Skip to main content
Scientific Reports logoLink to Scientific Reports
. 2021 Feb 4;11:3148. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-82342-w

Elevated preoptic brain activity in zebrafish glial glycine transporter mutants is linked to lethargy-like behaviors and delayed emergence from anesthesia

Michael J Venincasa 1, Owen Randlett 2,10, Sureni H Sumathipala 1, Richard Bindernagel 1, Matthew J Stark 1, Qing Yan 1, Steven A Sloan 1,11, Elena Buglo 1,3,4, Qing Cheng Meng 5, Florian Engert 2, Stephan Züchner 3,4, Max B Kelz 5,6,7,8, Sheyum Syed 9, Julia E Dallman 1,
PMCID: PMC7862283  PMID: 33542258

Abstract

Delayed emergence from anesthesia was previously reported in a case study of a child with Glycine Encephalopathy. To investigate the neural basis of this delayed emergence, we developed a zebrafish glial glycine transporter (glyt1 − / −) mutant model. We compared locomotor behaviors; dose–response curves for tricaine, ketamine, and 2,6-diisopropylphenol (propofol); time to emergence from these anesthetics; and time to emergence from propofol after craniotomy in glyt1−/− mutants and their siblings. To identify differentially active brain regions in glyt1−/− mutants, we used pERK immunohistochemistry as a proxy for brain-wide neuronal activity. We show that glyt1−/− mutants initiated normal bouts of movement less frequently indicating lethargy-like behaviors. Despite similar anesthesia dose–response curves, glyt1−/− mutants took over twice as long as their siblings to emerge from ketamine or propofol, mimicking findings from the human case study. Reducing glycine levels rescued timely emergence in glyt1−/− mutants, pointing to a causal role for elevated glycine. Brain-wide pERK staining showed elevated activity in hypnotic brain regions in glyt1−/− mutants under baseline conditions and a delay in sensorimotor integration during emergence from anesthesia. Our study links elevated activity in preoptic brain regions and reduced sensorimotor integration to lethargy-like behaviors and delayed emergence from propofol in glyt1−/− mutants.

Subject terms: Neurological disorders, Genetics, Neuroscience

Introduction

Delayed emergence from anesthesia was reported in a case study of a girl with Glycine Encephalopathy (also known as non-ketotic hyperglycemia, NKH)1. GE is an inherited condition that causes elevated glycine28. In newborns, this elevated glycine inhibits motor circuits, suppressing both respiration and motor tone9. For those who survive this post-natal period, neural circuits adapt to high glycine. A few months after birth, infants have improved motor tone and breathe without a ventilator but continue to struggle with seizures, increased lethargy, and delays in reaching developmental milestones9. In this case study, a girl with GE was anesthetized for a surgical procedure, but after surgery, exhibited unexpected, delayed emergence from anesthesia1. Here we model delayed emergence from anesthesia in glyt1−/− mutant zebrafish larvae and identify differentially active brain regions in glyt1−/− mutants and their siblings.

Emergence from anesthesia is known to be promoted by conserved vertebrate brain arousal pathways including hypocretin, dopamine, acetylcholine, and noradrenaline1016. These pathways have been best studied in rodents, where glycine has been shown to regulate hypocretin-releasing neurons and cholinergic neurons in the basal forebrain1719. Both hypocretin and basal forebrain cholinergic neurons express glycine receptors17,19 and they are also both innervated by ascending glycinergic brainstem neurons17,19. Moreover, electrophysiological recordings show that application of glycine inhibits action potentials in hypocretin neurons18 and that glycinergic inhibitory post-synaptic potentials are present in cholinergic neurons of the basal forebrain19. These data support the idea that high glycine in glyt1−/− mutants could inhibit arousal pathways and thereby delay emergence from anesthesia.

Our zebrafish model of GE harbors a homozygous mutation in the glial glycine transporter gene (glyt1te301;20,21) and is phenocopied by raising zebrafish in a Glyt1 blocker (N[3-(4′-fluorophenyl)-3-(4′-phenylphenoxy)-propyl]sarcosine) indicating that this point mutation limits transporter function20. In humans, mutations in GLYT1 have recently been shown to cause a form of GE7,8. In this form of GE, glycine is mildly elevated in the nervous system7, while in other forms caused by mutations in glycine cleavage system genes3,4, glycine is dramatically elevated throughout the body. Nonetheless there are overlapping symptoms between these forms of GE such as breathing difficulties and reduced motor tone7.

Here, we show that prior to anesthesia glyt1−/− mutant zebrafish have normal bouts of movement but initiate these bouts less frequently. glyt1−/− mutants take more than twice as long as their siblings to emerge from two structurally distinct general anesthetics: ketamine and 2,6-diisopropylphenol (the active ingredient of propofol). Whole-brain activity mapping during baseline conditions shows that preoptic brain regions are more active in glyt1−/− mutants while widespread sensorimotor activation is suppressed. Reducing brain glycine in glyt1−/− mutants restores timely emergence from anesthesia, pointing to elevated glycine as causal. Cumulatively, our results support a model whereby high glycine in glyt1−/− mutants promotes activity in preoptic brain regions and interferes with sensorimotor integration to produce lethargy-like behaviors and delayed emergence from anesthesia.

Methods

Animals

Wild type (Tubingen Longfin and Brian’s Wild Type) and glyt1te30120 heterozygous adult Danio rerio strains were maintained on a 14-h light, 10-h dark circadian cycle at 28.5 °C in recirculating Aquatic Habitats (Apopka, FL) aquaria. The glyt1 shocked te301 genotype was determined using PCR followed by restriction enzyme digest as in Mongeon 200820. All larvae were raised in ‘system water’ (reverse osmosis water conditioned with salts and bicarbonate that houses adults). Larvae at five and six days of age have not completed sexual maturation and therefore sex is not indicated. All procedures were reviewed and approved by the University of Miami Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee and are described in protocols 13-212 and 16-217 entitled ‘A zebrafish model of delayed emergence from anesthesia in patients with glycine encephalopathy.’ The University of Miami has an Animal Welfare Assurance on file with the Office of Laboratory Animal Welfare (OLAW), National Institutes of Health (assurance number: A-3377-01). It has had continuous accreditation by the Association for Assessment and Accreditation of Laboratory Animal Care (AALAC) since 1960. All experiments were performed in accordance with the relevant guidelines and regulations of these agencies.

Kinematic analyses

High-speed videos were captured at 1000 frames per second (fps) with a frame resolution of 512 × 512 pixels using a FastCAM 1024 PCI high-speed camera (Photron, San Diego, CA). Larvae were filmed in a custom plexiglass enclosure with the camera and associated Fujinon 1:1.4/25 mm CF25HA1 lens (Fujifilm North America Corporation, Valhalla NY) mounted 8.25 cm below a 35 mm Petri dish containing eight to twelve larval zebrafish. Backlighting mounted above the larvae was provided by a CS420 constant current source LED array (Advanced Illumination, Rochester, VT). To evoke escape responses, we used brief vibration stimuli provided by a Mini-shaker 4810 (Brüel and Kjær, Denmark) attached to a titanium rod and platform that held the Petri dish of zebrafish larvae. The vibration was controlled by a Dell Quad Duo computer via a data acquisition card (PCI-6221; National Instruments, Austin, TX), connection block (BNC-2110; National Instruments, Austin, TX) and a Grass S48 Stimulator (Astro-Med, Inc., W. Warwick, RI).

Four-hour recordings in lanes

For four-hour daytime recordings, five-day-old larvae were placed in custom-made lanes. Lanes were fabricated by milling a 0.5 × 7.25 × 10 cm (h × w × l) piece of plexiglass with 15 0.5 × 0.3 × 6.5 cm (h × w × l) wells. A Logitech HD 720p webcam was placed below the wells to avoid reflections associated with the meniscus.

Code

Videos were analyzed with custom-written MATLAB (MathWorks Inc., MA) scripts: https://github.com/sheyums/ChiyuanLI-Fish-Code. Larval positions in each image were determined by the ‘background subtraction’ method. Position was defined by larva’s center-of-mass, calculated from larval average × and y pixel locations. To compensate for slowly changing environment due to water evaporation and mechanical fluctuations in the tank, the algorithm automatically updated the background every ~ 1000 frames.

Anesthesia

Stock solutions of 100 µM of 2,6-Diisopropylphenol (Propofol; SAFC supply, St. Louis, MO) and 0.4% buffered tricaine methanesulfonate salt (Sigma, St Louis, MO) were diluted in system water. Fresh stock solutions were prepared at the beginning of each experiment. Ketamine was purchased from the Division of Veterinary Services at the University of Miami as a 10 mg/mL stock solution in water (Vedco, St. Joseph, MO) and also diluted in system water.

Propofol solutions were made in two ways, with and without sonication. Without sonication, 1 µL of neat propofol was added to 1 mL of system water followed by 30 s of vortexing. This mixture was further diluted into 50 mL and vortexed again for 30 s. This method was used for data in Figs. 3, 4, 5, and 6. With sonication, a calibrated, drawn out glass pipet was used to measure and transfer neat propofol to a concentration of ~ 100 µM (1 µL/50 mL system water in a glass bottle) before vortexing for 30 s and sonicating (Cole-Parmer Sonogen 60,626) for five minutes22. Sonication reduces the amount of bath propofol needed to anesthetize animals by ten-fold. This method was used for data in Figs. 2 and Supplementary Fig. 2 and 3. To compare how much propofol reached the brain tissue using each method, we carried out HPLC on dissected brains exposed to bath propofol without or with sonication (Supplementary Fig. 1).

Figure 3.

Figure 3

glyt1−/− mutant zebrafish larvae show delayed emergence from anesthesia. (A) Experimental workflow is indicated. i-Progeny from glyt1 ± adult in-cross were raised for five days and ii-transiently exposed to one of three anesthetics:10 µM propofol, ketamine (20 mM 45 min. or 10 mM 120 min.) and buffered 765.4 µM tricaine. iii-After removal of anesthetic, time of first response to vibration (stimuli were delivered at five-minute intervals) was recorded and iv-individual genotypes were subsequently determined using a PCR/restriction enzyme-based assay. A representative, cropped image of the genotyping gel with each lane corresponding to a larvae is shown. (B) Scatter plots of individual emergence times are overlaid with median and interquartile range. Kruskal Wallis ANOVAs followed by Dunn’s multiple comparisons were conducted for each anesthetic and incubation time. Asterisks indicate p-value: *p < 0.05, **p < 0.001, ***p < 0.001, ****p < 0.0001. Compared to their siblings, glyt1−/− mutants took more than twice as long to emerge from both ketamine and propofol. By contrast, glyt1−/− mutants and their siblings took similar amounts of time to emerge from tricaine. (C) Kaplan–Meier plots of the proportions of responsive fish against time-post-anesthesia for the same glyt1 + / + , glyt1 + /-, and glyt1−/− larvae presented in B are shown post-120-min exposures to propofol (top), ketamine (middle), and tricaine (bottom). p-values are calculated using log rank tests.

Figure 4.

Figure 4

Reducing glycine accelerates emergence from propofol in glyt1−/− mutants. (A) To test the role of glycine in delayed emergence from anesthesia, we anesthetized five-day-old glyt1 sibling (+ /- light gray; + / + dark gray) and mutant (− / − orange) larvae with tricaine, surgically exposed brain ventricles to a 0-glycine bath solution, tested larvae for intact swimming behavior and then anesthetized them with propofol for thirty minutes and measured their time to recovery. Numbers in each experimental group are indicated in parentheses on the plots in B and C. (B) Scatter plots of times to emergence post-propofol are plotted for larvae that were not surgically manipulated (left) and larvae with surgically exposed brain ventricles (right). Results were analyzed with a two-way ANOVA (treatment and genotype) followed by Wilcoxon/Kruskal Wallis test of significance. Asterisks indicate p-value: *p < 0.05, **p < 0.001, ***p < 0.001, ****p < 0.0001. glyt1−/− mutants emerged significantly more slowly than wild type siblings and glyt1−/− mutants with surgically exposed brain ventricles. (C) Kaplan–Meier plots of the proportions of responsive fish against time-post-propofol for the same glyt1 + / + , glyt1 + /-, and glyt1−/− larvae presented in B are shown for No Surgery (top) and Surgery/0 glycine (bottom). p-values are calculated using log rank tests.

Figure 5.

Figure 5

Brain-wide activity in both glyt1−/− mutants and wild type siblings is significantly reduced by exposure to propofol. Four batches of sixteen to eighteen, six-day-old larvae were placed in baskets (diagramed to the left) to enable easy transfer of larvae from system water to 10 µM propofol solutions. Half contained glyt1 siblings (A) and the other half glyt1−/− mutants (B). Larvae from each treatment were fixed and stained with pERK and tERK antibodies and z-stacks of the brain were captured on a confocal microscope. Images boxed in green correspond to baseline conditions while images boxed in purple correspond to exposure to 10 µM propofol for twenty minutes. Images show standard deviation projections of pERK, tERK, pERK/tERK, and median values. To compare activity between baseline and propofol treatments, voxels with more intense pERK/tERK at a p < 0.00005 threshold under baseline conditions are shown in green while voxels with more intense pERK/tERK in propofol are shown in purple for glyt1 siblings (A) and glyt1−/− mutants in (B). It is clear that both glyt1 siblings and glyt1−/− mutants have reduced activity at anesthetic doses of propofol as expected from their similar dose/response curves. Yellow brackets, curly brackets and arrows in median images highlight staining patterns that differ significantly between glyt1−/− mutants and their siblings. Under Baseline conditions, brackets point to a region encompassing the subpallium and preoptic that has more pronounced staining in the glyt1−/− mutants while curly brackets point to a region encompassing the optic tectum, cerebellum, and hindbrain that has more intense staining in the glyt1 siblings. Under propofol, arrows point to the area postrema region of the hindbrain that has more pronounced staining in glyt1 siblings. CB cerebellum, HB hindbrain, OB olfactory bulb, OT optic tectum, P pallium, PO preoptic, SC Spinal Cord, SP Sub-pallium, VT ventral tegmentum. Scale bars = 100 µm.

Figure 6.

Figure 6

Compared to glyt1 siblings, glyt1−/− mutants show delayed activation of pre-motor, and motor regions during emergence from propofol. Four batches of sixteen to eighteen, six-day-old larvae were placed in baskets (diagramed to the left) to enable easy transfer of animals from 10 µM propofol to system water solutions. Half contained glyt1 siblings after (A) ten minutes and (B) twenty minutes in system water. The other half contained glyt1−/− mutants after (C) ten minutes and (D) twenty minutes in system water. To compare activity between propofol and wash treatments, voxels with more intense pERK/tERK at a p < 0.00005 threshold under propofol conditions are shown in purple while voxels with more intense pERK/tERK in system water during emergence are shown in green for glyt1 siblings (A, B) and glyt1−/− mutants in (C, D). glyt1 siblings show greater activation of preoptic, subpallium, and spinal cord regions under propofol, with the olfactory bulb and a regions of the diencephalon that encompasses the arousal pathways (yellow arrowhead in A) being the first brain region to become more active during emergence from propofol, followed ten minutes later by the optic tectum, cerebellum and hindbrain. glyt1−/− mutants have greater activation of preoptic and subpallium, but also optic tectum neuropil and cerebellar neuropil under propofol. As with their glyt1 siblings, the olfactory bulb is the first brain region to become activated but there is no activation of other sensory, pre-motor, and motor regions by the twenty-minute timepoint consistent with delays seen in glyt1−/− mutants recovery of locomotor behaviors after propofol. Scale bars = 100 µm.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

glyt1 siblings and glyt1−/− mutants have similar tricaine, ketamine, and propofol dose–response curves. (A) Our experimental approach is diagrammed. Larvae in 16-well plates, with five larvae per basket insert, were exposed to alternating thirty second intervals of light and dark, with a tap delivered after 20 s in dark; this sequence of stimuli was repeated five times for a total of five minutes. Measurements of larval swimming were made at minutes four and five (red boxes). Larvae were then exposed to increasingly higher concentrations of anesthetic by transferring baskets until all stopped moving in response to tap. (B) The percentage of glyt1 sibling (gray) and glyt1−/− mutant (orange) moving are plotted for three behavioral endpoints: swimming in light (top row), swimming in response to a dark transition (middle row), and swimming in response to a tap (bottom row). Percentage responding are shown for each batch in the absence of anesthetic (left-most column; n = 21 batches of five larvae for both glyt1−/− mutants and their siblings). To the right, normalized mean percentages moving are plotted against log M concentrations of Tricaine (second column; n = 3 batches of five larvae for both glyt1−/− mutants and their siblings), Ketamine (third column; n = 5 batches of five larvae for both glyt1−/− mutants and their siblings), and propofol (fourth column; n = 14 batches of five larvae for both glyt1−/− mutants and their siblings). Dose–response data were fitted with nonlinear four parameter curves constraining Hill slope in Prism. Horizontal dashed lines show 50% response and EC50 for each curve is indicated on the graphs.

Dose/Response

Twelve-well plates with basket inserts were used. Five larvae (6 days post-fertilization) were placed in each basket/well containing 1.5 ml of system water (Fig. 2A). The well plate was then placed in the Noldus DanioVision chamber for 1 h so larvae could dark adapt. Following dark adaptation, a visual-motor response assay (VMR) coupled with a vibrational stimulus was used to assess larval state. Larvae were exposed to 30 s of lights-on (12% intensity in Noldus, 1200 lx) and 20 s of lights-off conditions followed by a tap (at intensity 5) and left in the dark for another 10 s. This cycle was repeated for five minutes. Next, larvae were transferred to increasing concentrations of anesthetic, the stimulation protocol repeated, and the proportion of larvae responding was recorded. The proportion responding in the 5 s before and after tap/light responses during 4th and 5th VMR cycles at each dose was subsequently fitted with a sigmoidal dose–response curve using Prism (GraphPad) as in22. For tricaine, the concentrations tested were 76.5 µM, 95.7 µM, 114.8 µM, 153.1 µM, 191.4 µM, 306.2 µM, 421 µM, and 765.4 µM. For ketamine, the concentrations tested were 50 µM, 100 µM, 500 µM, 1 mM, 5 mM, and 10 mM. For propofol the concentrations tested were 0.05 µM, 1 µM, 2 µM, 4 µM, 6 µM, 8 µM, and 10 µM.

Emergence from anesthesia

After washout of each anesthetic, larvae were tested at five-minute intervals for recovery of movement in response to vibration. The larval vibration-elicited escape response is similar to assays used in frog tadpoles23 and the righting reflex in mice used to assess emergence from anesthesia24. Mixed genotype larvae from a glyt1 heterozygous cross were exposed to anesthesia, allowed to recover, and then euthanized with tricaine before genotyping.

Craniotomy

glyt1−/− mutant larvae were anesthetized in tricaine, placed dorsal side up on a slanted plate made with 1% agarose in system water, and surgically manipulated to introduce a hole in the roof of the brain using sharpened tungsten needles as in20. Larvae were then placed in modified Hanks solution: in mM 140 NaCl, 0.1 Na2PO4, 3 KCl, 0.2 K2PO4, 3 mM D-Glucose so as not to osmotically shock exposed brain ventricles, a protocol previously used to normalize glycine levels in glyt1−/− mutants20.

MAP-mapping with pERK/tERK staining compared across treatments and genotypes.

MAP-mapping was carried out as in Randlett et al. 201525. 16–18 larvae per batch were fixed and stained with pERK and tERK antibodies (Cell Signaling Tech) followed by fluorescent secondary antibodies. Samples were imaged dorsal side up on an upright confocal with voxel size 0.08 × 0.08 × 2 µm using a 20 × water immersion objective. To capture the entire brain, two regions were stitched together. Using t-ERK staining, brain z-stacks were warped to a reference brain using CMTK. After warping, stacks were down-sampled to resolutions of 300, 679, and 80 sections in x, y, and z planes respectively and smoothed with a 2D Gaussian filter using the macro “PrepareStacksForMAPmapping.ijm” in Fiji. To compare staining across treatments, pERK values were divided by tERK values on a per voxel basis to normalize for individual variability in staining.

Experimental design

The majority of experiments were carried out blind to genotype on five and six-day-old larvae. No statistical power calculation was conducted prior to the study and sample sizes were based on the available data. Homozygous glyt1−/− mutants were compared to their siblings (glyt1 ± and +/+). To enrich for glyt1−/− mutants and thus reduce the total number of animals used, embryos were dechorionated at 28–30 h post-fertilization and sorted for their ability to move (glyt1−/− mutants are paralytic at this stage), then remixed with a similar number of siblings, and reared to days five and six for experiments. At least three batches of larvae were used per experiment to control for batch effects.

Statistical analysis

Prism (GraphPad) and Matlab were used for graphing and statistical analyses. Datasets were analyzed for normal distributions in Prism and their majority were not normally distributed. Therefore, nonparametric analyses were used. For basal behavior (Fig. 1) and emergence from anesthesia experiments (Table 1 and Figs. 3B, 4B and 7), non-parametric Kruskal Wallis ANOVAs are followed by Dunn’s Multiple Comparisons to calculate p-values and 2way-ANOVAs are followed by Sidak’s multiple comparisons. For datasets in Figs. 1, 3, and 4 medians ± interquartile range are reported with p-values based on non-parametric Mann–Whitney test (abbreviated MW). For the pERK/tERK MAP mapping approach in Figs. 5 and 6, voxel intensities between two groups of 16–18 larvae were compared with the Mann–Whitney U-statistic Z score as in25. A false discovery rate (FDR)-based method was used to set the significance threshold. The color intensity (0–65,535) assigned to the voxel is proportional to the difference between the median values. For the projections, intensity is scaled linearly with saturation at 60% of the maximum pixel intensity. This analysis was carried out in Matlab using the function “MakeTheMAPMap.m.” Regions that exceed the threshold for significance are indicated in either green (greater relative activity in treatment 1) or purple (greater relative activity in treatment 2).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

glyt1−/− mutants show normal bouts of locomotion with reduced bout frequency. (A) Body curvature is graphed over time for representative wild type sibling (grey) and glyt1−/− mutant (orange). Images of each larva at times of peak curvature are shown. (B) Median swim velocity with interquartile ranges are plotted for wild type siblings (n = 18) and glyt1−/− mutants (n = 21). (C) We designed custom lanes for measuring basal locomotor activity. Single larvae (white arrows; left panel) are placed in each lane and recorded for four hours. Binary image (white, right panel) shows larvae used for quantitative image analysis in Matlab. (D) Raw data of fish position versus time are plotted for a representative glyt1 sibling and glyt1−/− mutant. Periods of inactivity are interrupted by periodic bouts of swimming. Inset box plots show that glyt1−/− mutant fish are less active and more variable in activity than their wild type siblings. The probability densities (y-axis label in E also serves for F) with insets showing median and interquartile interval for (E) swim bout distance and (F) dwell times for glyt1−/− mutants (n = 16,427 bouts from 24 larvae) and glyt1 siblings (n = 125,784 bouts from 30 larvae). Mann Whitney rank tests show bout size is not different (p = 0.99) while dwell times are significantly longer in glyt1−/− mutants p = 1.18 × 10^(− 5). Asterisks indicate p-value: *p < 0.05, **p < 0.001, ***p < 0.001, ****p < 0.0001. (G) Position of an individual fish over fifteen minutes is plotted to show lap behavior. While glyt1−/− mutants have larger lap amplitudes than their siblings (p = 0.03) (H), glyt1−/− mutants do not exhibit prolonged durations for single lap behaviors (I). Clustered lap behaviors were less frequent in glyt1−/− mutants than in their siblings (p = 0.0003; see arrows).

Table 1.

glyt1−/− mutant larvae take longer to emerge from propofol and ketamine but not tricaine.

Experiment ANOVA
(Kruskal Wallis)
Dunn’s multiple comparisons
Anesthesia
(minutes)
glyt1 + / + vs. glyt1 ±  glyt1 + / + vs. glyt1−/− glyt1 + / − vs. glyt1−/−
H statistic p Rank p Rank p Rank p
Propofol (30) 28.5  < 0.0001  − 4.6

 > 0.99

ns

 − 30.5  < 0.0001  − 25.8  < 0.0001

Propofol

(120)

32.1  < 0.0001 5.1

 > 0.99

ns

 − 28.8 0.0001  − 34.0  < 0.0001

Ketamine

(45)

14.8 0.0006 1.9

 > 0.99

ns

 − 12.8 0.01  − 14.7 0.0004

Ketamine

(120)

27.7  < 0.0001 4.2

 > 0.99

ns

 − 16.4 0.007  − 20.6  < 0.0001

MS222

(60)

4.1

0.13

ns

1.6

 > 0.99

ns

 − 7.5

0.61

ns

 − 9.1

0.14

ns

MS222

(120)

1.2

0.55

ns

 − 1.8

 > 0.99

ns

 − 4.2

0.85

ns

 − 2.4

 > 0.99

ns

No Surgery

Propofol

(30)

20.6  < 0.0001 7.1 0.47  − 13.9 0.03  − 21.0  < 0.0001

Surgery

Propofol (30)

1.2

0.56

ns

3.5

0.96

ns

1.4

 > 0.99

ns

 − 2.1

 > 0.99

ns

Times to emergence from different anesthetics are compared across glyt1 genotypes. Shown are Kruskal Wallis ANOVA H-statistic and p-value followed by Dunn’s multiple comparisons rank and p-value.

Figure 7.

Figure 7

glyt1−/− mutants’ sensory ganglia are more active while arousal and motor circuits are less active than their siblings during recovery from propofol. (A) Brain regions of interest (ROIs) that are significantly different between glyt1−/− mutants and their siblings are shown on reference brains for Baseline , Propofol, 10 min. wash and 20 min. wash conditions with sample size indicated at the base of each image. ROIs are labeled with the corresponding anatomical brain regions; an abbreviation key can be found at the end of the figure legend. (BD) Violin plots of pERK/tERK ratios for twelve different brain regions are shown with a dashed line indicating median pERK/tERK levels in baseline glyt1 siblings. For each brain region, we conducted a 2-way ANOVA for genotype and condition followed by Sidak’s multiple comparisons of genotype across conditions. Asterisks indicate p-value: *p < 0.05, **p < 0.001, ***p < 0.001, ****p < 0.0001. (A) Three sensory ganglia are shown that are elevated in glyt1 mutants during recovery. (B) Olfactory bulb and arousal pathways are shown. (C) Premotor and motor regions elevated in baseline glyt1 siblings are shown. (D) Hypnotic regions elevated in baseline glyt1−/− mutants are shown. Anatomical abbreviations: OE olfactory epithelium, subpallium SP, pallium P, Preoptic nucleus PO, Hypothalamic nucleus enriched in Qrfp-expressing neuronal cell bodies Qrfp, Ventral Thalamus VT, Dopaminergic Cluster 2 in the posterior tuberculum Dop 2, Tectum Stratum Periventriculare TSPv, Tectum Neuropil TN, Cerebellum CB, vagal motor nucleus X, posterior Lateral Line Neuromasts along the body LLN D. Scale bar = 100 µm.

We did not exclude any animals from our analyses.

Results

glyt1−/−mutants show normal bouts of locomotion but reduced bout frequency

Previous work had shown that glyt1−/− mutant larvae are initially paralyzed by elevated glycine but recover coordinated locomotion by four days post fertilization20,26. Locomotion is central to assessing anesthesia in this model, therefore, here we carried out a more thorough assessment of swim bout coordination, frequency, and stamina in five-day-old glyt1−/− mutants and their siblings.

We first used high-speed recordings to compare discrete bouts of locomotion. As shown previously in Mongeon et al. (2008), both glyt1−/− mutants and siblings responded to vibration with a pronounced C-bend away from the stimulus followed by alternating flexions along the body axis (Fig. 1A). There was no difference between gylt1 mutants and their siblings in swim velocity (Fig. 1B; p = 0.35; MW). These results show that five-day-old glyt1−/− mutants have normal bouts of locomotion.

We next made long-term (4-h) recordings at reduced temporal resolution sufficient to capture the beginning and end of each bout of locomotion. We placed larvae in lanes (Fig. 1C) that allowed them to both swim long distances relative to their body length (Fig. 1D) and to easily turn and swim in the opposite direction. From these recordings, we extracted quantitative measurements and constructed probability densities of both distance traveled per swim bout (Fig. 1E) and dwell times (the time in between bouts; Fig. 1F). We found that while swim bouts were similar in size between glyt1−/− mutants and their sibling (Fig. 1E; p = 0.999; MW), dwell times between movements were longer in glyt1−/− mutants, indicating that mutants initiate swim bouts less frequently (Fig. 1F; p = 1.18 × 10−5; MW).

In these arenas, zebrafish larvae also showed lap swimming during which larvae swam back and forth along the lane by producing many sequential bouts of locomotion with only short intervening rests (Fig. 1G; hatched box). This lap behavior had characteristic lengths (Fig. 1H), periods (Fig. 1I), and duration (Fig. 1J). Although glyt1−/− mutant laps were slightly larger in amplitude than their siblings (p = 0.03, MW), individual siblings showed the ability to swim for longer periods than any of the glyt1−/− mutants (Fig. 1I; arrow). glyt1 siblings also maintained swimming for longer periods of time than glyt1−/− mutants (Fig. 1J; p = 0.0003; MW; arrow). In sum, these long-term recordings show that despite normal glyt1−/− mutant bouts of locomotion, glyt1−/− mutants initiate these bouts less frequently and exhibit shorter sustained periods of movement. We define reduced frequency of movement as ‘lethargy-like behaviors’ similar to those described in other animal models27 and in individuals with GE28.

glyt1−/− mutants have similar propofol dose–response curves to aversive tap stimuli

We compared anesthesia induction in glyt1−/− mutants and their siblings with three anesthetics that act through different pathways: tricaine, a non-specific Na channel blocker and the most commonly used anesthetic in zebrafish29, ketamine, a general anesthetic and NMDA receptor blocker30, and propofol, a general anesthetic and potentiator of GABA and glycine receptors. We also directly compared three behavioral endpoints for dose–response curves: loss of spontaneous swimming behavior, loss of response to transitions from light to darkness (visual motor response, VMR), and loss of response to tap/vibrational stimuli (Fig. 2A). In the absence of anesthesia, larvae are more active in response to dark transitions than in the light (glyt1 sib p = 0.0013; glyt1−/− mutant p = 0.0229) and more active in response to tap than in the light (glyt1 sib p = 0.0005; glyt1−/− mutant p = 0.0002; Fig. 2B).

The proportions of larvae responsive at different anesthetic doses were first normalized and then fit with polynomial four parameter curves in Prism. By testing these three behavioral endpoints sequentially, we observed that zebrafish tend to stop swimming and lose their VMR response at lower anesthetic concentrations than what is required to suppress the tap response (Fig. 2B; Supplementary Fig. 2; Supplementary Table 1). As such, we consider loss of swimming/VMR response similar to sedation as an endpoint while loss of the tap response, a more aversive stimulus, as more similar to anesthesia. While glyt1−/− mutants tended to become sedated at lower anesthetic doses than their siblings, they show similar or reduced anesthetic sensitivity in the tap assay. Doses of ketamine required to suppress the tap response also reduced larval heart rate (data not shown), therefore, for subsequent ketamine experiments, we used doses that were sedative but not fully anesthetic.

Time to emergence from propofol and ketamine is delayed in glyt1−/− mutants

To determine the relationship between glyt1 genotype and time to emergence from anesthesia, we compared time to regain tap response after exposure to the three anesthetics in glyt1 + / + , glyt1 ± , and glyt1−/− larvae (Fig. 3A; Supplementary Fig. 3). While there was no difference between glyt1 genotypes in time to emergence from 765.4 µM tricaine, glyt1−/− mutants took significantly longer than their siblings to emerge from either 10 mM ketamine or 10 µM propofol (Fig. 3B; Table 1). As would be predicted, longer exposure to anesthetics was associated with correspondingly longer times to emergence (Fig. 3B; propofol 30 vs. 120 min. exposure, p < 0.0001; ketamine 45 vs. 120 min. exposure, p = 0.0004; tricaine 60 vs. 120 min. exposure, p < 0.0001; MW). Moreover, failure-time-analyses showed that glyt1−/− mutants took more than twice as long as their siblings to emerge from anesthesia after two-hour exposures to propofol and ketamine but not to tricaine (Fig. 3C; Log rank test).

Craniotomy accelerates emergence from propofol in glyt1−/− mutants

To test whether elevated glycine in glyt1−/− mutants could explain delayed emergence, we surgically exposed brain ventricles to a bath solution by incising the skin over the fourth ventricle (craniotomy)20, thus equilibrating brain and bath glycine levels (Fig. 4A). This procedure was previously shown to reduce brain glycine levels sufficiently to restore movement to pre-recovery, two-day-old glyt1−/− embryos that would otherwise be paralyzed by elevated glycine20. Here, we used tricaine to anesthetize five-day-old glyt1−/− mutant and their sibling larvae for craniotomy surgery and let them recover swimming behaviors prior to exposing them to 10 µM propofol for 30 min and measuring time to emergence (Fig. 4B; Table 1). With surgery, glyt1−/− mutants had emergence times similar to their siblings and emerged significantly faster than glyt1−/− mutants without surgery. These results indicate that delayed emergence from propofol in glyt1−/− mutants is caused by a humoral factor, most likely glycine, in the cerebral spinal fluid of glyt1−/− mutants.

In both glyt1−/− mutant and their siblings, propofol inhibits brain-wide activity

To compare brain-wide activity in glyt1−/− mutants and their siblings, we used the MAP-mapping approach25. Twenty larvae were placed in baskets to facilitate transfers between system water, 10 µM propofol, and fixative (diagramed in Figs. 5 and 6). After fixation, larvae were stained with antibodies against phospho-ERK (pERK; Fig. 5 left-most column) and total-ERK (tERK; Fig. 5 second column). The pERK/tERK ratio (Fig. 5 third column) was used as a proxy for neuronal activity integrated over the 15 min just prior to fixation as in25. Median values per voxel were then calculated for different treatment conditions (Fig. 5; fourth column). To compare the relative activity between baseline and 10 µM propofol, pERK/tERK values were compared on a per voxel basis and the threshold for significance was corrected for multiple comparisons (p < 10−5). Green voxels indicate higher pERK/tERK values in the baseline condition while purple voxels indicate higher pERK/tERK values during exposure to 10 µM propofol (Fig. 5; right-most column). As expected, both glyt1−/− mutants and their siblings showed significantly more intense brain-wide activity under baseline conditions (green) than in 10 µM propofol (purple).

To identify brain regions associated with propofol exposure and emergence from propofol, we compared activity in 10 µM propofol to activity both 10 and 20 min after propofol washout in glyt1 siblings (Figs. 6A,B and 7) and glyt1−/− mutants (Figs. 6C–D and 7). During exposure to 10 µM propofol, both glyt1−/− mutants and their siblings showed more activity in sub-pallial (homologous to mammalian striatum/septum31) and preoptic areas (homologous to mammalian preoptic32). Moreover, sensory ganglia and the olfactory bulb were relatively resistant to anesthesia and were also the first brain regions to become activate post-propofol (Figs. 6A,D, 7). Remarkably, even though sensory ganglia were more active in glyt1−/− mutants than their siblings post-propofol, motor regions were less active, indicating reduced sensorimotor integration in glyt1−/− mutants (Fig. 7B–D). These brain regions align with mammalian literature linking activity in preoptic circuits to hypnotic states of sleep and anesthesia33,34, and heightened activity in sensory circuits to emergence from anesthesia35.

glyt1−/− mutants show elevated activity in the preoptic, a hypnotic brain region

We next compared activity between glyt1−/− mutants (orange) and their siblings (gray) during baseline, exposure to 10 µM propofol, and at both 10 and 20 min after washing out propofol (Figs. 7; Table 2). Under baseline conditions, glyt1−/− mutants had increased activity in the subpallium, preoptic hypothalamic and the broader hypothalamus; by contrast, glyt1 siblings had increased activity in their optic tectum, cerebellum, hindbrain, and spinal cord (Fig. 7A). These patterns indicate increased activity in sensory and motor regions in glyt1 siblings compared to glyt1−/− mutants even under baseline conditions. These differences can also be seen in the median images indicated by yellow brackets and arrows (Fig. 5). Under baseline conditions, preoptic and sub-pallial brain regions show stronger staining in glyt1−/− mutants (straight yellow brackets) while optic tectum, cerebellum, and hindbrain show stronger staining in their siblings (curlicue yellow brackets).

Table 2.

Brainwide differences between glyt1−/− mutant and sibling larvae are most pronounced in baseline and 20 min. wash conditions.

Brain region 2Way ANOVA
Genotype/condition
Sidak’s multiple comparisons
glyt1−/− vs. Siblings p-value
Interact Geno Cond Baseline Propofol 10 wash 20 wash
OE  < 0.0001  < 0.0001  < 0.0001

0.5997

ns

0.1782

ns

0.9969

ns

 < 0.0001
S01 0.0025  < 0.0001  < 0.0001

0.9793

ns

0.0375

0.0547

ns

 < 0.0001
S02 0.0956  < 0.0001  < 0.0001

0.4619

ns

0.3674

ns

0.0538

ns

 < 0.0001
OB  < 0.0001  < 0.0001 0.0178

0.9985

ns

0.0290 0.0024  < 0.0001
PT-Dop 2 0.0539  < 0.0001  < 0.0001 0.0023

 > 0.9999

ns

0.2273

ns

0.0127
Qrfp 0.1401  < 0.0001 0.0034

 > 0.9999

ns

0.9412

ns

0.1095

ns

0.0141
VT 0.0059  < 0.0001  < 0.0001  < 0.0001

0.9992

ns

0.8347

ns

0.0420
PT 0.1298  < 0.0001 0.0003 0.0085

0.9997

ns

0.5204

ns

0.0265
Isl 1 0.0004  < 0.0001  < 0.0001  < 0.0001

 > 0.9999

ns

0.9587

ns

0.0051
SP-Dop 0.0056  < 0.0001 0.0001  < 0.0001

0.3229

ns

0.9899

ns

0.1927

ns

AC  < 0.0001  < 0.0001  < 0.0001  < 0.0001 0.0082

0.9971

ns

0.9715

ns

PO-Glut  < 0.0001  < 0.0001  < 0.0001  < 0.0001

0.2256

ns

0.9986

ns

0.9970

ns

For the twelve brain regions shown in Fig. 7, 2way ANOVA p-values for interaction, genotype, and condition are shown in columns 2–4 are followed by p-values for Sidak’s multiple comparisons of glyt1−/− mutants and siblings across the four conditions in columns 5–8.

Surprisingly, during 10 µM propofol, glyt1−/− mutants showed more activity overall than glyt1 siblings (Figs. 6A,C and 7A). One exception to this pattern was that glyt1 siblings show stronger staining in the area postrema (Fig. 6; yellow arrows), a brain region commonly associated with nausea, the most prevalent post-anesthesia symptom reported in humans36.

Ten minutes into emergence from propofol, glyt1 siblings showed more activity in the olfactory bulb, and optic tectum, and in areas encompassing the diencephalon D2 dopaminergic, QRFP, and hypocretin/orexin arousal pathways, consistent with the glyt1 siblings’ earlier recovery from propofol (Fig. 6A; Fig. 7A). By contrast, glyt1−/− mutants still showed increased activity in sub-pallial and preoptic regions, likely hypnotic circuits since these are the same regions that are more active in both glyt1−/− mutants and their siblings during propofol exposure (Fig. 6C,D).

At twenty minutes post-propofol, glyt1−/− mutants showed more activity in their sensory ganglia and olfactory bulb consistent with initiating their behavioral recovery (Supplemental Fig. 2; Fig. 6D; Fig. 7B-C). Unlike their siblings however, glyt1−/− mutants did not show the coordinate activation of arousal pathways in the diencephalon (Fig. 6 A,D). Moreover, at the 20 min after washing out propofol, glyt1 siblings showed more activity in their optic tectum and cerebellum, consistent with the return of movement in behavioral assays (Supplemental Fig. 2; Fig. 6B). These comparisons indicate that during emergence in glyt1 siblings, activity first increases in sensory before spreading to diencephalon brain regions encompassing QRFP, dopamine and hypocretin arousal pathways and motor-associated regions. By contrast, glyt1−/− mutants showed elevated activity in hypnotic preoptic brain regions under baseline conditions and, despite increased activity in sensory ganglia post-propofol, were delayed in activating motor brain regions during emergence from anesthesia.

Discussion

Our work provides an animal model of delayed emergence from anesthesia in GE. GE is a rare genetic disorder impacting ~ 1:76,000 people (National Organization of Rare Disorders)4,5, making clinical studies of how to best anesthetize individuals with GE difficult if not impossible to conduct1,37,38. As such, our ability to model this phenomenon in the zebrafish glyt1−/− mutant provides a way to investigate underlying mechanisms of delayed emergence. As in people with GE who struggle with seizures, lethargy and failure in reaching developmental milestones4,9, the glyt1−/− mutation in zebrafish has negative impacts on long-term health, with the majority of glyt1−/− mutant zebrafish failing to survive juvenile periods of rapid growth. At the pre-feeding, larval stages studied here however, glyt1−/− mutants can produce normal bouts of swimming, having compensated for high glycine by down-regulating expression of glycine receptors, a known target of general anesthesia20; despite quantifiable lethargy-like behaviors reported herein, they have 100% survival and are impossible to sort by eye from their siblings based on either behavior or morphology20. Our findings reveal that larval glyt1−/− mutants have elevated activity in hypnotic, preoptic brain regions that likely explains both their lethargy-like behaviors and delayed emergence from anesthesia.

Zebrafish are a relatively unexplored model39 in which to study the pharmacogenetics of anesthesia15,22,40,41. As such, there is still variation in both the behavioral endpoints used to generate dose–response curves and in the reported doses required for anesthesia. For example, three recent studies each used different behavioral endpoints to construct dose–response curves: loss of the visual motor response (VMR)40 (increased swimming in larvae evoked by sudden decreases in light), loss of touch-induced escape swims41, and loss of the tap-induced response22. By directly comparing loss of spontaneous swimming, loss of VMR, and loss of tap response, we show that an order of magnitude higher dose of anesthetic was required for loss of the tap response compared to the other two behavioral endpoints, consistent with tap being a more aversive stimulus25. As such, we suggest that loss of VMR and spontaneous swims indicate sedation while loss of the tap response indicates a deeper state of anesthesia.

In regard to anesthetic dose, because zebrafish are aquatic, anesthetics are delivered directly to the bath. Less water soluble anesthetics like propofol form micelles in water22; because strategies to mitigate micelles vary, anesthetic doses reported in the literature also vary substantially22,40,42,43. Nonetheless, physiological measurements42, dose–response curves22,40, and HPLC measurements of brain propofol from this study support that effective anesthetic concentrations reflect differences in the way the solutions are made rather than individual differences in anesthetic sensitivity.

A challenge for zebrafish models of human inherited disorders is that zebrafish and human brains differ structurally44. As such, drawing parallels between zebrafish and human brain activity during different behavioral states requires physiological studies to establish that brain regions are functionally related in the two species35. In zebrafish, the ability to image activity in brain-wide neuronal ensembles has helped to address this problem. For example, in zebrafish as in mammals, arousal pathways including QRFP45, hypocretin/orexin46,47 and dopamine48 have been directly linked to increases in locomotion49. Moreover, in zebrafish as in mammals, activity galanin-producing neurons in the preoptic are associated with sleep50. Using MAP-mapping25, we show that activity in preoptic brain regions is also associated with anesthesia in both glyt1−/− mutants and their siblings, consistent with activity in the preoptic causing hypnotic states. In mammals, reciprocal inhibition between preoptic brain regions and arousal pathways is known to regulate transitions between sleep or anesthetized states and awake states33,5154. What sets glyt1−/− mutants apart from their siblings is persistently elevated activity in preoptic brain regions that could suppress arousal pathways and contribute to their delayed emergence from anesthesia. Critically, these results highlight that delayed emergence of glyt1−/− mutant zebrafish is not due simply to a motor deficit.

We show that preoptic brain regions are not only more active during emergence from anesthesia but also under baseline conditions prior to exposure to anesthesia. Consistent with elevated activity in the preoptic, glyt1−/− mutant exhibited lethargy-like behaviors quantified by their reduced frequency of movement and reduced responsiveness to sensory stimuli. Another much less severe condition that, like glycine encephalopathy, causes both daytime lethargy55 and delayed emergence from anesthesia56,57 is narcolepsy. Narcolepsy is known to be caused by deficits in the hypocretin/orexin arousal pathway58, suggesting that the similar glyt1−/− mutant and narcolepsy phenotypes could reflect dysregulation of arousal pathways. Our study contributes to a growing body of literature supporting that distinct neural circuits mediate induction into and emergence from anesthesia with arousal pathways playing a critical role in emergence16,59,60.

In summary, our work follows up a case study by showing that we can recapitulate delayed emergence from anesthesia in a zebrafish glyt1−/− mutant model. We use this model to identify elevated activity in preoptic brain regions as likely explaining both their daytime lethargy-like behaviors and their delayed emergence from anesthesia. Rescuing time-to-emergence from anesthesia and brain-wide activity mapping support a model whereby elevated glycine promotes hypnotic pathways to delay transitions to active locomotor states. Future work that directly tests approaches to boost excitability in arousal pathways and/or suppress activity in hypnotic brain regions of glyt1−/− mutant could suggest therapeutic strategies for individuals with GE.

Supplementary Information

Acknowledgements

We extend our thanks to Deanna Kallil, Mateus C. C. Lima, Courtney Hibbs, and Chabely Cowley for helping with initial experiments, Manuel Collazo and Angel Morciego for fashioning chambers and the titanium rod for behavior studies, and Will Scudder for brain dissections for HPLC. We thank Kevin Collins and April Mann for their review and editing of the manuscript. Developing the glyt1−/− mutant as a model for delayed emergence from anesthesia was conceived for a Neuroscience Lab course (Neu403), students in which contributed their ideas, enthusiasm, and confirming data. Zebrafish facility manager Ricardo Cepeda provided excellent care of the zebrafish.

Author contributions

JED, OR, and MBK designed research. MJV, OR, SS, RB, MJS, QY, SAS, EB, QCM, and JED performed research. FE, SZ, and SSyed contributed unpublished analytic tools. MJV and JED wrote the paper.

Funding

This work was supported by NIH NINDS K01NS048200 Career award, start-up funds, and Provost Award from University of Miami to JED and “Beyond-the-Books” undergraduate summer fellowships from the University of Miami College of Arts and Sciences to MJS and MJV, an Honors Program stipend to MJV, and NIH Brain Initiative grants U19NS104653, R24 NS086601, and R43OD024879, as well as Simons Foundation grants SCGB nos. 542973 and 325207 to FE.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Footnotes

Publisher's note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

The online version contains supplementary material availlable at 10.1038/s41598-021-82342-w.

References

  • 1.Liu CM, Fan SZ. Glycine encephalopathy and delayed emergence from anesthesia. Anesth. Analg. 2006;103:1631. doi: 10.1213/01.ane.0000247193.72647.d7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Hayasaka K, Tada K, Kikuchi G, Winter S, Nyhan WL. Nonketotic hyperglycinemia: two patients with primary defects of P-protein and T-protein, respectively, in the glycine cleavage system. Pediatr. Res. 1983;17:967–970. doi: 10.1203/00006450-198312000-00008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Schutgens RB, Ket JL, Hayasaka K, Tada K. Non-ketotic hyperglycinaemia due to a deficiency of T-protein in the glycine cleavage system in liver and brain. J. Inherit. Metab. Dis. 1986;9:208–214. doi: 10.1007/BF01799462. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Toone JR, Applegarth DA, Levy HL, Coulter-Mackie MB, Lee G. Molecular genetic and potential biochemical characteristics of patients with T-protein deficiency as a cause of glycine encephalopathy (NKH) Mol. Genet. Metab. 2003;79:272–280. doi: 10.1016/S1096-7192(03)00115-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Korman SH, et al. Persistent NKH with transient or absent symptoms and a homozygous GLDC mutation. Ann. Neurol. 2004;56:139–143. doi: 10.1002/ana.20159. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Flusser H, et al. Mild glycine encephalopathy (NKH) in a large kindred due to a silent exonic GLDC splice mutation. Neurology. 2005;64:1426–1430. doi: 10.1212/01.WNL.0000158475.12907.D6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Kurolap A, et al. Loss of glycine transporter 1 causes a subtype of glycine encephalopathy with arthrogryposis and mildly elevated cerebrospinal fluid glycine. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 2016 doi: 10.1016/j.ajhg.2016.09.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Alfadhel M, et al. Mutation in SLC6A9 encoding a glycine transporter causes a novel form of non-ketotic hyperglycinemia in humans. Hum. Genet. 2016;135:1263–1268. doi: 10.1007/s00439-016-1719-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Boneh A, et al. Clinical, ethical and legal considerations in the treatment of newborns with non-ketotic hyperglycinaemia. Mol. Genet. Metab. 2008;94:143–147. doi: 10.1016/j.ymgme.2008.02.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Dong HL, Fukuda S, Murata E, Zhu Z, Higuchi T. Orexins increase cortical acetylcholine release and electroencephalographic activation through orexin-1 receptor in the rat basal forebrain during isoflurane anesthesia. Anesthesiology. 2006;104:1023–1032. doi: 10.1097/00000542-200605000-00019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Kelz MB, et al. An essential role for orexins in emergence from general anesthesia. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 2008;105:1309–1314. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0707146105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Kushikata T, Yoshida H, Kudo M, Kudo T, Hirota K. Plasma orexin A increases at emergence from sevoflurane-fentanyl anesthesia in patients undergoing ophthalmologic surgery. Neurosci. Lett. 2010;482:212–215. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2010.07.037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Shirasaka T, Yonaha T, Onizuka S, Tsuneyoshi I. Effects of orexin-A on propofol anesthesia in rats. J. Anesth. 2011;25:65–71. doi: 10.1007/s00540-010-1071-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Zhang LN, et al. Orexin-A facilitates emergence from propofol anesthesia in the rat. Anesth. Analg. 2012;115:789–796. doi: 10.1213/ANE.0b013e3182645ea3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Du WJ, et al. The locus coeruleus modulates intravenous general anesthesia of zebrafish via a cooperative mechanism. Cell Rep. 2018;24:3146–3155. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2018.08.046. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Kelz MB, Garcia PS, Mashour GA, Solt K. Escape from oblivion: neural mechanisms of emergence from general anesthesia. Anesth. Analg. 2019;128:726–736. doi: 10.1213/ANE.0000000000004006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Hondo M, Furutani N, Yamasaki M, Watanabe M, Sakurai T. Orexin neurons receive glycinergic innervations. PLoS ONE. 2011;6:e25076. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0025076. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Karnani MM, Venner A, Jensen LT, Fugger L, Burdakov D. Direct and indirect control of orexin/hypocretin neurons by glycine receptors. J. Physiol. 2011;589:639–651. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2010.198457. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Bardoczi Z, et al. Glycinergic input to the mouse basal forebrain cholinergic neurons. J. Neurosci. 2017;37:9534–9549. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3348-16.2017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Mongeon R, et al. Synaptic homeostasis in a zebrafish glial glycine transporter mutant. J. Neurophysiol. 2008;100:1716–1723. doi: 10.1152/jn.90596.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Granato M, et al. Genes controlling and mediating locomotion behavior of the zebrafish embryo and larva. Development. 1996;123:399–413. doi: 10.1242/dev.123.1.399. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Bedell V, et al. Zebrafish: a pharmacogenetic model for anesthesia. Methods Enzymol. 2018;602:189–209. doi: 10.1016/bs.mie.2018.02.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Krasowski MD, et al. General anesthetic potencies of a series of propofol analogs correlate with potency for potentiation of gamma-aminobutyric acid (GABA) current at the GABA(A) receptor but not with lipid solubility. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 2001;297:338–351. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Campagna JA, Miller KW, Forman SA. Mechanisms of actions of inhaled anesthetics. New Engl. J. Med. 2003;348:2110–2124. doi: 10.1056/NEJMra021261. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Randlett O, et al. Whole-brain activity mapping onto a zebrafish brain atlas. Nat. Methods. 2015;12:1039–1046. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.3581. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.http://www.nature.com/nmeth/journal/v12/n11/abs/nmeth.3581.html#supplementary-information (2015).
  • 27.Cui WW, et al. The zebrafish shocked gene encodes a glycine transporter and is essential for the function of early neural circuits in the CNS. J. Neurosci. 2005;25:6610–6620. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5009-04.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Harrington ME. Neurobiological studies of fatigue. Prog. Neurobiol. 2012;99:93–105. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2012.07.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Allee J, Tobias JD. Perioperative care of a child with non-ketotic hyperglycinemia. Saudi J. Anaesth. 2010;4:197–201. doi: 10.4103/1658-354X.71578. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Sneddon LU. Clinical anesthesia and analgesia in fish. J. Exotic Pet. Med. 2012;21:32–43. doi: 10.1053/j.jepm.2011.11.009. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Alkire MT, Hudetz AG, Tononi G. Consciousness and anesthesia. Science. 2008;322:876–880. doi: 10.1126/science.1149213. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Rink E, Wullimann MF. Connections of the ventral telencephalon (subpallium) in the zebrafish (Danio rerio) Brain Res. 2004;1011:206–220. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2004.03.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Herget U, Wolf A, Wullimann MF, Ryu S. Molecular neuroanatomy and chemoarchitecture of the neurosecretory preoptic-hypothalamic area in zebrafish larvae. J. Comput. Neurol. 2014;522:1542–1564. doi: 10.1002/cne.23480. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Moore JT, et al. Direct activation of sleep-promoting VLPO neurons by volatile anesthetics contributes to anesthetic hypnosis. Curr. Biol. 2012;22:2008–2016. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2012.08.042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Jiang-Xie LF, et al. A common neuroendocrine substrate for diverse general anesthetics and sleep. Neuron. 2019 doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2019.03.033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Purdon PL, et al. Electroencephalogram signatures of loss and recovery of consciousness from propofol. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 2013;110:E1142–1151. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1221180110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Philip BK, Kallar SK, Bogetz MS, Scheller MS, Wetchler BV. A multicenter comparison of maintenance and recovery with sevoflurane or isoflurane for adult ambulatory anesthesia. The Sevoflurane Multicenter Ambulatory Group. Anesth. Analg. 1996;83:314–319. doi: 10.1213/00000539-199608000-00019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.August DA, Nguyen HG. Nitrous oxide for glycine encephalopathy. Paediatr. Anaesth. 2010;20:974–976. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9592.2010.03412.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Barker C, Jefferson P, Ball DR. Glycine encephalopathy and anesthesia. Anesth. Analg. 2007;105:544. doi: 10.1213/01.ane.0000265701.64299.92. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Nash HA. In vivo genetics of anaesthetic action. Br. J. Anaesth. 2002;89:143–155. doi: 10.1093/bja/aef159. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Yang X, et al. High-throughput screening in larval zebrafish identifies novel potent sedative-hypnotics. Anesthesiology. 2018;129:459–476. doi: 10.1097/ALN.0000000000002281. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Ehrlich O, et al. Clove oil and AQUI-S efficacy for zebrafish embryo, larva, and adult anesthesia. Zebrafish. 2019 doi: 10.1089/zeb.2019.1737. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Warren KS, Baker K, Fishman MC. The slow mo mutation reduces pacemaker current and heart rate in adult zebrafish. Am. J. Physiol. Heart Circ. Physiol. 2001;281:H1711–1719. doi: 10.1152/ajpheart.2001.281.4.H1711. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Martins T, Diniz E, Felix LM, Antunes L. Evaluation of anaesthetic protocols for laboratory adult zebrafish (Danio rerio) PLoS ONE. 2018;13:e0197846. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0197846. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Kozol RA, et al. Function over form: modeling groups of inherited neurological conditions in zebrafish. Front. Mol. Neurosci. 2016;9:55. doi: 10.3389/fnmol.2016.00055. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Chen A, et al. QRFP and its receptors regulate locomotor activity and sleep in zebrafish. J. Neurosci. 2016;36:1823–1840. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2579-15.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Prober DA, Rihel J, Onah AA, Sung RJ, Schier AF. Hypocretin/orexin overexpression induces an insomnia-like phenotype in zebrafish. J. Neurosci. 2006;26:13400–13410. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4332-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Naumann EA, Kampff AR, Prober DA, Schier AF, Engert F. Monitoring neural activity with bioluminescence during natural behavior. Nat. Neurosci. 2010;13:513–520. doi: 10.1038/nn.2518. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Jay M, De Faveri F, McDearmid JR. Firing dynamics and modulatory actions of supraspinal dopaminergic neurons during zebrafish locomotor behavior. Curr. Biol. 2015;25:435–444. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2014.12.033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Chiu CN, Prober DA. Regulation of zebrafish sleep and arousal states: current and prospective approaches. Front. Neural Circuits. 2013;7:58. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2013.00058. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Reichert S, Pavon Arocas O, Rihel J. The neuropeptide galanin is required for homeostatic rebound sleep following increased neuronal activity. Neuron. 2019;104:370–384. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2019.08.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Saper CB, Fuller PM, Pedersen NP, Lu J, Scammell TE. Sleep state switching. Neuron. 2010;68:1023–1042. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.11.032. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Gallopin T, et al. Identification of sleep-promoting neurons in vitro. Nature. 2000;404:992–995. doi: 10.1038/35010109. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Sherin JE, Elmquist JK, Torrealba F, Saper CB. Innervation of histaminergic tuberomammillary neurons by GABAergic and galaninergic neurons in the ventrolateral preoptic nucleus of the rat. J. Neurosci. 1998;18:4705–4721. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-12-04705.1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Steininger TL, Gong H, McGinty D, Szymusiak R. Subregional organization of preoptic area/anterior hypothalamic projections to arousal-related monoaminergic cell groups. J. Comput. Neurol. 2001;429:638–653. doi: 10.1002/1096-9861(20010122)429:4&#x0003c;638::AID-CNE10&#x0003e;3.0.CO;2-Y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Droogleever Fortuyn HA, et al. Severe fatigue in narcolepsy with cataplexy. J. Sleep Res. 2012;21:163–169. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2011.00943.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Mesa A, Diaz AP, Frosth M. Narcolepsy and anesthesia. Anesthesiology. 2000;92:1194–1196. doi: 10.1097/00000542-200004000-00040. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Cavalcante AN, Hofer RE, Tippmann-Peikert M, Sprung J, Weingarten TN. Perioperative risks of narcolepsy in patients undergoing general anesthesia: a case–control study. J. Clin. Anesth. 2017 doi: 10.1016/j.jclinane.2017.04.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Lin L, et al. The sleep disorder canine narcolepsy is caused by a mutation in the hypocretin (orexin) receptor 2 gene. Cell. 1999;98:365–376. doi: 10.1016/S0092-8674(00)81965-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Mashour GA, Woodrum DT, Avidan MS. Neurological complications of surgery and anaesthesia. Br. J. Anaesth. 2015;114:194–203. doi: 10.1093/bja/aeu296. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Solt K, et al. Methylphenidate actively induces emergence from general anesthesia. Anesthesiology. 2011;115:791–803. doi: 10.1097/ALN.0b013e31822e92e5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials


Articles from Scientific Reports are provided here courtesy of Nature Publishing Group

RESOURCES