Skip to main content
Springer Nature - PMC COVID-19 Collection logoLink to Springer Nature - PMC COVID-19 Collection
. 2021 Feb 3;22(3):167–179. doi: 10.1038/s41583-021-00428-w

Cognitive and behavioural flexibility: neural mechanisms and clinical considerations

Lucina Q Uddin 1,2,
PMCID: PMC7856857  PMID: 33536614

Abstract

Cognitive and behavioural flexibility permit the appropriate adjustment of thoughts and behaviours in response to changing environmental demands. Brain mechanisms enabling flexibility have been examined using non-invasive neuroimaging and behavioural approaches in humans alongside pharmacological and lesion studies in animals. This work has identified large-scale functional brain networks encompassing lateral and orbital frontoparietal, midcingulo-insular and frontostriatal regions that support flexibility across the lifespan. Flexibility can be compromised in early-life neurodevelopmental disorders, clinical conditions that emerge during adolescence and late-life dementias. We critically evaluate evidence for the enhancement of flexibility through cognitive training, physical activity and bilingual experience.

Subject terms: Cognitive control, Human behaviour


Flexibility is critical for the optimal adaptation of thoughts and actions under changing circumstances. In this Review, Uddin summarizes research that has identified cognitive processes and neural systems supporting flexibility and discusses ways to improve flexibility across the lifespan.

Introduction

The year 2020 will be remembered as a time marked by an unprecedented need for flexibility. In response to the global COVID-19 pandemic, governments, institutions, businesses and individuals made necessary and creative adaptations to cope with an uncertain, rapidly evolving situation1. This public health and economic crisis necessitated a great degree of cognitive and behavioural flexibility on the part of individuals adapting to the novel situation with which they were confronted. Responses to the pandemic, ranging from denial and maintenance of the status quo to swift and decisive action to curtail the spread of the causative virus, provided a real-world example of why an optimal level of flexibility is adaptive.

Developmental and lifespan research suggests that flexibility promotes academic achievement, employment success2, successful transitioning to adulthood3 and other optimal life outcomes. Likewise, flexibility in later life can mitigate the effects of ageing on cognitive decline4. Flexibility is typically thought to comprise both cognitive and behavioural components. ‘Cognitive flexibility’ is broadly defined as the mental ability to switch between thinking about two different concepts according to the context of a situation5. ‘Behavioural flexibility’ refers to the adaptive change of behaviour in response to changing environmental contingencies6. The constructs of cognitive flexibility and behavioural flexibility are thus closely intertwined. Since most laboratory tasks used to assess cognitive flexibility require behavioural outputs, they in effect measure aspects of both cognitive and behavioural flexibility. Likewise, it is hard to imagine a flexible behavioural response that is not associated with flexible cognition. The terms ‘cognitive flexibility’ and ‘behavioural flexibility’ are often used interchangeably in the neuroscience literature, and the differentiation in terminology is most likely attributable to the different disciplines (cognitive psychology and behavioural neuroscience, respectively) from which they arose.

Components of flexibility

Cognitive and behavioural flexibility fall under the broader category of executive functions, or processes necessary for the control of goal-directed behaviour7. Projects such as the Cognitive Atlas8 that aim to systematically characterize psychological processes classify flexibility under executive and cognitive control. The question of whether different processes falling under the executive function umbrella can be considered unitary reflections of the same underlying mechanism9 has been approached using latent variable analysis to examine the extent of unity or diversity of executive functions. In one influential account, executive functions are postulated to comprise three latent variables, described as mental set-shifting (‘shifting’), information updating and monitoring in working memory (‘updating’) and inhibition of prepotent responses (‘inhibition’), that are moderately correlated with one another, yet clearly separable7. This framework has helped address the task impurity problem — the issue that because executive functions necessarily manifest themselves by operating on other cognitive processes, any executive task strongly implicates other processes not directly relevant to the target executive function. When we use the term ‘flexibility’, we mean to invoke the aspect of executive function that is typically associated with shifting.

Relatedly, a large and growing literature on flexibility comes from the study of working memory gating, or the process by which relevant contextual information is updated in working memory while distracting information is kept out10. Studies investigating neural mechanisms underlying flexibility in working memory are reviewed elsewhere11,12.

Box 1 describes two classic paradigms in cognitive and behavioural neuroscience that have historically been used to assess flexibility in human and animals. The Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) is a neuropsychological task developed for humans that measures the ability to infer rules to guide behaviour, create an attentional set based on abstract categories, and switch attention and adjust behaviour with changing task demands13. Performance on the WCST is strongly related to shifting (also referred to as ‘attention switching‘ or ‘task switching’), which involves the disengagement of an irrelevant task set and subsequent active engagement of a relevant task set7. Reversal learning tasks are often used to study behavioural flexibility in humans as well as rodents and non-human primates14. These paradigms assess the ability to respond adaptively in the face of changing stimulus–outcome or response–outcome contingencies15. What are referred to as ‘switch trials’ in cognitive flexibility studies are paralleled by ‘reversals’ in behavioural flexibility experiments. Both switches and reversals are points at which shifting from one task or mode of response to another is required. The first aim of this Review is to draw information from these and related neuroscience studies (Box 2) to summarize what is known regarding the brain systems and processes underlying cognitive and behavioural flexibility.

In the clinical realm, although diagnosis based on the fifth edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-V) is still the norm in psychiatry, there has been a push from the US National Institute of Mental Health to shift towards consideration of behaviour dimensionally — that is, along a continuum — rather than categorically. This research domain criteria (RDoC) approach recognizes that dimensions of behaviour can cut across traditional diagnostic categories and urges the integration of multiple levels of information from genomics to neural circuits to behaviour and self-report (for example, using questionnaires that are filled out by the participants themselves) to understand basic dimensions of functioning spanning the full range of human behaviour16. This framework may lead to a revised diagnostic nosology that is more firmly grounded in biology17. The ‘cognitive systems’ domain of the RDoC matrix includes constructs labelled ‘cognitive control’ and ‘working memory’, which contain subconstructs (goal selection/performance monitoring and flexible updating) that are closely tied to the constructs of cognitive and behavioural flexibility. Consensus regarding which cognitive tasks best probe flexibility can potentially be built by adopting the RDoC framework, which itself is continuously undergoing refinement18.

Cognitive and behavioural flexibility are compromised in clinical conditions affecting early life such as autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD); those that emerge in adolescence, including schizophrenia and mood disorders; and dementias with later-life onset. While many of these conditions share flexibility deficits, the heterogeneous nature, severity and patterns of co-morbid symptoms complicate efforts to develop treatment strategies for enhancing flexibility. The scope of this Review will span these clinical considerations with the goal of identifying common cognitive, pharmacological and neurobiological factors contributing to inflexibility transdiagonostically. Finally, we critically evaluate potential avenues for flexibility training and discuss future directions for translational neuroscience.

Box 1 Experimental paradigms used to assess cognitive and behavioural flexibility in humans and animals.

The Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (see the figure, part a) was first developed in 1948 to assess perseveration, abstract reasoning and set-shifting142. The test takes 20–30 minutes to administer as follows: four cards incorporating three stimulus parameters (colour, shape and number of objects) are presented to the participants, who are then asked to sort individual response cards according to different principles. Four different ways of classifying each card are possible, and participants must learn using feedback provided by the experimenter whether a given classification is correct or not. After the participant has correctly sorted several cards according to one learned rule, the experimenter changes the rule without letting the participant know that the rule has been changed. Individuals with frontal lobe damage and children younger than 4 years tend to persist in sorting cards according to previously learned rules and have difficulty flexibly switching to new sorting rules143. In reversal learning paradigms (see the figure, part b) animals form associations between two choices and their reward outcomes initially over a series of trials. After a successful learning period, the choice–outcome mapping is reversed. The ability of the animal to adapt and change behaviour after the first reversal is a measure of behavioural flexibility144. Part a is reproduced by special permission of the Publisher, Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc., 16204 North Florida Avenue, Lutz, Florida 33549, from the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, Copyright 1981, 1993 by PAR, Inc. Further reproduction is prohibited without permission of PAR, Inc. Part b is adapted from Brady, A. M. & Floresco, S. B. Operant procedures for assessing behavioural flexibility in rats. J. Vis. Exp. 96, e52387, 10.3791/52387 (2015).Box 1 Experimental paradigms used to assess cognitive and behavioural flexibility in humans and animals

Box 2 How is creativity related to flexibility?

Flexible thinking is a critical component of creativity, or the ability to think of new ideas or make new things. Flexibility and creativity have not historically been studied in tandem, despite the obvious parallels between the constructs. While cognitive flexibility is conceptualized as an aspect of executive function and is associated with a rich human neuroimaging literature, creativity has only recently become the topic of cognitive neuroscientific investigations. A query of researchers from academic societies focused on creativity (the Society for the Neuroscience of Creativity and the Society for the Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts) yielded several cognitive constructs deemed relevant to creativity, including ‘flexibility’, ‘cognitive control’ and ‘divergent thinking145. A meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies of divergent thinking indicates that brain networks underlying creative idea generation are composed of lateral prefrontal, posterior parietal and anterior cingulate cortices, as well as the caudate146. A study examining neuroimaging predictors of creativity assessed with visual and verbal tests of divergent thinking, everyday creative behaviour and creative achievement revealed that greater creativity was broadly predicted by grey matter of the inferior frontal gyrus and inferior parietal lobule as well as white matter integrity of the basal ganglia147. These findings align with functional activation studies showing inferior frontal gyrus involvement in verbal creative problem-solving148. The overlap in lateral frontoparietal and striatal involvement for both flexibility and creativity points to potential shared neural substrates for these related constructs. Future work in creativity could thus benefit from closer integration with the literature on cognitive flexibility.

Neural substrates of flexibility

Cognitive flexibility follows a protracted, inverted U-shaped developmental trajectory from early childhood through adolescence and adulthood, peaking between the second and third decades of life, and declining in late life19. Here we will summarize the role of lateral and orbital frontoparietal, midcingulo-insular and frontostriatal functional brain networks in supporting flexibility across the lifespan. The cognitive processes and neural properties contributing to the development of flexibility, its maturation in young adulthood and its decline with ageing will be delineated.

Cognitive flexibility in humans

In studies of the neural basis of cognitive flexibility, participants perform task-switching or set-shifting paradigms while their brain activity is monitored using functional MRI (fMRI)20. It is important to keep in mind that laboratory-based measures and neuropsychological tests have high construct validity but may not always converge with real-world flexible behaviours as indexed using self-report or informant-report questionnaires, which typically have greater ecological validity21. The Behaviour Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF) is an assessment available in versions for both children and adults that includes a measure of an individual’s ability to shift, or make transitions, tolerate change, flexibly problem-solve, switch attention and change focus from one topic to another22,23. Adult participants complete the BRIEF as a self-report, and parents and teachers can complete this assessment to evaluate school-aged children. Test batteries that include assessments of flexibility in children and adults include the WCST, the Dimensional Change Card Sort24, the Delis–Kaplan Executive Function System (D-KEFS)25, NEPSY-II26 and the Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery Intra–Extra Dimensional Set Shift task27.

Cognitive flexibility is difficult to isolate, as it requires the confluence of several aspects of executive function20,28. Neurosynth is a tool for synthesizing the results of human neuroimaging studies to produce mappings between neural activation patterns and cognitive states using text mining and automated meta-analyses29. Entering the terms describing the latent variables comprising executive function into Neurosynth reveals that the brain maps associated with these interrelated cognitive constructs are highly overlapping7 (Fig. 1a).

Fig. 1. Core cognitive processes and brain network interactions underlying flexibility in the human brain.

Fig. 1

a | Three latent variables that constitute executive function are referred to as ‘shifting (flexibility)’, ‘updating (working memory)’ and ‘inhibition’7. Automated meta-analyses of published functional neuroimaging studies can be conducted with Neurosynth, a Web-based platform that uses text mining to extract activation coordinates from studies reporting on a specific psychological term of interest and machine learning to estimate the likelihood that activation maps are associated with specific psychological terms, thus creating mapping between neural and cognitive states (see ref.29 for detailed methods). Neurosynth reveals that brain imaging studies including the terms ‘shifting’, ‘updating’ and ‘inhibition’ report highly overlapping patterns of activation in lateral frontoparietal and midcingulo-insular brain regions, underscoring the difficulty of isolating the construct of flexibility from associated executive functions. a | Maps created by first, entering the terms ‘shifting’, ‘updating’ and ‘inhibition’ individually into Neurosynth; second, displaying the ‘uniformity test’ results to view z scores corresponding to the degree to which each voxel in the brain is consistently activated in studies that use each of the selected terms; third, downloading the resulting brain images (with thresholding at a false discovery rate of 0.01) in the form of NIfTi files; and fourth, displaying the brain images using the image viewer MRIcron with the following settings: 2.3 < z < 8 (scale); x = 45 (Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) coordinate for sagittal slice), y = 19 (MNI coordinate for coronal slice) and z = 45 (MNI coordinate for axial slice). The uniformity test map depicts z scores from a one-way ANOVA testing whether the proportion of studies that report activation at a given voxel differs from the rate that would be expected if activations were uniformly distributed throughout grey matter. b | Brain regions supporting executive function and flexibility operate within the context of the broader networks shown in part a. During performance of a flexible item selection task, participants directly engage the inferior frontal junction (IFJ), which influences activity in other lateral frontoparietal and midcingulo-insular regions. ACC, anterior cingulate cortex; AG, angular gurus; AI, anterior insula; dlPFC, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex; IPL, inferior parietal lobule. Part b adapted with permission from ref.33, Massachusetts Institute of Technology.

A large body of literature on human functional neuroimaging studies using task-switching and set-shifting paradigms points to a central role for the lateral frontoparietal network (L-FPN) and the midcingulo-insular network (M-CIN) in supporting executive function and cognitive flexibility20,30. The L-FPN is also referred to as the executive control network and includes lateral prefrontal cortices (PFCs; dorsolateral PFC (dlPFC), ventrolateral PFC and inferior frontal junction (IFJ)), the inferior parietal lobule (IPL), posterior inferior temporal lobes and portions of the midcingulate gyrus. The M-CIN is sometimes referred to as the salience network or the cingulo-opercular network, and includes bilateral anterior insulae (AI), the anterior midcingulate cortex and subcortical nodes, including the amygdala and thalamus31.

While whole-brain activation patterns reveal how effortful control and executive functions broadly engage these systems, approaches for estimating task-modulated network connectivity are beginning to reveal how specific experimental manipulations are associated with dynamic relationships among brain regions. For example, one study found that the IFJ is engaged during the updating of task representations, a core aspect of flexibility32. During a task requiring flexible selection of items based on different stimulus dimensions, participants initially directly engaged the IFJ, leading to recruitment of other L-FPN and M-CIN regions, including the dlPFC, IPL, anterior midcingulate cortex and AI via functional connections33. Considerable individual variability in functional network topography supporting cognitive flexibility was observed, and the strength of functional connectivity between selected brain regions was related to individual differences in task performance (Fig. 1b). This finding is in line with earlier work demonstrating domain-general task-switching activation in the IFJ34, a brain region that exhibits meta-analytic co-activation and resting state functional connectivity with the AI, dlPFC and IPL35.

Behavioural flexibility in animals

Assessment of behavioural flexibility in marmoset monkeys reveals that animals with lateral PFC lesions are not impaired in reversal learning or in shifting behavioural responses to a previously rewarded alternative. These monkeys are, however, impaired with regard to extradimensional shifts. Monkeys with orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) lesions show the opposite behaviour: impairment in reversal learning but no deficits in extradimensional shifts. These findings have led to the proposal that the lateral PFC is necessary for shifting of responding between abstract perceptual dimensions, whereas the OFC and associated corticostriatal loops are necessary for shifting of responding between different stimuli with specific associations with reinforcement36. Similar findings have been observed in rodents engaging in reversal learning paradigms. OFC inactivation in rats impairs reversal learning owing to perseverance of previously learned choices15. Neurons in the mouse OFC respond saliently and transiently to rule switches during reversal learning37. Dorsomedial striatal inactivation impairs both reversal learning and strategy switching, resulting in an inability to maintain new choice patterns once they are selected. The dorsomedial striatum is thought to dynamically interact with multiple prefrontal subregions that generate new strategies to facilitate behavioural flexibility38.

For humans, reversal learning is much easier to perform than extradimensional shifts, but similar neuroanatomy to that seen in animals has been observed using fMRI39. Neuroimaging additionally reveals the role of the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex and the inferior frontal gyrus in suppression of prior learned responses and response inhibition during reversal learning40.

Brain dynamics supporting flexibility

Brain dynamics underlie complex forms of cognition and behaviour, including flexibility. Recent work has examined time-varying or dynamic changes in functional coupling between brain regions4143. Sliding window functional connectivity analyses can be used to quantify brain dynamic metrics, including ‘dwell time’ (the time spent in a particular brain state), ‘frequency of occurrence’ (the number of times a given brain state occurs) and ‘transitions’ (the number of times transitions between brain states are observed) (Fig. 2a,b). With use of this approach, it has been shown that certain patterns of whole-brain dynamics are associated with elevated levels of cognitive flexibility. Individuals who score higher on the WCST exhibit more episodes of more frequently occurring brain states, and fewer episodes of less frequently occurring brain states that have previously been associated with low vigilance and arousal44 (Fig. 2c). Dynamic patterns among specific networks have also been linked with flexible behaviours. Time-varying functional connectivity of the M-CIN predicts individual differences in cognitive flexibility45. Dynamics between the default mode or medial frontoparietal network (M-FPN) and the L-FPN have also been linked to cognitive flexibility46. A study using hidden Markov models demonstrates that the proportion of time an individual spends in a brain state characterized by functional connectivity of M-FPN and L-FPN regions relates to individual differences in cognitive flexibility47. Multimodal investigations considering both anatomical connectivity and activation dynamics find that greater alignment between white matter networks and functional signals is associated with greater cognitive flexibility48.

Fig. 2. Brain dynamics underlying individual differences in flexibility.

Fig. 2

a | In sliding window dynamic functional connectivity analyses, time-varying patterns of connectivity between brain regions are quantified as follows. Whole-brain functional connectivity matrices computed for each window (for example, 45 seconds of functional MRI time-series data) are subjected to clustering, and each window is assigned to a ‘brain state’, here labelled 1, 2 and 3. b | Dynamic metrics, including frequency, dwell time and transitions between states, can then be computed on the basis of trajectories of brain state evolution over time141. c | Brain states are ordered from most frequently occurring on the left (state 1, characterized by weak correlations among brain regions) to least frequently occurring on the right (state 5, characterized by strong correlations among brain regions). Higher executive function performance measured outside the scanner is associated with greater episodes of more frequently occurring states and fewer episodes of less frequently occurring states. In the colour bar, hot colours (red) represent high correlation values and cool colours (blue) represent low correlation values. WCST, Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. Parts a, b and c adapted with permission from44, Elsevier.

The emerging links between brain dynamics and flexible behaviours in neurotypical adults49 have set the stage for understanding how these processes are affected in development and ageing. Neural flexibility, or the frequency with which brain regions change allegiance between functional modules, has recently been shown to increase with age during the first 2 years of life50. At the other end of the lifespan, older adults performing well on a cognitive test battery were found to exhibit brain states characterized by global coherence, whereas those performing poorly exhibited greater frequency of switching between dynamic brain states51. Ease of transitions between brain states distinguishes younger from older individuals, and is further linked with executive function indexed by the D-KEFS. In younger adults, executive function ability is correlated with efficiency in brain dynamics of the M-CIN, whereas for older adults this ability is associated with efficiency in the M-FPN52. Brain regions in higher-order association cortices exhibit high levels of functional flexibility, with dissociable age-related changes observed in frontal and parietal regions across the lifespan53. Several recent studies have further shown how individual differences in task performance are related to patterns of dynamic brain organization54,55. Taken together, this emerging literature is in line with the notion that the ability of the brain to flexibly reconfigure itself in response to changing demands may underlie individual differences in flexible behaviours.

Brain variability and flexibility

Variability in neural signals, while initially conceptualized as noise56, has more recently been linked with cognitive capacity. Between childhood and mid-adulthood, brain signal variability increases with age, shows negative correlations with reaction time variability and positive correlations with accuracy57. Brain variability appears to increase during task performance compared with rest in younger and faster-performing adults, whereas older and slower-performing adults exhibit less differentiation in brain variability across experimental conditions58. These findings build on work demonstrating that blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) variability is a better predictor of age than BOLD mean59. Across the age range from 6 to 85 years, BOLD signal variability decreases linearly across most of the brain, with the exception of the AI, a critical M-CIN node involved in flexibility, which shows the opposite pattern60 (Fig. 3). In line with findings from functional activation studies, it has been shown that increased IFJ variability is linked to better performance on a cognitive flexibility task61. Older adults aged 59–73 years who exhibit upregulated brain signal variability show higher levels of task performance62. The suggestion is that higher variability might reflect a broader repertoire of metastable brain states and transitions between them to enable optimal responses57.

Fig. 3. Quantifying brain signal variability.

Fig. 3

a | Mean squared successive difference is one approach for computing brain signal variability. Applied to neural time-series data, mean squared successive difference is calculated according to the equation shown. b | Regionally specific increases and decreases in brain signal variability across the lifespan may be associated with changes in behavioural performance. Brain signal variability decreases linearly across the lifespan in most brain regions, with the exception of the anterior insula, which exhibits linear age-related increases in variability. In early and late life, the speculation is that larger differences in variability between brain regions may lead to suboptimal behavioural performance. Optimal behavioural performance may be associated with a balance between high and low variability in different brain regions (black arrows) during midlife. Part b is adapted from ref.60, CC BY 4.0 (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).

Flexibility in clinical conditions

Executive function impairments broadly, and flexibility impairments specifically, are observed across many forms of psychopathology and may serve as transdiagnostic intermediate phenotypes63. All of the DSM-V categories (with the possible exception of sleep–wake disorders) include clinical conditions in which domains of executive function are compromised; this raises the question of the extent to which the construct of flexibility has discriminative value. In several of the disorders in which flexibility deficits have been documented, these impairments can be observed even while performance on basic perceptual and motor tasks remains unaltered.

Flexibility deficits are observed in neurodevelopmental conditions with early life onset, such as ASD and ADHD, as well as psychiatric conditions that emerge in adolescence, including mood disorders, obsessive–compulsive disorder (OCD) and schizophrenia. Late life onset dementias, including Parkinson disease and Alzheimer disease, are also marked by rigidity and cognitive inflexibility. The extent to which common and distinct neural mechanisms underlie the variety of flexibility deficits observed across the lifespan in these conditions will be explored in this section (Table 1).

Table 1.

Psychiatric and neurological disorders affecting flexibility across the lifespan

Category Disorder Cognitive and behavioural manifestation of flexibility deficit Putative neural substrate and/or neurotransmitter systems affected
Neurodevelopmental disorders Autism spectrum disorder Restricted and repetitive behaviours M-CIN, L-FPN, striatum
Neurodevelopmental disorders Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder Inattention M-CIN, L-FPN, striatum, dopamine
Depressive disorders Major depressive disorder Repetitive negative thinking M-CIN, L-FPN, M-FPN
Anxiety disorders Generalized anxiety disorder Repetitive negative thinking M-CIN, L-FPN, M-FPN
Obsessive–compulsive disorder and related disorders Obsessive–compulsive disorder Recurrent and persistent thoughts OFC, frontostriatal
Schizophrenia spectrum disorders and other psychotic disorders Schizophrenia Excessive cognitive rigidity M-CIN, L-FPN, striatum, dopamine
Neurocognitive disorders Alzheimer disease Excessive cognitive rigidity M-FPN
Neurocognitive disorders Parkinson disease Excessive cognitive rigidity Striatum, dopamine

L-FPN, lateral frontoparietal network; M-CIN, midcingulo-insular network; M-FPN, medial frontoparietal network; OFC, orbitofrontal cortex.

Flexibility in developmental disorders

ASD and ADHD are two prevalent, heterogeneous neurodevelopmental disorders typically diagnosed in the first 5 years of life. In children with ASD or ADHD, executive function and flexibility deficits are often observed in laboratory settings and in day-to-day activities64,65. Although children with ASD, ADHD or co-morbid ASD and ADHD may all exhibit flexibility deficits, the nature and severity of these issues can differ across and even within these disorders.

Early work in developmental psychopathology66 and recent meta-analyses confirm broad executive dysfunction in ASD across domains67 as well as more specific impairments in flexibility, typically assessed with the WCST64,68. Restricted and repetitive behaviours (RRBs), considered core deficits in ASD, can include stereotyped movements, insistence on sameness, and circumscribed or perseverative interests69. The severity of RRBs is associated with measures of cognitive inflexibility in ASD70. Studies of the neural circuitry underlying RRBs transdiagostically point to a critical role for frontostriatal systems in mediating these behaviours71. A recent review of neural mechanisms underlying cognitive and behavioural flexibility in autism additionally points to atypical patterns of L-FPN and M-CIN activation in response to task switching and set-shifting72.

While ASD is characterized by difficulty in flexibly adapting to changes in routines, children with ADHD have difficulty with attentional focus and exhibit high levels of variability in moment-to-moment behaviours73. Diagnostic criteria for ADHD include inattention, hyperactivity and impulsivity69, which can be thought of as manifestations of distractibility or too much flexibility. Still, the story is not as simple as ‘impaired flexibility in ASD’ versus ‘heightened flexibility in ADHD’, as there is a very high degree of co-morbidity between these two disorders74 such that the combination of impaired flexibility and inattention can manifest itself in the same individual. Some reports claim that executive dysfunction is more pervasive and more severe in ADHD than in ASD75, yet studies targeting flexibility document that children with ASD perform poorer on the WCST than do children with ADHD76. Age-related improvements in executive function are more clearly observed in ASD than in ADHD77. Even though not all children with a primary diagnosis of ASD exhibit executive dysfunction78, the vast majority of children with co-morbid ASD and ADHD do exhibit executive function impairments65.

Only a handful of neuroimaging studies have examined ASD and ADHD together. One found evidence for shared and distinct patterns of intrinsic functional connectivity centrality in children with these disorders79. Another reported no evidence for group differences in functional network connectivity across diagnostic groups80. Although it is hypothesized that the common behavioural manifestations of cognitive inflexibility across ASD and ADHD should be reflected in shared neural substrates, few assessments of brain circuitry supporting flexibility across these disorders have been conducted. Data-driven techniques are now being used to identify key dimensions of functioning that overlap across diagnostic categories and also present heterogeneously within diagnostic categories81. For example, transdiagnostic executive function subtypes have been identified with use of community detection algorithms, and children within the subtype characterized by inflexibility showed a failure to modulate parietal lobe activation in response to increasing executive task demands82. Other work examining ASD, ADHD and co-morbid ASD and ADHD using latent profile analysis also provides evidence for transdiagnostic executive function classes65. A study using magnetoencephalography found that during an intradimensional/extradimensional set-shift task, children with ASD exhibited greater parietal activity than children with ADHD, and both groups showed sustained parietal activation with an absence of sequential progression of brain activation from parietal to frontal regions83. Further work is needed to understand the brain activation patterns and dynamics underlying reduced or heightened flexibility in these neurodevelopmental disorders, as well as the paradoxical combinations (for example, inflexibility alongside distractibility) that can sometimes be observed. This work might focus on how dynamics within specific brain networks might support different domains of executive function. For example, intrinsic dynamics of the M-CIN (but not the L-FPN) have been shown to relate to individual differences in distractibility in neurotypical adults84.

Measurement issues complicate the assessment of flexibility deficits and their neural bases in ASD and ADHD, as different combinations of laboratory-based measures, neuropsychological tests and informant-report questionnaires have been used across studies72. It is important to note that well-documented inflexible everyday behaviours in ASD are not necessarily directly related to cognitive flexibility deficits assessed experimentally85. Standardized informant-report assessments specifically targeting flexible behaviours in autism have been developed, such as the Flexibility Scale, which reveals factors related to routines/rituals, transitions/change, special interests, social flexibility and generativity86. Still, these types of nuanced measure of flexibility are not yet routinely used in transdiagnostic assessment settings, leaving several open questions as to the specific profile of executive function and flexibility deficits that characterize neurodevelopmental disorders.

Flexibility in adolescence and midlife

Adolescence is a critical developmental period marked by dramatic physical, social and emotional changes that require cognitive flexibility for successful navigation. Adolescence also coincides with a period of vulnerability and risk of the onset of psychopathologies, including anxiety, depression, OCD and schizophrenia. Brain circuitry supporting cognitive control is still undergoing development during adolescence87, in part owing to differential development of limbic and executive control systems88. These asymmetries are evident in studies demonstrating that adolescents learn faster from negative reward prediction errors than adults, and recruit the right AI to a greater degree during probabilistic reversal learning89.

Signs of mood disorders, including anxiety and depression, can develop during the adolescent years. Pathological anxiety involves excessive worry or the tendency to dwell on difficulties and perceive future problems as more likely than they are in reality, whereas depression involves rumination or passively focusing on distressing thoughts in response to sad mood and experiences69. Worry and rumination may reflect the same underlying construct of repetitive negative thinking, which is likely a product of inflexible thinking and difficulty engaging the L-FPN executive control systems in the service of emotion regulation90.

Another adolescent-onset disorder characterized by severe flexibility impairments is OCD. Flexibility deficits in OCD manifest themselves as maladaptive patterns of recurrent and persistent thoughts, urges and impulses that are intrusive, as well as compulsions, including repetitive behaviours that an individual feels driven to perform69. Neuroimaging investigations across OCD and ASD provide evidence that increased functional connectivity within frontostriatal circuitry relates to more severe symptoms of repetitive behaviour91. In OCD, reduced activation of the OFC and frontostriatal regions during cognitive flexibility task performance is regularly reported92,93.

Schizophrenia is another condition emerging during late adolescence that is associated with reduced cognitive flexibility, often accompanied by frontal lobe hypometabolism94. Individuals with schizophrenia perform worse than individuals with OCD on the WCST, suggesting the involvement of different subsystems within basal–corticofrontal circuits in these two disorders95. Just as in the general population, frontostriatal circuitry appears to be linked with variability of cognitive flexibility performance in schizophrenia96.

Flexibility in neurological disorders

While executive function and flexibility deficits are observed in normal ageing, these issues can be further exacerbated in neurological disorders that affect later life. Older adults exhibit reduced efficiency of lateral prefrontal control regions, and compensate for age-related declines in task-switching performance by relying on enhanced frontotemporal connectivity compared with younger adults97. The default–executive coupling hypothesis of ageing proposes that declining performance on executive control tasks and reduced flexibility in older adulthood are underpinned by inflexible coupling of the M-FPN and lateral prefrontal regions98. A recent meta-analysis of fMRI studies of executive function in ageing reveals that the IFJ is recruited to a different degree in younger versus older adults. Furthermore, decreased functional connectivity between the IFJ and other executive function-related brain regions is observed with increasing age99. Whole-brain computational models permit quantification of metastability and recalibration processes underlying changes in cognitive performance over the lifespan. Such models can help clarify how dedifferentiation observed at the network level, such as that proposed by the default–executive coupling hypothesis of ageing, can be seen as compensation for the decline of structural integrity in the ageing brain100.

One of the signs of dementia is heightened executive function impairment compared with that from normal ageing, including a deterioration of mental flexibility and the onset of cognitive rigidity. A burgeoning functional neuroimaging literature including task-switching and set-shifting tasks adapted from neuropsychological assessments (most notably the WCST) investigates cognitive flexibility deficits in ageing and dementia, confirming the critical role of PFC recruitment in maintaining these functions101. Flexibility deficits observed in Parkinson disease may result from dysfunction of frontostriatal loops resulting from dopamine depletion102. Across neurological disorders, different aspects of cognitive flexibility may be impaired. For example, frontoparietal changes affecting set-shifting ability characterize patients with amyotrophic lateral sclerosis, whereas frontostriatal changes affecting rule inference are seen in primary dystonia and Parkinson disease103.

Dysexecuitve syndrome, which involves impairment of working memory, cognitive flexibility and inhibitory control, is seen in progressive dementia syndrome due to Alzheimer disease. This syndrome is accompanied by frontoparietal hypometabolism as demonstrated by positron emission tomography104. Taken together, the literature on flexibility in ageing and dementia points to frontoparietal and frontostriatal dysfunction, as might be predicted from the human and animal research.

While we focus on maladaptive outcomes associated with flexibility deficits here, flexibility reductions can also be associated with adaptive or healthy traits, and the level of flexibility required can fluctuate depending on the context. Therefore, alterations in flexibility might in some cases represent normative adaptations to the perceived characteristics of the environment. In Parkinson disease, cognitive impairments such as slowed thinking and cognitive inflexibility parallel motor impairments102, suggesting that reduced flexibility might be an appropriate reaction to a world that is experienced as more stationary. Cognitive stability — the opposite of cognitive flexibility — can likewise be beneficial during tasks requiring focused attention and distractor inhibition105. Thus, reduced flexibility may paradoxically be optimal under specific conditions.

Drugs and training of flexibility

Animal studies have revealed how specific neurotransmitter systems underlie flexible cognition and behaviour. In humans, cognitive training paradigms and physical activity have been touted as means to bolster flexibility, and there is some initial evidence from studies of development and ageing that bilingualism may confer greater flexibility. This section will summarize what is known regarding the pharmacology of cognitive and behavioural flexibility, then critically review the research on cognitive flexibility enhancement and training.

Pharmacology supporting flexibility

Serotonin and striatal dopamine neurotransmitter systems have a modulatory role in reversal learning, as evidenced by human and animal lesion, stimulation and neuroimaging studies106. In humans, transient cerebral serotonin depletion affects processing of negative feedback during reversal learning107. l-DOPA withdrawal studies demonstrate that patients with Parkinson disease not receiving medication show inflexibility in the form of increased switch costs when switching between tasks108. Methylphenidate, a psychostimulant influencing dopamine and noradrenaline activity, has long been used to treat ADHD and other developmental disorders109. There is some evidence from studies of rhesus monkeys given therapeutic doses of methylphenidate that the drug can impair task-switching performance. This indicates that the improved ability to focus attention may come at the expense of hindering flexibility110. Taken together, these findings suggest that serotonergic and dopaminergic signalling are critically involved in flexible cognition and behaviour.

The striatal cholinergic systems also appear to play a role in behavioural flexibility. Proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy studies in humans during reversal learning show that lower levels of choline in the dorsal striatum are associated with a lower number of perseverative trials111. Studies of the contributions of the cholinergic system to flexibility are complicated, however, by the fact that many cholinergic neurons co-release glutamate or GABA along with acetylcholine112.

Interventions to improve flexibility

Computerized cognitive training, physical activity and specialized curricula have been described as potential interventions to improve flexibility in children, yet the evidence supporting the efficacy of these interventions is mixed. Successful programmes involve repeated practice and progressive increases in challenge to executive functions, and children who are more impaired initially benefit the most from cognitive training and physical activity interventions2. Generally, training in a specific aspect of executive function can produce short-term narrow transfer, but does not generalize to other aspects of executive function. For example, working memory training can improve working memory performance, but not inhibitory processing or other skills113. Implementing a game-based flexibility training designed to increase motivation in children, one study found long-term transfer effects in untrained executive control tasks. The study authors also reported greater performance improvements in the game-based flexibility training group on reading comprehension, an effect that appeared only at the 6-week follow up. These findings suggest that the addition of game elements to executive control training tasks may result in enhanced complexity that facilitates transfer to academic abilities114.

Flexibility training in neurodevelopmental disorders has also produced mixed results. One computerized working memory and cognitive flexibility training designed for children with ASD did not result in differential improvement in a randomized controlled trial115. An executive function intervention known as Unstuck and On Target aims to address insistence on sameness, flexibility, goal setting and planning using a cognitive behavioural programme. This intervention has been shown to be effective for improving classroom behaviour, flexibility and problem-solving in children with ASD116.

Cognitive training has been used to combat age-related cognitive decline, and training-induced structural and functional brain changes in healthy older adults (60 years of age and older) have been demonstrated117. A task-switching study reported training-related improvements in task performance, but limited transfer to untrained similar flexibility tasks and no improvements for untrained domains of executive function after 1 year118.

Studies examining the effects of aerobic exercise or resistance training interventions without a cognitive component seem to suggest little or no executive function benefit, although exercise that is cognitively challenging, such as martial arts, can produce measurable benefits119. In adults of around 60 years of age and older, aerobic exercise interventions may contribute to salutary effects on cognition through prevention of volumetric decreases of hippocampal volume over time120. The small effects reported in studies of physical activity interventions on executive function stand in contrast to the fact that children with greater cardiovascular fitness perform better on executive function components, including information processing and control, visuospatial working memory and attention efficiency121. Likewise, individuals who are generally more physically active have better executive function than those who are more sedentary122.

Effects of bilingualism on flexibility

More than 50% of the global population is bilingual, or able to use two languages with equal fluency. The concept of a ‘bilingual advantage’ suggests that individuals fluent in two languages may develop cognitive advantages, particularly within the executive function domain. Evidence supporting the bilingual advantage identifies inhibition and monitoring as potential mechanisms conferring enhanced executive control in individuals with diverse language experiences123. This model suggests that both languages in a bilingual individual’s repertoire are always active to a degree, and there is a constant competition for selection. Lifelong experience of managing and resolving competition between languages imposes demands that require brain regions not typically used for language processing124. This bilingual experience reorganizes brain networks to create more effective mechanisms for executive control and results in cognitive benefits when non-linguistic processing draws on the same executive control networks125. As language switching involves the same frontal systems involved in executive control and inhibitory processes, it is thought that the bilingual experience results in general enhancement of these brain systems123,126.

Current research in bilingualism has produced mixed results, and there is no consensus regarding the relationship between bilingualism and cognitive advantages in the executive function domain. Some researchers report cognitive benefits in bilingual individuals127, while others fail to replicate these findings in typically developing children128 and adults129. However, the bilingual advantage has been observed in children of lower socio-economic status124,127. Likewise, in individuals experiencing age-related cognitive decline, a ‘cognitive reserve’ has been observed whereby the bilingual brain is more resistant to neurodegeneration and dementia123. The observation that bilingual experience helps offset age-related losses in executive processes has led to the proposal that bilingualism may act as a neuroprotective factor against dementia by buffering against the decline in cognitive control abilities typically observed in later life130,131. Thus, the bilingual advantage may manifest itself under specific circumstances, but further research is needed on this topic.

Summary and future directions

The global COVID-19 pandemic highlighted the critical need for optimal levels of flexibility at the level of institutions and individuals. Neuroscience research has probed flexibility using paradigms that are capable of spanning both human and animal investigations. This research has demonstrated that cognitive and behavioural flexibility involve executive control processes that rely on the coordinated functioning among several large-scale frontoparietal and frontostriatal brain networks enacting salience detection, attention, inhibition, working memory and switching processes20. Understanding the typical development of these networks, their stabilization in adulthood and their potential for breakdown with ageing is the first step towards pinpointing effective strategies for addressing flexibility deficits in psychiatric and neurological disorders and enhancing flexibility across the lifespan. For example, the identification of unique brain profiles supporting various degrees of flexibility across clinical and neurotypical populations could aid in identifying interventions with the highest probability of success for a particular individual. Capturing mechanistic insights with the aid of neuroimaging will help to improve our current diagnostic nosology and move us towards achieving the goals of precision medicine.

Future directions include addressing issues of measurement to maximize ecological and construct validity in research on flexibility. It is important to acknowledge that highly reliable self-report or informant-report measures may better predict individual differences in real-life outcomes, whereas laboratory performance-based measures that are sensitive to within-person experimental manipulations can reveal processes underlying task performance21. Standardized, transdiagnostic assessments that are normed for targeted age ranges must be developed and universally adopted to permit characterization of common and unique aspects of flexibility that are affected across clinical conditions. Several self-report scales have been developed for use in adults, including the Cognitive Control and Flexibility Questionnaire132, the Cognitive Flexibility Scale133 and the Psychological Flexibility Questionnaire134. Consistent use of questionnaires in future studies will provide a clearer picture of the clinical profile of flexibility deficits. Generally however, self-report/informant-report and behavioural measures are only weakly correlated, as behavioural measures index responses during structured situations, whereas self-report/informant-report queries how individuals behave in real-life situations21. Going forward, the challenge of how to bridge laboratory-based, objective behavioural measures of flexibility with real-world indices of flexible behaviour must be tackled. Recent approaches focus on measurement of ‘hot’ or emotionally salient flexibility135,136, and have also turned towards implicit rather than explicit flexibility performance measures33 as possible bridges between real-world and laboratory performance.

Translational neuroscience research adopting the RDoC framework will likely continue to build on findings that interactions among the M-CIN, L-FPN and M-FPN are implicated as common neurobiological substrates for mental illness28,137. The emerging field of computational psychiatry that strives to use data-driven approaches and machine-learning to improve disease classification and predict treatment outcomes138 will benefit by focusing on transdiagnostic constructs such as flexibility, with clear links to real-life outcomes. The success of these clinically oriented endeavours, however, hinges on progress in neuroinformatics efforts to construct biologically informed taxonomies of psychological processes139.

At present, there have been no interventional studies demonstrating the role of changing brain network dynamics in supporting successful training of flexibility. Following similar work providing evidence for dynamic reconfiguration of brain networks with working memory training140, future research should focus efforts towards delineating how effective cognitive and behavioural flexibility training alters brain dynamics. Studies of cognitive training generally provide limited support for far transfer of skills. Similarly, while the cumulative effects of exercise are clearly beneficial for the brain and cognition, more research is needed to determine the type and dosage of physical activity intervention that is most suited to enhance executive function and flexibility. If bilingualism can confer a flexibility advantage in some instances, then encouraging bilingualism might be a ‘natural intervention’ strategy to improve flexibility. The bolstering of flexibility that may be conferred by bilingualism provides an added incentive to promote the learning of multiple languages from a young age. The next frontier of flexibility research will likely involve collaborations among clinical psychologists, medical professionals, neuroscientists, engineers, computer scientists and educators.

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by the US National Institute of Mental Health (R01MH107549), the Canadian Institute for Advanced Research and a Gabelli Senior Scholar award from the University of Miami to L.Q.U.

Glossary

Latent variable analysis

A statistical approach for identifying clusters based on a series of continuous variables or indicators. This type of analysis assumes that there are unobserved latent profiles that generate responses on indicator items.

Mental set-shifting

Also referred to as ‘shifting’, this refers to the ability to switch back and forth between multiple tasks.

Prepotent responses

Automatic behavioural responses with which immediate reinforcement is associated. Executive functions are necessary for overriding prepotent responses.

Diagnostic nosology

A system for classification of diseases.

RDoC matrix

The research domain criteria (RDoC) matrix is a tool to help implement the principles of RDoC in research studies.

Construct validity

In psychology, the idea that a test is valid if it measures what it claims to measure or is designed to measure.

Divergent thinking

In the study of creativity, the type of thinking used in an open-ended task, such as coming up with multiple uses for a given object.

Ecological validity

In psychology, the idea that something measured with a laboratory test translates to performance in real-life settings.

Cognitive constructs

In psychology, cognitive constructs are terms used to described mental processes. Examples of cognitive constructs include ‘attention‘, ‘memory’ and ‘perception’.

Extradimensional shifts

In set-shifting tasks, an extradimensional shift is one in which the important aspect of a stimulus switches from one category to another (for example, in a discrimination task, when colour is no longer an informative aspect of the stimulus, and shape becomes the discriminating characteristic).

Hidden Markov models

Statistical models in which the system being modelled is assumed to be a Markov process (where the probability of each event depends on the state in the previous event) with unobservable or hidden states.

Metastability

A state of a dynamical system other than the state of least energy. In a non-linear system such as the brain, ‘metastability’ refers to a state in which signals fall outside their natural equilibrium state, but persist for an extended period of time.

Transfer effects

Phenomena in which training or learning in one context applies to another.

Competing interests

The author declares no competing interests.

Footnotes

Peer review information

Nature Reviews Neuroscience thanks L. Kenworthy, R. Cools and the other, anonymous, reviewer for their contribution to the peer review of this work.

Publisher’s note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Related links

MRIcron: https://www.nitrc.org/projects/mricron

Neurosynth: https://neurosynth.org/

University of Miami Brain Connectivity and Cognition Laboratory: https://bccl.psy.miami.edu/

Twitter: @LucinaUddin

References

  • 1.Moradian N, et al. The urgent need for integrated science to fight COVID-19 pandemic and beyond. J. Transl Med. 2020;18:205. doi: 10.1186/s12967-020-02364-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Diamond A, Lee K. Interventions shown to aid executive function development in children 4 to 12 years old. Science. 2011;333:959–964. doi: 10.1126/science.1204529. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Burt KB, Paysnick AA. Resilience in the transition to adulthood. Dev. Psychopathol. 2012;24:493–505. doi: 10.1017/S0954579412000119. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Burke SN, et al. What are the later life contributions to reserve, resilience, and compensation? Neurobiol. Aging. 2019;83:140–144. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2019.03.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Scott WA. Cognitive complexity and cognitive flexibility. Sociometry. 1962;25:405–414. doi: 10.2307/2785779. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Brown VJ, Tait DS. Behavioral flexibility: attentional shifting, rule switching, and response reversal. Encycl. Psychopharmacol. 2014 doi: 10.1007/978-3-642-27772-6_340-2. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Miyake A, et al. The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex ‘frontal lobe’ tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cogn. Psychol. 2000;41:49–100. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1999.0734. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Poldrack RA, et al. The cognitive atlas: toward a knowledge foundation for cognitive neuroscience. Front. Neuroinform. 2011;5:17. doi: 10.3389/fninf.2011.00017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Teuber H-L. Unity and diversity of frontal lobe functions. Acta Neurobiol. Exp. 1972;32:615–656. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Badre D. Opening the gate to working memory. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2012;109:19878–19879. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1216902109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Chatham CH, Badre D. Multiple gates on working memory. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 2015;1:23–31. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2014.08.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Ott T, Nieder A. Dopamine and cognitive control in prefrontal cortex. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2019;23:213–234. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2018.12.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Banich MT. Executive function: the search for an integrated account. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2009;18:89–94. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01615.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Butter CM. Perseveration in extinction and in discrimination reversal tasks following selective frontal ablations in Macaca mulatta. Physiol. Behav. 1969;4:163–171. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(69)90075-4. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Izquierdo A, Brigman JL, Radke AK, Rudebeck PH, Holmes A. The neural basis of reversal learning: an updated perspective. Neuroscience. 2017;345:12–26. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2016.03.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Insel TR. The NIMH research domain criteria (RDoC) project: precision medicine for psychiatry. Am. J. Psychiatry. 2014;171:395–397. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2014.14020138. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Saggar M, Uddin LQ. Pushing the boundaries of psychiatric neuroimaging to ground diagnosis in biology. eNeuro. 2019 doi: 10.1523/ENEURO.0384-19.2019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.National Institute of Mental Health. National Advisory Mental Health Council Workgroup on Tasks and Measures for Research Domain Criteria. Behavioral assessment methods for RDoC constructs (NIH, 2016).
  • 19.Cepeda NJ, Kramer AF, Gonzalez de Sather JCM. Changes in executive control across the life span: examination of task-switching performance. Dev. Psychol. 2001;37:715–730. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.37.5.715. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Dajani DR, Uddin LQ. Demystifying cognitive flexibility: Implications for clinical and developmental neuroscience. Trends Neurosci. 2015;38:571–578. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2015.07.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Dang J, King KM, Inzlicht M. Why are self-report and behavioral measures weakly correlated? Trends Cogn. Sci. 2020;24:267–269. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2020.01.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Isquith, P. K., Roth, R. M., Gioia, G. A. & Par, S. Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function–Adult Version (BRIEF-A) Interpretive Report (Psychological Assessment Resources, 2006).
  • 23.Gioia GA, Isquith PK, Guy SC, Kenworthy L. Behavior rating inventory of executive function. Child. Neuropsychol. 2000;6:235–238. doi: 10.1076/chin.6.3.235.3152. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Zelazo PD. The Dimensional Change Card Sort (DCCS): a method of assessing executive function in children. Nat. Protoc. 2006;1:297–301. doi: 10.1038/nprot.2006.46. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Delis, D. C., Kaplan, E. & Kramer, J. H. Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System (American Psychological Association, 2001).
  • 26.Brooks BL, Sherman EMS, Strauss E. NEPSY-II: a developmental neuropsychological assessment, second edition. Child. Neuropsychol. 2009;16:80–101. doi: 10.1080/09297040903146966. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Cambridge Cognition. Intra-Extra Dimensional Set Shift (IED). https://www.cambridgecognition.com/cantab/cognitive-tests/executive-function/intra-extra-dimensional-set-shift-ied/ (2021).
  • 28.Uddin LQ. Salience processing and insular cortical function and dysfunction. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2015;16:55–61. doi: 10.1038/nrn3857. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Yarkoni T, Poldrack RA, Nichols TE, Van Essen DC, Wager TD. Large-scale automated synthesis of human functional neuroimaging data. Nat. Methods. 2011;8:665–670. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.1635. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Seeley WW, et al. Dissociable intrinsic connectivity networks for salience processing and executive control. J. Neurosci. 2007;27:2349–2356. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5587-06.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Uddin LQ, Thomas Yeo BT, Nathan Spreng R. Towards a universal taxonomy of macro-scale functional human brain networks. Brain Topogr. 2019;32:926–942. doi: 10.1007/s10548-019-00744-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Derrfuss J, Brass M, Neumann J, von Cramon DY. Involvement of the inferior frontal junction in cognitive control: meta-analyses of switching and Stroop studies. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2005;25:22–34. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20127. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Dajani DR, et al. Measuring cognitive flexibility with the flexible item selection task: from MRI adaptation to individual connectome mapping. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2020;32:1026–1045. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_01536. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Kim C, Johnson NF, Cilles SE, Gold BT. Common and distinct mechanisms of cognitive flexibility in prefrontal cortex. J. Neurosci. 2011;31:4771–4779. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5923-10.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Sundermann B, Pfleiderer B. Functional connectivity profile of the human inferior frontal junction: involvement in a cognitive control network. BMC Neurosci. 2012;13:119. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-13-119. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Robbins TW. Shifting and stopping: fronto-striatal substrates, neurochemical modulation and clinical implications. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 2007;362:917–932. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2007.2097. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Banerjee A, et al. Value-guided remapping of sensory cortex by lateral orbitofrontal cortex. Nature. 2020;585:245–250. doi: 10.1038/s41586-020-2704-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Ragozzino ME. The contribution of the medial prefrontal cortex, orbitofrontal cortex, and dorsomedial striatum to behavioral flexibility. Ann. NY Acad. Sci. 2007;1121:355–375. doi: 10.1196/annals.1401.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Hampshire A, Owen AM. Fractionating attentional control using event-related fMRI. Cereb. Cortex. 2006;16:1679–1689. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhj116. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Ghahremani, D. G., Monterosso, J., Jentsch, J. D., Bilder, R. M. & Poldrack, R. A. Neural components underlying behavioral flexibility in human reversal learning. Cereb. Cortex20, 1843–1852. This functional neuroimaging study reveals how human reversal learning and guidance of actions consistent with current reward contingencies engages the lateral OFC, dorsal anterior cingulate cortex and right inferior frontal cortex. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 41.Chang C, Glover GH. Time–frequency dynamics of resting-state brain connectivity measured with fMRI. Neuroimage. 2010;50:81–98. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Hutchison RM, et al. Dynamic functional connectivity: promise, issues, and interpretations. Neuroimage. 2013;80:360–378. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.079. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Calhoun VD, Miller R, Pearlson G, Adalı T. The chronnectome: time-varying connectivity networks as the next frontier in fMRI data discovery. Neuron. 2014;84:262–274. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.10.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Nomi JS, et al. Chronnectomic patterns and neural flexibility underlie executive function. Neuroimage. 2017;147:861–871. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.10.026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Chen T, Cai W, Ryali S, Supekar K, Menon V. Distinct global brain dynamics and spatiotemporal organization of the salience network. PLoS Biol. 2016;14:e1002469. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1002469. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Douw L, Wakeman DG, Tanaka N, Liu H, Stufflebeam SM. State-dependent variability of dynamic functional connectivity between frontoparietal and default networks relates to cognitive flexibility. Neuroscience. 2016;339:12–21. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2016.09.034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Vidaurre D, Smith SM, Woolrich MW. Brain network dynamics are hierarchically organized in time. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2017;114:12827–12832. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1705120114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Medaglia JD, et al. Functional alignment with anatomical networks is associated with cognitive flexibility. Nat. Hum. Behav. 2018;2:156–164. doi: 10.1038/s41562-017-0260-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Cohen JR. The behavioral and cognitive relevance of time-varying, dynamic changes in functional connectivity. Neuroimage. 2018;180:515–525. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.09.036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Yin W, et al. The emergence of a functionally flexible brain during early infancy. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2020;117:23904–23913. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2002645117. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Cabral J, et al. Cognitive performance in healthy older adults relates to spontaneous switching between states of functional connectivity during rest. Sci. Rep. 2017;7:5135. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-05425-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Ezaki T, Sakaki M, Watanabe T, Masuda N. Age-related changes in the ease of dynamical transitions in human brain activity. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2018;39:2673–2688. doi: 10.1002/hbm.24033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Yin D, et al. Dissociable changes of frontal and parietal cortices in inherent functional flexibility across the human life span. J. Neurosci. 2016;36:10060–10074. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1476-16.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Allegra M, et al. Brain network dynamics during spontaneous strategy shifts and incremental task optimization. NeuroImage. 2020;217:116854. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116854. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Saggar M, et al. Towards a new approach to reveal dynamical organization of the brain using topological data analysis. Nat. Commun. 2018;9:1399. doi: 10.1038/s41467-018-03664-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Uddin LQ. Bring the noise: reconceptualizing spontaneous neural activity. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2020 doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2020.06.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.McIntosh AR, Kovacevic N, Itier RJ. Increased brain signal variability accompanies lower behavioral variability in development. PLoS Comput. Biol. 2008;4:e1000106. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Garrett DD, Kovacevic N, McIntosh AR, Grady CL. The modulation of BOLD variability between cognitive states varies by age and processing speed. Cereb. Cortex. 2013;23:684–693. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs055. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Garrett DD, Kovacevic N, McIntosh AR, Grady CL. Blood oxygen level-dependent signal variability is more than just noise. J. Neurosci. 2010;30:4914–4921. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5166-09.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Nomi JS, Bolt TS, Ezie C, Uddin LQ, Heller AS. Moment-to-moment BOLD Signal variability reflects regional changes in neural flexibility across the lifespan. J. Neurosci. 2017 doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3408-16.2017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Armbruster-Genç DJN, Ueltzhöffer K, Fiebach CJ. Brain signal variability differentially affects cognitive flexibility and cognitive stability. J. Neurosci. 2016;36:3978–3987. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2517-14.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Garrett DD, Epp SM, Kleemeyer M, Lindenberger U, Polk TA. Higher performers upregulate brain signal variability in response to more feature-rich visual input. Neuroimage. 2020;217:116836. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116836. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Snyder HR, Miyake A, Hankin BL. Advancing understanding of executive function impairments and psychopathology: bridging the gap between clinical and cognitive approaches. Front. Psychol. 2015;6:328. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00328. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Lai CLE, et al. Meta-analysis of neuropsychological measures of executive functioning in children and adolescents with high-functioning autism spectrum disorder. Autism Res. 2017;10:911–939. doi: 10.1002/aur.1723. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Dajani DR, Llabre MM, Nebel MB, Mostofsky SH, Uddin LQ. Heterogeneity of executive functions among comorbid neurodevelopmental disorders. Sci. Rep. 2016;6:36566. doi: 10.1038/srep36566. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Pennington BF, Ozonoff S. Executive functions and developmental psychopathology. J. Child. Psychol. Psychiatry. 1996;37:51–87. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1996.tb01380.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Demetriou EA, et al. Autism spectrum disorders: a meta-analysis of executive function. Mol. Psychiatry. 2018;23:1198–1204. doi: 10.1038/mp.2017.75. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Landry O, Al-Taie S. A meta-analysis of the Wisconsin Card Sort Task in autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2016;46:1220–1235. doi: 10.1007/s10803-015-2659-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5®). (American Psychiatric Association Publishing, 2013).
  • 70.Lopez BR, Lincoln AJ, Ozonoff S, Lai Z. Examining the relationship between executive functions and restricted, repetitive symptoms of autistic disorder. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2005;35:445–460. doi: 10.1007/s10803-005-5035-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Wilkes BJ, Lewis MH. The neural circuitry of restricted repetitive behavior: Magnetic resonance imaging in neurodevelopmental disorders and animal models. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2018;92:152–171. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.05.022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Uddin LQ. Brain mechanisms supporting flexible cognition and behavior in adolescents with autism spectrum disorder. Biol. Psychiatry. 2021;89:172–183. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2020.05.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Kuntsi J, Klein C. Intraindividual variability in ADHD and its implications for research of causal links. Curr. Top. Behav. Neurosci. 2012;9:67–91. doi: 10.1007/7854_2011_145. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Leitner Y. The co-occurrence of autism and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in children - what do we know? Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2014;8:268. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00268. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Bloemen AJP, et al. The association between executive functioning and psychopathology: general or specific? Psychol. Med. 2018;48:1787–1794. doi: 10.1017/S0033291717003269. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Sergeant JA, Geurts H, Oosterlaan J. How specific is a deficit of executive functioning for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder? Behav. Brain Res. 2002;130:3–28. doi: 10.1016/S0166-4328(01)00430-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Happe F, Booth R, Charlton R, Hughes C. Executive function deficits in autism spectrum disorders and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: examining profiles across domains and ages. Brain Cogn. 2006;61:25–39. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2006.03.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Baez AC, et al. Parsing heterogeneity of executive function in typically and atypically developing children: a conceptual replication and exploration of social function. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2019 doi: 10.1007/s10803-019-04290-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Di Martino A, et al. Shared and distinct intrinsic functional network centrality in autism and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol. Psychiatry. 2013;74:623–632. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.02.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Dajani DR, et al. Investigating functional brain network integrity using a traditional and novel categorical scheme for neurodevelopmental disorders. Neuroimage Clin. 2019;21:101678. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2019.101678. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Cordova M, et al. Heterogeneity of executive function revealed by a functional random forest approach across ADHD and ASD. Neuroimage Clin. 2020;26:102245. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2020.102245. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Vaidya CJ, et al. Data-driven identification of subtypes of executive function across typical development, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, and autism spectrum disorders. J. Child. Psychol. Psychiatry. 2020;61:51–61. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.13114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Mogadam A, et al. Magnetoencephalographic (MEG) brain activity during a mental flexibility task suggests some shared neurobiology in children with neurodevelopmental disorders. J. Neurodev. Disord. 2019;11:19. doi: 10.1186/s11689-019-9280-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Steimke R, et al. Salience network dynamics underlying successful resistance of temptation. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2017;12:1928–1939. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsx123. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Geurts HM, Corbett B, Solomon M. The paradox of cognitive flexibility in autism. Trends Cognit. Sci. 2009;13:74–82. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.11.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Strang JF, et al. The Flexibility Scale: development and preliminary validation of a cognitive flexibility measure in children with autism spectrum disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2017;47:2502–2518. doi: 10.1007/s10803-017-3152-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Luna B, Paulsen DJ, Padmanabhan A, Geier C. The teenage brain: cognitive control and motivation. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2013;22:94–100. doi: 10.1177/0963721413478416. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Casey BJ, Jones R, Hare T. The adolescent brain. The year in cognitive neuroscience. Ann. NY Acad. Sci. 2008;11:84–94. [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Hauser TU, Iannaccone R, Walitza S, Brandeis D, Brem S. Cognitive flexibility in adolescence: neural and behavioral mechanisms of reward prediction error processing in adaptive decision making during development. Neuroimage. 2015;104:347–354. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.09.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Burrows CA, Timpano KR, Uddin LQ. Putative brain networks underlying repetitive negative thinking and comorbid internalizing problems in autism. Clin. Psychol. Sci. 2017;5:522–536. doi: 10.1177/2167702616683506. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Akkermans SEA, et al. Frontostriatal functional connectivity correlates with repetitive behaviour across autism spectrum disorder and obsessive–compulsive disorder. Psychol. Med. 2019;49:2247–2255. doi: 10.1017/S0033291718003136. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Gu B-M, et al. Neural correlates of cognitive inflexibility during task-switching in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Brain. 2008;131:155–164. doi: 10.1093/brain/awm277. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Gruner P, Pittenger C. Cognitive inflexibility in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Neuroscience. 2017;345:243–255. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2016.07.030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Weinberger DR. Schizophrenia and the frontal lobe. Trends Neurosci. 1988;11:367–370. doi: 10.1016/0166-2236(88)90060-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Cavallaro R, et al. Basal-corticofrontal circuits in schizophrenia and obsessive-compulsive disorder: a controlled, double dissociation study. Biol. Psychiatry. 2003;54:437–443. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01814-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 96.Waltz JA. The neural underpinnings of cognitive flexibility and their disruption in psychotic illness. Neuroscience. 2017;345:203–217. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2016.06.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 97.Hakun JG, Zhu Z, Johnson NF, Gold BT. Evidence for reduced efficiency and successful compensation in older adults during task switching. Cortex. 2015;64:352–362. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2014.12.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 98.Spreng RN, Turner GR. The shifting architecture of cognition and brain function in older adulthood. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2019;14:523–542. doi: 10.1177/1745691619827511. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 99.Heckner, M. K. et al. The aging brain and executive functions revisited: implications from meta-analytic and functional connectivity evidence. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 1–36 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 100.Naik S, Banerjee A, Bapi RS, Deco G, Roy D. Metastability in senescence. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2017;21:509–521. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2017.04.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 101.McDonald AP, D’Arcy RCN, Song X. Functional MRI on executive functioning in aging and dementia: a scoping review of cognitive tasks. Aging Med. 2018;1:209–219. doi: 10.1002/agm2.12037. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 102.Robbins TW, Cools R. Cognitive deficits in Parkinson’s disease: a cognitive neuroscience perspective. Mov. Disord. 2014;29:597–607. doi: 10.1002/mds.25853. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 103.Lange F, Seer C, Kopp B. Cognitive flexibility in neurological disorders: cognitive components and event-related potentials. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2017;83:496–507. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.09.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 104.Townley RA, et al. Progressive dysexecutive syndrome due to Alzheimer’s disease: a description of 55 cases and comparison to other phenotypes. Brain Commun. 2020 doi: 10.1093/braincomms/fcaa068. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 105.Ueltzhöffer K, Armbruster-Genç DJN, Fiebach CJ. Stochastic dynamics underlying cognitive stability and flexibility. PLoS Comput. Biol. 2015;11:e1004331. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004331. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 106.Clark L, Cools R, Robbins TW. The neuropsychology of ventral prefrontal cortex: decision-making and reversal learning. Brain Cognition. 2004;55:41–53. doi: 10.1016/S0278-2626(03)00284-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 107.Evers EAT, et al. Serotonergic modulation of prefrontal cortex during negative feedback in probabilistic reversal learning. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2005;30:1138–1147. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300663. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 108.Cools R, Barker RA, Sahakian BJ, Robbins TW. L-Dopa medication remediates cognitive inflexibility, but increases impulsivity in patients with Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychologia. 2003;41:1431–1441. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(03)00117-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 109.Martino AD, Di Martino A, Melis G, Cianchetti C, Zuddas A. Methylphenidate for pervasive developmental disorders: safety and efficacy of acute single dose test and ongoing therapy: an open-pilot study. J. Child. Adolesc. Psychopharmacol. 2004;14:207–218. doi: 10.1089/1044546041649011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 110.Rajala AZ, Populin LC, Jenison RL. Methylphenidate affects task-switching and neural signaling in non-human primates. Psychopharmacology. 2020;237:1533–1543. doi: 10.1007/s00213-020-05478-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 111.Bell T, Lindner M, Langdon A, Mullins PG, Christakou A. Regional striatal cholinergic involvement in human behavioral flexibility. J. Neurosci. 2019;39:5740–5749. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2110-18.2019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 112.Prado VF, Janickova H, Al-Onaizi MA, Prado MAM. Cholinergic circuits in cognitive flexibility. Neuroscience. 2017;345:130–141. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2016.09.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 113.Melby-Lervåg M, Hulme C. Is working memory training effective? A meta-analytic review. Dev. Psychol. 2013;49:270–291. doi: 10.1037/a0028228. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 114.Johann VE, Karbach J. Effects of game-based and standard executive control training on cognitive and academic abilities in elementary school children. Dev. Sci. 2020;23:197. doi: 10.1111/desc.12866. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 115.Vries M, de, de Vries M, Prins PJM, Schmand BA, Geurts HM. Working memory and cognitive flexibility-training for children with an autism spectrum disorder: a randomized controlled trial. J. Child. Psychol. Psychiatry. 2015;56:566–576. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12324. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 116.Kenworthy L, et al. Randomized controlled effectiveness trial of executive function intervention for children on the autism spectrum. J. Child. Psychol. Psychiatry. 2014;55:374–383. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12161. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 117.Nguyen L, Murphy K, Andrews G. Cognitive and neural plasticity in old age: a systematic review of evidence from executive functions cognitive training. Ageing Res. Rev. 2019;53:100912. doi: 10.1016/j.arr.2019.100912. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 118.Gaál ZA, Czigler I. Task-switching training and transfer. J. Psychophysiol. 2018;32:106–130. doi: 10.1027/0269-8803/a000189. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 119.Diamond A, Ling DS. Conclusions about interventions, programs, and approaches for improving executive functions that appear justified and those that, despite much hype, do not. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2016;18:34–48. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2015.11.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 120.Firth J, et al. Effect of aerobic exercise on hippocampal volume in humans: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Neuroimage. 2018;166:230–238. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.11.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 121.Meijer A, et al. Cardiovascular fitness and executive functioning in primary school-aged children. Dev. Sci. 2020 doi: 10.1111/desc.13019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 122.Stillman CM, Esteban-Cornejo I, Brown B, Bender CM, Erickson KI. Effects of exercise on brain and cognition across age groups and health states. Trends Neurosci. 2020;43:533–543. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2020.04.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 123.Bialystok E, Craik FIM, Luk G. Bilingualism: consequences for mind and brain. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2012;16:240–250. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.03.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 124.Carlson SM, Meltzoff AN. Bilingual experience and executive functioning in young children. Dev. Sci. 2008;11:282–298. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00675.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 125.Rodríguez-Pujadas A, et al. Bilinguals use language-control brain areas more than monolinguals to perform non-linguistic switching tasks. PLoS ONE. 2013;8:e73028. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0073028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 126.Buchweitz A, Prat C. The bilingual brain: flexibility and control in the human cortex. Phys. Life Rev. 2013;10:428–443. doi: 10.1016/j.plrev.2013.07.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 127.Hartanto A, Toh WX, Yang H. Bilingualism narrows socioeconomic disparities in executive functions and self-regulatory behaviors during early childhood: evidence from the early childhood longitudinal study. Child. Dev. 2019;90:1215–1235. doi: 10.1111/cdev.13032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 128.Dick AS, et al. No evidence for a bilingual executive function advantage in the ABCD study. Nat. Hum. Behav. 2019;3:692–701. doi: 10.1038/s41562-019-0609-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 129.Nichols ES, Wild CJ, Stojanoski B, Battista ME, Owen AM. Bilingualism affords no general cognitive advantages: a population study of executive function in 11,000 people. Psychol. Sci. 2020;31:548–567. doi: 10.1177/0956797620903113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 130.Dash T, Berroir P, Joanette Y, Ansaldo AI. Alerting, orienting, and executive control: the effect of bilingualism and age on the subcomponents of attention. Front. Neurol. 2019;10:1122. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2019.01122. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 131.Costumero V, et al. A cross-sectional and longitudinal study on the protective effect of bilingualism against dementia using brain atrophy and cognitive measures. Alzheimers. Res. Ther. 2020;12:11. doi: 10.1186/s13195-020-0581-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 132.Gabrys RL, Tabri N, Anisman H, Matheson K. Cognitive control and flexibility in the context of stress and depressive symptoms: the cognitive control and flexibility questionnaire. Front. Psychol. 2018;9:2219. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02219. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 133.Martin MM, Rubin RB. A new measure of cognitive flexibility. Psychol. Rep. 1995;76:623–626. doi: 10.2466/pr0.1995.76.2.623. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 134.Ben-Itzhak S, Bluvstein I, Maor M. The Psychological Flexibility Questionnaire (PFQ): development, reliability and validity. WebmedCentral Psychol. 2014 doi: 10.9754/journal.wmc.2014.004606. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 135.Kenworthy L, et al. Preliminary psychometrics for the executive function challenge task: a novel, ‘hot’ flexibility, and planning task for youth. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2020;26:725–732. doi: 10.1017/S135561772000017X. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 136.Zelazo P. D., C. W. A. Executive function: mechanisms underlying emotion regulation. in Handbook of Emotion Regulation (ed. Gross, J. J.) 135–158 (Guilford, 2007).
  • 137.Goodkind M, et al. Identification of a common neurobiological substrate for mental illness. JAMA Psychiatry. 2015;72:305–315. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2014.2206. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 138.Huys QJM, Maia TV, Frank MJ. Computational psychiatry as a bridge from neuroscience to clinical applications. Nat. Neurosci. 2016;19:404–413. doi: 10.1038/nn.4238. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 139.Bolt T, Nomi JS, Yeo BTT, Uddin LQ. Data-driven extraction of a nested model of human brain function. J. Neurosci. 2017;37:7263–7277. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0323-17.2017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 140.Finc K, et al. Dynamic reconfiguration of functional brain networks during working memory training. Nat. Commun. 2020;11:2435. doi: 10.1038/s41467-020-15631-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 141.Allen EA, et al. Tracking whole-brain connectivity dynamics in the resting state. Cereb. Cortex. 2014;24:663–676. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs352. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 142.Berg EA. A simple objective technique for measuring flexibility in thinking. J. Gen. Psychol. 1948;39:15–22. doi: 10.1080/00221309.1948.9918159. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 143.Grant DA, Berg EA. Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. PsycTESTS Dataset. 2014 doi: 10.1037/t31298-000. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 144.Bartolo R, Averbeck BB. Prefrontal cortex predicts state switches during reversal learning. Neuron. 2020;106:1044–1054. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2020.03.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 145.Kenett YN, et al. Developing a neurally informed ontology of creativity measurement. Neuroimage. 2020;221:117166. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117166. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 146.Wu X, et al. A meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies on divergent thinking using activation likelihood estimation. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2015;36:2703–2718. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22801. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 147.Sunavsky A, Poppenk J. Neuroimaging predictors of creativity in healthy adults. Neuroimage. 2020;206:116292. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116292. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 148.Becker M, Sommer T, Kühn S. Inferior frontal gyrus involvement during search and solution in verbal creative problem solving: a parametric fMRI study. Neuroimage. 2020;206:116294. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116294. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Nature Reviews. Neuroscience are provided here courtesy of Nature Publishing Group

RESOURCES