Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2021 Feb 8;16(2):e0242897. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0242897

Type 1 dopamine receptor (D1R)-independent circadian food anticipatory activity in mice

Dina R Assali 1, Michael Sidikpramana 1, Andrew P Villa 1, Jeffrey Falkenstein 1, Andrew D Steele 1,*
Editor: Paul A Bartell2
PMCID: PMC7869994  PMID: 33556069

Abstract

Circadian rhythms are entrained by light and influenced by non-photic stimuli, such as feeding. The activity preceding scheduled mealtimes, food anticipatory activity (FAA), is elicited in rodents fed a limited amount at scheduled times. FAA is thought to be the output of an unidentified food entrained oscillator. Previous studies, using gene deletion and receptor pharmacology, implicated dopamine type receptor 1 (D1R) signaling in the dorsal striatum as necessary for FAA in mice. To further understand the role of D1R in promoting FAA, we utilized the Cre-lox system to create cell type-specific deletions of D1R, conditionally deleting D1R in GABA neurons using Vgat-ires-Cre line. This conditional deletion mutant had attenuated FAA, but the amount was higher than expected based on prior results using a constitutive knockout of D1R, D1R KODrago. This result prompted us to re-test the original D1R KODrago line, which expressed less FAA than controls, but only moderately so. To determine if genetic drift had diminished the effect of D1R deletion on FAA, we re-established the D1R KODrago knockout line from cryopreserved samples. The reestablished D1R KODrago-cryo had a clear impairment of FAA compared to controls, but still developed increased activity preceding mealtime across the 4 weeks of timed feeding. Finally, we tested a different deletion allele of D1R created by the Knockout Mouse Project. This line of D1R KOKOMP mice had a significant impairment in the acquisition of FAA, but eventually reached similar levels of premeal activity compared to controls after 4 weeks of timed feeding. Taken together, our results suggest that D1R signaling promotes FAA, but other dopamine receptors likely contribute to FAA given that mice lacking the D1 receptor still retain some FAA.

Introduction

Circadian rhythms link molecular processes within cells to large scale phenomena within organisms such as metabolism, hormonal signaling, and behavior [13]. The circadian timekeeping mechanism for photoentrainment has been linked to core transcriptional-translational feedback loops in many types of cells [4]. In mammals, the suprachiasmatic nucleus (SCN) serves as the main time keeper, orchestrating the 24 hour rhythms in many other brain regions and peripheral organs [5]. Through photoreceptors within the retina, the SCN is synchronized by daily light exposure [6,7].

Aside from photic input, the availability of food is an external cue that synchronizes circadian clocks in the brain and peripheral tissues independently of the SCN [8]. This is demonstrated in rodents with time-restricted food access during their rest phase, which causes a shift of circadian activity cycles while retaining nocturnal activity driven by the SCN. This shift is associated with a bout of locomotor activity that anticipates the daily scheduled meal by about two hours [9]. Remarkably, this food anticipatory activity (FAA) is robustly maintained in rodents with SCN lesions, indicating the presence of a food entrained oscillator separate from the SCN [10]. It is not clear if the molecular components of the food entrained oscillator are fully, partially, or non-overlapping with the light entrained oscillator present within the SCN [1113]. The location of the neurons responsible for the anticipation of food and other related stimuli have been elusive [9,1417]. Recent studies have implicated everything from liver-derived ketones (requiring intact period gene in the liver) [18] to neuronal expression of Reverb-ɑ (Nr1d1) [19] to dopamine receptors 1 and 2 as important for mediating FAA [2022].

Within the brain, dopamine signaling is essential for orchestrating movement and plays a major role in reward and motivation [2325]. The dopamine neurons in the substantia nigra (SN) and ventral tegmental area (VTA) innervate several parts of the brain, with SN neurons projecting primarily to the dorsal striatum which regulates goal-directed locomotion, while VTA neurons project to the nucleus accumbens to drive motivation and learning [25]. Studies have shown functional overlap and interactions between the SN→dorsal striatum and VTA→nucleus accumbens dopamine circuits [26,27]. Dopamine neurons function by activating two distinct types of G-protein coupled receptors, D1-like and D2-like, which are expressed in abundant, intermingled populations in the striatum and nucleus accumbens, among other regions [28,29]. Activation of D1-like receptors increases intracellular cAMP production to increase neuronal activity and excitability while D2-like receptors function to lower the intracellular production of cAMP, thereby depressing activity in D2-expressing neurons [29].

Dopamine signaling is modulated by circadian machinery and, conversely, dopamine signaling influences circadian rhythms [2022,3035]. For example, Reverb-α, a circadian nuclear receptor, is a negative regulator of tyrosine hydroxylase (TH) mRNA levels; TH levels were found to be highest at night when REV-ERBα levels were lowest [34]. In addition, mice with REV-ERBα gene deletion had higher dopamine production and firing rates than did controls [34]. Another study reports that extracellular levels of dopamine are controlled by the dopamine transporter protein (DAT or Slc6a3) with levels of dopamine correlating the behavioral activity cycle [32]. Furthermore, Hood and colleagues demonstrated that period 2 (Per2) protein expression in the striatum follows a circadian pattern and is dependent on dopamine signaling, as rats with unilateral lesions to dopamine neurons lost their Per2 rhythm in the lesioned side but not contralateral side [31]. Therefore, further understanding of the timing and release of dopamine is necessary for the treatment of many disorders, including drug addiction [36].

Daily access to food leads to anticipatory behavioral activity that may also be dependent on dopamine signaling [30,37,38]. Previous studies of mice with a constitutive knockout (KO) of D1R showed attenuated FAA, whereas D2R KO mice showed normal FAA [21,39]. The D1R KO mice had a weakened PER2 RNA expression in their striatum as well. In mice lacking TH, viral reintroduction of dopamine production in the dorsal striatum was sufficient for FAA, suggesting that dopamine signaling to D1R neurons in the dorsal striatum permits the expression of FAA [21]. In addition, timed daily D1R agonist injection caused anticipatory activity even in mice fed ad libitum (AL), suggesting that D1R neurons can control behavioral rhythms [21]. Taken together, these results suggested that the SN dopamine-striatal D1R neurons may constitute a critical component of the circadian response to scheduled feeding. Thus, resolving the D1R-dopamine circuitry required for FAA would advance the field toward a circuit-level understanding of food as a zeitgeber. To that end we examined conditional deletion mutants of D1R and also re-examined constitutive D1R mutants.

Materials and methods

Mouse husbandry and strains

The Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee at the California State Polytechnic University Pomona approved the experiments described herein. Mice were maintained on a 12:12 light:dark cycle in static microisolator cages. By convention, zeitgeber time (ZT) 0 was defined as lights on, while ZT 12 was designated as lights-off. Home cages contained sani-chip bedding (Envigo, 7090) and a cotton nestlet. Temperatures ranged between 22-24°C and humidity between 20–45%. For all experiments, except those described in Fig 2 with D1R KODrago, mice were fed rodent chow (2018 Teklad Diet Envigo), which has a caloric density of 3.1 kcal/gram and macronutrient composition of 18% fat, 24% protein, 58% carbohydrates. For the experiments with D1R KODrago mice, n = 10 control and n = 10 KO were fed formulated chow (Research Diets, D12450K), which had a caloric density of 5.49 kcal/gram and a macronutrient composition of 10% fat, 20% protein, 70% carbohydrates. For this experiment we combined data from n = 9 controls and n = 13 D1R KODrago fed standard chow with those fed unformulated chow since both diets yielded similar results on feeding, body weight, activity, and FAA.

Fig 2. Immunostaining, food intake, body weight, and circadian behavior of mice with constitutive deletion of D1R (D1R KODrago).

Fig 2

Forebrain staining of (A) control TH (pink) and DAPI (blue), (B) D1R KODrago TH and DAPI, (C) control D1R (green) and DAPI, (D) D1R KODrago and DAPI (E) control merge and (F) D1R KODrago merge. (G) Average daily food intake for control (n = 20) and D1R KODrago (n = 23) mice in grams and (H) normalized to body weight. (I) Body weight in grams prior to CR and during 4 weeks of CR and (J) expressed as a fraction of Day 0 weight. (K) Mean ± SEM fraction of high activity behaviors on day 0 of CR. n = 14–23 control and 16–19 KO for all behavioral measurements. (L) Mean ± SEM fraction of high activity behaviors after 14 days of timed CR feeding. (M) Mean ± SEM fraction of high activity behaviors after 28 days of timed CR feeding. (N) Mean ± SEM total 24 hour high activity data in seconds. (O) Mean ± SEM high activity data in seconds for the 3 h preceding scheduled feeding across the experiment. (P) Normalized high activity data shown in (O) in the 3 h preceding scheduled feeding across the experiment.

Drd1tm1e(KOMP)Wtsi mice (Denoted as D1R KOKOMP) were obtained from the KOMP repository [40], whereas Cre-driver mice were obtained from the Jackson Laboratory (Bar Harbor, ME) (Table 1). Drd1tm1Jcd/J heterozygote (+/-) mice (Denoted as D1R KODrago) were bred for 3–5 generations before being tested for FAA, with only WT/WT (+/+) and KO/KO (-/-) mice being used for CR experiments. Tg(Drd1a-Cre)120MXu (denoted as D1-Cre) and Slc32a1tm2(cre)Lowl (denoted as Vgat-Cre) were crossed with mouse strain Drd1tm2.1Stl/J, which contain LoxP flanked D1R (denoted as D1Rflox/flox; these mice were kindly provided by Dr. Susumu Tonegawa [MIT] and are now available from Jackson Labs stock 025700). For CR experiments with Drd1tm1e(KOMP)Wtsi (Denoted as D1R KOKOMP) and Vgat-Cre mice, both homozygous wild-type and heterozygous wild-types were used as controls. Both males and females were used for all studies.

Table 1. Mouse strains used in this study.

A list of the mouse strains along with their abbreviated names used in this study.

Strain Allele Intended deletion Reference
Drd1tm1Jcd/J “D1R KODrago D1R constitutive, null Allele all 48
Drd1tm1Jcd/J “D1R KODrago” cryo-recovered D1R constitutive, null Allele all 48
Drd1tm1a(KOMP)WtsiD1R KOKOMP Drd1 constitutive, Null Allele all 40
Drd1tm2.1Stl/J “D1Rflox/flox LoxP flanked Drd1 allele all 43
Tg(Drd1a-Cre)120MXuD1-Cre Random integration transgenic; Cre recombinase driven by D1 promoter D1R in all D1R expressing cells 44
Slc32a1tm2(cre)LowlVgat-Cre Cre recombinase driven by vesicular GABA transporter- expressing cells by an internal ribosomal entry site GABAergic cells 45

To genotype mice, DNA was obtained from tail clippings from 2-week old mice, which were then digested with proteinase K and DNA was purified by isopropanol precipitation. Genomic PCR was performed to amplify D1R and Cre alleles. The D1R KOKOMP mice were genotyped through a third party using qPCR (Transnetyx). Genomic DNA of the D1R KODrago and D1R KOKOMP lines was assessed using single polymorphic nucleotides (SNPs) for differences between genetic background comparable to C57BL/6J and C57BL/6N WT mice (Table 2) by the Jackson Laboratory.

Table 2. Single nucleotide polymorphism in genomic DNA from Drd1tm1Jcd and Drd1tm1e(KOMP)Wtsi mouse line to assess purity of genetic background.

Mouse Strain Relative to C57BL/6J Relative to C57BL/6N Relative to 129S1/SvImJ-FVB/NJ
Drd1tm1Jcd 99.65% -- 0.3533%
Drd1tm1e(KOMP)Wtsi 83.34% 16.66% --

Drd1tm1Jcd mice (n = 3) were tested at 144 positions across the genome for the presence of C57BL/6J or 129S1/SvImJ-FVB/NJ alleles. Drd1tm1e(KOMP)Wtsi line (n = 4) was tested for the presence C57BL/6J or C57BL/6N strain at 144 positions across the genome. Percentages were in respects to SEM values.

Food intake, behavioral measurements and calorie restriction conditions

Food intake was measured at 9–10 weeks of age. To calculate 60% CR food allotment per cohort, daily food intakes were measured per mouse over 48 hours. These values were then averaged per group and 60% of the daily average was determined as the daily CR value. Food intakes were adjusted by 0.1 g per day to prevent excessive weight loss for some individual mice (experimenters were blind to genotypes during the experiment).

On video recording days, home cages only contained a minimum of sani-chip bedding (100ml) and no cotton nestlet. During video recording, dim red lighting was provided with High Power 42 SMT RED LED PAR38 from LEDwholesalers.com. Home cage behavior measurements were obtained by video recording mice from a perpendicular angle to their home cages and analyzing these videos using an automated-behavior recognition software, HomeCageScan 3.0 [41], which annotates for the following behaviors: remain low, pause, twitch, awaken, distance traveled, turn, sniff, groom, food bin entry, chew, drink, stretch, hanging, jumping, rearing, walking, and unassigned behaviors. These data were binned in 24 one-hour bins to quantify the temporal structure of activity. The behaviors hanging, jumping, rearing, and walking are designated as high activity. Total high activity was then determined by summation of high activity bins, while FAA ratios were calculated by dividing the final 3h of high activity over the total high activity of each mouse.

All mice entering CR were at least 10 weeks of age. Mice were individually housed on AL for 5–7 days prior to beginning CR with food intake studies conducted at 2–4 days preceding CR. Mice were then fed 60% of their daily caloric intake every day for 28 days at ZT 6 beginning on Day 0, or the start of CR. Body weights were measured every 7th day prior to scheduled mealtime, beginning from Day -7 and ending at Day 28.

Tissue histology and antibody labeling

For immunolabeling, mice were euthanized with CO2 gas and their brains were perfusion-fixed using 4% paraformaldehyde (Sigma) and post-fixed for 24h before being sectioned at room temperature using a vibrating blade microtome (VT-1000S, Leica Instruments). Antibody staining was performed using a rat monoclonal D1R antibody 1:250 (Sigma) and a chicken polyclonal tyrosine hydroxylase (TH) 1:500 (Aves). An Alexa Fluor 647-conjugated AffiniPure Goat Anti-Chicken IgY 1:500 and an AffiniPure Goat Anti-Rat 488 1:500 were used as fluorotags, along with DAPI (4’, 6-Diamidino-2-Phenylindole, Dihydrochloride) 1:1000 for nuclear staining. Sections were imaged by confocal microscopy using a Nikon Eclipse Ti-E inverted microscope system linked to a computer system with NIS-Elements Imaging Software and FIJI was used for image processing [42].

Statistical analysis

Behavioral data were exported from HomeCageScan 3.0 as excel files, which were analyzed using MATLAB programs to sum, average, and visualize data. Statistical tests and graphs for all data described were performed using GraphPad Prism 9. Sample sizes for each experiment are indicated in the figure legends. Food intake data, which fell on a normal distribution, were tested for significance using an Unpaired T Test. Body weights were analyzed for differences using mixed-effects analysis with Tukey’s post-test to examine differences between groups at each time point. For home-cage behavioral data, we also used mixed-effects analysis to examine the effects of genotype, time, and the interaction between time and genotye. We used Sidak’s multiple comparisons test to compare behavior at each time point. To analyze a contingency table for mouse genotypes, a Chi-Square Test was performed to determine the dependency of genotype on viable offspring.

Results

Conditional deletion of D1R using D1R-Cre may be embryonically lethal

To follow up on our previous work, which utilized a constitutive deletion mutant of D1R (denoted as D1R KODrago), we attempted to conditionally delete D1R by crossing D1Rflox/flox mice [43] to D1-Cre transgenic mice [44] (Table 1). Surprisingly, backcrossing F1 progeny (D1-Cre+/-; D1Rwt/flox,) to the parental D1Rflox/flox line did not produce cKO progeny (D1-Cre+/-,D1Rflox/flox): out of 47 progeny examined, all were negative for this genotype (Table 3; P<0.0001, Chi-Square). These results suggest that conditionally deleting D1R in all D1R-Cre expressing cells is embryonically lethal. However, since the location of the D1R-Cre transgene was not determined, the lack of cKO progeny can also be attributable to a genetic linkage of the D1R-Cre transgene with the D1R locus. Nevertheless, the focus of our investigation does not concern the cause of embryonic lethality and therefore we turned to a different Cre driver line to achieve deletion of D1R.

Table 3. Observed and expected genotypes for the Tg(Drd1a-Cre)120MXu mouse line.

F2 Possible Genotypes Expected Observed
WT D1R/flox D1R; D1R-Cre + 11.75 23
WTD1R/flox D1R; D1R-Cre – 11.75 15
Flox D1R/flox D1R; D1R-Cre – 11.75 9
Flox D1R/flox D1R; D1R-Cre + 11.75 0

Four possible genotypes were expected as a result of crossing D1Rwt/flox, D1R-Cre+ and D1Rflox/flox, D1R-Cre–mice. Of 47 offspring observed, an expected offspring of 11.75 for each genotype did not match observed, yielding a Chi-square statistic of 24.064 (P<0.0001, Chi-Square).

Conditional deletion of D1R in GABAergic neurons using Vgat-Cre reduces food anticipatory activity

A second attempt at a broad conditional deletion of D1R was made using a well-characterized Cre driver for inhibitory neurons, Vgat-Cre [45]. The only known D1R-expressing neurons in the dorsolateral striatum are GABAergic medium-sized spiny neurons [46]; thus, we expected that D1R should be deleted throughout the striatum in Vgat-Cre; D1Rflox/flox mice. We verified deletion of D1R protein using antibody labeling. Both D1R and TH staining can be seen in control striatum tissue (Fig 1A, 1C and 1E), whereas D1R antibody labeling is completely absent in Vgat-Cre; D1Rflox/flox forebrain tissue (Fig 1B, 1D and 1F).

Fig 1. Immunostaining, food intake, body weight, and circadian behavior of mice with conditional deletion of D1R using vgat-ires-Cre.

Fig 1

Forebrain staining of (A) control TH (pink) and DAPI (blue), (B) Vgat-Cre; FloxD1R TH and DAPI, (C) control D1R (green) and DAPI, (D) Vgat-Cre; FloxD1R D1R and DAPI (E) control merge and (F) Vgat-Cre; FloxD1R merge. (G) Average daily food intake for control (n = 12) and Vgat-Cre; FloxD1R mice (n = 11) in grams and (H) normalized to body weight. (I) Body weight in grams prior to CR and during 4 weeks of CR and (J) expressed as a fraction of Day 0 weight. (K) Mean ± SEM fraction of high activity behaviors (hanging, jumping, rearing, and walking) day 0 of CR, the first day of scheduled CR feeding. The yellow bar indicates lights on and the gray area indicates lights off. n = 14–23 control and 16–23 cKO for all behavioral measurements. (L) Mean ± SEM fraction of high activity behaviors after 14 days of timed CR feeding. (M) Mean ± SEM fraction of high activity behaviors after 28 days of timed CR feeding. (N) Mean ± SEM total 24 hour high activity data in seconds. (O) Mean ± SEM high activity data in seconds for the 3 h preceding scheduled feeding across the experiment (sum of ZT 5, 6, and 7). (P) Normalized high activity data shown in (O) in the 3 h preceding scheduled feeding across the experiment.

We measured daily food intake levels for control and Vgat-Cre; D1Rflox/flox mice, and found no difference in mean food intakes: control animals ate 4.2g (+/-0.6g SD) and Vgat-Cre; D1Rflox/flox 4.5 (+/- 0.8 SD) for cKO (P = 0.385, unpaired t test; Fig 1G). When food intake is normalized to body weight, the cKO mice are hyperphagic compared to controls (unpaired t test, P = 0.0009, Fig 1H). Notably, cKO body weights are much lower than controls: prior to starting CR, the average weight of controls was 23.0g (+/- 3.3 SD) while Vgat-Cre; D1Rflox/flox mice weighed only 17.9g (+/- 2.9 SD). At all time points prior to and during timed CR, the body weights of Vgat-Cre; D1Rflox/flox mice were reduced to a greater extent when compared to controls (mixed-effects analysis: fixed effects of time P<0.001, genotype P<0.0001, and time x genotype P = 0.485; for pairwise comparisons using Sidak’s multiple comparison, P<0.0001 for days -7, 0, 7, 14 and P = 0.0002 for day 21 and P = 0.001 for day 28; Fig 1I). However, when body weight is normalized by dividing each value by the day 0 body weight, it is clear that the response of body weight to timed 60% CR feeding is similar between Vgat-Cre; D1Rflox/flox and control mice, which both lost similar fractions of weight across the experiment (mixed-effects analysis, fixed effects: time P<0.0001, genotype P = 0.26, time x genotype P = 0.77; P>0.05 for all pairwise comparisons; Fig 1J).

Both groups displayed similar waveforms of normalized high activity behaviors (defined as the sum of walking, cuddling, hanging vertically, hanging upside down, and jumping) on day 0, the first day of timed CR feeding (Fig 1K). On day 14 of CR, the duration of nighttime activity appeared prolonged in the Vgat-Cre; D1Rflox/flox mice while the premeal activity was lower amplitude and had a later oneset (Fig 1L). On day 28 of timed CR feeding, both groups showed premeal activity but the control group had an increased amplitude of FAA in comparison to Vgat-Cre; D1Rflox/flox mice (Fig 1M). We plotted total activity levels (in seconds), observing that the Vgat-Cre; D1Rflox/flox mice were considerably more active overall than controls: on days 0, 7, 12, and 21 activity was significantly increased (mixed-effects analysis, fixed effects: time P = 0.023, genotype P < 0.0001, time x genotype P = 0.187; for pairwise comparisons P = 0.038 day 0, P < 0.0001 day 7, P = 0.0154 for day 14, P = 0.0196 for day 21). For pre-meal (ZT 4–6) high activity behaviors in seconds, there was a strong effect of time (P<0.0001) but no interaction of genotype (P = 0.565), and a significant effect of time x genotype (P = 0.0269, mixed-effects analysis; Fig 1O). However, pairwise comparison of these data yielded no significant differences at any time point (P>0.05). To account for differences in total activity levels between Vgat-Cre; D1Rflox/flox and control groups, we divided the ZT 4–6 high activity by the 24-hour total high activity, expressing activity as a fraction of total (Fig 1P). This normalization revealed strong fixed effects of time (P < 0.0001), genotype (P = 0.0029) and time x genotype (P < 0.0001). Pairwise comparisons demonstrated that Vgat-Cre; D1Rflox/flox had significantly less FAA as compared to controls on days 14, 21, and 28 of CR (Sidak’s multiple comparison, P < 0.0001 day 14, P = 0.0076 day 21, P = 0.024 for day 28). We also noted that by day 28 of timed CR, all control mice showed at least 20% of their activity during ZT 4–6 whereas only a small fraction of the Vgat-Cre; D1Rflox/flox mice exceed 20% and many showed very low activity during this preprandial time window.

Constitutive deletion of D1R in the Drd1Drago strain moderately reduces food anticipatory activity

Given that Vgat-Cre; D1Rflox/flox mice demonstrated more FAA than we had predicted (we expected a near complete loss of FAA), we returned to the constitutive deletion strain D1R KODrago that we previously demonstrated had a large impairment in FAA [21]. We verified D1R deletion using antibody staining of adult mice; D1R KODrago mice did not have any D1R staining in the striatum or nucleus accumbens whereas control samples showed abundant staining for D1R (Fig 2A–2F).

Prior to CR, D1R KODrago mice were individually housed and measured for daily food intakes while on an AL diet. Mean food intake for control animals was 3.6g (+/- 0.8 SD) while D1R KODrago ate only 3.0g (+/-0.8 SD), which was significantly reduced (Fig 2G, P = 0.020, unpaired t test). However, when food intake is normalized to body weight, the D1R KODrago mice are hyperphagic compared to controls (P = 0.035, unpaired t test, Fig 2H). Notably, the D1R KODrago body weights are much lower than controls: prior to starting CR, the average weight of controls was 25.5g (+/- 3.8 SD) while D1R KODrago mice weighed only 18.3g (+/- 2.8 SD) (Fig 2I). At time points prior to and during timed CR, the body weights of D1R KODrago mice were less than that of controls (mixed-effects analysis: fixed effects of time P<0.0001, genotype P<0.0001, and time x genotype P<0.001; for pairwise comparisons using Sidak’s multiple comparison, P<0.0001 for days -7, 0, 7, 14, 21, and 28; Fig 2I). However, when body weight is normalized by dividing each value by the day 0 body weight, it is clear that the response of body weight to timed 60% CR feeding is similar between D1R KODrago and control mice, which both lost similar fractions of weight across the experiment (mixed-effects analysis, fixed effects: time P<0.0001, genotype P = 0.32, time x genotype P = 0.013; P>0.05 for all pairwise comparisons; Fig 2J).

Both groups displayed similar waveforms of normalized high activity behaviors on day 0 (Fig 2K). On day 14 of CR, the amplitude of nighttime activity was higher in the D1R KODrago mice, while their premeal activity was lower than controls (Fig 2L). On day 28 of timed CR feeding, both groups showed premeal activity but the control group had an earlier and stronger increase in the amplitude of FAA in comparison to D1R KODrago mice (Fig 2M). We plotted the total activity levels (in seconds), observing that the D1R KODrago mice were considerably more active overall on days -7, 0, and 7 but not at later measurements (mixed-effects analysis, fixed effects: time P = 0.401, genotype P = 0.0008, time x genotype P = 0.040; for pairwise comparisons, P = 0.013 day -7, P = 0.0051 day 0, P = 0.0070 for day 7, and P>0.05 days 14, 21, and 28). For pre-meal (ZT 4–6) high activity behaviors in seconds, there was a strong effect of time (P<0.0001) but no interaction of genotype (P = 0.146), and a significant effect of time x genotype (P<0.001, mixed-effects analysis; Fig 2O). Pairwise comparison of these data showed significant differences only on days 21 and 28 of CR (P = 0.038 for day 21, and P = 0.0135 for day 28; P>0.05 at all other measurements, Sidak’s multiple comparisons test). To account for differences in total activity levels between D1R KODrago and control groups, we divided the ZT 4–6 high activity by the 24-hour total high activity, expressing activity as a fraction of total (Fig 2P). This normalization revealed strong fixed effects of time (P<0.0001), genotype (P = 0.0155) and time x genotype (P< 0.0001, mixed-effects analysis). Pairwise comparisons demonstrated that D1R KODrago mice had significantly less FAA as compared to controls on days 14, 21, and 28 of CR (P = 0.0077 day 14, P = 0.0027 day 21, P = 0.002 for day 28, Sidak’s multiple comparison). We noted that in contrast to what was observed with Vgat-Cre; D1Rflox/flox, by day 28 of timed CR, several of the D1R KODrago mice had ample FAA, exceeding 20 or even 30% of their daily activity during ZT 4–6 (Fig 2P).

Cryorestored Drd1Drago knockouts have impaired food anticipatory activity

Given that the Drd1Drago KO strain showed more FAA than expected based on prior results (Gallardo et al., 2014), we speculated that mutations or selection for alleles present in this strain that promote FAA appeared in our line of D1R KODrago mice. To test this hypothesis, we obtained the line again from the cryorepository at Jackson lab (Bar Harbor, ME, USA); we denote this line as D1R KODrago-cryo to indicate that it is newly established in our colony (Table 1). Prior to CR, mice were individually housed and measured for daily food intakes while on an AL diet. Mean food intake for control animals was 3.8g (+/- 0.4 SD) while D1R KODrago-cryo ate 4.4g (+/-0.6 SD) per day, which was significantly increased (Fig 3A, P = 0.012, unpaired t test). When food intake is normalized to body weight, D1R KODrago mice are even more obviously hyperphagic compared to controls (P = 0.0001, unpaired t test, Fig 3B). As with the other deletion strains, the D1R KODrago-cryo body weights are much lower than controls: prior to starting CR, the average weight of controls was 25.7g (+/- 5.8 SD) while D1R KODrago-cryo mice weighed only 18.4g (+/- 2.8 SD) (Fig 3C). At time points prior to and during timed CR, the body weights of D1R KODrago-cryo mice were reduced as compared to controls at all time points except day 28 (mixed-effects analysis: fixed effects of time P<0.0001, genotype P = 0.0005, and time x genotype P = 0.018; for pairwise comparisons using Sidak’s multiple comparison, P<0.0001 for day -7, P = 0.003 for day 0, P = 0.0012 for day 14, P = 0.0036 for Day 14, P = 0.0138 for day 21, P = 0.0585 for 28; Fig 3C). However, when body weight is normalized by dividing each value by the day 0 body weight, it is clear that the response of body weight to timed 60% CR feeding is similar between D1R KODrago-cryo and control mice, which both lost similar fractions of weight across the experiment (mixed-effects analysis, fixed effects: time P<0.0001, genotype P = 0.597, time x genotype P = 0.0656; P>0.05 for all pairwise comparisons; Fig 3D).

Fig 3. Food intake, body weight, and circadian behavior of D1R KODrago mice from the cryorepository.

Fig 3

(A) Average daily food intake for control (n = 16) and D1R KODrago-cryo (n = 8) mice in grams and (B) normalized to body weight. (C) Body weight in grams prior to CR and during 4 weeks of CR and (D) expressed as a fraction of Day 0 weight. (E) Mean ± SEM fraction of high activity behaviors on day 0 of CR. n = 19–21 for control and 9–10 for KO for all behavioral measurements. (F) Mean ± SEM fraction of high activity behaviors after 14 days of timed CR feeding. (G) Mean ± SEM fraction of high activity behaviors after 28 days of timed CR feeding. (H) Mean ± SEM total 24 hour high activity data in seconds. (I) Mean ± SEM high activity data in seconds for the 3 h preceding scheduled feeding across the experiment. (J) Normalized high activity data shown in (I) in the 3 h preceding scheduled feeding across the experiment.

Both groups displayed similar waveforms of normalized high activity behaviors on day 0, with the D1R KODrago-cryo KO showing a wider amplitude of nighttime activity (Fig 3E). On day 14 of CR, the amplitude of nighttime activity was higher in the D1R KODrago-cryo mice, while their premeal activity was lower than controls (Fig 3F). On day 28 of timed CR feeding, nocturnal activity continued to be pronounced in D1R KODrago-cryo KO mice and they had much less noticeable premeal activity compared to the control group (Fig 3G). We plotted the total activity levels (in seconds), observing that the D1R KODrago-cryo mice were more active than controls on days 0 and 7, but not at other measurements (mixed-effects analysis, fixed effects: time P<0.0001, genotype P = 0.257, time x genotype P<0.0001; for pairwise comparisons P = 0.807 day -7, P = 0.0009 day 0, P = 0.0430 for day 7, and P>0.05 days 14, 21, and 28; Fig 3H). For pre-meal (ZT 4–6) high activity behaviors in seconds, there were strong effects of time (P<0.0001), genotype (P = 0.0056), time x genotype (P = 0.0002, mixed-effects analysis; Fig 3I). Pairwise comparison of these data showed significant differences on days 14, 21 and 28 of CR (P = 0.0087 for day 14, P = 0.0330 for Day 21, and P<0.0001 for day 28; P>0.05 at all other measurements, Sidak’s multiple comparisons test). To account for differences in total activity levels between D1R KODrago-cryo and control groups, we divided the ZT 4–6 high activity by the 24-hour total high activity, expressing activity as a fraction of total (Fig 3J). This normalization revealed strong fixed effects of time (P<0.0001), genotype (P = 0.0002) and time x genotype (P = 0.0002, mixed-effects analysis). Pairwise comparisons demonstrated that D1R KODrago-cryo mice had significantly less FAA as compared to controls on days 14, 21, and 28 of CR (P = 0.0060 day 14, P = 0.0016 day 21, P<0.0001 for day 28, Sidak’s multiple comparison). We noted that similar to the D1R KODrago mice, by day 28 of timed CR, several D1R KODrago-cryo mice had ample FAA, exceeding 20% of their daily activity during ZT 4–6, while the other half failed to show activity during this time window (Fig 3J).

A novel constitutive deletion of D1R (Knockout Mouse Project), D1R KOKOMP, has delayed acquisition of food anticipatory activity

We next tested a novel constitutive KO of D1R, which we term D1R KOKOMP (Table 1). This particular deletion strategy was attractive because it used a “knockout first” approach, creating a mutant allele that can later be reactivated by FLP recombinase [40]. In addition, the “flipped” allele can be deleted in a conditional manner by Cre recombinase [40]. As such, we first confirmed that the allele was disrupted by antibody labeling forebrain tissue using D1R antibody (Fig 4A–4F). While TH staining is apparent in both WT and D1R KOKOMP forebrain tissue, no D1R was present in the D1R KOKOMP forebrain tissue (Fig 4A–4F). In contrast, control samples had ample D1R staining in the striatum and other areas within the forebrain (Fig 4C and 4E).

Fig 4. Immunostaining, food intake, body weight, and circadian behavior of mice with constitutive deletion of D1R (D1R KOKOMP).

Fig 4

Forebrain staining of (A) control TH (pink) and DAPI (blue), (B) D1R KOKOMP TH and DAPI, (C) control D1R (green) and DAPI, (D) D1R KOKOMP and DAPI (E) control merge and (F) D1R KOKOMP merge. (G) Average daily food intake for control (n = 21) and D1R KOKOMP (n = 18) mice in grams and (H) normalized to body weight. (I) Body weight in grams prior to CR and during 4 weeks of CR and (J) expressed as a fraction of Day 0 weight. (K) Mean ± SEM fraction of high activity behaviors on day 0 of CR. n = 18–22 for control and 16–19 for KO for all behavioral measurements. (L) Mean ± SEM fraction of high activity behaviors after 14 days of timed CR feeding. (M) Mean ± SEM fraction of high activity behaviors after 28 days of timed CR feeding. (N) Mean ± SEM total 24 hour high activity data in seconds. (O) Mean ± SEM high activity data in seconds for the 3 h preceding scheduled feeding across the experiment (sum of ZT 5, 6, and 7). (P) Normalized high activity data shown in (O) in the 3 h preceding scheduled feeding across the experiment.

Daily average food intake was similar between controls, which at 3.2g (+/- 0.7 SD), and D1R KOKOMP mice, which ate 3.0g (+/- 0.7 SD) (P = 0.43, unpaired t test; Fig 4G). When food intake is normalized to body weight, there was no difference between groups, unlike for all the other D1R mutants tested (P = 0.62, unpaired t test, Fig 4H). In contrast to the other D1R deletion strains described above, the D1R KOKOMP mice had only a subtle decrease in body weight (Fig 4I). Prior to starting CR, the average weight of controls was 25.7g (+/- 4.8 SD) grams and D1R KOKOMP mice weighed 22.8g (+/- 2.4 SD) (Fig 4I). The only significant difference in body weight occurred at day 0, when the D1R KOKOMP mice weighed less than controls (mixed-effects analysis: fixed effects of time P<0.0001, genotype P = 0.0532, and time x genotype P = 0.011; for pairwise comparisons using Sidak’s multiple comparison, P = 0.0338 on day 0 and P>0.05 at all other time points). The graph of normalized body weight demonstrates that the response of body weight to timed 60% CR feeding is similar between D1R KOKOMP and control mice: both lost similar fractions of weight throughout the experiment (mixed-effects analysis, fixed effects: time P<0.0001, genotype P = 0.607, time x genotype P = 0.374; P>0.05 for all pairwise comparisons; Fig 4J).

In this experiment, all mice displayed similar waveforms of normalized high activity behaviors on day 0 (Fig 4K). On day 14 of CR, the D1R KOKOMP mice clearly developed pre-meal activity, but it was of lower amplitude than controls (Fig 4L). By day 28 of timed CR feeding, both groups showed strong pre-meal activity (Fig 4M). We plotted the total activity levels (in seconds), observing that the D1R KOKOMP mice were similar to controls and that both groups tended to be less active overall as the experiment progressed (mixed-effects analysis, fixed effects: time P<0.0001, genotype P = 0.550, time x genotype P = 0.029; for pairwise comparisons P>0.05 for all measurements, Sidak’s multiple comparison; Fig 4N). For pre-meal (ZT 4–6) high activity behaviors in seconds, there was a strong effect of time (P<0.0001), genotype (P<0.0001), and time x genotype (P<0.0001, mixed-effects analysis; Fig 4O). Pairwise comparison of these data showed significant differences on days 14, 21 and 28 of CR (P = 0.0313 for day 14, P = 0.0019 for day 21, and P = 0.0057 for day 28; P>0.05 at all other measurements, Sidak’s multiple comparisons test). To account for differences in total activity levels between D1R KOKOMP and control groups, we assessed activity as a fraction of total (Fig 4P), which revealed a strong fixed effect of time (P<0.0001), genotype (P = 0.0003) and time x genotype (P<0.0001, mixed-effects analysis). Pairwise comparisons demonstrated that D1R KOKOMP mice had significantly less FAA as compared to controls on days 7, 14, and 21 of CR (P = 0.0286 day 7, P = 0.0014 day 14, P<0.001 for day 21, Sidak’s multiple comparison). However, by day 28 of CR, the normalized FAA was similar between groups (P = 0.152), demonstrating that the D1R KOKOMP mice eventually reach the same level of circadian entrainment to scheduled feeding as controls.

Discussion

Previously, we provided evidence that deletion of D1R in mice nearly eliminated behavioral anticipation but not the body temperature increase associated with a daily meal; we proposed that the circuitry sufficient for FAA involved D1R mediated signaling from the SN into the dorsal striatum [21]. More recently, we demonstrated that mice with hypomorphic mutations in paired-like homeodomain transcription factor 3, a transcription factor necessary for survival of 95% of SN dopamine neurons, defines a minimal population of dopamine neurons sufficient for FAA and metabolic entertainment to timed feeding [47]. To further resolve the circuitry of D1R-expressing neurons and their role promoting FAA, we created a conditional knockout of D1R in Vgat-Cre expressing neurons. This deletion eliminated D1R expression in all brain regions examined and significantly attenuated FAA, but some FAA was still present in the conditional KO mice. This led us to retrace our original findings: we repeated testing of FAA in D1R KODrago and observed a less prominent attenuation of FAA than we expected based on prior findings. Similarly, testing of other constitutive knockouts of D1R (D1R KODrago-cryo and D1R KOKOMP) showed an impairment in FAA but also that the behavior was still evident (i.e. D1R promotes FAA but is not necessary for it to occur). In sum, our current study suggests that D1R is a key component of FAA but not essential for it to occur.

We considered methodological issues that could have influenced the outcomes of our study. Firstly, the use of D1R-Cre line to delete D1R with its endogenous promoter appeared to affect mice embryonically, such that no experimental mice were produced (Table 3). This was surprising because of the several existing constitutive mutants of this allele, which show only slight decreases in viability, but have clear growth defects [48,49]. We speculate that depending on the timing of the deletion of the Drd1a gene, compensatory changes occur in the brain to permit viability. Another methodological issue that may be important in comparing results of food entrainment studies relates to the severity of calorie restriction. For example, LeSateur and colleagues (2018) observed that transgenic over-expression of D2R reduced FAA but only on a very mild restriction, whereas the same mice had ample FAA when temporally restricted to a short feeding time window. In prior studies, we have noted that 80% CR induced much lower FAA as compared to 60% CR [50]. It is possible that if we had increased the food allotment in our studies, that the amount of FAA demonstrated by the various D1R knockout strains may have been lessened.

The dissection of the neural circuitry required for FAA has remained elusive. One study demonstrated a strong role for liver-derived ketones in driving food entrainment, yet the brain region(s) mediating responses to ketones has yet to be identified [18]. Furthermore, it appears that there are several brain regions capable of controlling activity cycles in dopamine-dependent manners [51]. Future studies using conditional genetic approaches may be able to delineate the contributions of separable dopamine and/or D1R populations in mediating the behavioral expression of FAA and/or the underlying food-entrained oscillator. Furthermore, the recently appreciated connection between dopamine signaling in the SCN and out of phase eating resulting in diet-induced obesity demonstrates that this is a complex area of research that deserves additional study [52].

Supporting information

S1 File

(XLSX)

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to David Cun, Elizabeth Hill, and Amanda Ng for assistance with mouse behavioral experiments and to Craig LaMunyon for assistance with confocal microscopy.

Data Availability

All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting Information files.

Funding Statement

The Whitehall Foundation provided a Research Grant to AS, and the National Institute of General Medical Sciences of the National Institutes of Health provided funding under Award Number SC3GM125570 to AS. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

  • 1.Ramsey KM, Yoshino J, Brace CS, Abrassart D, Kobayashi Y, Marcheva B, et al. Circadian clock feedback cycle through NAMPT-mediated NAD+ biosynthesis. Science. 2009;324(5927):651–4. Epub 2009/03/21. 10.1126/science.1171641 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Rudic RD, McNamara P, Curtis AM, Boston RC, Panda S, Hogenesch JB, et al. BMAL1 and CLOCK, two essential components of the circadian clock, are involved in glucose homeostasis. PLoS Biol. 2004;2(11):e377 Epub 2004/11/04. 10.1371/journal.pbio.0020377 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Panda S. Circadian physiology of metabolism. Science. 2016;354(6315):1008–15. Epub 2016/11/26. 10.1126/science.aah4967 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Takahashi JS. Transcriptional architecture of the mammalian circadian clock. Nat Rev Genet. 2017;18(3):164–79. Epub 2016/12/19. 10.1038/nrg.2016.150 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Gekakis N, Staknis D, Nguyen HB, Davis FC, Wilsbacher LD, King DP, et al. Role of the CLOCK protein in the mammalian circadian mechanism. Science. 1998;280(5369):1564–9. Epub 1998/06/11. 10.1126/science.280.5369.1564 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Welsh DK, Takahashi JS, Kay SA. Suprachiasmatic nucleus: cell autonomy and network properties. Annu Rev Physiol. 2010;72:551–77. Epub 2010/02/13. 10.1146/annurev-physiol-021909-135919 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.LeGates TA, Fernandez DC, Hattar S. Light as a central modulator of circadian rhythms, sleep and affect. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2014;15(7):443–54. Epub 2014/06/12. 10.1038/nrn3743 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Mistlberger RE. Circadian food-anticipatory activity: formal models and physiological mechanisms. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 1994;18(2):171–95. Epub 1994/01/01. 10.1016/0149-7634(94)90023-x . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Pendergast JS, Yamazaki S. The Mysterious Food-Entrainable Oscillator: Insights from Mutant and Engineered Mouse Models. J Biol Rhythms. 2018;33(5):458–74. Epub 2018/07/24. 10.1177/0748730418789043 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Takasu NN, Kurosawa G, Tokuda IT, Mochizuki A, Todo T, Nakamura W. Circadian regulation of food-anticipatory activity in molecular clock-deficient mice. PLoS One. 2012;7(11):e48892 Epub 2012/11/13. 10.1371/journal.pone.0048892 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Storch KF, Weitz CJ. Daily rhythms of food-anticipatory behavioral activity do not require the known circadian clock. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2009;106(16):6808–13. Epub 2009/04/16. 10.1073/pnas.0902063106 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Mendoza J, Albrecht U, Challet E. Behavioural food anticipation in clock genes deficient mice: confirming old phenotypes, describing new phenotypes. Genes Brain Behav. 2010;9(5):467–77. Epub 2010/02/26. 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2010.00576.x . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Pendergast JS, Wendroth RH, Stenner RC, Keil CD, Yamazaki S. mPeriod2 (Brdm1) and other single Period mutant mice have normal food anticipatory activity. Sci Rep. 2017;7(1):15510 Epub 2017/11/16. 10.1038/s41598-017-15332-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Davidson AJ. Lesion studies targeting food-anticipatory activity. Eur J Neurosci. 2009;30(9):1658–64. Epub 2009/10/30. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2009.06961.x . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Gunapala KM, Gallardo CM, Hsu CT, Steele AD. Single gene deletions of orexin, leptin, neuropeptide Y, and ghrelin do not appreciably alter food anticipatory activity in mice. PLoS One. 2011;6(3):e18377 Epub 2011/04/06. 10.1371/journal.pone.0018377 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Mistlberger RE. Neurobiology of food anticipatory circadian rhythms. Physiol Behav. 2011;104(4):535–45. Epub 2011/04/30. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2011.04.015 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Challet E. The circadian regulation of food intake. Nat Rev Endocrinol. 2019;15(7):393–405. Epub 2019/05/11. 10.1038/s41574-019-0210-x . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Chavan R, Feillet C, Costa SS, Delorme JE, Okabe T, Ripperger JA, et al. Liver-derived ketone bodies are necessary for food anticipation. Nat Commun. 2016;7:10580 Epub 2016/02/04. 10.1038/ncomms10580 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Delezie J, Dumont S, Sandu C, Reibel S, Pevet P, Challet E. Rev-erbalpha in the brain is essential for circadian food entrainment. Sci Rep. 2016;6:29386 Epub 2016/07/07. 10.1038/srep29386 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Smit AN, Patton DF, Michalik M, Opiol H, Mistlberger RE. Dopaminergic regulation of circadian food anticipatory activity rhythms in the rat. PLoS One. 2013;8(11):e82381 Epub 2013/12/07. 10.1371/journal.pone.0082381 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Gallardo CM, Darvas M, Oviatt M, Chang CH, Michalik M, Huddy TF, et al. Dopamine receptor 1 neurons in the dorsal striatum regulate food anticipatory circadian activity rhythms in mice. Elife. 2014;3:e03781 Epub 2014/09/14. 10.7554/eLife.03781 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.LeSauter J, Balsam PD, Simpson EH, Silver R. Overexpression of striatal D2 receptors reduces motivation thereby decreasing food anticipatory activity. Eur J Neurosci. 2020;51(1):71–81. Epub 2018/10/27. 10.1111/ejn.14219 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Wise RA. Dopamine, learning and motivation. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2004;5(6):483–94. Epub 2004/05/21. 10.1038/nrn1406 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Bjorklund A, Dunnett SB. Dopamine neuron systems in the brain: an update. Trends Neurosci. 2007;30(5):194–202. Epub 2007/04/06. 10.1016/j.tins.2007.03.006 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Solinas M, Belujon P, Fernagut PO, Jaber M, Thiriet N. Dopamine and addiction: what have we learned from 40 years of research. J Neural Transm (Vienna). 2019;126(4):481–516. Epub 2018/12/21. 10.1007/s00702-018-1957-2 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Darvas M, Palmiter RD. Restricting dopaminergic signaling to either dorsolateral or medial striatum facilitates cognition. J Neurosci. 2010;30(3):1158–65. Epub 2010/01/22. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4576-09.2010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Darvas M, Palmiter RD. Contributions of striatal dopamine signaling to the modulation of cognitive flexibility. Biol Psychiatry. 2011;69(7):704–7. Epub 2010/11/16. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.09.033 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Araki KY, Sims JR, Bhide PG. Dopamine receptor mRNA and protein expression in the mouse corpus striatum and cerebral cortex during pre- and postnatal development. Brain Res. 2007;1156:31–45. Epub 2007/05/19. 10.1016/j.brainres.2007.04.043 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Beaulieu JM, Gainetdinov RR. The physiology, signaling, and pharmacology of dopamine receptors. Pharmacol Rev. 2011;63(1):182–217. Epub 2011/02/10. 10.1124/pr.110.002642 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Liu YY, Liu TY, Qu WM, Hong ZY, Urade Y, Huang ZL. Dopamine is involved in food-anticipatory activity in mice. J Biol Rhythms. 2012;27(5):398–409. Epub 2012/09/27. 10.1177/0748730412455913 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Hood S, Cassidy P, Cossette MP, Weigl Y, Verwey M, Robinson B, et al. Endogenous dopamine regulates the rhythm of expression of the clock protein PER2 in the rat dorsal striatum via daily activation of D2 dopamine receptors. J Neurosci. 2010;30(42):14046–58. Epub 2010/10/22. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2128-10.2010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Ferris MJ, Espana RA, Locke JL, Konstantopoulos JK, Rose JH, Chen R, et al. Dopamine transporters govern diurnal variation in extracellular dopamine tone. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2014;111(26):E2751–9. Epub 2014/07/01. 10.1073/pnas.1407935111 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Mendoza J, Challet E. Circadian insights into dopamine mechanisms. Neuroscience. 2014;282:230–42. Epub 2014/10/05. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2014.07.081 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Chung S, Lee EJ, Yun S, Choe HK, Park SB, Son HJ, et al. Impact of circadian nuclear receptor REV-ERBalpha on midbrain dopamine production and mood regulation. Cell. 2014;157(4):858–68. Epub 2014/05/13. 10.1016/j.cell.2014.03.039 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Grippo RM, Purohit AM, Zhang Q, Zweifel LS, Guler AD. Direct Midbrain Dopamine Input to the Suprachiasmatic Nucleus Accelerates Circadian Entrainment. Curr Biol. 2017;27(16):2465–75 e3. Epub 2017/08/07. 10.1016/j.cub.2017.06.084 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Blum ID, Zhu L, Moquin L, Kokoeva MV, Gratton A, Giros B, et al. A highly tunable dopaminergic oscillator generates ultradian rhythms of behavioral arousal. Elife. 2014;3 Epub 2014/12/30. 10.7554/eLife.05105 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Mistlberger RE. Food as circadian time cue for appetitive behavior. F1000Res. 2020;9 Epub 2020/02/13. 10.12688/f1000research.20829.1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Honma K, Honma S. The SCN-independent clocks, methamphetamine and food restriction. Eur J Neurosci. 2009;30(9):1707–17. Epub 2009/11/03. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2009.06976.x . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Michalik M, Steele AD, Mistlberger RE. A sex difference in circadian food-anticipatory rhythms in mice: Interaction with dopamine D1 receptor knockout. Behav Neurosci. 2015;129(3):351–60. Epub 2015/06/02. 10.1037/bne0000058 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Skarnes WC, Rosen B, West AP, Koutsourakis M, Bushell W, Iyer V, et al. A conditional knockout resource for the genome-wide study of mouse gene function. Nature. 2011;474(7351):337–42. Epub 2011/06/17. 10.1038/nature10163 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Steele AD, Jackson WS, King OD, Lindquist S. The power of automated high-resolution behavior analysis revealed by its application to mouse models of Huntington’s and prion diseases. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2007;104(6):1983–8. Epub 2007/01/31. 10.1073/pnas.0610779104 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Schindelin J, Arganda-Carreras I, Frise E, Kaynig V, Longair M, Pietzsch T, et al. Fiji: an open-source platform for biological-image analysis. Nat Methods. 2012;9(7):676–82. Epub 2012/06/30. 10.1038/nmeth.2019 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Sarinana J, Kitamura T, Kunzler P, Sultzman L, Tonegawa S. Differential roles of the dopamine 1-class receptors, D1R and D5R, in hippocampal dependent memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2014;111(22):8245–50. Epub 2014/05/21. 10.1073/pnas.1407395111 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Zhang J, Zhang L, Jiao H, Zhang Q, Zhang D, Lou D, et al. c-Fos facilitates the acquisition and extinction of cocaine-induced persistent changes. J Neurosci. 2006;26(51):13287–96. Epub 2006/12/22. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3795-06.2006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Vong L, Ye C, Yang Z, Choi B, Chua S Jr., Lowell BB. Leptin action on GABAergic neurons prevents obesity and reduces inhibitory tone to POMC neurons. Neuron. 2011;71(1):142–54. Epub 2011/07/13. 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.05.028 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Gangarossa G, Espallergues J, Mailly P, De Bundel D, de Kerchove d’Exaerde A, Herve D, et al. Spatial distribution of D1R- and D2R-expressing medium-sized spiny neurons differs along the rostro-caudal axis of the mouse dorsal striatum. Front Neural Circuits. 2013;7:124 Epub 2013/08/03. 10.3389/fncir.2013.00124 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Scarpa LLWB, Smidt M, Mistlberger RE, Steele AD. Mice hypomorphic for Pitx3 define a minimal dopamine neuron population sufficient for entraining behavior and metabolism to scheduled feeding. BioRxiv. 2020; 10.1101/2020.10.23.353193 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Drago J, Gerfen CR, Lachowicz JE, Steiner H, Hollon TR, Love PE, et al. Altered striatal function in a mutant mouse lacking D1A dopamine receptors. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1994;91(26):12564–8. Epub 1994/12/20. 10.1073/pnas.91.26.12564 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Kobayashi M, Iaccarino C, Saiardi A, Heidt V, Bozzi Y, Picetti R, et al. Simultaneous absence of dopamine D1 and D2 receptor-mediated signaling is lethal in mice. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2004;101(31):11465–70. Epub 2004/07/24. 10.1073/pnas.0402028101 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Gallardo CM, Hsu CT, Gunapala KM, Parfyonov M, Chang CH, Mistlberger RE, et al. Behavioral and neural correlates of acute and scheduled hunger in C57BL/6 mice. PLoS One. 2014;9(5):e95990 Epub 2014/05/09. 10.1371/journal.pone.0095990 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Steele AD, Mistlberger RE. Activity is a slave to many masters. Elife. 2015;4:e06351 Epub 2015/02/13. 10.7554/eLife.06351 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Grippo RM, Tang Q, Zhang Q, Chadwick SR, Gao Y, Altherr EB, et al. Dopamine Signaling in the Suprachiasmatic Nucleus Enables Weight Gain Associated with Hedonic Feeding. Curr Biol. 2020;30(2):196–208 e8. Epub 2020/01/07. 10.1016/j.cub.2019.11.029 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

Paul A Bartell

8 Dec 2020

PONE-D-20-35295

Type 1 dopamine receptor (D1R)-independent circadian food anticipatory activity in mice

PLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Steele,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

It is important that, in light of the sex differences that have been observed in FAA, that the authors analyze their data for differences among sexes. 

Please submit your revised manuscript by Jan 22 2021 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Paul A. Bartell

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

2. To comply with PLOS ONE submissions requirements, please provide methods of sacrifice in the Methods section of your manuscript.

3. Please include your tables as part of your main manuscript and remove the individual files. Please note that supplementary tables should be uploaded as separate "supporting information" files.

4. Thank you for stating the following in the Acknowledgments Section of your manuscript:

"Funding was provided by the Whitehall Foundation, California State

Polytechnic University Pomona, CSU Program for Education and Research in

Biotechnology, and the National Institute of General Medical Sciences of the

National Institutes of General Medical Sciences of the National Institutes of Health

under Award Number SC3GM125570."

We note that you have provided funding information that is not currently declared in your Funding Statement. However, funding information should not appear in the Acknowledgments section or other areas of your manuscript. We will only publish funding information present in the Funding Statement section of the online submission form.

Please remove any funding-related text from the manuscript and let us know how you would like to update your Funding Statement. Currently, your Funding Statement reads as follows:

"National Institute of General Medical Sciences of the National Institutes of General Medical Sciences of the National Institutes of Health under Award Number SC3GM125570 to AS. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health."

Please include your amended statements within your cover letter; we will change the online submission form on your behalf.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Partly

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: The authors used several gene ablation approaches to study the role of D1 dopamine receptor singaling in food anticipatory activity in mice fed 60% of their average ad lib food intake. In all, their data reveal a small effect of loss of D1DR signaling on the level of activity prior to daily feeding. Although not addressed in the paper, these manipulations appeared not to influence the timing of increased activity. This is nicely written paper attempting to shed new light on a long-standing issue in the study of circadian entrainment by feeding schedules, specifically the mechanisms that mediate the heightened activity observed prior to feeding. Unfortunately, in its present form it falls short of adding significant information on the topic. In particular, all experiments were carried-out in both male and female mice (as they should!). However, the reported results do not distinguish between the sexes and data from males and females are pulled together. This is an important issue given evidence of sex differences in food anticipatory activity, and evidence of an important role of female sex hormones on striatal DA receptor-mediated signaling. To gain better insight into the importance of D1DR signaling in FAA, and possibly to discover sexual dimorphism in their contribution, the authors are encouraged to reanalyze and report the data for males and females separately.

Reviewer #2: This is a follow up study on Gallardo et al. 2014 published by the same group. In the initial study, they found marked reduction in FAA upon global DRD1 KO. Here, the authors now conclude that the reduction is rather moderate suggesting that other dopamine receptors might contribute to FAA. In support of their revised claim, the authors present FAA data on Vgat-Cre FloxD1R, global D1RKO, global D1RKO resurrected from cryopreserved embryos, and a different D1RKO line.

The authors used timed 60% caloric restriction to induce FAA assessed by quantifying video-taped ‘high’ activities (jumping hanging rearing ambulating) during the 2hrs prior food replenishment which occurred at ZT6.

The authors found statistical differences in the absolute and relative amounts of FAA in all 4 models, however the dot plots clearly indicate that a fraction of the KO animals still showed FAA more or less indifferent from controls which is also acknowledged by the authors.

The authors confirm that D1 receptor expression is undetectable in the striatum across all models used, arguing against inefficiencies in gene disruption to explain the ‘milder’ phenotype.

Based on these findings, the authors arrive at the conclusion that DRD1 signalling contributes to FAA manifestation but that other dopamine receptors are likely involved as well.

This work is important as it revises previous findings central to the topic of food anticipation, even though the revision is slight.

Perhaps more importantly, the presented data puts the notion of an involvement of DRD1 in FAA on a stronger footing due to the use of different models reporting largely similar deficits.

Comments:

As the Cryo D1KO showed the strongest phenotype it can be argued that some genetic drift perhaps happened, the authors may want to discuss this point (it seems a bit buried). The other genetic model, is it a transcriptional stop knock in? Some detail would be helpful here, considering that stops can be leaky (although the immuno indeed tells otherwise, at least as far as detectable DRD1 levels go).

Are there any GABA- DRD1 neurons? This should be perhaps addressed in the discussion.

I am not quite sure if the lack of obtaining KO offspring from the D1-Cre floxd1 cross should be mentioned in the abstract. The conclusion that the Cre transgene landed on the D1 encoding chromosome seems most parsimonious given that the D1KO phenotypes described in the literature are quite distant from embryonic lethality. Thus being likely only a technical issue, a brief mentioning in the results should suffice.

“The location of the neurons responsible for the anticipation of food and other related stimuli have been elusive”

Do we know if FAA relies on neurons solely? The FAA generator/mediator might include other cells and tissues….

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: No

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

Decision Letter 1

Paul A Bartell

28 Jan 2021

Type 1 dopamine receptor (D1R)-independent circadian food anticipatory activity in mice

PONE-D-20-35295R1

Dear Dr. Steele,

We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication.

An invoice for payment will follow shortly after the formal acceptance. To ensure an efficient process, please log into Editorial Manager at http://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the 'Update My Information' link at the top of the page, and double check that your user information is up-to-date. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

Kind regards,

Paul A. Bartell

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments (optional):

Reviewers' comments:

Acceptance letter

Paul A Bartell

29 Jan 2021

PONE-D-20-35295R1

Type 1 dopamine receptor (D1R)-independent circadian food anticipatory activity in mice

Dear Dr. Steele:

I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact onepress@plos.org.

If we can help with anything else, please email us at plosone@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access.

Kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Dr. Paul A. Bartell

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE


Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

RESOURCES