Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2021 Feb 9.
Published in final edited form as: Dev Psychopathol. 2020 Oct;32(4):1230–1239. doi: 10.1017/S0954579420000838

The role of caregiver speech in supporting language development in infants and toddlers with autism spectrum disorder

Meghan R Swanson 1
PMCID: PMC7872436  NIHMSID: NIHMS1662803  PMID: 32893764

Abstract

Parents play an essential role in supporting child development by providing a safe home, proper nutrition, and rich educational opportunities. In this article we focus on the role of caregiver speech in supporting development of young children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD). We review studies from typically developing children and children with autism showing that rich and responsive caregiver speech supports language development. Autism intervention studies that target caregiver speech are reviewed as are recent scientific advances from studies of typical development. The strengths and weakness of different techniques for collecting language data from caregivers and children are reviewed, and natural language samples are recommended as best practice for language research in autism. We conclude that caregivers play a powerful role in shaping their children’s development and encourage researchers to adapt parent-mediated intervention studies to acknowledge individual differences in parents by using a personalized medicine approach.

Keywords: autism spectrum disorder, ASD, caregiver speech, high familial risk, home language environment, language

Introduction

Parents play an essential role in supporting child development by providing a safe home, proper nutrition, and rich educational opportunities. In this article we focus on the role of caregiver speech in supporting development of young children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD). Autism is a neurodevelopmental disorder with a strong, but complex genetic basis, that is typically diagnosed after 4 years of age (Baio et al., 2018). Children with autism have deficits in social interaction and communication, and have restrictive or repetitive patterns of behaviors, interests, or activities (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). In this article, the history of researching parents of children with autism will be reviewed, and results from modern studies will be synthesized. To highlight future directions for autism research, we will review recent scientific advances from studies of typical development. We conclude that caregivers play a powerful role in shaping their children’s development. We also advocate that individual differences in caregivers should be taken into consideration if early intervention efforts are going to be maximally effective.

Early History of Research on Parents of Children with Autism

Before examining current research on caregiver speech to children with autism one must first acknowledge the historical context and sensitive nature of this topic. Early descriptions by Leo Kanner described parents of children with autism as displaying “coldness” and “obsessiveness” (Kanner, 1949). These descriptions led others to popularize the term “refrigerator mother” and to propagate the theory that a lack of maternal warmth causes autism in children. It is important to note that Kanner himself viewed autism as innate, and later in his career he was an outspoken advocate against blaming parents for their children’s autism diagnoses. Regardless, the “refrigerator mother” theory took hold in the 1950s and 1960s, facilitated primarily by Bruno Bettelheim who was instrumental in spreading the theory. Bettelheim himself has long been a controversial figure with accusations that he engaged in plagiarism, abusive treatment of students, and misrepresentation of credentials (he did not have a degree in child psychology, as claimed) (Pollak, 1998).

In the 1960s the prevailing theory of the cause of autism in both the public and the medical community was cold parenting. Bettelheim recommended that children with autism be removed from their homes and live in a therapeutic milieu institutional setting (Bettelheim & Sylvester, 1948). He also supported early research on the parents of institutionalized children which concluded that parents were not loving their children “right” (e.g., Henry, 1973). The popularization of this theory had profound effects on families of children with autism. Many parents experienced intense guilt and shame and were left trying to convince professionals that their children were indeed wanted and loved. Along the way, many parents lost trust in the professionals charged with helping their children. Belief in this misguided theory also meant that many researchers and psychotherapists were failing to intervene on actionable treatment targets.

By the mid 1960s critics of Bettelheim and the “refrigerator theory” of autism began to gain traction. Bernald Rimland was an early outspoken critic of Bettelheim, challenging his psychoanalytic perspective on autism and instead proposing a neurodevelopmental perspective (Rimland, 1964). Rimland’s neurodevelopmental perspective, informed by the observation of co-occurring seizures in many children, asserted that autism was caused by aberrant growth and development of the brain very early in life. This theory is directly supported by recent magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) studies showing atypical brain growth in young infants who go on to develop autism (Hazlett et al., 2017).

Rimland was an advocate for families affected by autism, a natural alliance since he was a parent of a child with autism himself. In 1965 Rimland, with the help of Ruth Sullivan and group of parents, went on to form the advocacy group National Society for Autistic Children (now called Autism Society of America). Since the founding of the National Society for Autistic Children and continuing today, parent advocacy groups have played an instrumental role in informing public policy, funding autism research, and shaping public perception.

Today, the “refrigerator theory” of autism has been widely disproven; however, the damage of this era of autism research is enduring. It is because of this history that some in the autism community feel that mothers of children with autism should not be the focus of research. For a discussion on the topic see Tager-Flusberg (2016). In this article, we discuss how caregiver behaviors support child development. This article does not subscribe to the notion, nor promote the idea that caregiver behavior can cause autism. Rather, this article promotes a strength-based approach to understanding individual differences in caregiving. We also adopt an integrated resilience framework, in a broad sense, where parents promote positive infant development through the way they nurture, socialize, and communicate with infants (Lai & Szatmari, 2019; Masten, 2018). In the traditional resilience framework, resilience is demonstrated when adaptive outcomes are observed in the face of adversity. We endorse a broader framework where autism is not viewed as an adversity (Lai & Szatmari, 2019), while acknowledging that the genetic underpinning of autism contributes to risk of higher rates of language delay (Marrus et al., 2018; Swanson et al., 2017). In this framework children with autism who go on to have positive outcomes in terms of language and communication have demonstrated resilience. We present data showing that, much like how good nutrition supports physical growth, rich and responsive caregiver speech contributes to resilience by supporting language and communication development in young children with autism.

Early Language Skills are a Harbinger of Later Development in Typically Developing Children

Supporting early language skills is crucial because these early skills are related to school readiness (Fiorentino & Howe, 2004) and long-term development (Pace, Alper, Burchinal, Golinkoff, & Hirsh-Pasek, 2019). Early language skills are the best predictor of later academic outcomes across broad domains (Pace et al., 2019). For example, preschool language skills are positively associated with school-age language, reading, and mathematics skills (Pace et al., 2019). Early language skills also predict gains in subsequent language skills. Children who enter elementary school with the best language skills have the largest gains in reading between 1st and 3rd grade (Pace et al., 2019).

These cascading developmental events can be disrupted by exposure to a variety of social risk factors, like poverty, maternal depression, and low parental education. For example, exposure to multiple risk factors is related to lower levels of reading and mathematics skills (Burchinal, Roberts, Zeisel, Hennon, & Hooper, 2006). However, the negative effects of social risk factors can be mitigated by exposure to protective factors such as responsive and sensitive parenting. Research has shown that a stimulating childcare environment can mitigate the negative effects of being exposed to social risk factors (Burchinal et al., 2006). Early language skills also play a significant role in the predictive association between social risk factors and school readiness: early language skills mediate the relationship between social risk factors and academic achievement (Burchinal et al., 2006; Forget-Dubois et al., 2009).

The Role of Caregiver Speech in Supporting Typical Child Development

In the 1990s two seminal studies were published showing that hearing more caregiver speech during infancy results in better language skills later in life (Hart & Risley, 1995; Huttenlocher, Haight, Bryk, Seltzer, & Lyons, 1991). For both studies research staff went into the families’ homes and recorded mothers interacting with their children. They then transcribed the audio files, a time-consuming process that yields estimates of the quantity of caregiver language exposure and estimates of infant vocabulary growth. Huttenlocher et al. (1991) found that girls and boys experienced similar levels of caregiver speech, but girls had faster vocabulary growth when compared to boys. These sex differences in vocabulary growth started to decline after 20 months and were no longer observable by the end of the second year of life. For both sexes, those children that heard the most caregiver speech also had the largest vocabulary growth.

Hart and Risley (1995) generated rich profiles of the language environment experienced by children of varied socioeconomic backgrounds. Every month for over two years they collected home language samples that were transcribed and annotated, generating a remarkable data set. Results indicated that parent’s socioeconomic status (SES), assigned based on occupation of both parents, was related to the amount of caregiver speech an infant experienced with high SES parents talking to their children more than parents from lower SES households. Hart and Risley famously coined the term “word gap,” espousing that by 3 years of age, low SES children heard 30 million fewer words than high SES children (Hart & Risley, 2003). In addition to disparities in quantity of caregiver speech, they also found SES differences in qualitative aspects of caregiver speech. Higher SES caregivers used more encouraging and less discouraging words when compared to lower SES caregivers. These early differences in experience had lasting developmental impacts. Children who heard the most caregiver speech as infants had the best vocabularies when they were 3 years old. Discussion of this study often focuses on the amount of speech, but Hart and Risley’s data actually showed that the richness of caregiver speech was more strongly related to later child development than the amount of caregiver speech.

All of the children in these seminal studies by Huttenlocher et al. (1991) and Hart and Risley (1995) learned to walk and talk, and by all accounts they developed typically. The lasting contribution of this work is the understanding that parents varied greatly in how much they talked to their children, and this variation was significantly associated with later performance on vocabulary and IQ tests. Hart and Risley (1995) concluded that rather than providing early intervention services to children who experience less caregiver speech, we should rather support and help parents. This call to action spurred decades of research and wide-scale public policy campaigns aimed to increase caregiver speech (e.g., Providence Speaks, Too Small to Fail).

The finding that early caregiver speech is associated with child language skills has been replicated many times since the initial studies were published (Cartmill et al., 2013; Hirsh-Pasek et al., 2015; Hoff, 2003; Hurtado, Marchman, & Fernald, 2008; Huttenlocher, Waterfall, Vasilyeva, Vevea, & Hedges, 2010; Pan, Rowe, Singer, & Snow, 2005; Rowe, 2008, 2012; Weisleder & Fernald, 2013), as have the findings that there are SES differences in caregiver speech (Cartmill et al., 2013; Hoff, 2003; Huttenlocher et al., 2010; Rowe, 2008, 2012). A recent paper challenged the size of the word gap (Sperry, Sperry, & Miller, 2018); however, there are noteworthy methodological distinctions between the Sperry et al. (2018) report and the original work by Hart and Risley (1995). For example, Sperry et al. (2018) do not include a high SES group, and their definition of language input is much broader than the original work by Hart and Risley (1995). Given the methodological differences between studies, claims that the Sperry et al. (2018) study refutes the findings of Hart and Risley (1995) should be evaluated with caution.

More recent studies have aimed to understand what types of caregiver speech best support language skills across development. There is now an understanding that caregiver speech patterns change as infants develop new communication skills, but our understanding of this developmental sequence is still being refined. Rowe and Zuckerman (2016) put forward a conceptual framework for how caregiver speech changes as infants develop. In this framework infants 0–6 months benefit the most from infant-directed speech, a speech style characterized by slower speech, elongated vowels, and wide ranges in pitch. Between 6 and 18 months infant’s gesture repertoire greatly expands and language learning is supported when parents point with their infants and contingently respond with object labeling to points by the infant. During the toddler years the use of diverse vocabulary, especially the use of wh-questions supports children’s expanding vocabularies. After the age 3 years, caregivers can support language skills by using more sophisticated language and using speech that includes the past and present tense. Several studies support this general shift from speech quantity to speech quality as the “key ingredients” to supporting infant language skills (Jones & Rowland, 2017; Ramírez-Esparza, García-Sierra, & Kuhl, 2014, 2017; Rowe, 2012).

Children with Autism Show Early and Persistent Difficulties with Language

Many children with autism show early and persistent difficulties with language. However, language skills in children with autism are highly variable, as are early trajectories of language development. Children with autism may show delays in reaching early language milestones such as canonical babbling and first word acquisition (Iverson & Wozniak, 2007; Mayo, Chlebowski, Fein, & Eigsti, 2013; Patten et al., 2014). When group-level language skills are explored, children with autism show deficits in language at 12 months of age, with these deficits becoming more pronounced at 24 months of age (Estes et al., 2015; Hudryet al., 2014; Swanson et al., 2017). These early difficulties with language persist for a substantial proportion of children with autism. About 29% of school-age children with autism display minimal language and another 24% produce words but not sentences (Anderson et al., 2007; Tager-Flusberg & Kasari, 2013). Language difficulties may also be more subtle, presenting as difficulties in sematic and pragmatic language (for a review of this literature see Tager-Flusberg, Edelson, & Rhiannon, 2011).

Language difficulties aggregate in families of children with autism, even for siblings without an autism diagnosis themselves. Siblings of children with autism have been shown to have higher rates of language delay (Gamliel, Yirmiya, & Sigman, 2007; Iverson & Wozniak, 2007; Marrus et al., 2018), and lower language scores (Gangi, Ibañez, & Messinger, 2014; Miller et al., 2015; Mitchell et al., 2006; Paul, Fuerst, Ramsay, Chawarska, & Klin, 2011; Seery, Tager-Flusberg, & Nelson, 2014; Swanson et al., 2017; Toth, Dawson, Meltzoff, Greenson, & Fein, 2007) than children with no familial risk for autism. Further, early language skills in children with autism represent a reliable predictor of later skills. A younger age of first word acquisition is significantly associated with better cognitive and adaptive skills (Kover, Edmunds, & Ellis Weismer, 2016; Mayo et al., 2013).

Caregiver Speech and Later Language Skills in Toddlers and Children with Autism

In 2002 Michael Siller and Marian Sigman published a study examining how mothers communicated with their young children with autism (Siller & Sigman, 2002). The study had two major findings. First, the authors reported that mothers of children with autism provided similar amounts of responsive verbal utterances as mothers of typically developing children. Second, the study was the first to report that responsive verbal utterances were significantly associated with better language 10 and 16 years later for children with autism (Siller & Sigman, 2002). Responsive verbal utterances, or “synchronized caregiver speech” were defined as instances when the caregiver makes a comment or reinforcing statement related to the child’s ongoing activity. Verbal responsiveness supports early child language skills by providing labels for the object or activity at the center of the child’s attention. This style of communication is responsive to the child’s focus of attention and does not require them to shift attention. This strategy may be especially effective for children with autism who may have relative deficits in social communication, including joint attention. Siller and Sigman went on to replicate their findings in an independent sample of children with autism (Siller & Sigman, 2008). These studies played a role in spurring others to investigate the role of caregivers in supporting language and cognitive development in children with autism.

Recent studies support early reports by showing that responsive caregiver communication is associated with gains in language skills across a 6-month time period when children with autism were 3–4 years old (McDuffie & Yoder, 2010). However, the potential benefit of responsive caregiving may rely on the child’s cognitive skills. Haebig, McDuffie, & Weismer (2013) found that children with autism with minimal language skills had larger language gains from responsive caregiver communication when compared to children with autism with fluent language skills. The engagement state of children also seems to influence potential benefits. Responsive caregiver language during times when the caregiver and child are playing collaboratively seems to support later language skills more so than when responsive caregiver language is used when the child and caregiver are not playing collaboratively (Bottema-Beutel, Yoder, Hochman, & Watson, 2014).

Caregiver Speech and the Home Language Environment in Infants with a Family History of Autism Spectrum Disorder

The advent of the “baby-sibling” research design where infant siblings of older children with autism are prospectively followed was another paradigm shifting moment for the autism field. For families with one child with autism, the risk for autism is higher in subsequently born infants. Population-based cohort studies estimate recurrence risk to be around 10%, whereas baby-sibling convenience samples estimate recurrent risk to be closer to 20% (Ozonoff et al., 2011; Sandin et al., 2014). These recurrence risk rates indicate that most families impacted by autism are simplex families, meaning there is one child with autism in the family. Multiplex families, where there is more than one child with autism in the family, represent the minority of families.

The baby-sibling research design provided a window into the development of infants who would later be diagnosed with autism. For the first time researchers could describe autism during infancy, before clinically reliable diagnoses are available. Over the last decade, these studies have documented a pre-symptomatic period early in infancy where the defining behavioral features of autism are not yet present (Estes et al., 2015; Ozonoff et al., 2010; Wolff et al., 2014). However, atypical neurodevelopment does begin during this period (Shen et al., 2017; Wolff et al., 2012), and pre-symptomatic brain features can accurately predict later autism diagnoses (Emerson et al., 2017; Hazlett et al., 2017). The results of these studies could drastically lower the age of identification and age of entry into treatment for children from multiplex families. Further research using population-based sampling strategies will be needed to determine if these findings extend to children with autism from simplex families.

The baby-sibling design has been used to examine early parent–child interactions and to quantify aspects of the home language environment. Overall, mothers of high- and low-risk infants provide equally rich linguistic environments (Campbell, Leezenbaum, Mahoney, Day, & Schmidt, 2015; Leezenbaum, Campbell, Butler, & Iverson, 2014; Swanson et al., 2018; Talbott, Nelson, & Tager-Flusberg, 2015). However, there are subtle differences in parent–infant vocal and play interactions when comparing high- and low-risk dyads. For example, among 9-month-old high-risk infants and their mothers, a greater amount of simultaneous speech was associated with later language delay (Northrup & Iverson, 2015). Simultaneous speech was defined as times when the infant and parent were talking at the same time.

Several studies have reported that high-and low-risk mothers show statistically equivalent levels of parental responsiveness (e.g., appropriate engagement to the infant that is contingent and developmentally appropriate) both when high risk is compared to low risk, and when infants who go on to have autism are separated into their own group (Wan et al., 2013, 2012). These studies show that mothers of high-risk infants are just as sensitive when responding to their infant as low-risk mothers. However, levels of non-directiveness do seem to differ by group, with high-risk parents being rated as more directive than their low-risk peers (Wan et al., 2013, 2012). Non-directive behaviors focus on the infant’s experience and current activity, while directive behaviors include prohibiting infant behavior, vocal demands, and requesting the infant to shift attention from a preferred activity. This finding that parents of high-risk infants are more directive may be a spillover effect learned from interacting with an older child with autism. The older child with autism may have limited functional play skills and in response the parent may have adapted a more directive play style.

Subtle group differences in caregiver speech may also emerge as high-risk infants display difficulties with the development of social-communicative skills. Choi et al. (Choi, Nelson, Rowe, & Tager-Flusberg, 2020) found that parents of high-risk infants and low-risk parents used similarly complex sentences when speaking to their 12-month-old infants, but high-risk parents used sentences with simpler complexity when speaking to their 18-month-old infants. It may be the case that parents of high-risk infants adjust their language to match their infant’s communication skills (Choi et al., 2020).

A recent report took a different approach and examined caregiver speech to infants using day-long home language recordings (Swanson et al., 2019). Home language recordings were collected when infants were 9 and 15 months of age and language skills were assessed when infants were 24 months of age. Across all infants in the study, including those with autism diagnoses, infants who heard more caregiver speech at 9 or 15 months had better language skills later in life. This pattern of results held true for both measures of the quantity and quality of early caregiver speech. This study also showed that caregiver speech mediated the relationship between maternal education and later child language skills. Mothers in the study with higher educational attainment talked more to their children, and in turn their children had better language skills (Swanson et al., 2019).

Results also revealed that there are dynamic changes in parent–child communication across the first two years of life, and these patterns may differ in typically developing children and children with autism (Swanson et al., 2019). Typically developing children experience a decrease in adult words and a compensatory increase in conversational turn counts over the 9–15-month time period. These findings likely reflect the expanding communication repertoire of typically developing children during this time frame. Swanson et al. (2019) saw a different pattern of results in children later diagnosed with autism. These infants also experienced a decrease in adult words counts across 9–15 months, but they did not experience an increase in conversational turn counts. These results could be reflecting an early disruption in the social feedback loop (Warlaumont, Richards, Gilkerson, & Oller, 2014). In summary, caregiver behaviors during infancy have a significant impact on development and caregiver speech may be a critical medium to support development in infants who go on to receive an autism diagnosis.

Caregiver Speech as an Intervention Target

Promising results from studies of typically developing children and children with autism showing the effects of responsive caregiving on child language skills contributed to the development of intervention studies targeting responsive caregiving. To date, at least 27 studies have been published outlining results of parent-training interventions on language development of children with autism (Roberts, Curtis, Sone, & Hampton, 2019). On average, these studies report large effect sizes on parent outcomes and medium effect sizes on child language skills (Roberts et al., 2019). These intervention studies provide evidence that, with instruction, parents can learn responsive communication strategies in the course of a short intervention, and that these changes support child language skills. We outline a selection of notable studies below.

Based on their early results Michael Siller and Marian Sigman designed Focused Playtime Intervention, a parent-mediated intervention for toddlers and young children with autism designed to support family capacity building. They tested the approach first in minimally verbal preschoolers with autism and results indicated positive treatment effects for parent responsive verbal behaviors (Siller, Hutman, & Sigman, 2013) and parents’ capacity for reflection and self-evaluation (Siller et al., 2018). There were also positive treatment effects for child language gains (Siller et al., 2013) and child attachment behaviors (Siller, Swanson, Gerber, Hutman, & Sigman, 2014). Results from a subsequent multisite study of toddlers showing early signs of autism (identified in the community) indicated positive treatment effects for responsive verbal behaviors, but these effects were not maintained at the one-year follow-up (Kasari et al., 2014).

Green and colleagues carried out the Preschool Autism Communication Trial (PACT), a large and rigorous randomized study of toddlers with autism, employing trial registration, double-blind assessment procedures, an independent statistician, and long-term follow-up. Children who had participated in PACTS had positive treatment effects for autism severity and responsive verbal behaviors, but insignificant treatment effects for child language skills (Green et al., 2010). Follow-up analyses indicated that the treatment effects on autism severity were mediated by responsive verbal behaviors (Pickles et al., 2015). Children who participated in PACT had reduced autism symptoms at a long-term follow-up assessment over 6 years later, but the treatment and control groups no longer differed in responsive verbal behaviors (Pickles et al., 2016). One interpretation of these findings is that responsive verbal behaviors have a larger impact early in development when pivotal skills are emerging when compared to later in development.

Another large randomized trial of preschoolers with autism echoes the PACT results, with positive treatment effects for responsive parental behaviors, but no significant effects for child language skills (Solomon, Egeren, Van Egeren, Mahoney, Huber, & Zimmerman, 2014). These null results for language skills could be a result of many causes, including issues related to the outcome measure (e.g., insufficient dimensionality, floor effects) and the intervention (e.g., approach not specific enough, low fidelity).

Caregiver Speech as an Intervention Target for Pre-symptomatic Infant Studies

Parent-mediated interventions including components of responsive caregiver behaviors have been downward extended into infancy using the baby-sibling research design. The overarching goal of many of these “pre-emptive interventions” is to mitigate risk and modify developmental trajectories, not to eliminate the disorder or condition (Insel, 2007). Reliable diagnostics are not available for autism until the second year of life so the infant intervention studies carried out to date have randomized across high-risk participants, with no additional selection criteria (with one exception, see Rogers et al., 2014 below), regardless of eventual diagnostic status. Since the intervention is then applied to infants who will go on to have autism as well as those who go on to be typically developing, ethical considerations require that the intervention be beneficial to all participants (and not only those with autism). It should be noted that parents find these approaches acceptable and enjoyable, and they find that the intervention provides tools to better understand their infants’ behavior (Green et al., 2013).

The iBASIS trial was a low-intensity randomized control trial testing a social communication intervention for infants at high familial risk for autism (Green et al., 2015, 2017). Results for this trial indicated significant intervention effects for parental synchrony and infant attentiveness to caregiver (Green et al., 2015). However, they did not find intervention effects for diagnostic outcome or child language skills (Green et al., 2017). In a different study of similar intensity, the “Promoting First Relationships” intervention for high-risk infants showed intervention effects on neurocognitive metrics including reduction in habitation time to faces during an eye-tracking task and increased frontal theta power during an electroencephalography (EEG) task (Jones, Dawson, Kelly, Estes, & Webb, 2017). The use of two “low-risk” control samples allowed the researchers to determine that the intervention resulted in more normalized neurocognitive metrics. Rogers and colleagues used yet a different approach and tested an infant version of the Early Start Denver Model called Infant Start in a small pilot study (n = 7 in the treatment group). In Infant Start, infants either had an older sibling with autism or were identified through the community (e.g., parents or clinicians were concerned about early signs of autism and referred the infant) (Rogers et al., 2014). Results of this pilot study found lower rates of autism and higher developmental functioning in infants who had received the treatment when compared to infants in the control group.

The studies reviewed have several limitations to consider, but the preliminary results are promising and warrant further investigation. Of the studies reviewed, two of the three were randomized control trials (Green et al., 2015, 2017; Jones et al., 2017), and all have yet to be replicated. The samples of all studies were small, with the largest study treating 28 infants (Green et al., 2015, 2017). Last, the samples in these studies were overwhelmingly white and from middle and upper SESs. All of the studies were either parent-mediated or included parent coaching studies. However, only one study directly tested the effects of the intervention on parent behaviors, reporting that parents in the intervention decreased their directiveness during play (Green et al., 2015).

Future infant intervention efforts will also likely use parent-mediated approaches. Parent-mediated approaches are advantageous as they make infant intervention feasible, ecologically valid, scalable, and cost-effective. These approaches are also in line with recommended practice for early intervention services for toddlers with autism (Schertz, Baker, Hurwitz, & Benner, 2011) and the guiding principles from Part C (Individuals with Disabilities Education Improvement Act of 2004, IDEIA). For example, key principles for early intervention for autism include, in part, that (a) families play a central role in the intervention, (b) the resources, priorities, and concerns of the family should be taken into consideration, as should the family’s cultural and ethnic context, (c) the intervention should be carried out in the child’s natural environment, and (d) the intervention should promote child-initiative learning and supporting functional skills (Schertz et al., 2011). Taking a developmental perspective, commencing intervention before an infant shows delays in reaching language milestones and before difficulties in social interaction are present would have the largest impact on later development (Adamson, Kaiser, Tamis-LaMonda, Owen, & Dimitrova, 2020).

Questions about the design of infant intervention are still being debated, but there is also the question of who should be the target population. It would certainly be unfeasible to provide infant intervention to all younger siblings of children with autism. Studies by the Infant Brain Imaging Study (IBIS) Network showing that brain development in the first year of life can independently predict later autism diagnoses at an individual level (Emerson et al., 2017; Hazlett et al., 2017) highlight how future studies could leverage neurobiology to target only those at ultra-high risk for later autism. A criticism of this approach, using MRI-based screening of high-risk infants to determine who should receive treatment, is the cost associated with MRI brain scans. A recent report counters this claim, with analyses showing that MRI-based screening is a cost-effective strategy for determining which high-risk infants should receive intensive early intervention (Williamson, Elison, Wolff, & Runge, 2020). Early intensive behavioral intervention is a costly endeavor, but these upfront costs are offset by long-term societal and economic savings (Jacobson, Mulick, & Green, 1998; Penner et al., 2015).

Methodological Considerations Related to Measuring Infant Language Skills and Caregiver Speech

There are several methodological considerations related to measuring caregiver speech and infant language development that warrant discussion. The majority of infant autism studies of parent–child interactions measure caregiver speech and infant vocalizations from short, 10–13 min videos (Campbell et al., 2015; Chericoni et al., 2016; Heymann et al., 2018; Ozonoff et al., 2010; Schwichtenberg, Kellerman, Young, Miller, & Ozonoff, 2018; Walton & Ingersoll, 2015). Short video recordings produce a different account of the language heard by a child when compared to day-long audio recordings collected in the home (Bergelson, Amatuni, Dailey, Koorathota, & Tor, 2018). Specifically, assessments of short videos, compared to day-long audio recordings, revealed caregivers produced more questions, fewer declaratives, more nouns, and higher lexical diversity during short videos (Bergelson et al., 2018). Bergelson and colleagues concluded that short videos provide a dense sample of the infant’s experience, rather than an account of the typical experience. In sum, short videos were similar to peak audio hours. Day-long audio recordings may generate more ecologically valid data because families may find it easier to behave freely when long-format audio recordings are collected versus short video recordings. Given these findings, day-long language recorders may be the optimal strategy if the goal of a study is to provide an account of what the infant typically experiences.

Measuring infant language and communication skills also requires a unique set of methodological considerations. Measures of expressive language skills are often an outcome measure for treatment studies, so the field requires language measures that are easy to obtain and are sensitive to change. Historically, treatment studies have used standardized assessments or parent-report language questionnaires as outcome measures. Standardized assessments provide a direct test of the child’s language skills, but during infancy standard scores on these tests may be subject to floor effects that limit the dimensionality of the data (Klein-Tasman, Phillips, Lord, Mervis, & Gallo, 2009). Standardized tests are often administered before, during, and after an intervention, all within a brief time frame, making practice effects a potential issue. Practice effects may inflate scores on standardize assessments resulting in an inaccurate picture of the child’s actual abilities. Both of these potential issues, floor effects and practice effects, can add noise to data and conceal treatment effects. An alternative to direct assessments is parent-report language measures, like the MacArthur-Bates Communication Development Inventories. A major limitation of parent-report measures is that they are susceptible to placebo effects in the context of an intervention (Guastella et al., 2015).

Both direct assessments and parent report measures provide normed summary-level data; however, they do not capture the multivariate aspect of pre-linguistic skills. Natural language samples, like day-long home language recordings, are an ecologically valid, multivariate approach that are not subject to many of the limitations of direct assessments or parent-report measures (Barokova & Tager-Flusberg, 2018; Tager-Flusberg, 2000). Day-long home language recordings capture spontaneous expressive language in an environment where the infant is comfortable. After data collection these recordings can be analyzed and coded for multivariate language and communication features. Coding can be tailored to the goals of the intervention. This approach is not susceptible to placebo effects or practice effects, and floor effects can be mitigated by selecting appropriate variables for coding. Importantly, day-long home language recordings capture the infant’s actual abilities and how they communicate in everyday life. Day-long home language recordings may be an approach that combats the extreme sampling bias in developmental science. The approach has been used successfully with diverse samples including families that are low-income, from varied ethnic and racial backgrounds, and non-English speaking (Suskind et al., 2016; Weisleder & Fernald, 2013; Wood, Diehm, & Callender, 2016). The relative methodological advantages of natural language samples have led experts to recommend natural language samples as best practice for language research in autism (Barokova & Tager-Flusberg, 2018; Tager-Flusberg, 2000).

Considerations and Limitations Specific to Day-long Home Language Samples

Day-long home language recordings provide the researcher with a window into the child’s linguistic world. The benefits to this research strategy are outlined above, but there are also limitations and unique considerations to consider when using this approach. For a comprehensive guide to collecting day-long home language recordings and the ethics of such data collection we refer the reader to Casillas and Cristia (2019) and Cychosz et al. (2020), respectively. We review some of these practical and ethical considerations below.

The collection of day-long home language recordings poses complex ethical considerations related to privacy. If a child is wearing the recorder in public there is the potential to capture the voice of someone who has not provided informed consent. Researchers should be informed of state consent laws related to audio recordings that could impact their ability to record in public. One strategy to mitigate these privacy concerns is to have families complete the recording when they are at home for the day. It is more feasible to notify anyone coming into a participant’s home of the recording than to notify everyone encountered while at the grocery store, for example. Similar privacy concerns would pertain to recordings collected when the child is at daycare or school.

When collecting home language recordings, the consent process should be given special attention. Researchers should ensure that families understand that they can withdraw from the research study at any time. Families can also be given the additional option to have a recording deleted but stay enrolled in the study. Consent forms should outline these options and include any relevant information on mandatory reporting laws.

Investigators should carefully consider the reliability and validity of the research strategy before collecting data. Day-long home language recordings are best suited to answer research questions about broad phenomenon (e.g., quantity of speech), and less well suited to ask questions about rare phenomenon or linguistic features without a salient acoustic signature (detection of wh-questions) (Casillas & Cristia, 2019). A priori research questions will help to ensure the approach is valid. For example, if a researcher is interested in capturing the daily experience of the child (e.g., how many adult words does the child hear during the day), then accounting for time periods when the infant is asleep may not be necessary for valid data. However, if researchers are interested in capturing how many words the child hears per waking hour then accounting for nap time and sleep time is necessary.

The recording parameters related to the environment should also be carefully considered. Researchers may limit recordings to the home if the research question relates to speech exclusively by parents. This approach comes with the limitation that environments outside of the home (e.g., daycare) are not being measured. To understand how representative the recording is of the child’s environment parents can complete a childcare questionnaire. Some studies have limited recording sessions to weekend days (Thompson, Benítez-Barrera, Angley, Woynaroski, & Tharpe, 2020). However, this approach should be used with caution as the “work week” varies across sectors of employment. Low-income families are more likely to have individuals working non-standard hours than high-income families, so limiting recordings to weekend days has the potential to introduce unwanted bias in the data (Enchautegui, 2013).

Data from day-long home language recordings can be processed using automated analysis software or hand coded. Limitations of automated software include variability in accuracy and precision of available tools (Räsänen et al., 2019) and lack of information on the context of recorded speech. If the recorded data are to be annotated or transcribed, an appropriate sampling procedure needs to be used as it is often not feasible to annotate the entire day-long recording (see Casillas & Cristia (2019) for a description of different sampling procedures).

Future Directions and Conclusion

The research outlined in this report underscores the influential role caregivers play in children’s long-term development, while also highlighting the first year of life as a time when parent training could substantially support child development. If caregiver speech is pursued as a target for pre-symptomatic intervention studies, further research is needed to identify modifiable factors that could increase intervention efficacy. For example, there is a growing appreciation that SES is a distal factor influencing caregiver speech. More modifiable and proximal factors include: parent knowledge of child development, views on teaching and learning, and the stimulation, support, and structure in the home (Rowe, 2008). Knowledge of child development fully mediates the relationship between SES and caregiver speech (e.g., the relationship between SES and caregiver speech was no longer significant when knowledge of child development was added to the model) (Rowe, 2008).

Expanding upon this work, Rowe & Leech (2019) administered a parent gesture intervention study that included growth mindset training. A growth mindset is the belief that intelligence in malleable. A fixed mindset, on the other hand, is the belief that intelligence is fixed. Parents who entered the study with a fixed mindset, but then received growth mindset training, had children with the largest language gains. Children had smaller language gains if they had parents with a fixed mindset that did not receive intervention (Rowe & Leech, 2019). In other reports, parents in a reading intervention with embedded growth mindset training who reduced their “fixedness” belief had children with the largest gains in reading scores (Andersen & Nielsen, 2016), and mothers primed to use a growth mindset were more supportive of children during a difficult task (Moorman & Pomerantz, 2010). This research highlights how individual differences in parental beliefs can impact child learning and development in meaningful ways.

Applying a personalized medicine approach to parent-mediated autism interventions could increase intervention efficacy, lower costs, and improve child outcomes. As the autism field advances toward first-year autism detection and pre-symptomatic intervention, an evidence base to inform these interventions is needed. Future research should aim to understand what specific features of caregiver speech make ideal treatment targets, and what individual differences in the parents are related to these features. Additionally, large, rigorous treatment studies are needed to determine if innovative personalized medicine approaches are feasible. We conclude with two recommendations. First, we encourage researchers to use long-format, natural language samples to measure caregiver speech and infant vocal behavior. Second, we encourage researchers to adapt parent-mediated intervention studies to acknowledge individual differences in parents by using a personalized medicine approach.

Acknowledgments.

This work was supported by grants through the National Institutes of Health (R00-MH108700 PI Swanson). The funders had no role in study design, data collection, analysis, data interpretation, or the writing of the report. The author thanks Drs. Jed Elison and Dante Cicchetti for editing this Special Issue of Development and Psychopathology and for their helpful comments on earlier versions of this manuscript.

Footnotes

Conflict of Interest. None

References

  1. Adamson LB, Kaiser AP, Tamis-LaMonda CS, Owen MT, & Dimitrova N (2020). The developmental landscape of early parent-focused language intervention. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 50, 59–67. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2018.11.005 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  2. American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-5®). [DOI] [PubMed]
  3. Andersen SC, & Nielsen HS (2016). Reading intervention with a growth mindset approach improves children’s skills. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 113, 12111–12113. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1607946113 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Anderson DK, Lord C, Risi S, DiLavore PS, Shulman C, Thurm A, … Pickles A (2007). Patterns of growth in verbal abilities among children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 75, 594–604. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.75.4.594 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Baio J, Wiggins L, Christensen DL, Maenner MJ, Daniels J, Warren Z, … Dowling NF (2018). Prevalence of autism spectrum disorder among children aged 8 years—Autism and developmental disabilities monitoring network, 11 sites, United States, 2014. MMWR. Surveillance Summaries, 67, 1–23. doi: 10.15585/mmwr.ss6706a1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Barokova M, & Tager-Flusberg H (2018). Commentary: Measuring language change through natural language samples. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. Advance online publication. doi: 10.1007/s10803-018-3628-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bergelson E, Amatuni A, Dailey S, Koorathota S, & Tor S (2018). Day by day, hour by hour: Naturalistic language input to infants. Developmental Science, 22(1), e12715. doi: 10.1111/desc.12715. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Bettelheim B, & Sylvester E (1948). A therapeutic milieu. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 18, 191–206. doi: 10.1111/j.1939-0025.1948.tb05078.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bottema-Beutel K, Yoder PJ, Hochman JM, & Watson LR (2014). The role of supported joint engagement and parent utterances in language and social communication development in children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44, 2162–2174. doi: 10.1007/s10803-014-2092-z [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Burchinal M, Roberts JE, Zeisel SA, Hennon EA, & Hooper S (2006). Social risk and protective child, parenting, and child care factors in early elementary school years. Parenting, 6, 79–113. doi: 10.1207/s15327922par0601_4 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  11. Campbell SB, Leezenbaum NB, Mahoney AS, Day TN, & Schmidt EN (2015). Social engagement with parents in 11-month-old siblings at high and low genetic risk for autism spectrum disorder. Autism : The International Journal of Research and Practice, 19, 915–924. doi: 10.1177/1362361314555146 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Cartmill EA, Armstrong BF, Gleitman LR, Goldin-Meadow S, Medina TN, & Trueswell JC (2013). Quality of early parent input predicts child vocabulary 3 years later. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110, 11278–11283. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1309518110 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Casillas M, & Cristia A (2019). A step-by-step guide to collecting and analyzing long-format speech environment (LFSE) recordings. Collabra: Psychology, 5, 24. doi: 10.1525/collabra.209 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  14. Chericoni N, de Brito Wanderley D, Costanzo V, Diniz-Gonçalves A, Leitgel Gille M, Parlato E, … Muratori F (2016). Pre-linguistic vocal trajectories at 6–18 months of age as early markers of autism. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1595. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01595 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Choi B, Nelson CA, Rowe ML, & Tager-Flusberg H (2020). Reciprocal influences between parent input and child language skills in dyads involving high- and low-risk infants for autism spectrum disorder. Autism Research, aur.2270. Advance online publication. doi: 10.1002/aur.2270. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Cychosz M, Romeo R, Soderstrom M, Scaff C, Ganek H, Cristia A, … Weisleder A (2020). Longform recordings of everyday life: Ethics for best practices. Behavior Research Methods, 1–19. doi: 10.3758/s13428-020-01365-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Emerson RW, Adams C, Nishino T, Hazlett HC, Wolff JJ, Zwaigenbaum L, … Network I (2017). Functional neuroimaging of high-risk 6-month-old infants predicts a diagnosis of autism at 24 months of age. Science Translational Medicine, 9, eaag2882. doi: 10.1126/scitranslmed.aag2882 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Enchautegui ME (2013). Nonstandard Work Schedules and the Well-Being of Low-Income Families Low-Income Working Families. https://www.urban.org/sites/default/files/publication/32696/412877-Nonstandard-Work-Schedules-and-the-Well-being-of-Low-Income-Families.PDF
  19. Estes AM, Zwaigenbaum L, Gu H, St John T, Paterson S, Elison JT, …Network, I. B. I. S. (2015). Behavioral, cognitive, and adaptive development in infants with autism spectrum disorder in the first 2 years of life. Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders, 7, 24. doi: 10.1186/s11689-015-9117-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Fiorentino L, & Howe N (2004). Language competence, narrative ability, and school readiness in low-income preschool children. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science / Revue Canadienne Des Sciences Du Comportement, 36, 280–294. doi: 10.1037/h0087237 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  21. Forget-Dubois N, Dionne G, Lemelin J-PP, Pérusse D, Tremblay RE, & Boivin M (2009). Early child language mediates the relation between home environment and school readiness. Child Development, 80, 736–749. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01294.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Gamliel I, Yirmiya N, & Sigman M (2007). The development of young siblings of children with autism from 4 to 54 months. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 171–183. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0341-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Gangi DN, Ibañez LV, & Messinger DS (2014). Joint attention initiation with and without positive affect: Risk group differences and associations with ASD symptoms. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44, 1414–1424. doi: 10.1007/s10803-013-2002-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Green J, Charman T, McConachie H, Aldred C, Slonims V, Howlin P, … Pickles A (2010). Parent-mediated communication-focused treatment in children with autism (PACT): A randomised controlled trial. The Lancet, 375, 2152–2160. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(10)60587-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Green J, Charman T, Pickles A, Wan MW, Elsabbagh M, Slonims V, …Johnson MH (2015). Parent-mediated intervention versus no intervention for infants at high risk of autism: A parallel, single-blind, randomised trial. The Lancet Psychiatry, 2, 133–140. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(14)00091-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Green J, Pickles A, Pasco G, Bedford R, Wan MW, Elsabbagh M, … McNally J (2017). Randomised trial of a parent-mediated intervention for infants at high risk for autism: Longitudinal outcomes to age 3 years. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 58, 1330–1340. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12728 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Green J, Wan MW, Guiraud J, Holsgrove S, McNally J, Slonims V, …Johnson MH (2013). Intervention for infants at risk of developing autism: A case series. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43, 2502–2514. doi: 10.1007/s10803-013-1797-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Guastella AJ, Gray KM, Rinehart NJ, Alvares GA, Tonge BJ, Hickie IB, … Einfeld SL (2015). The effects of a course of intranasal oxytocin on social behaviors in youth diagnosed with autism spectrum disorders: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 56, 444–452. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12305 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Haebig E, Mcduffie A, & Weismer SE (2013). Brief report: Parent verbal responsiveness and language development in toddlers on the autism spectrum. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43(9), 2218–2227. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Hart B, & Risley TR (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children. Baltimore: P. H. Brookes. [Google Scholar]
  31. Hart B, & Risley TR (2003). The early catastrophe: The 30 million word gap by age 3. American Educator, 27, 4–9. [Google Scholar]
  32. Hazlett HC, Gu H, Munsell BC, Kim SH, Styner M, Wolff JJJ, …Network, I. B. I. S. (2017). Early brain development in infants at high risk for autism spectrum disorder. Nature, 542, 348–351. doi: 10.1038/nature21369 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Henry J (1972). Pathways to madness. USA: Vintage Books; ISBN-13: 9780224007146; ISBN-10: 0224007149 [Google Scholar]
  34. Heymann P, Northrup JB, West KL, Parladé MV, Leezenbaum NB, & Iverson JM (2018). Coordination is key: Joint attention and vocalisation in infant siblings of children with Autism Spectrum Disorder. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 53(5), 1007–1020. doi: 10.1111/1460-6984.12418. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Hirsh-Pasek K, Adamson LB, Bakeman R, Owen MT, Golinkoff RM, Pace A, … Suma K (2015). The contribution of early communication quality to low-income children’s language success. Psychological Science, 26, 1071–1083. doi: 10.1177/0956797615581493 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Hoff E (2003). The specificity of environmental influence: Socioeconomic status affects early vocabulary development via maternal speech. Child Development, 74, 1368–1378. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00612 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Hudry K, Chandler S, Bedford R, Pasco G, Gliga T, Elsabbagh M, … Charman T (2014). Early language profiles in infants at high-risk for autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44, 154–167. doi: 10.1007/s10803-013-1861-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Hurtado N, Marchman VA, & Fernald A (2008). Does input influence uptake? Links between maternal talk, processing speed and vocabulary size in Spanish-learning children. Developmental Science, 11, F31–F39. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00768.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Huttenlocher J, Haight W, Bryk A, Seltzer M, & Lyons T (1991). Early vocabulary growth: Relation to language input and gender. Developmental Psychology, 27, 236–248. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.27.2.236 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  40. Huttenlocher J, Waterfall H, Vasilyeva M, Vevea J, & Hedges LV (2010). Sources of variability in children’s language growth. Cognitive Psychology, 61, 343–365. doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2010.08.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Insel TR (2007). The arrival of preemptive psychiatry. Early Intervention in Psychiatry, 1, 5–6. doi: 10.1111/j.1751-7893.2007.00017.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Iverson JM, & Wozniak RH (2007). Variation in vocal-motor development in infant siblings of children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 158–170. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0339-z [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Jacobson JW, Mulick JA, & Green G (1998). Cost-benefit estimates for early intensive behavioral intervention for young children with autism—general model and single state case. Behavioral Interventions, 13, 201–226. doi: [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  44. Jones EJH, Dawson G, Kelly J, Estes A, & Webb SJ (2017). Parent-delivered early intervention in infants at risk for ASD: Effects on electrophysiological and habituation measures of social attention. Autism Research, 10, 961–972. doi: 10.1002/aur.1754 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Jones G, & Rowland CF (2017). Diversity not quantity in caregiver speech: Using computational modeling to isolate the effects of the quantity and the diversity of the input on vocabulary growth. Cognitive Psychology, 98, 1–21. doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2017.07.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Kanner L (1949). Problems of nosology and psychodynamics of early infantile autism. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 19, 416–426. doi: 10.1111/j.1939-0025.1949.tb05441.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Kasari C, Siller M, Huynh LN, Shih W, Swanson M, Hellemann GS, & Sugar CA (2014). Randomized controlled trial of parental responsiveness intervention for toddlers at high risk for autism. Infant Behavior and Development, 37, 711–721. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2014.08.007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Klein-Tasman BP, Phillips KD, Lord C, Mervis CB, & Gallo FJ (2009). Overlap with the autism spectrum in young children with Williams syndrome. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 30, 289–299. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0b013e3181ad1f9a [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Kover ST, Edmunds SR, & Ellis Weismer S (2016). Brief report: Ages of language milestones as predictors of developmental trajectories in young children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46, 2501–2507. doi: 10.1007/s10803-016-2756-y [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Lai M-C, & Szatmari P (2019). Resilience in autism: Research and practice prospects. Autism, 23, 539–541. doi: 10.1177/1362361319842964 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Leezenbaum NB, Campbell SB, Butler D, & Iverson JM (2014). Maternal verbal responses to communication of infants at low and heightened risk of autism. Autism : The International Journal of Research and Practice, 18, 694–703. doi: 10.1177/1362361313491327 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Marrus N, Hall LP, Paterson SJ, Elison JT, Wolff JJ, Swanson MR, … Constantino JN (2018). Language delay aggregates in toddler siblings of children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders, 10, 29. doi: 10.1186/s11689-018-9247-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Masten AS (2018). Resilience theory and research on children and families: Past, present, and promise. Journal of Family Theory & Review, 10, 12–31. doi: 10.1111/jftr.12255 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  54. Mayo J, Chlebowski C, Fein DA, & Eigsti I-MM (2013). Age of first words predicts cognitive ability and adaptive skills in children with ASD. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43, 253–264. doi: 10.1007/s10803-012-1558-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. McDuffie A, & Yoder P (2010). Types of parent verbal responsiveness that predict language in young children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 53, 1026. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2009/09-0023) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Miller M, Young GS, Hutman T, Johnson S, Schwichtenberg AJ, & Ozonoff S (2015). Early pragmatic language difficulties in siblings of children with autism: implications for DSM-5 social communication disorder? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 56, 774–781. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12342 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Mitchell S, Brian J, Zwaigenbaum L, Roberts W, Szatmari P, Smith I, & Bryson S (2006). Early language and communication development of infants later diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics : JDBP, 27, S69–78. doi: 10.1097/00004703-200604002-00004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Moorman EA, & Pomerantz EM (2010). Ability mindsets influence the quality of mothers’ involvement in children’s learning: An experimental investigation. Developmental Psychology, 46, 1354–1362. doi: 10.1037/a0020376 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Northrup JB, & Iverson JM (2015). Vocal coordination during early parent–infant interactions predicts language outcome in infant siblings of children with autism spectrum disorder. Infancy, 20, 523–547. doi: 10.1111/infa.12090 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Ozonoff S, Iosif A, Baguio F, Cook IC, Hill MM, Hutman T, …Young GS (2010). A prospective study of the emergence of early behavioral signs of autism. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 49, 256–66.e1–2. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2009.11.009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Ozonoff S, Young GS, Carter AS, Messinger D, Yirmiya N, Zwaigenbaum L, … Stone WL (2011). Recurrence risk for autism spectrum disorders: A Baby Siblings Research Consortium study. Pediatrics, 128, e488–95. doi: 10.1542/peds.2010-2825 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Pace A, Alper R, Burchinal MR, Golinkoff RM, & Hirsh-Pasek K (2019). Measuring success: Within and cross-domain predictors of academic and social trajectories in elementary school. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 46, 112–125. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2018.04.001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  63. Pan BA, Rowe ML, Singer JD, & Snow CE (2005). Maternal correlates of growth in toddler vocabulary production in low-income families. Child Development, 76(4), 763–782. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00876.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Patten E, Belardi K, Baranek GT, Watson LR, Labban JD, & Oller DK (2014). Vocal patterns in infants with autism spectrum disorder: canonical babbling status and vocalization frequency. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44, 2413–2428. doi:DOI 10.1007/s10803-014-2214-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Paul R, Fuerst Y, Ramsay G, Chawarska K, & Klin A (2011). Out of the mouths of babes: vocal production in infant siblings of children with ASD. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 52, 588–598. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02332.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Penner M, Rayar M, Bashir N, Roberts SW, Hancock-Howard RL, & Coyte PC (2015). Cost-effectiveness analysis comparing pre-diagnosis autism spectrum disorder (ASD)-targeted intervention with ontario’s autism intervention program. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45, 2833–2847. doi: 10.1007/s10803-015-2447-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Pickles A, Harris V, Green J, Aldred C, McConachie H, Slonims V, … Charman T (2015). Treatment mechanism in the MRC preschool autism communication trial: Implications for study design and parent-focussed therapy for children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 56, 162- 170. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12291 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Pickles A, Le Couteur A, Leadbitter K, Salomone E, Cole-Fletcher R, Tobin H, … Green J (2016). Parent-mediated social communication therapy for young children with autism (PACT): Long-term follow-up of a randomised controlled trial. The Lancet, 388(10059), P2501–2509. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(16)31229-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Pollak R (1998). The creation of doctor B: A biography of Bruno Bettelheim. New York, NY: Simon and Schuster. [Google Scholar]
  70. Ramírez-Esparza N, García-Sierra A, & Kuhl PK (2014). Look who’s talking: Speech style and social context in language input to infants are linked to concurrent and future speech development. Developmental Science, 17, 880–891. doi: 10.1111/desc.12172 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Ramírez-Esparza N, García-Sierra A, & Kuhl PK (2017). Look who’s talking NOW! Parentese speech, social context, and language development across time. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1008. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Räsänen O, Seshadri S, Karadayi J, Riebling E, Bunce J, Cristia A, … Soderstrom M (2019). Automatic word count estimation from daylong child-centered recordings in various language environments using language-independent syllabification of speech. Speech Communication, 113, 63–80. doi: 10.1016/J.SPECOM.2019.08.005. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  73. Rimland B (1964). Infantile autism: The syndrome and its implications for a neural theory of behavior. East Norwalk, CT, US: Appleton-Century-Crofts. [Google Scholar]
  74. Roberts MY, Curtis PR, Sone BJ, & Hampton LH (2019). Association of parent training with child language development: A systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Pediatrics, 173, 671–680. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2019.1197 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Rogers SJ, Vismara L, Wagner AL, McCormick C, Young G, & Ozonoff S (2014). Autism treatment in the first year of life: A pilot study of infant start, a parent-implemented intervention for symptomatic infants. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44, 2981–2995. doi: 10.1007/s10803-014-2202-y [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Rowe ML (2008). Child-directed speech: Relation to socioeconomic status, knowledge of child development and child vocabulary skill. Journal of Child Language, 35, 185–205. doi: 10.1017/S0305000907008343 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Rowe ML (2012). A longitudinal investigation of the role of quantity and quality of child-directed speech vocabulary development. Child Development, 83, 1762–1774. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01805.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Rowe ML, & Leech KA (2019). A parent intervention with a growth mindset approach improves children’s early gesture and vocabulary development. Developmental Science, 22, e12792. doi: 10.1111/desc.12792 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Rowe ML, & Zuckerman B (2016). Word gap redux. JAMA Pediatrics, 170, 827. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2016.1360 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Sandin S, Lichtenstein P, Kuja-Halkola R, Larsson H, Hultman CM, & Reichenberg A (2014). The familial risk of autism. JAMA, 311, 1770–1777. doi: 10.1001/jama.2014.4144 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Schertz HH, Baker C, Hurwitz S, & Benner L (2011). Principles of early intervention reflected in toddler research in autism spectrum disorders. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 31, 4–21. doi: 10.1177/0271121410382460 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  82. Schwichtenberg A, Kellerman AM, Young GS, Miller M, & Ozonoff S (2018). Mothers of children with autism spectrum disorders: Play behaviors with infant siblings and social responsiveness. Autism, 13623613187822223 (4), 821–833. doi: 10.1177/1362361318782220. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Seery A, Tager-Flusberg H, & Nelson CA (2014). Event-related potentials to repeated speech in 9-month-old infants at risk for autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders, 6, 43. doi: 10.1186/1866-1955-6-43 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Shen MD, Kim SH, McKinstry RC, Gu H, Hazlett HC, Nordahl CW, … Gu H (2017). Increased extra-axial cerebrospinal fluid in high-risk infants who later develop autism. Biological Psychiatry, 82, 186–193. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2017.02.1095 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Siller M, Hotez E, Swanson MR, Delavenne A, Hutman T, & Sigman M (2018). Parent coaching increases the parents’ capacity for reflection and self-evaluation: results from a clinical trial in autism. Attachment & Human Development, 20, 287–308. doi: 10.1080/14616734.2018.1446737 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Siller M, Hutman T, & Sigman M (2013). A parent-mediated intervention to increase responsive parental behaviors and child communication in children with ASD: A randomized clinical trial. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43, 540–555. doi: 10.1007/s10803-012-1584-y [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Siller M, & Sigman M (2002). The behaviors of parents of children with autism predict the subsequent development of their children’s communication. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 32, 77–89. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Siller M, & Sigman M (2008). Modeling longitudinal change in the language abilities of children with autism: parent behaviors and child characteristics as predictors of change. Developmental Psychology, 44, 1691–1704. doi: 10.1037/a0013771 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Siller M, Swanson MR, Gerber A, Hutman T, & Sigman M (2014). A parent-mediated intervention that targets responsive parental behaviors increases attachment behaviors in children with ASD: Results from a randomized clinical trial. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44, 1–13. doi: 10.1007/s10803-014-2049-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Solomon R, Van Egeren LA, Mahoney G, Huber MSQ, & Zimmerman P (2014). PLAY project home consultation intervention program for young children with autism spectrum disorders: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 35, 475. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0000000000000096 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Sperry DE, Sperry LL, & Miller PJ (2018). Reexamining the verbal environments of children from different socioeconomic backgrounds. Child Development, 00, 1–16. doi: 10.1111/cdev.13072 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Suskind DL, Leffel KR, Graf E, Hernandez MW, Gunderson EA, Sapolich SG, … LeVine SC (2016). A parent-directed language intervention for children of low socioeconomic status: A randomized controlled pilot study. Journal of Child Language, 43, 366–406. doi: 10.1017/S0305000915000033 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Swanson MR, Donovan K, Paterson S, Wolff JJ, Parish-Morris J, Meera SS, … Piven J (2019). Early language exposure supports later language skills in infants with and without autism. Autism Research, 12(12). doi: 10.1002/aur.2163. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Swanson MR, Shen MD, Wolff JJ, Boyd B, Clements M, Rehg J, …Network, I. B. I. S. (2018). Naturalistic language recordings reveal “hyper-vocal” infants at high familial risk for autism. Child Development, 89, e60–e73. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12777 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Swanson MR, Shen MD, Wolff JJ, Elison JT, Emerson RW, Styner MA, … Network, I. B. I. S. (2017). Subcortical brain and behavior phenotypes differentiate infants with autism versus language delay. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, 2, 664–672. doi: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2017.07.007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Tager-Flusberg H (2000). The challenge of studying language development in children with autism In Menn L & Ratner NB (Eds.), Methods for studying language production (pp. 313–332). Mahwah, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. [Google Scholar]
  97. Tager-Flusberg H (2016). Should scientists study mothers of children with autism? Spectrum. 1–4, https://www.spectrumnews.org/opinion/viewpoint/should-scientists-study-mothers-of-children-with-autism/ [Google Scholar]
  98. Tager-Flusberg H, Edelson L, & Rhiannon L (2011). Language and communication in autism spectrum disorders In Amaral D, Geschwind D & Dawson G (Eds.), Autism spectrum disorders (pp. 172–185). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press. doi: 10.1093/med/9780195371826.003.0011. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  99. Tager-Flusberg H, & Kasari C (2013). Minimally verbal school-aged children with autism spectrum disorder: The neglected end of the spectrum. Autism Research, 6, 468–478. doi: 10.1002/aur.1329 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Talbott MR, Nelson CA, & Tager-Flusberg H (2015). Maternal gesture use and language development in infant siblings of children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45, 4–14. doi: 10.1007/s10803-013-1820-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Thompson EC, Benítez-Barrera CR, Angley GP, Woynaroski T, & Tharpe AM (2020). Remote microphone system use in the homes of children with hearing loss: Impact on caregiver communication and child vocalizations. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 63, 633–642. doi: 10.1044/2019_JSLHR-19-00197 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Toth K, Dawson G, Meltzoff AN, Greenson J, & Fein D (2007). Early social, imitation, play, and language abilities of young non-autistic siblings of children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 145–157. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0336-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Walton KM, & Ingersoll BR (2015). The influence of maternal language responsiveness on the expressive speech production of children with autism spectrum disorders: A microanalysis of mother-child play interactions. Autism, 19, 421–432. doi: 10.1177/1362361314523144 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Wan MW, Green J, Elsabbagh M, Johnson M, Charman T, & Plummer F (2013). Quality of interaction between at-risk infants and caregiver at 12–15 months is associated with 3-year autism outcome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 54, 763–771. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12032 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Wan MW, Green J, Elsabbagh M, Johnson MH, Charman T, Plummer F, & Wai M (2012). Parent–infant interaction in infant siblings at risk of autism. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 33, 924–932. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2011.12.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Warlaumont AS, Richards JA, Gilkerson J, & Oller DK (2014). A social feedback loop for speech development and its reduction in autism. Psychological Science, 25, 1314–1324. doi: 10.1177/0956797614531023 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Weisleder A, & Fernald A (2013). Talking to children matters: Early language experience strengthens processing and builds vocabulary. Psychological Science, 24, 2143–2152. doi: 10.1177/0956797613488145 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  108. Williamson IO, Elison JT, Wolff JJ, & Runge CF (2020). Cost-effectiveness of MRI-based identification of presymptomatic autism in a high-risk population. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 11, 60. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00060 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  109. Wolff JJ, Botteron KN, Dager SR, Elison JT, Estes AM, Gu H, … Network IBIS (2014). Longitudinal patterns of repetitive behavior in toddlers with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 55, 945–953. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12207 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  110. Wolff JJ, Gu H, Gerig G, Elison JT, Styner M, Gouttard S, … Network IBIS (2012). Differences in white matter fiber tract development present from 6 to 24 months in infants with autism. American Journal of Psychiatry, 169, 589–600. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.11091447 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. Wood C, Diehm EA, & Callender MF (2016). An investigation of language environment analysis measures for Spanish–English bilingual preschoolers from migrant low-socioeconomic-status backgrounds. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 47, 123–134. doi: 10.1044/2015_LSHSS-14-0115 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES