Abstract
The rise of large cohort-based health research that includes genetic components has increased the communication challenges for researchers. Controversies have been amplified over requirements for re-consent, return of results, and privacy protections, among other issues. This study extended research on the impact that the perceived role of “research participant” might have on communication expectations to illuminate research participants’ preferences for re-consent. The study employed an on-line survey of participants in a long-standing cancer genetics registry. Results confirmed previous exploratory findings that research participants endorse multiple mental models of participant roles in research (doctor-patient, collaborator, donor, legal contract, etc.). Regression analyses indicated that high and low salience of different models of the role of research participant are related to different communication expectations. However, the pattern of relationships among roles is relevant. The results of the regression analysis also indicated that preference for mandatory re-consent and its relationship to mental models of roles are related to attitudes of trust, benefits, and informational risks. The discussion identifies implications as including the use of explicit approaches to address role relationships in communication with research participants. It also points to implications for methodological approaches in mental model research.
Keywords: Mental Models, Roles, Re-consent, Research Ethics, Health Communication
Research involving human participants often involves communication between researchers and participants. Much of that communication has been made more complex by recent developments. Notably, the increasing expense, complexity, and participant burden of “big data” studies have encouraged re-use of data from existing studies or maintaining registries or cohorts from whom additional data can be requested. The National Institutes of Health have, for example, increased their promotion of data sharing (https://www.nlm.nih.gov/NIHbmic/nih_data_sharing_policies.html). These issues have also been intensified by the Precision Medicine Initiative (https://www.nih.gov/precision-medicine-initiative-cohort-program).
Much of the existing research on these issues has focused on genetic research, in part because of the large social investment in genetics. That research stream has indicated divergence of expectations among researchers, participants, and IRBs on key communication obligations including re-consent and return of results (Bledsoe et al., 2013; Ziniel et al., 2014). The use of on-line communication in these studies has likewise intensified concerns about the protection of privacy of communications.
The process of obtaining informed and autonomous consent for participation is the most common communicative experience required in research involving human subjects. Therefore, this study focuses especially on that process. Past research shows that expectations regarding re-consent diverge between IRBs and researchers (Edwards et al., 2012). The expectations of those groups also appear to differ in some dimensions from opinions of participants (Ludman et al., 2010; Edwards et al., 2016; Edwards et al., under review). Similar divergences seem to exist with regard to the related alternative of “broad” or “blanket” consent (i.e., consent documents that do not specify purposes and limits of use for the data collected from participants; Kelly, Spector, Cherkas, Prainsack, & Harris, 2015; Platt, Bollinger, Dvoskin, Kardia, & Kaufman, 2014). Other potential divergences may occur with regard to the role of consent in medical practice research (Cho et al., 2015).
Such divergences in expectations might be accounted for by differences in the way researchers and participants envision the role of research participant. Role theories of communication hold that communicative expectations arise in part from the intuitive beliefs people have about the character of different social roles (e.g., friendship, parent-child, doctor-patient; Dillard, Solomon, & Palmer, 1999; Dillard, Solomon, & Samp, 1996; Fiske, 1992; Koerner, 2006; Solomon, Dillard, & Anderson, 2002). These theories and previous research warrant the present investigation of whether participants might hold models of the relationship between researchers and participants that might lead to different communicative expectations. Additionally, the research literature in both human subjects ethics and on role relationships in health care warrant specific inclusion of trust and risk-benefit as potential variables affecting the impact of such models.
Roles and Health Communication Expectations
Studies outlining expectations for the roles of physicians and patients have been on-going for decades (Adams, Parrott, & Segall, 1976; Berkanovic, 1972; Hebdon, Fahnestock, & McComb, 2014; Stone, 1979). As articulated by Larsen and Rootman (1976), role theory holds that “a role consists of a set of behavioral expectations, which contain a normative (ought or should) component, applied to an incumbent of a particular social position” (p. 29). Research assessing the communicative interactions between physicians and health care providers has shown that role expectations have measurable impacts. Larsen & Rootman (1976), for example, showed that patient satisfaction increases when physicians fulfill patient’s role expectations. Of particular interest given the unfamiliarity of the role of “research participant” to many people, the research stream has also indicated that “role ambiguity increases the probability that a person will be dissatisfied with a role” (Adams & Parrott, 1994, p. 39). Additional research has indicated a lack of convergence in role expectations between patients and health care providers (Cichon & Masterson, 1993). To pursue further people’s perceptions of roles in research, we turned to the theoretical and methodological framework of mental models research.
Mental Models Theory and Roles
Morgan, Fischhoff, Bostrom, and Atman (2002) introduced research on mental models in order to improve success in communicating expert knowledge to lay people. They argued that it was as important to understand what people already believed as it was to outline clearly what experts needed to communicate. This was true because people’s existing “mental models” shaped their predispositions to accept and understand the models offered by experts. Mental models are considered to consist of representations of a context (including key actors, scenic factors, available tools, etc.). Research in specific contexts generally has focused on those aspects of mental representations that account for behavioral choices. Mental models of lay individuals in multiple health contexts have been examined (Anderson et al., 2005; Byram, Fischoff, Embry, de Bruin & Thorne, 2001; Damman & Timmermans, 2012; Henderson & Maguire, 2000; Kealey & Berkman, 2010). Some of this research has focused on the models shared among teams of health experts (Burtscher, Kolbe, Wacker, & Manser, 2011; Biemann, Ellwart, & Rack, 2014).
Candidates for mental models for this research project were initially derived from prescriptive studies that suggested that the researcher-participant relationship should be analogized to specific familiar roles. Common recommendations among medical researchers have been to suggest that research participants should employ the “donor” model (Clayton & McGuire, 2012; Dressler et al., 2012; Miller, Mello, & Joffe, 2008). Common among IRBs and ethicists has been a preference for the “legal contract” (Bredenoord, Onland-Moret, & Van Delden, 2011; Zawati & Rioux, 2012) or “collaborative” models (Beskow, Burke, Fullerton, & Sharp, 2012; Murphy, Scott, Kaufman, Geller, LeRoy, & Hudson, 2008). There was also evidence that among research participants, the “doctor-patient” or “altruistic” models might be operative (Hunter, Corcoran, Leeder & Phelps, 2012; Kelley et al., 2015; Michie, Henderson, Garrett, & Corbie-Smith, 2011; Ruiz-Canela, Valle-Mansilla, & Sulmasy, 2011).
In an exploratory interview-based study, Condit and colleagues (2015) found that rather than selecting a single one of these mental models (i.e., “gift,” “doctor-patient,” “legal”, “collaborator”) to form the expectations for the role of research participant, interviewees accepted several different familiar models as applicable. In the face of the availability of several models, it was unclear how and whether mental models would guide communication expectations. It was possible that in the absence of a strong, clear or singular norm, lay participants in research effectively have no governing role expectations for communication. Given the research stream in attitude accessibility, a more likely possibility was that a particularly salient model might shape the communication (Higgins, 1996; Bargh, Lombardi, & Higgins, 1988). Activation of one model might have facilitated and/or resulted in activation of other similar mental models through an associative process (cf. Taylor & Crocker, 1981). Alternatively, there might have been synergistic effects among multiple models. We therefore framed the following hypothesis: where roles endorsed by participants are important and salient, those roles will be associated with specific communicative expectations, even in the face of the participants’ recognition of other models for the role. Because of the importance of informed consent (as described above), and because of its clarity as a communicative expectation, we employed support of mandatory policies for re-consent as our operational measure of a concrete communicative expectation.
Trust, and Risk-Benefit in Medical Genetics Practice & Research
Trust and risk-benefit trade-offs are a significant component of the requirements for informed consent (Erlich et al., 2014; Hill, Turner, Martin, & Donovan, 2013; Picillo, Kou, Barone, & Fasano, 2015). They also play a prominent role in the literature on health related outcomes (Berry et al., 2008; Gabay, 2015; Hillen, de Haes and Smets, 2011; Jauffret-Roustide et al., 2012; Lee & Lin, 2009). Therefore, trust and risk-benefit should be actively considered in any effort to develop a model of role expectations for genetic research. Empirical studies had begun to identify the impact of trust on research participation (Gao et al., 2014; Hatfield & Pearce, 2014), including the existence of a correlation between trust and enrollment in a National Cancer Genetics Registry (Skinner et al. 2008). Some studies had addressed trust at the institutional level (Overby, et al., 2015). However, most research has emphasized trust at the interpersonal level, and hence as an aspect of a social role (as illustrated by the mapping of 596 empirical papers on trust in the health-care setting by Brennan and colleagues, 2013). These studies pointed up the possibility that trust may be important both in the conceptualization or choice of specific models of roles for research participation and in enrollment. However, there had been no empirical research assessing their relationship to specific roles or preferences for re-consent in research. Trust in the researcher and the research institution was therefore included as an exploratory variable in the analysis.
Risk/Benefit
Because of the essentially probabilistic nature of most genetic information, concerns with the balance of benefits and risks (or harms) have been a long-standing focus of studies about research in genetic medicine. Recent empirical studies have confirmed that participants’ perceptions of risks (or harms) and benefits play a substantial role in decisions about research participation (Christensen et al., 2015; Gollust et al., 2011; Hatfield & Pearce, 2014; Jamal et al., 2014; Wood, Kowalczuk, Elwyn, Mitchell, & Gallacher, 2011; Tarrant et al., 2015). Perceived benefit and risks were therefore included as exploratory variables in a research question framed as follows: What are the relationships among role, trust, benefit, risk, and communication expectations? A scale based in perceived information risks was employed to operationalize trust, because information risks are of potentially greatest salience in large research cohorts (Hawkins, 2010; Homer et al., 2008; McGuire, et al., 2011),
Method
Participants
Participants were recruited for an on-line survey from a long-standing genetics cancer registry. The Northwest Cancer Genetics Registry (NWCGR) was established in 2010 as the continuation of the Northwest Cancer Genetics Network (NWCGN) established in 1998 as one of the eight original sites of the Cancer Genetics Network, funded by the National Cancer Institute to provide centers specializing in the study of inherited predispositions to cancer. The NWCGR includes as recruited cases persons with cancer (n=2027), family members with and without cancer (n=451), a population-based comparison sample (n=527), and a self-referral group (n=340 with cancer of 904), and their relatives (n=464). Participants in the Registry will have had experience at least with the registry core data research questionnaires and subsequent follow-ups, and may have had additional research participation through the NWCGN and outside of it.
Members of the NWCGR (n=3352) were recruited for the present study via letters sent by US mail. Up to two follow-up letters were sent at approximately two-week intervals. The study was reviewed and approved by the University of Washington’s Human Subjects Division, and by the University of California, Irvine Institutional Review Board. All participants provided informed consent prior to participation.
A total of 450 participants completed the survey (Age: M = 63.64 years, SD=11.81). There were more females (64.9%) than males. Ninety-four percent identified themselves as white, 3.3% as multi-racial, 1.6% as Asian, and 0.9% as African American, and one participant not answering. Regarding education, 61.3% are with bachelor degrees, 23.8% had some college, 8.9% with high school or less, and 6.0% failed to disclose their education level.
Survey Development
The survey was designed to document the concerns of potential research participants with regard to human genetics in a manner that maximized the possibility for comparison with previously reported data gathered from researchers and IRB professionals (the “GRIPP” survey; Edwards et al., 2012). The survey was developed through a multi-stage process. First, 31 individuals participated in qualitative interviews (described in Condit et al., 2015). These interviews were used 1) to format or word those questions aligned with the GRIPP survey in language intelligible to research participants, and 2) to identify new topic areas and sub-topics of specific concern to research participants. Changes based on the qualitative survey included, for example, a shift to “donor” from “gift” based on participant explanations and recommendations, and the addition of a distinction in the goals of “collaboration” (whether focused on economically valuable products or health care).
Preliminary versions of the present survey were reviewed by internal and external experts. Changes were made to enhance clarity after these reviews and after cognitive interviews with 37 eligible participants. Final pilot testing on the web platform was used to assess technical difficulties and completion time of the survey.
Survey Features
The final instrument had 22 questions in six topic areas, including factors affecting decisions to participate in research, elements of relationships between participants and researchers (the focus of the current report), re-consent and blanket consent, return of results, privacy, and family communication relating to health issues.
The survey was delivered through Catalyst, a software developed by the University of Washington. Survey confidentiality was established by employing a unique participation number for each participant and an individual URL to access the survey. Links to individual surveys were retained to enable the use of existing demographic and other participation information and thus reduce the survey length for participants.
Measures
Benefit.
Participants reported how important the following were to them when making decisions to give consent to participation on a 1–4 scale, where 1 = “very important”, 2 = “somewhat important”, 3 = “not very important”, and 4 = “not at all important.” The items were: “the value of incentive from participation”; “the research must be meaningful to me personally”; “the research will improve my health”; “the research will provide information I can use to improve my health”; “the research will provide information about me I didn’t know”; “the research can benefit my family”; and “the research could benefit people I know.” The items were reverse coded and then averaged into a composite score (α =.83) such that higher scores indexed more benefits.
Trust.
Participants reported how important (1 = “very important”, 2 = “somewhat important”, 3 = “not very important”, and 4 = “not at all important”) each of the following were when asked to provide health information about themselves and a biological sample. The items were: “the reputation of the research institution or researcher”; “the researcher is trustworthy”; “the institution where the research is being conducted is trustworthy”; and “data and sample are stored securely.” The items were reverse coded and then averaged into a composite score (α =.60) such that higher scores reflected higher levels of trust.
Risk.
Participants estimated the likelihood of certain events occurring in genetic research studies in the next five years (1 = “very likely”, 2= “somewhat likely”, 3= “neither likely nor unlikely”, 4=“somewhat unlikely”, and 5= “very unlikely”): “A research participant would be personally identified in a study involving de-identified data by someone other than the researchers”, “ A research participant would be harmed as a result of identification from de-identified genetic data”, and “a federal agency or other law-enforcement agency might compel researchers to disclose information about genetic research participants.” The items were reverse coded and then averaged into a composite score (α =.81) such that higher scores meant more severe risk.
Roles.
Roles were measured by single items. Participants reported their opinions about what the relationship between genetic researchers and participants should be like (1 = “strongly agree”, 2 = “somewhat agree”, 3= “somewhat disagree”, and 4 = “strongly disagree”). The relationship statements were: “collaborators working together to make scientific discoveries to improve health”; “a legal contract between two parties”; “a doctor-patient relationship”; “collaborators working together to produce commercial health products”; “participants making a donation to the researcher’s work”, and “a personal relationship.” These variables were also reverse coded such that higher scores reflected more salient roles.
Mandatory re-consent.
Participants reported their opinion regarding a national policy that would make it mandatory for researchers to re-contact participants and ask for permission again in each of the following situations (1 = “strongly agree”, 2= “somewhat agree”, 3= “neither agree nor disagree”, and 4= “somewhat disagree” 5=”strongly disagree”): when the researcher wants “to investigate a different, but related health condition”, “to investigate an unrelated health condition or cancer”, “to look at genetic factors in a study that did not originally include those genetic factors”, “to share samples or data with an investigator at another institution”, “the original consent was given by the parents of a child, and the child is not old enough to decide for him or herself”. These items were reverse coded and then averaged into a composite score (α =.92) such that higher scores indicated stronger opinion that re-consent should be mandatory.
Data Analysis Strategy
To test the hypothesis (i.e., that salient and important models of roles predict preferences for re-consent, even in the face of the participants’ recognition of multiple models) and to answer the research question (i.e., What are the relationships among role, trust, benefit, risk, and preferences for re-consent?), a series of hierarchical linear regression analyses were conducted to predict re-consent: age, gender and education were entered in the first block, and the role variables were entered in the second block. The regression model was estimated first with the overall data, and then when each of the roles were salient (strongly agree and somewhat agree), and when non-salient (strongly disagree and somewhat disagree). With these parameters, the sample size of 450 yielded statistical power of about .56 to detect an effect size equivalent to r =.10 when α < .05. To enhance statistical power, and given the relative exploratory nature of this study, the significance level was adjusted to α < .10.
Results
Table 1 presents the descriptive statistics, which indicate that participants endorse multiple roles. Table 2 presents the zero order correlation matrix among the role variables.
Table 1:
1 Strongly Agree % (N) | 2 Somewhat Agree % (N) | 3 Somewhat Disagree % (N) | 4 Strongly Disagree % (N) | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Collaborators working together to make scientific discoveries to improve health | 57% (256) | 32% (143) | 8% (36) | 3% (13) |
A doctor-patient relationship | 42% (185) | 35% (154) | 17% (73) | 7% (29) |
Participants making a donation to the researcher’s work | 32% (142) | 42% (185) | 16% (72) | 10% (44) |
A legal contract between two parties | 24% (104) | 48% (210) | 19% (85) | 9% (39) |
A relationship between an individual and a community | 23% (98) | 54% (233) | 17% (72) | 7% (32) |
Collaborators working together to produce commercial health products | 16% (71) | 32% (143) | 30% (134) | 21% (93) |
A personal relationship | 9% (40) | 27% (121) | 31% (138) | 33% (145) |
Note: Percentages are rounded to nearest whole number. Raw number totals are different because participants were free to skip questions.
Table 2:
1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1. Scientific collaborator | 1.0 | ||||||
2. legal contract | .23 | 1.0 | |||||
3. Doctor-patient | .36 | .34 | 1.0 | ||||
4. Commercial partner | .40 | .30 | .43 | 1.0 | |||
5. Donation | .08a | .09a | .09a | .18 | 1.0 | ||
6. Personal | .39 | .33 | .49 | .49 | .17 | 1.0 | |
7. Community | .21 | .19 | .14 | .15 | .21 | .26 | 1.0 |
Note: all correlations significant at p <.01 except.
The Impact of Demographics
Table 3 presents the results from the regression analyses. There was impact from demographic variables overall. While there was no impact from education, there was impact from age (β =−.10, p =.06), which means that older participants were less likely to believe that re-consent should be mandatory. There was no gender effect (female coded as “0” and male coded as “1”) overall, but gender had a positive impact when the following roles were not salient: doctor-patient, donation, legal contract, and personal relationship. The negative coefficients (ranging from −.14 to −.22) indicated that females were more likely than males to believe that re-consent should be mandatory than were males.
Table 3:
Predictor | Overall | Scientific Collaborator | Doctor-patient | Donation | Legal contract | Community | Commercial collaborator | Personal | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
High | Low | High | Low | High | Low | High | Low | High | Low | High | Low | High | Low | ||
Age | −.10 (.06) | − | −.29 (.09) | −.10 (.08) | - | - | - | - | - | - | −.24 (.02) | - | - | - | −.14 (.03) |
Gender | - | - | - | - | −.22 (.049) | - | −.21 (.04) | - | −.22 (.04) | - | - | - | - | - | −.14 (.04) |
Benefit | .16 (.01) | .13 (.04) | - | .14 (.04) | - | .17 (.03) | - | .16 (.04) | - | .12 (.08) | - | .15 (.07) | - | .21 (.04) | .15 (.06) |
Trust | .13 (.03) | .11 (.08) | - | .12 (.07) | - | - | - | .12 (.08) | - | .14 (.04) | - | - | .21 (.02) | - | .16 (.03) |
Risk | .15 (.003) | .14 (.01) | - | .16 (.006) | - | .17 (.006) | - | .14 (.02) | .22 (.03) | .16 (.007) | - | .17 (.02) | .15 (.06) | .17 (.05) | .12 (.06) |
Doctor-patient | - | - | - | - | - | −.13 (.07) | - | −.13 (.07) | - | −.19 (.01) | - | - | - | - | - |
Legal contract | .09 (.095) | - | - | - | .22 (.057) | - | - | - | - | - | - | .17 (.04) | - | - | - |
Commercial collaborator | -.12 (.07) | -.14 (.04) | - | - | - | - | - | - | −.20 (.06) | - | - | - | - | - | - |
Personal | .15 (.02) | .16 (.02) | - | .16 (.02) | - | - | .27 (.02) | .14 (.07) | - | .16 (.04) | - | .22 (.02) | - | .24 (.01) | - |
Notes:
Non-significant coefficients suppressed for the sake of clarity.
Education, donation and community did not have any significant effect, hence, dropped from the table.
p-values in parentheses.
The Impact from Benefit, Trust, & Risk
The group of benefit (β =.16, p =.01), trust (β =.13, p =.03), risk (β = .15, p =.003) had robust effects on re-consent preferences. The positive coefficients mean that 1) the more benefits the participants believed there were, 2) the greater the trust they had for the institution and researcher, and 3) the more risk of identification they perceived in genetic research, the more likely they were to agree that re-consent should be mandatory. In other words, the higher the perceived stake, the more likely they were to agree that re-consent should be mandatory. Notably, the influence on participants’ opinion regarding mandatory re-consent was inhibited when the roles were not salient, particularly for scientific collaborator, doctor-patient relationship, donation, and community relationship. This is consistent with the notion that where there is an absence of a strong, clear or singular norm, lay participants in research in effect have no governing role expectations for communication.
The Impact of Roles
Except for donation and community relationship, the other roles all had impact on the opinion of whether re-consent should be mandatory. Personal relationship had the most robust impact: It had an overall positive effect (β =.15, p =.02) and such an effect was most salient when other goals were salient, with donation as the only exception: When the donation role was most salient, the role of personal relationship no longer had any impact; on the other hand, when the donation role was not activated, personal relationship enhanced likelihood of believing that re-consent should be mandatory. Legal contract had a positive, overall effect (β =.09, p =.095). Such effects were manifest mainly when the roles of donation and commercial collaboration were activated and salient. The negative coefficient (β = −.12, p =.07) suggested that the role of commercial collaboration reduces the opinion that re-consent should be mandatory, especially when the expectations of scientific collaboration and legal contract were dominant. The expectation of doctor-patient relationship did not have an overall effect, but it tended to mitigate the preference for mandatory re-consent, especially when the norms of donation, legal contract, and community relationship were at play.
Combined with the mutual correlations among the roles (Table 2), these results showed that it was probably rare that roles functioned by themselves; rather they formed an associative network: 1) the majority of roles had significant and substantial correlations; 2) the roles that did not correlate (i.e., donation) or had low correlations (i.e., community) with other roles did not have any impact on re-consent preference; and 3) most importantly, the impact from roles on re-consent preference decision making was most pronounced when multiple roles are activated and salient.
Discussion
This project added survey-based support to previous interview-based findings that research participants do not employ a singular role as an analogy for research participation, but rather endorse multiple analogies. The results further indicated that particularly high or low salience roles nonetheless predicted communication expectations, specifically the expectation for re-consent. Third, the results indicated that trust and assessments of risk and benefit play a role in participants’ expectations and are related to some role expectations, but the relationships are not homogeneous across roles.
The generalizability of these results is limited by the profile of these respondents, who are older and less ethnically diverse than the general population. As members of a cancer genetics registry, they bring more research participation experience to bear than would people who are new to research participation.
Although this study has not resolved the issue, it has provided substantial grounds to justify additional research that would move beyond the exploratory stage. In addition to including other types of participants, next stage research will need to develop instruments for measuring role perceptions that are based on scales rather than single questions. That research stage may also profit from integrating the above results with the approaches to roles embodied in relational framing theory and “working models” theories.
Pursuing this line of research would be valuable because these findings have implications for policies related to re-consent in large cohort studies that examine the role of genetics in medical outcomes. The findings that there are multiple role models employed by research participants for the research relationship mean that researchers cannot simply take for granted a singular model for the research participant role and therefore for the researcher-participant relationship. However, our findings also suggested that these role expectations should not be ignored because they have concrete implications for communication expectations. Research by Adams and Parrott (1994) offers a potential resolution of this challenge. Their experimental results showed that formal communication related to roles (in their case articulated as rules) increased the satisfaction of nurses and patients and reduced role ambiguity. This suggests that explicit address of role relationships in consent, recruitment, or other materials framing research participation in large cohort studies such as the Precision Medicine Initiative might be productive. Empirical research to confirm the added value of such explicit address of roles in communication with research participants seems warranted.
These results also extend and support developments in mental models theory that emphasize the creative and emergent character of mental models (Roskos-Ewoldsen, Davies, & Roskos-Ewoldsen, 2004; Zhou & Wang, 2010), especially as related to role theory. The results indicate that, even in the presence of multiple models recognized by participants as relevant, models of particularly high or low salience may inform communication expectations. The finding that the impact was least when roles were least correlated with other roles and that impact was greater with higher activation of multiple roles further suggests the importance of methodological approaches to mental models research that seek to assess the presence of multiple roles and trace their interactions rather than seeking to identify a single dominant mental representation.
The exploratory results have also pointed to the role of salience-guiding variables (in this case, trust and risk/benefit) as involved in the selection of a particular mental model and/or its expectations. Because this exploratory work cannot definitively establish the causal directions of the relationships among these variables, further research is needed to assess these dynamics. However, these results do suggest that in addition to the general variables that influence role expectations across contexts as specified by relational framing theory (dominance/affiliation) or working models of relationship theory (communal sharing, authority ranking, equality matching, and market pricing), adding more role variables specific to particular relational contexts (e.g., risk/benefit, trust) may add additional explanatory power for particular types of roles (such as the health context or research context).
Finally, these results add further stimulation to on-going ethical debates. There is a robust discussion of which consent approaches are most appropriate for biobank research (Brown, Drake, Gehlert, et al., 2016; Ewing, Erby, Bollinger, Tetteyfio, Ricks-Santi, & Kaufman, 2015; Grady, Eckstein, & Berkman, 2015; Tindana & de Vries, 2016). Frameworks include blanket or broad consent, sometimes in the context of opting in or opting out, as well as tiered consent, in which a participant indicates which types of additional research would be acceptable (Garrison, Sathe, Antommaria, et al., 2015). Processes for re-consent may be seen by some as one way to fill gaps left by the difficulties involved in projecting the range of future information that may become relevant for evolving biobank usages, especially in the context of increasing roles for the internet in research processes (Kaye, Curren, Anderson, et al., 2012; Trinidad, Fullerton, Bares, Jarvik, Larson, Burke, 2012). The present research results raise the intriguing possibility that understanding more about how both researchers and participants frame the role of “participant” might better inform the presentation of consent and re-consent options to participants. Research to explore how different formats and depths of communicative interactions are affected by and, in a spiral fashion, may affect particular role perceptions, might be both interesting and useful.
Acknowledgements:
The authors acknowledge and thank Lesley Pfeifer, Anne Renz, Joan Scott and David Kaufmann for their work contributing to the earlier stages of this project. They thank Jennifer Samp for her advice on the literature on role theory in communication studies. This research was supported by NIH grant# R01CA149051 to Karen Edwards (PI), “Identification of Issues and Expectations of Subjects Participating in Genetic Studies of Cancer”.
Contributor Information
Celeste M. Condit, Distinguished Research Professor, Department of Communication, 610 Caldwell Hall, University of Georgia, Athens, GA 30602
Lijiang Shen, Department of Communication Arts and Sciences, 200 Sparks Building University Park, PA, 16802.
Karen L. Edwards, Department of Epidemiology, Rm 224 Irvine Hall, School of Medicine, 1001 Health Science Rd, University of California, Irvine, Irvine CA, 92697-7550.
Deborah J. Bowen, Department of Bioethics and Humanities, 1107 NE 45th St, Suite 305, University of Washington, Seattle, WA 98195
Diane M. Korngiebel, Biomedical Informatics and Medical Education, Box 357240, University of Washington, Seattle, WA 98195
Catherine O. Johnson, Department of Epidemiology, Rm 224 Irvine Hall, School of Medicine, 1001 Health Science Rd, University of California, Irvine, Irvine CA, 92697-7550
References
- Adams RJ & Parrott R. (1994). Pediatric nurses’ communication of role expectations to parents of hospitalized children. Journal of Applied Communication Research, 22, 36–47. [Google Scholar]
- Anderson RA, Ammarell N, Bailey D Jr., Colón-Emeric C, Corazzini KN, Lillie M, Piven MLS, Utley-Smith Q, McDaniel RR Jr. (2005). Nurse assistant mental models, sensemaking, care actions, and consequences for nursing home residents. Qualitative Health Research, 15, 1006–1021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bargh JA, Lombardi WJ, & Higgins ET (1988). Automaticity of Person x Situation effects on impression formation: It’s just a matter of time. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 599–605. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berkanovic E. (1972). Lay conceptions of the sick role. Social Forces, 51, 53–64. [Google Scholar]
- Beskow LM, Burke W, Fullerton SM, & Sharp RR (2012). Offering aggregate results to participants in genomic research: opportunities and challenges. Genetics in Medicine, 14(4), 490–496. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berry LL, Parrish JT, Janakiraman R, Ogburn-Russell L, Couchman GR.., Rayburn WL, Grisel J. (2008). Patients’ commitment to their primary physician and why it matters. Annals of Family Medicine, 6, 6–13. doi: 10.1370/afm.757. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Biemann T, Ellwart T, & Rack O. (2014). Quantifying similarity of team mental models: An introduction of the rRG index. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 17(1), 125–140. doi: 10.1177/1368430213485993 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Bledsoe MJ, Clayton EW, McGuire AL, Grizzle WE, O’Rourke PP, Zeps N. (2013). Return of research results from genomic biobanks: Cost matters. Genetics In Medicine: Official Journal Of The American College Of Medical Genetics, 15(2), 103–5. doi: 10.1038/gim.2012.105 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bredenoord AL, Onland-Moret NC, & Van Delden JJM (2011). Feedback of individual genetic results to research participants: In favor of a qualified disclosure policy. Human Mutation, 32(8), 861–867. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brennan N, Barnes R, Calnan M, Corrigan O, Dippe P & Entwistle V. (2013). Trust in the health-care provider-patient relationship: a systematic mapping review of the evidence base. International Journal for Quality in Health Care, 25, 682–688. doi: 10.1093/intqhc/mzt063 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brown KM, Drake BF, Gehlert S, Wolf LE, DuBois J, Seo J, Woodward KL, , Perkins H, Goodman MS, Kaphingst KA (2016). Differences in preferences for models of consent for biobanks between Black and White women. Journal of Community Genetics,7(1), 41–49. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burtscher MJ, Kolbe M, Wacker J, Manser T. (2011). Interactions of team mental models and monitoring behaviors predict team performance in simulated anesthesia inductions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 17(3), 257–269. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Byram S, Fischhoff B, Embrey M, de Bruin WB, & Thorne S. (2001). Mental models of women with breast implants: Local complications. Behavioral Medicine, 27, 4–15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cho MK, Magnus D, Constantine M, Lee SS, Kelley M, Alessi S, Korngiebel D, James C, Kuwana E, Gallagher TH, Diekema D, Capron AM, Joffe S, Wilfond BS (2015). Attitudes toward risk and informed consent for research on medical practices: A cross-sectional survey. Annals of Internal Medicine, 19;162(10),731–2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Christensen KD, Roberts JS, Zikmund-Fisher BJ, Kardia SLR, McBride CM, Linnenbringer E, Green RC, & for the REVEAL Study Group. (2015). Associations between self-referral and health behavior responses to genetic risk information. Genome Medicine, 7, 10. doi 10.1186/s13073-014-0124-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cichon EJ, & Masterson JT (1993). Physician-patient communication: Mutual role expectations. Communication Quarterly, 41(4), 477–489. [Google Scholar]
- Clayton EW, & McGuire AL (2012). The legal risks of returning results of genomics research. Genetics in Medicine, 14(4), 473–477. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Condit CM, Korngiebel DM, Pfeifer M, Renz AD, Bowen DJ, Kaufman D, Mercer Kollar LM, & Edwards K. (2015). What should be the character of the researcher-participant relationship? Views of participants in a longstanding cancer genetic registry. IRB: Ethics & Human Research, 37(4), 1–10. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cordon CG (1966). Role theory and illness: A sociological perspective. New Haven, CN: College & University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Damman OC, & Timmermans DRM (2012). Educating health consumers about cardio-metabolic health risk: What can we learn from lay mental models of risk? Patient Education and Counseling, 89, 300–308. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dillard JP, Solomon DH, & Palmer MT (1999). Structuring the concept of relational communication. Communication Monographs, 66, 49–65. [Google Scholar]
- Dillard JP, Solomon DH, & Samp JA (1996). Framing social reality: The relevance of relational judgments. Communication Research, 23, 703–723. [Google Scholar]
- Dressler LG, Smolek S, Ponsaran R, Markey JM, Starks H, Gerson N, & Consortium G. (2012). IRB perspectives on the return of individual results from genomic research. Genetics in Medicine, 14(2), 215–222. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Edwards KL, Korngiebel DM, Pfeifer L, Goodman D, Renz A, Wenzel L, Bowen DJ, Condit CM (2016). Participant views on consent in cancer genetics research: preparing for the precision medicine era. Journal of Community Genetics, 7(2),133–43. doi: 10.1007/s12687-015-0259-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Edwards KL, Lemke AA, Trinidad SB, Lewis SM, Starks H, Snapinn KW, Griffin MQ, Wiesner GL, Burke W, & the GRRIP Consortium. (2012). Genetics researchers’ and IRB professionals’ attitudes toward genetic research review: a comparative analysis. Genetics in Medicine, 14(2), 236–242. doi: 10.1038/gim.2011.57 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Erlich Y, Williams JB, Glazer D, Yocum K, Farahany N, Olson M, Narayanan A, Stein LD, Witkowski JA, & Kain RC (2014). Redefining genomic privacy: Trust and empowerment. PLOS Biology, 12(11) e1001983. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ewing AT, Erby LA, Bollinger J, Tetteyfio E, Ricks-Santi LJ, Kaufman D. (2015). Demographic differences in willingness to provide broad and narrow consent for biobank research. Biopreservation and Biobanking, 13(2), 98–106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fiske AP (1992). The four elementary forms of sociality: Framework for a unified theory of social relations. Psychological Review, 689–723. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gabay G. (2015). Perceived control over health, communication and patient-physician trust. Patient Education and Counseling, 98, 1550–1557. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gao W, Ma GX, Tan Y, Fang C, Weaver J, Jin M, Lai P. (2014). Factors associated with willingness to participate in biospecimen research among Chinese Americans. Biopreservation and Biobanking, 12(2), 131–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Garrison NA, Sathe NA, Antommaria AHM, Holm IA, Sanderson SC, Smith ME, McPheeters ML, & Clayton EW (2015). A systematic literature review of individuals’ perspectives on broad consent and data sharing in the United States, Genetics in Medicine, published online 19 November 2015. doi: 10.1038/gim.2015.138 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gollust SE, Gordon ES, Zayac C, Griffin G, Christman MF, Pyeritz RE, Wawak L, & Bernhardt BA (2012). Motivations and perceptions of early adopters of personalized genomics: Perspectives from research participants. Public Health Genomics, 15, 22–30. doi: 10.1159/000327296 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grady C, Eckstein L, Berkman B, Brock D, Cook-Deegan R, Fullerton SM, Greely H, Hansson MG, Hull S, Kim S Lo B, Pentz R, Rodriguez L, Well C, Wilfond BS, & Wendler D. (2015). Broad consent for research with biological samples: Workshop Conclusions. The American Journal of Bioethics : AJOB. 15(9), 34–42. DOI: 10.1080/15265161.2015.1062162 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hatfield LA, & Pearce MM (2014). Factors influencing parents’ decision to donate their healthy infant’s DNA for minimal-risk genetic research. Journal of Nursing Scholarship, 46 (6), 398–407. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hawkins AK (2010). Biobanks; Importance, implications and opportunities for genetic counselors. Journal of Genetic Counseling, 19, 423–429. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hebdon M, Fahnestock O, & McComb S. (2014). Shared mental models of patients, oncology providers, and primary care providers regarding roles in cancer survivorship care. Western Journal of Nursing Research, 36(10), 1384–1385. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Henderson BJ, & Maguire BT (2000). Three lay mental models of disease inheritance. Social Science and Medicine, 50, 293–301. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Higgins ET (1996). Knowledge activation: Accessibility, applicability, and salience In Higgins ET & Kruglanski AW (Eds.). Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles (pp. 133–168). New York, NY: Guilford Press. [Google Scholar]
- Hill EM, Turner EL, Martin RM, & Donovan JL (2013). “Let’s get the best quality research we can”: Public awareness and acceptance of consent to use existing data in health research: A systematic review and qualitative study. BMC Medical Research Methodology, 13 (72), http://www.biomedcentral.com/1471-2288/13/72 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hillen MA, de Haes HCJM, & Smets EMA (2011). Cancer patients’ trust in their physician-a review. Psycho-Oncology, 20, 277–241. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Homer N, Szelinger S, Redman M, Duggan D, Tembe W, Muehling J, Pearson JV, Stephan DA, Nelson SF, Craig DW (2008). Resolving individuals contributing trace amounts of DNA to highly complex mixtures using high-density SNP genotyping microarrays. PLOS Genetics, 4. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.1000167 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hunter J, Corcoran K, Leeder S, & Phelps K. (2012). Appealing to altruism is not enough: Motivators for participating in health services research. Journal of Empirical Research on Human Research Ethics, 7 (3), 84–90. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jamal L, Sapp JC, Lewis K, Yanes T, Facio FM, Biesecker LG, Biesecker BB (2014). Research participants’ attitudes towards the confidentiality of genomic sequence information. European Journal of Human Genetics, 22, 964–968. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jauffret-Roustide M, Cohen J, Poisot-Martin I, Spire B, Gossop M, Carrieri MP, & the Manife 2000 Study Group, (2012). Distributive sharing among HIV-HCV co-infected injecting drug users: The preventive role of trust in one’s physician. AIDS Care, 24, 232–238. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaye J, Curren L, Anderson N, Edwards K, Fullerton SM, Kanelopoulou N, Lund D, MacArthur DG, Mascalzoni D, Shepherd J, Taylor PL, Terry SF, & Winter SF (2012). From patients to partners: participant-centric initiatives in biomedical research. Nature Reviews. Genetics, 13(5), 371–376. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kealey E, & Berkman CS (2010). The relationship between health information sources and mental models of cancer: Findings from the 2005 Health Information National Trends Survey. Journal of Health Communication, 15(Suppl 3), 236–251. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kelley M, James C, Alessi Kraft S, Korngiebel D, Wijangco I, Rosenthal E Joffe S, Cho MK, Wilfond B, & Lee SS (2015). Patient perspectives on the Learning Health System: The importance of trust and shared decision making. American Journal of Bioethics, 15(9):4–17. doi: 10.1080/15265161.2015.1062163. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kelly SE, Spector TD, Cherkas LF, Prainsack B, & Harris JM (2015). Evaluating the consent preferences of UK research volunteers for genetic and clinical studies. PLoS ONE, 10, e0118027. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0118027 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koerner AF (2006). Models of relating—not relationship models: Cognitive representations of relating across interpersonal relationship domains. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 23, 629–653. [Google Scholar]
- Larsen DE, & Rootman L (1976). Physician role performance and patient satisfaction. Social Science and Medicine, 10, 29–32. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee Y-Y, & Lin JL (2009). The effects of trust in physician on self-efficacy, adherence and diabetes outcomes. Social Science & Medicine, 68,1060–1068. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ludman EJ, Fullerton SM, Spangler L, Trinidad SB, Fujii MM, Jarvik GP, Larson EB, & Burke W. (2010). Glad you asked: Participants’ opinions of re-consent for dbGap Data Submission. Journal of Empirical Research on Human Research Ethics: An International Journal, 5 (3), 9–16. doi: 10.1525/jer.2010.5.3.9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Michie M, Henderson G, Garrett J, & Corbie-Smith G. (2011). “If I could in a small way help”: Motivations for and beliefs about sample donation for genetic research. Journal of Empirical Research on Human Research Ethics, 6 (2), 57–70. DOI: 10.1525/jer.2011.6.2.57 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McGuire AL, Basford M, Dressler LG, Fullerton SM, Koenig BA, Li R, McCarty CA, Ramos E, Smith ME, Somkin CP, Waudby C, Wolf WA, & Clayton EW (2011). Ethical and practical challenges of sharing data from genome-wide association studies: the eMERGE Consortium experience. Genome Research, 21, 1001–1007. doi: 10.1101/gr.120329.111 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miller FG, Mello MM & Joffe S. (2008). Incidental findings in human subjects research: What do investigators owe research participants? Journal of Law, Medicine, and Ethics, 36 (2), 271–279. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morgan MG, Fischhoff B, Bostrom A, & Atman CJ (2002). Risk communication. A mental models approach. New York: Cambridge University Press; 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Murphy J, Scott J, Kaufman D, Geller G, LeRoy L, & Hudson K. (2008). Public expectations for return of results from large-cohort genetic research. The American Journal of Bioethics: AJOB, 8(11), 36–43. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Overby CL, Maloney KA, Alestock TD, Chavez J, Berman D, Sharaf RM, Fitzgerald T, Kim E-Y., Palmer K, Shuldiner AR, & Mitchel BD (2015). Prioritizing approaches to engage community members and build trust in biobanks: A survey of attitudes and opinions of adults within outpatient practices at the University of Maryland, Journal of Personalized Medicine, 5, 264–279. doi: 10.3390/jpm5030264 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Picillo M, Kou N, Barone P, & Fasano A. (2015). Recruitment strategies and patient selection in clinical trials for Parkinson’s disease: Going viral and keeping science and ethics at the highest standards. Parkinsonism and Related Disorders, 21, 1041–1048. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Platt J, Bollinger J, Dvoskin R, Kardia SLR, & Kaufman D. (2014). Public preferences regarding informed consent models for participation in population-based genomic research. Genetics in Medicine, 16, 11–18. doi: 10.1038/gim.2013.59 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roskos-Ewoldsen B, Davies J, & Roskos-Ewoldsen DR (2004). Implications of the mental models approach for cultivation theory. European Journal of Communications Research, 29, 345–363. [Google Scholar]
- Ruiz-Canela M, Valle-Mansilla JI, & Sulmasy DP (2011). What research participants want to know about genetic research results: The impact of “genetic exceptionalism.” Journal of Empirical Research on Human Research Ethics, 6 (3), 39–46. doi: 10.1525/jer.2011.6.3.39 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Segall A. (1976). The sick role concept: Understanding illness behavior. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 17, 162–170. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Skinner CS, Schildkraut JM, Calingaert B, Hoyo C, Cranksaw SS, Fish L, Susswein L, Jasper C, & Reid L. (2008). Factors associated with African Americans enrollment in a National Cancer Genetics Registry. Community Genetics, 11, 224–233. doi: 10.1159/000116883 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Solomon DH, Dillard JP, & Anderson JW (2002). Episode type, attachment orientation, and frame salience: Evidence for a theory of relational framing. Human Communication Research, 28, 136–152. [Google Scholar]
- Stone CC (1979). Patient compliance and the role of the expert. Journal of Social Issues, 35, 34–59. [Google Scholar]
- Tarrant C, Jackson C, Dixon-Woods M, McNicol S, Kenyon S, & Armstrong N. (2015). Consent revisited: the impact of return of results on participants’ views and expectations about trial participation. Health Expectations, 18, 2042–2053. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taylor SE, & Croker J. (1981). Schematic bases of social information processing In Higgins ET, Heran CP, & Zanna MP (Eds.), Social cognition: The Ontario Symposium Vol. 1 (pp. 89–134). Hillsdale, NJ: LEA. [Google Scholar]
- Tindana P, & de Vries J. (2016). Broad consent for genomic research and biobanking: Perspectives from low- and middle-income countries. Annual Review of Genomics and Human Genetics. Posted online Feb 22 2016. DOI: 10.1146/annurev-genom-083115-022456 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Trinidad SB, Fullerton SM, Bares JM, Jarvik GP, Larson EB, Burke W. (2012). Informed consent in genome-scale research: What do prospective participants think? AJOB primary research. 3(3), 3–11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wood F, Kowalczuk J, Elwyn G, Mitchell C, & Gallacher J. (2011). Achieving online consent to participation in large-scale gene-environment studies: a tangible destination. Journal of Medical Ethics, 37(8), 487–492. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zawati MNH, & Rioux A. (2011). Biobanks and the return of research results: Out with the old and in with the new? The Journal of Law, Medicine & Ethics, 39(4), 614–620. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ziniel SI, Savage SK, Huntington N, Amatruda J, Green RC, Weitzman ER, Taylor P, & Holm IA (2014). Parents’ preferences for return of results in pediatric genomic research. Public Health Genomics,17(2), 105–14. doi: 10.1159/000358539 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhou Y, & Wang E. (2010). Shared mental models as moderators of team process-performance relationships. Social Behavior and Personality, 38(4), 433–444. doi 10.2224/sbp.2010.38.4.433 [DOI] [Google Scholar]