominant 30.65 ± 7.74# 27.23 ± 7.44 43.73 ± 7.90##
High 33.32 ± 7.61## 30.51 ± 8.66## 43.01 ± 9.02##

p < 0.001; #p < 0.05; ##p < 0.001; reflect mean values that differ significantly from the low achievement goal orientation group.

4. Discussion

The purpose of the current study was to identify latent profiles of athletes based on within-person patterns of achievement goal orientations and test for differences in motivation and mental toughness across each of the subgroups. The results supported 3 unique configurations that reflected variations in athletes’ achievement goal preferences. Based on the profiles extracted, a number of subgroup differences in motivation and mental toughness were identified.

4.1. Heterogeneity in achievement goal orientations

The findings revealed 3 distinct achievement goal orientation profiles that varied primarily in the approach–avoidance valence. Whereas two of the subgroups were characterized by high and low levels of achievement goal-oriented pursuits on all 6 dimensions, the third group preferred approach goals over avoidance goals. Supplementing the substantive literature that has typically used variable-centered approaches to study achievement goal orientations in sport, these findings demonstrate that athletes pursue an assortment of achievement goals and highlight the importance of examining goal orientations collectively rather than in isolation. The approach-dominant group comprised a relatively low proportion of athletes (<10%), and the majority of athletes tended to report similar levels on both approach and avoidance valences of the achievement goal orientations they pursue.

Of particular interest is that many athletes endorsed high levels of both approach and avoidance goals. With emerging evidence supporting situational fluctuations in athletes’ achievement goal pursuits,12 there may be circumstances in which athletes consider avoidance goals to be useful. Avoidance motives induce attention to detail, systematic information processing, vigilance, and the recruitment of cognitive resources.54 This factor may be particularly relevant in the competitive tennis context because the sport requires athletes to execute complex and accurate motor movements.55 Indeed, avoidance goals may result in the immediate recruitment of cognitive resources to deal with problems arising during sports tasks with strong time constraints and to avoid failure and its negative effects.56, 57 Middle-to-late adolescents are more sensitive to insecurity and failure, especially if they are competitive athletes.58 Participants were mainly young tennis players (65.02% were <18 years of age), and they were actively partaking in national-level singles tennis tournaments. Consequently, they may more likely to endorse avoidance goals (complementing approach goals) if these goals have immediate positive effects on their sport performance.

4.2. Validity of achievement goal orientation profiles

Profile comparisons were consistent with the expectation that autonomous motivation would be higher among athletes who reported higher levels of approach types of goal orientations. Participants in the approach-dominant and high achievement goal orientation groups (who endorsed all 3 types of approach goals) self-reported similar levels of autonomous motivation, yet the approach-dominant group endorsed comparably lower levels of avoidance types of goals. Although self- and task-based goals have been more closely aligned with autonomous forms of motivation in prior research,12 the present findings suggest that other-based goals are not necessarily detrimental to autonomous motivation, provided that athletes are also oriented by mastery forms of goals. This coincides with research indicating that intrinsic forms of motivation are positively related to performance-approach goals,59 but not to other types of goal orientations,26 and has evidenced no clear pattern of relations with avoidance-based goals.3 Only the low and high achievement goal orientation groups differed in controlled motivation, with the approach-dominant group reporting similar levels of controlled motivation to each group. Even though avoidance goals, as compared with approach goals, have typically been associated with forms of motivation that are more controlled,37 controlled motivation may also be influenced by athletes’ pursuit of approach types of goals.

The subgroups of participants in the approach-dominant and high achievement goal orientation groups reported higher levels of mental toughness than the low achievement goal orientation group. Mental toughness differences between the 3 profiles were in the expected direction, with athletes who favored approach types of goals (irrespective of avoidance goal preferences) reporting greater mental toughness. These findings coincide with previous research suggesting that mental toughness is associated with self-, task-, and other-oriented approach goals,37 yet provide an indication of the mix of achievement goals that are favored by athletes with higher levels of mental toughness. Another advantage of the person-centered approach used was the finding that mentally tougher athletes may also pursue avoidance types of goals, suggesting that avoidance types of achievement goals, when favored alongside approach goals, may not necessarily detract from mental toughness. Although prior research has advocated emphasis on approach goals in developing mental toughness,37 evidence suggests that there may be different ways to cultivate mental toughness.60, 61 Thus, mental toughness might also arise from a collection of approach and avoidance achievement goal preferences.

Research has also found state-like fluctuations in mental toughness,62 suggesting there may also be changes in athletes’ mental toughness over time and across situations. Although athletes high in mental toughness may emphasize approach types of achievement goals more consistently, even athletes who have achieved the highest accolades in their sporting code (many of whom have been described as mentally tough) have been oriented by avoidance types of achievement goals during the course of their careers.63 Perhaps situational variations in mental toughness affect the types of achievement goal orientations athletes emphasize. An important follow-up to this study would be to ascertain whether athletes who strictly favor approach goals are more inclined to display consistent levels of mental toughness across situations.

4.3. Limitations and future research directions

A key strength of this study was the use of a person-centered approach to profile athletes according to the unique configurations of achievement goals. The findings extend prior research that has typically relied on variable-centered approaches to examine achievement goal orientations in sport. There are, however, selected methodological limitations that need to be considered when interpreting the present findings. First, causal-effect conclusions are precluded by the cross-sectional design used in this study. Second, the sample consisted of national-level competitors participating in an individual, noncontact racquet sport. Even though there were several similarities between the findings in the present study and prior research involving athletes participating in other kinds of sports (e.g., Australian Rules football37), caution should be applied when generalizing the findings to other sport types and levels of competitive participation. However, goal orientations are often held in a particular life domain64 and may be sport specific. Measurement precision and predictive utility may be decreased when achievement goals are assessed at a broad level (i.e., “when you play sport”) rather than at a sport-specific level (e.g., “when you play tennis”).3 Third, our measurement approach did not account for contextual or temporal changes in the constructs included in this study. Research is needed to identify the stability of the achievement goal profiles identified in this study and changes in relations with auxiliary variables of interest, such as whether certain combinations of achievement goals relate more strongly to mental toughness in some competitive situations than in others. Further investigations are also necessary to examine achievement goal profiles in relation to objective sport performance, which is a highly valued outcome in the approach–avoidance goals literature.2 Longitudinal designs may be an interesting way of determining whether the performance of athletes who strictly endorse approach types of achievement goals follows the same temporal pattern as athletes who tend to pursue both approach and avoidance goals. Fourth, each of the variables was measured using athletes’ self-report ratings, which may have resulted in socially desirable response sets or self-report bias. In future studies, researchers might consider using other-informant ratings or observations to accompany athletes’ self-reports.

5. Conclusion

We found evidence of 3 subgroups of athletes that favored unique combinations of achievement goals, which were primarily distinguishable based on valence of competence preferences (i.e., approach vs. avoidance). Differences on auxiliary outcome variables were largely consistent with expectations, such that higher levels of autonomous motivation and mental toughness were reported among groups of athletes who endorsed approach types of goals (irrespective of whether or not they endorsed avoidance goals). Overall, the findings indicate that athletes tend to pursue a number of achievement goals collectively rather than in isolation, and that avoidance goals may also be associated with desirable psychological characteristics provided they are pursued in conjunction with approach goals.

Authors’ contributions

RGC and NM conceived the article ideas and design, and drafted, revised, and edited the manuscript; TRD assisted in revising and editing the manuscript. All authors have read and approved the final version of the manuscript, and agree with the order of presentation of the authors.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Footnotes

Peer review under responsibility of Shanghai University of Sport.

References

  • 1.Gaudreau P., Braaten A. Achievement goals and their underlying goal motivation: does it matter why sport participants pursue their goals? Psychol Belg. 2016;56:244–268. doi: 10.5334/pb.266. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Lochbaum M., Gottardy J. A meta-analytic review of the approach-avoidance achievement goals and performance relationships in the sport psychology literature. J Sport Health Sci. 2015;4:164–173. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Mascret N., Elliot A.J., Cury F. Extending the 3 × 2 achievement goal model to the sport domain: the 3 × 2 Achievement Goal Questionnaire for Sport. Psychol Sport Exerc. 2015;17:7–14. [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Elliot A.J. A conceptual history of the achievement goal construct. In: Elliot A.J., Dweck C.S., editors. Handbook of competence and motivation. Guilford Publications; New York, NY: 2005. pp. 52–72. [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Conroy D.E., Elliot A.J., Hofer S.M. A 2 × 2 Achievement Goals Questionnaire for Sport: evidence for factorial invariance, temporal stability, and external validity. J Sport Exerc Psychol. 2003;25:456–476. [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Elliot A. Approach and avoidance motivation and achievement goals. Educ Psychol. 1999;34:169–189. [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Elliot A.J., Murayama K., Pekrun R. A 3 × 2 achievement goal model. J Educ Psychol. 2011;103:632–648. [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Laursen B.P., Hoff E. Person-centered and variable-centered approaches to longitudinal data. Merrill-Palmer Q. 2006;52:377–389. [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Morin A.J.S., Meyer J.P., Creusier J., Biétry F. Multiple group analysis of similarity in latent profile solutions. Organ Res Methods. 2016;19:231–254. [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Peugh J., Fan X. Modeling unobserved heterogeneity using latent profile analysis: a Monte Carlo simulation. Struct Equ Modeling. 2013;20:616–639. [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Collins L., Lanza S. John Wiley & Sons; Hoboken, NJ: 2010. Latent class and latent transition analysis: with applications in the social, behavioral, and health sciences. [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Vansteenkiste M., Mouratidis A., Van Riet T., Lens W. Examining correlates of game-to-game variation in volleyball players' achievement goal pursuit and underlying autonomous and controlling reasons. J Sport Exerc Psychol. 2014;36:131–145. doi: 10.1123/jsep.2012-0271. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Litalien D., Morin A.S., McInerney D.M. Achievement goal profiles among adolescent males and females. Dev Psychol. 2017;53:731–751. doi: 10.1037/dev0000288. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Morin A.J.S., Morizot J., Boudrias J., Madore I. A multifoci person-centered perspective on workplace affective commitment: a latent profile/factor mixture analysis. Organ Res Methods. 2011;14:58–90. [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Clancy R.B., Herring M.P., MacIntyre T.E., Campbell M.J. A review of competitive sport motivation research. Psychol Sport Exerc. 2016;27:232–242. [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Ryan R.M., Deci E.L. Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being. Am Psychol. 2000;55:68–78. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.55.1.68. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Pelletier L.G., Rocchi M.A., Vallerand R.J., Deci E.L., Ryan R.M. Validation of the revised sport motivation scale (SMS-II) Psychol Sport Exerc. 2013;14:329–341. [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Lonsdale C., Hodge K., Rose E. Athlete burnout in elite sport: a self-determination perspective. J Sports Sci. 2009;27:785–795. doi: 10.1080/02640410902929366. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Langan E., Hodge K., McGowan S., Carney S., Saunders V., Lonsdale C. The influence of controlled motivation alongside autonomous motivation: maladaptive, buffering, or additive effects? Int J Sport Exerc Psychol. 2016;14:57–71. [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Chalabaev A., Radel R., Ben Mahmoud I., Massiera B., Deroche T., d'Arripe-Longueville F. Is motivation for marathon a protective factor or a risk factor of injury? Scand J Med Sci Sports. 2017;27:2040–2047. doi: 10.1111/sms.12807. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Hodge K., Hargreaves E.A., Gerrard D., Lonsdale C. Psychological mechanisms underlying doping attitudes in sport: motivation and moral disengagement. J Sport Exerc Psychol. 2013;35:419–432. doi: 10.1123/jsep.35.4.419. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Jowett G.E., Hill A.P., Hall H.K., Curran T. Perfectionism and junior athlete burnout: the mediating role of autonomous and controlled motivation. Sport Exerc Perform Psychol. 2013;2:48–61. [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Gillet N., Vallerand R.J., Rosnet E. Motivational clusters and performance in a real-life setting. Motiv Emot. 2009;33:49–62. [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Mouratidis A., Michou A. Perfectionism, self-determined motivation, and coping among adolescent athletes. Psychol Sport Exerc. 2011;12:355–367. [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Wang C.J., Liu W.C., Lochbaum M.R., Stevenson S.J. Sport ability beliefs, 2 × 2 achievement goals, and intrinsic motivation: the moderating role of perceived competence in sport and exercise. Res Q Exerc Sport. 2009;80:303–312. doi: 10.1080/02701367.2009.10599565. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Adie J.W., Jowett S. Meta-perceptions of the coach–athlete relationship, achievement goals, and intrinsic motivation among sport participants. J Appl Soc Psychol. 2010;40:2750–2773. [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Cowden R.G. Competitive performance correlates of mental toughness in tennis: a preliminary analysis. Percept Mot Skills. 2016;123:341–360. doi: 10.1177/0031512516659902. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Cowden R.G. Mental toughness and success in sport: a review and prospect. Open Sports Sci J. 2017;10:1–14. [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Gucciardi D.F. Mental toughness: progress and prospects. Curr Opin Psychol. 2017;16:17–23. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.03.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Gucciardi D.F., Hanton S., Gordon S., Mallett C., Temby P. The concept of mental toughness: tests of dimensionality, nomological network, and traitness. J Pers. 2015;83:26–44. doi: 10.1111/jopy.12079. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Vaughan R., Hanna D., Breslin G. Psychometric properties of the Mental Toughness Questionnaire 48 (MTQ48) in elite, amateur and nonathletes. Sport Exerc Perform Psychol. 2018;7:128–140. [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Cook C., Crust L., Littlewood M., Nesti M., Allen-Collinson J. “What it takes”: perceptions of mental toughness and its development in an English Premier League Soccer Academy. Qual Res Sport Exerc Health. 2014;6:329–347. [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Jones G., Hanton S., Connaughton D. A framework of mental toughness in the world's best performers. Sport Psychol. 2007;21:243–264. [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Crust L., Swann C., Allen-Collinson J., Breckon J., Weinberg R. A phenomenological exploration of exercise mental toughness: perceptions of exercise leaders and regular exercisers. Qual Res Sport Exerc Health. 2014;6:441–461. [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Powell A.J., Myers T.D. Developing mental toughness: lessons from Paralympians. Front Psychol. 2017;8:1270. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01270. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Beck N., Petrie T.A., Harmison R.J., Moore E.W.G. Parent, coach, and peer created motivational climates: relationships to goal orientations and mental toughness. Int J Sport Psychol. 2017;48:185–205. [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Gucciardi D.F. Mental toughness profiles and their relations with achievement goals and sport motivation in adolescent Australian footballers. J Sports Sci. 2010;28:615–625. doi: 10.1080/02640410903582792. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Madigan D.J., Stoeber J., Passfield L. Perfectionism and achievement goals revisited: the 3 × 2 achievement goal framework. Psychol Sport Exerc. 2017;28:120–124. [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Li C., Kawabata M., Zhang L. Validity and reliability of the Sport Motivation Scale-II for Chinese athletes. Int J Sport Exerc Psychol. 2018;16:51–64. [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Li C., Zhang C., Zhang L. Further examination of the psychometric properties of the Mental Toughness Inventory: evidence from Chinese athletes and university students. Curr Psychol. 2019;38:1328–1334. [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Madigan D.J., Nicholls A.R. Mental toughness and burnout in junior athletes: a longitudinal investigation. Psychol Sport Exerc. 2017;32:138–142. [Google Scholar]
  • 42.R Core Team . R Foundation for Statistical Computing; Vienna: 2017. R: a language and environment for statistical computing. [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Stekhoven D., Bühlmann P. MissForest: non-parametric missing value imputation for mixed-type data. Bioinform. 2012;28:112–118. doi: 10.1093/bioinformatics/btr597. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.In'nami Y., Koizumi R. Structural equation modeling in educational research: a primer. In: Khine M., editor. Application of structural equation modeling in educational research and practice. Sense Publishers; Rotterdam: 2013. pp. 23–51. [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Curran P.J., West S.G., Finch J.F. The robustness of test statistics to nonnormality and specification error in confirmatory factor analysis. Psychol Methods. 1996;1:16–29. [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Dunn T.J., Baguley T., Brunsden V. From alpha to omega: a practical solution to the pervasive problem of internal consistency estimation. Br J Psychol. 2014;105:399–412. doi: 10.1111/bjop.12046. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Oberski D. Mixture models: latent profile and latent class analysis. In: Robertson J., Kaptein M., editors. Modern statistical methods for HCI. Springer; Cham: 2016. p. 275–87. [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Muthén B.O. Latent variable analysis: growth mixture modeling and related techniques for longitudinal data. In: Kaplan D., editor. Handbook of quantitative methodology for social sciences. Sage; Newbury Park, CA: 2004. p. 345–68. [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Nylund K., Asparouhov T., Muthén B. Deciding on the number of classes in latent class analyses and growth mixture modeling. A Monte Carlo simulation study. Struct Equ Modeling. 2007;14:535–569. [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Stanley L., Kellermanns F.W., Zellweger T.M. Latent profile analysis: understanding family firm profiles. Fam Bus Rev. 2017;30:84–102. [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Hipp J.R., Bauer D.J. Local solutions in the estimation of growth mixture models. Psychol Methods. 2006;11:36–53. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.11.1.36. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Ekstrøm C.T. Teaching “instant experience” with graphical model validation techniques. Teach Stat. 2014;36:23–26. [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Cribari-Neto F. Asymptotic inference under heteroskedastictiy of unknown form. Comput Stat Data Anal. 2004;45:215–253. [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Roskes M., Elliot A.J., Nijstad B.A., De Dreu C.K. Avoidance motivation and conservation of energy. Emot Rev. 2013;5:264–268. [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Defrancesco C., Burke K.L. Performance enhancement strategies used in a professional tennis tournament. Int J Sport Psychol. 1997;28:185–195. [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Baumeister R.F., Bratslavsky E., Finkenauer C., Vohs K.D. Bad is stronger than good. Rev Gen Psychol. 2001;5:323–370. [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Elliot A.J. The hierarchical model of approach–avoidance motivation. Motiv Emot. 2006;30:111–116. [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Coakley J. Burnout among adolescent athletes: a personal failure or social problem? Sociol Sport J. 1992;9:271–285. [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Wang C.K.J., Koh K.T., Chatzisarantis N.L.D. An intra-individual analysis of players’ perceived coaching behaviours, psychological needs, and achievement goals. Int J Sports Sci Coach. 2009;4:177–192. [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Bell J.J., Hardy L., Beattie S. Enhancing mental toughness and performance under pressure in elite young cricketers: a 2-year longitudinal intervention. Sport Exerc Perform Psychol. 2013;2:281–297. [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Connaughton D., Hanton S., Jones G. The development and maintenance of mental toughness in the world's best performers. Sport Psychol. 2010;24:168–193. doi: 10.1080/02640410701310958. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Weinberg R., Butt J., Mellano K., Harmison R. The stability of mental toughness across situations: taking a social-cognitive approach. Int J Sport Psychol. 2017;48:280–302. [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Newman H.J.H., Howells K.L., Fletcher D. The dark side of top level sport: an autobiographic study of depressive experiences in elite sport performers. Front Psychol. 2016;7:868. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00868. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Spray C.M., Wang C.K.J., Biddle S.J., Chatzisarantis N.L. Understanding motivation in sport: an experimental test of achievement goal and self-determination theories. Eur J Sport Sci. 2006;6:43–51. [Google Scholar]

Articles from Journal of Sport and Health Science are provided here courtesy of Shanghai University of Sport

RESOURCES