Abstract
With less than a million neurons, the western honeybee Apis mellifera is capable of complex olfactory behaviors and provides an ideal model for investigating the neurophysiology of the olfactory circuit and the basis of olfactory perception and learning. Here, we review the most fundamental aspects of honeybee’s olfaction: first, we discuss which odorants dominate its environment, and how bees use them to communicate and regulate colony homeostasis; then, we describe the neuroanatomy and the neurophysiology of the olfactory circuit; finally, we explore the cellular and molecular mechanisms leading to olfactory memory formation. The vastity of histological, neurophysiological, and behavioral data collected during the last century, together with new technological advancements, including genetic tools, confirm the honeybee as an attractive research model for understanding olfactory coding and learning.
Keywords: Honeybee, Olfaction, Olfactory system, Olfactory coding, Olfactory learning
Odorants and their meaning
Honeybees strongly rely on olfaction for various aspects of their life, including foraging, mating, navigation, and social interactions. For this, their olfactory system has adapted to detect a myriad of molecules and complex mixtures, creating many different odors. Some odorants are endowed with an intrinsic (innate) value, whereas the majority does not carry meaningful information per se, but can be associated with a food source or with danger, or can be used as olfactory landmark. Honeybees are generalist foragers, i.e., they are not bound to a single flower species to survive. Accordingly, their olfactory circuit is not optimized to detecting the fragrance of a few specific flowers, but to detect, discriminate and learn virtually an infinite number of odorants. However, individual bees forage within a radius of a few kilometers from the hive and for a time span of a few weeks only. As a result, they experience but a limited number of salient olfactory cues, which they must efficiently learn and discriminate.
Wording in olfaction can be confusing. In this review, we use “chemical” or “substance” for a (volatile) stimulus consisting of a single molecule type, “odorant,” “fragrance,” or “scent” for a volatile that can be smelled by the animal, mixture or not, and “odor” for the percept of the animal. Thus, “odorant” refers to a physical stimulus (e.g., “1-hexen-3-ol” or “40–60% mixture of A and B”), “odor” to the psychophysical entity created in the brain (e.g., “cut grass” or “that rewarding flower type”). Different odorants may elicit the same odor, or the same odorant may elicit different odor percepts in particular situations. As an analogy: in color vision, the physical stimulus would be described as a spectrum across wavelengths, while the percept would be the color (“blue” or “yellow”), and here too, the same spectrum might create different colors depending on the circumstances.
The most common chemicals in floral scents are fatty acid derivatives (e.g., alcohols, aldehydes, ketones), benzenoids (e.g., methyl-2-hydroxybenzoate, benzaldehyde, benzyl alcohol), and terpenoids including sesquiterpenes (e.g., limonene, linalool, ocimene) (Knudsen et al. 1993). Some floral odorants can be innately attractive, such as linalool, 2-phenylethanol and lavender (Nouvian et al. 2015), or may possess an aversive valence if they happen to correspond to innately aversive odorants, such as the bee alarm pheromonal compound isoamyl acetate (Boch et al. 1962), but most plant odorants do not possess any innate valence.
Pheromones
Pheromones are chemical substances secreted by an animal’s exocrine gland and perceived by another individual of the same species, in which they induce a specific response (Karlson and Lüscher 1959). Primer pheromones induce long-term physiological changes, while signal (or releaser) pheromones elicit a temporary behavior (Wilson and Bossert 1963). Honeybees are eusocial insects and use pheromone-based communication in different aspects of colony ecology, from promoting social cohesion and maintaining an equilibrium across the different castes, all the way to regulating reproduction and swarming (Bortolotti and Costa 2014; Free 1987; Jarriault and Mercer 2012; Slessor et al. 2005).
Brood pheromones
Behavior and physiology of worker bees can be modulated by a mixture of fatty-acid esters secreted by larvae salivary glands, the brood pheromone. Among the main components of this blend we find methyl and ethyl palmitate, oleate, stearate, and linoleate. Non-volatile compounds are distributed within the hive by physical interaction (Le Conte et al. 1990). The relative amounts of the different components vary with larval stage, and can promote different behaviors in the nurse bees—e.g., cell capping, increasing royal jelly production, accepting new queen larvae (Le Conte et al. 2001), or directly inhibit ovarian development (Mohammedi et al. 1998). Bee larvae produce also a highly volatile pheromone, E-β-ocimene, which disperses throughout the colony inhibiting workers’ sexual maturation (He et al. 2016; Maisonnasse et al. 2009), and regulates the numerical equilibrium of nurses versus forager bees (Le Conte et al. 2001; Sagili et al. 2011).
Workers’ pheromones
Workers use pheromones to communicate and to influence the activity of other workers. When honey receiving bees within the hive cannot get rid of their honey, the ethanol produced by nectar fermentation within their body is transformed in ethyl oleate, a low-volatility pheromone that can diffuse at short range or by physical contact, and provides a foraging inhibition signal (Leoncini et al. 2004). Worker bees possess a strongly developed Nasanov gland, located beneath the intersegmental membrane, and responsible for the production, storage, and release of a pheromonal blend (Pickett et al. 1980). Nasanov pheromone provides an attractive signal used in various contexts, e.g., colony recognition, social cohesion within the hive, or guidance to the entrance of a new nest (Avitabile et al. 1975; Free 1987). Scouts recognize the presence of Nasanov pheromone in a potential nest, which indicates that bees have previously occupied the cavity, making it an attractive choice (Schmidt 2001). Foragers exposed the Nasanov gland for longer durations on more rewarding feeders, suggesting a higher amount of released pheromone in presence of a valuable food source, leading to an increase in the visiting rate from other foragers (Fernández et al. 2003; Free and Williams 1983; Koethe et al. 2020).
A chemical with opposite valence and higher volatility is 2-heptanone, produced in large amount by the mandibular glands of foragers (Shearer and Boch 1965). At exceptionally high concentrations, this odorant triggers a defensive response, but at physiological concentrations, it induces a repulsive behavior: during a foraging flight, a bee marks a visited flower with a small amount of this pheromone, signaling to other foragers to avoid the already depleted flower (Giurfa and Núñez 1992; Stout and Goulson 2001). The effect is short-lived, given the high volatility of 2-heptanone, so that by the time the flower has replenished, the pheromone’s effect has vanished. Together, Nasanov pheromone and 2-heptanone have opposite effects, but exploiting their different volatility both could be deposited on the same flower, creating a temporal sequence of first repellence, then attraction—a hypothesis that remains to be tested.
Pheromones may also communicate danger to the colony and trigger a concerted defense response. Alarm pheromones are released by the Koschevnikov gland located within the sting apparatus, and its main active component, isoamyl acetate, provokes a defensive response and stinging behavior (Boch et al. 1962) by lowering the stinging response threshold, mechanistically elicited by increased brain levels of serotonin and dopamine (Nouvian et al. 2018). This triggers coordinated attacks and stinging behavior (Millor et al. 1999), while decreasing general appetitive learning efficiency (Urlacher et al. 2010). Though this response is genetically pre-determined, it is also modulated by environmental factors. For example, it can be reduced by exposure to floral fragrances with high appetitive value, such as linalool or lavender (Nouvian et al. 2015), suggesting that the bee brain integrates information from innately attractive or aversive stimuli to control which of the many possible behavioral responses to opt for.
One of the most fascinating displays of complex behavior in insects is the honeybee dance, with which bees communicate distance, direction, and quality of a food source (von Frisch 1966). Such behavior creates a multisensory experience: it takes place in the darkness of the hive and relies on sounds (Michelsen et al. 1992), vibrations (Tautz 1996), tactile cues (Rohrseitz and Tautz 1999), taste (Farina and Núñez 1991), and olfaction (Thom et al. 2007). Dancing bees were found to release four characteristic volatiles, which were present in higher amount on waggle dancers than in non-dancing foragers or non-foraging worker bees. Three of these compounds (Z-(9)-tricosene, tricosane, and pentacosane) significantly increase foraging flights, thus suggesting that at least one of the messages of this pheromone is to promote foraging activity (Gilley 2014; Thom et al. 2007).
Queen’s pheromones
The queen bee provides an important regulatory function within the hive, influencing numerous aspects of colony life, such as social cohesion, workers’ fertility, rearing of new queens, and colony swarming (Kocher et al. 2009). Such regulatory activity is largely mediated by pheromones produced by different glands and in variable amounts throughout the queen’s life. The main source of queen pheromones are the mandibular glands, responsible for the production of queen mandibular pheromone, a mixture of several chemicals, among which the strong sexual attractant 9-oxo-2-decenoic acid (9-ODA) (Butler et al. 1962). Queen mandibular pheromone is released by virgin queens during the nuptial flight to attract the male drones (Free 1987; Gary 1962), while within the hive it is used in combination with other pheromonal compounds (Keeling et al. 2003) to recruit workers for feeding and grooming, the so-called retinue behavior. Retinue pheromones have little volatility and require direct contact or close proximity between the queen and the workers to spread. An increase in honeybee population, or an ageing queen, leads to a reduction in perceived QMP concentration throughout the colony, providing an absence-of-queen signal and promoting the rearing of new queens (Slessor et al. 1988; Slessor et al. 2005). Queen pheromone inhibits ovary development in worker bees, thus maintaining the queen as the only reproductive individual in the colony, and regulates colony homeostasis (Hoover et al. 2003). Indeed, by artificially modulating its level within an experimental hive, it is possible to influence comb building activity, development of foragers, defensive behavior, and aversive learning (Jarriault and Mercer 2012; Slessor et al. 2005).
Drones’ pheromones
During mating season, honeybee drones from multiple colonies gather in congregation areas waiting for the queen arrival (Koeniger and Koeniger 2004). Such gathering takes place before the arrival of the queen, suggesting that it may not be catalyzed by the queen pheromone (Ruttner and Ruttner 1972). Laboratory preference assays suggested that drone-released pheromones may promote their cohesion in the mating congregation (Brandstaetter et al. 2014; Gary 1962). Analogously, sexually mature virgin queens (but not worker bees of the same age) are attracted to the odor of a group of drones (but not of workers), suggesting the role of a putative drone pheromone in attracting mating queens (Bastin et al. 2017). However, the drone aggregation pheromone has not been identified yet.
Olfactory coding: the problems
Odorant discrimination and generalization
Olfactory coding must balance two opposite concepts: discrimination and generalization (Sandoz 2011; Shepard 1987). On one hand, a bee must be capable of discriminating subtle differences between two olfactory objects. On the other hand, small differences in odorant composition might separate fragrances that have the same meaning and therefore should be categorized as the same. Variability in natural odorants means that animals cannot always search for physically the same odorant they associated with, say, a nectar-rich flower. Rather, the molecular composition of an olfactory stimulus may vary, e.g., across flowers of the same plant species, with the time of day, with plant development, or due to a recent landing of another pollinator (Wright and Schiestl 2009; Wright and Smith 2004). Honeybees need to be highly sensitive to small variation in the bouquet in some situations, but in other situations also capable to generalize to avoid discarding good flowers because of minor composition differences.
What is the generalization function in honeybee olfaction? Smith and Menzel investigated generalization by classically conditioning worker bees to an odorant with multiple rewarded trials, and testing them with a series of novel odorants. They observed that harnessed bees showed high generalization among odorants of the same chemical class, particularly for aldehydes, acetates and monoterpene alcohols (Smith and Menzel 1989). Similarly, using PER conditioning, Guerrieri et al. were able to construct a generalization matrix of 16 odorants varying in chain length and functional groups (Fig. 1), showing that generalization depends on the similarity between stimuli, and it occurs more frequently between long-chained molecules (Guerrieri et al. 2005). In a more naturalistic paradigm, bees were conditioned to an odorant present at a feeder and successively tested in a free-flight choice over 44 vials containing the learnt stimulus and 43 other aliphatic molecules that differed in chain length and functional group (Laska et al. 1999). All bees could correctly identify the trained odorant. Nonetheless, generalization was observed towards odorants belonging to the same chemical group and differing in carbon chain length by only one carbon atom from the conditioned stimulus. Notably, generalization is an asymmetrical phenomenon: a bee may generalize by confusing odorant A with odorant B, but does not necessarily show the opposite generalization behavior from B to A (Guerrieri et al. 2005; Laska et al. 1999).
Odorant concentration
The same odorant may appear at high concentration (e.g., a bee sitting on the flower collecting nectar), or at low concentration (e.g., when the bee encounters the whiff of that odorant at a distance)—thus, bees should generalize across concentrations. On the other hand, the absolute odorant concentration also contains information, e.g., within a flower about the amount of nectar to be expected. In this situation, a bee should not generalize across concentrations, but she should be able to discriminate among them. Indeed, honeybees can discriminate between different stimulus concentrations—although with faster learning rates at higher concentrations (Wright and Smith 2004)—and are also able to navigate a gradient in search of the learned/rewarded concentration in a non-turbulent environment (Ditzen et al. 2003; Kramer 1976). Notably, while freely moving animals were able to learn absolute concentration levels, restrained animals displayed a higher rate of generalization from high to low concentrations of the same odorant (Bhagavan and Smith 1997; Pelz et al. 1997).
Odorant mixtures and temporal complexity
Odorants consist of volatile molecules that are transported across space in the air (Mafra-Neto and Cardé 1994; Murlis et al. 1992). These movements are always turbulent: odorant pockets of airborne chemicals form eddies, and at the antenna of the recipient animal (at the olfactory receptors) they create complex temporal sequences of varying concentrations. Bouts of low or zero concentration can alternate with very high or with intermediate concentrations (Celani et al. 2014; Murlis et al. 1992). Due to these temporal complexities, in a turbulent environment, it is not possible to locate an odorant source by gradient ascent. However, the temporal structure of an odorant trail contains some information about the distance from its source, since more distant sources create higher intermittency rates (Riffell et al. 2008). Turbulent distribution of odorant eddies has another important property that animals use to gain information about their environment. Most odor sources release odorant mixtures, and not pure substances. Take, as example, two flowers in a meadow, each with its own characteristic bouquet. In the air, the two mixtures might mix, and thus form a third mixture. How does the bee know that this third mixture is not a third kind of flower? Or that the mixture from a single flower is not a superposition of two other, different sources? The answer lies in the dynamical temporal structure of the olfactory world. Bees exploit the temporal coherence of odorant trails: mixtures that vary consistently in concentration are interpreted as originating from a single source, allowing a bee to separate a target odorant from the olfactory background using the synchronicity of their fluctuation (Hopfield 1991; Nowotny et al. 2013; Szyszka and Stierle 2014; Szyszka et al. 2012). From the neural point of view, the solution might be similar to when humans separate the voice of a person in the noise of a cocktail party (Stierle et al. 2013).
In a natural environment, most olfactory stimuli consist of a bouquet of many substances with unequal relative concentration of the elements. Thus, one of the greatest challenges for understanding olfactory coding is understanding how mixtures are processed: when the components of a mixture blend into a new olfactory object, the perception of the mixture may be considered configural or synthetic (“synthetic coding,” e.g., recognizing a particular Chardonnay and its year); conversely, when a mixture is perceived as the ensemble of its components, we may refer to an elemental (or analytical) processing of the mixture (“elemental coding,” e.g., smelling that there is garlic in a dish) (Kay et al. 2005). By studying honeybee’s ability to discriminate complex mixtures, Laloi et al. showed that not all components are evaluated equally: some of them are more easily identified, suggesting that mixture recognition relies on the identification of a few “key compounds” (Laloi et al. 2000). Similarly, after mixture conditioning, bees were shown to generalize more towards some components than towards others, and that a mixture composed by the key compounds alone could induce the same behavioral response as the whole initial blend, suggesting that a subset of key compounds is sufficient for synthetic coding of complex mixtures (Reinhard et al. 2010). In another study, Locatelli et al. showed that honeybees could distinguish two varieties of snapdragon fragrance. By mimicking natural variations occurring in the two flower blends, they were able to produce novel versions of the two varieties, which bees were able to correctly classify as belonging to one or the other variety. Thus, bees were able to perform a categorization/generalization task including novel odorants in a complex mixture context. Behavioral generalization towards similar blends is supported also by neurophysiological data indicating that similarities among the neural correlates of flowers from the same variety were greater than between different varieties (Locatelli et al. 2016). A detailed analysis related the capacity of analytical mixture analysis to the generalization profile of odorants: those substances that induced less generalization were the components that were most dominant within a mixture (Schubert et al. 2015).
Honeybee olfactory system
The olfactory system is a complex neuronal network organized in multiple highly interconnected neuropils. Each of them is a complex neural circuit itself comprising thousands of neurons with feed-back and feed-forward interactions within and across neuropils. Thus, the neural correlate of an olfactory stimulus is the result of local processing within olfactory neuropils and global interactions among neuropils. Below, we provide a detailed review of the neuroanatomy and neurophysiology of the principal neuropils (i.e., “local circuits”) of the honeybee olfactory system (Fig. 2).
Sensory organs and olfactory receptors
The peripheral olfactory organs of the bee are the antennae: all olfactory sensory neurons (OSNs) are located there. Each antenna is composed of three anatomical structures, from proximal to distal: scapus, pedicel, and flagellum. The flagellum comprises ten flagellomeres (eleven in the drone), the eight more distal of which host the olfactory sensilla: porous cuticular structures containing a variable number of olfactory sensory neurons. Sensilla are classified based on morphology. In the honeybee, the most frequent types of sensilla are placodea, trichodea, basiconica and coeloconica (Esslen and Kaissling 1976; Nishino et al. 2009). The majority of OSNs are housed within sensilla placodea (pore plate sensilla), which are oval-shaped thin cuticular porous plates innervated by up to 35 sensory neurons each (Kelber et al. 2006; Schneider and Steinbrecht 1968). Volatile molecules reaching the honeybee antennae enter the sensillar pores and diffuse through the sensillar lymph until they reach the OSNs’ dendritic membrane. The sensillar lymph prevents the OSNs’ dendritic terminals from drying, and contains olfactory binding proteins (OBPs) and odorant degrading enzymes (ODEs) (Chertemps et al. 2015; Forêt and Maleszka 2006; Iovinella et al. 2018; Song et al. 2018; Vogt et al. 1991; Younus et al. 2014). OBPs are likely involved in facilitating the transition of the odorants from a gaseous to a liquid environment and in transporting them towards the olfactory receptors (Gomez-Diaz et al. 2013; Laughlin et al. 2008; Leal 2013), even if recent studies in Drosophila showed that deletion of the principal OBP genes does not impair odor transduction, suggesting that many OBP may not play an essential role in odorant-receptor interaction (Xiao et al. 2019). ODEs, instead, may have a role in the degradation of odorants, thus promoting signal termination by limiting the time an odorant is present in the sensillar lymph and preventing saturation of the olfactory receptors (Iovinella et al. 2018; Leal 2013; Vogt et al. 1985; Younus et al. 2014). Studies in Drosophila suggest that ODEs need not be odorant specific, but may be broad spectrum enzymes (e.g., esterases, glutatione-S-transferases, aldehyde dehydrogenases and oxidases, and cytochrome P450s) (Leal 2013; Younus et al. 2017). The neural response spectrum to olfactory stimuli derives both from OR tuning and sensitivity, and from the interaction dynamics with OBPs and ODEs (Larter et al. 2016).
After crossing the hemolymph, odorants reach the OSNs’ dendritic membrane, where they interact with the chemosensory receptors. In bees, most receptors belong to two molecular families: the olfactory (or odorant) receptors (ORs) and the ionotropic receptors (IRs). These receptors are the main drivers for the molecular receptive response ranges of the olfactory sensory neurons. The honeybee genome comprises 163 ORs and 21 IRs (Robertson 2019; Robertson and Wanner 2006). Olfactory receptors are C-terminus-out seven-transmembrane-domain proteins equipped with a ligand-binding domain, and are functionally active as heterodimers (or multimers) with the olfactory receptor co-receptor (Orco) (Benton et al. 2006). In bees, AmOr2 (Krieger et al. 2003) is the ortholog of the fly co-receptor Orco (DmOR38b) (Robertson and Wanner 2006; Sato et al. 2008). While ORs have evolved within the hexapoda clade from gustatory receptors, IRs are more ancient in evolution. They are also multimeric receptor proteins that evolved from ionotropic glutamate receptors, but their physiological properties have not yet been investigated in bees, and our knowledge is extrapolated from Drosophila work (Silbering et al. 2011).
The receptor for CO2 is not yet known in the bee. In Drosophila, a gustatory receptor multimer consisting of DmGR21a and DmGR63a has evolved to form a phasic CO2 receptor (Suh et al. 2004). While bees too can detect carbon dioxide (Lacher 1964; Stange and Diesendorf 1973), they do not use a receptor homologous to the fly CO2 receptors (Robertson and Kent 2009; Robertson and Wanner 2006; Suh et al. 2004). Within the hive, bees experience levels of CO2 far above atmospheric concentration, which they control by ventilating the hive with fanning behavior (Seeley 1974). Carbon dioxide also influences honeybee fertility (Koywiwattrakul et al. 2005; Mackensen 1947), providing a context- and caste-dependent signal able to induce a behavioral effect, such as fanning, and physiological changes, such as modulation of ovary development. For this, they need to detect absolute levels or CO2, rather than rapid changes, a necessity that may have led to the independent evolution of a different CO2 receptor.
Olfactory receptors’ expression levels vary during honeybee development and are influenced by experience. For example, ORs for linalool and queen pheromone 9-ODA are downregulated after appetitive classical conditioning for those odorants (Claudianos et al. 2014). Furthermore, expression of ORs involved in floral scents detection—but not the co-receptor AmOr2/Orco—show significant variability throughout the year, suggesting an influence of individual experience (Reinhard and Claudianos 2012; Wanner et al. 2007).
Ligand-binding properties
Vision and hearing are senses that rely on a few continuous dimensions to decode the most important properties of a stimulus, such as intensity, wavelength, frequency, or spatial origin. Chemical senses, and in particular airborne olfaction, employs multiple descriptors (such as molecular size, shape, charge, chain length, presence of functional groups) to identify an odorant and locate it in a multidimensional olfactory space, but which parameters are exploited by olfactory receptors is unknown, and indeed likely to differ among receptors (Münch and Galizia 2016; Sachse et al. 1999). This complexity is exacerbated when we also consider the intensity of an odorant, the multiple components of a mixture, or temporal fluctuations of volatiles.
Olfactory coding initiates with the biochemical interaction between an odorant and the binding pocket of the olfactory receptor, starting odor signal transduction. Many olfactory receptors are activated by multiple odorants and most odorants can interact with more than one receptor, with the specificity of the odorant-receptor interaction influencing the dynamics of the elicited neuronal activity (Getz and Akers 1993; Getz and Akers 1994). This allows for a limited number of receptors to encode a practically infinite number of olfactory combinations (Galizia et al. 1999b; Malnic et al. 1999). Still, every ligand-receptor pair has its own affinity, with higher affinity allowing OR-odorant interaction at lower concentration. Hence, when an odorant is present at low-concentration, it interacts with fewer receptors, while at high concentration also less specific neurons will be recruited, whereas some of the high-affinity OSNs might be saturated and may even stop responding. For this reason, minute variations in stimulus nature and concentration produce significant changes of odor representation across the sensory neurons’ population. It is the task of the olfactory system in the brain to either use these concentration dependent properties for an efficient detection of stimulus concentration, or to ignore their effect for concentration invariance (Strauch et al. 2012).
Floral bouquets may contain enantiomers, i.e., molecules with the same chemical composition but with chiral bonds. Multiple reports have shown that honeybees are capable of discriminating enantiomers for some substances (Aguiar et al. 2018; Laska and Galizia 2001; Lensky and Blum 1974). Although this may appear as a complex task, it is not surprising when considering that odorant detection relies on a ligand sterically interacting with a receptor’s binding pocket, and mirror-like structures do not necessarily fit despite having the same chemical formula, just like left and right foot cannot fit the same shoe. Some studies suggested that honeybees could discriminate between different isotopologues of the same molecule, i.e., molecules that differ, for some of their atoms, in the number of neutrons only (i.e., having different isotopes) (Gronenberg et al. 2014; Paoli et al. 2016a). The only physical mechanism known that could allow for this discrimination would involve reading the ligand’s vibrational spectrum, thus suggesting a mechanism for primary transduction (Turin 1996). However, proving that vibrations are used is experimentally contested: recent experiments in fruit flies revealed that an apparent different perception of isotopologues was in fact caused by minute traces of contaminants in the experimental samples (Paoli et al. 2017). That study showed that a contamination of 0.0006% (6 ppm) was sufficient for a full response, and thus may cause a distorted representation of receptive response profiles.
What is the time required for an odorant to enter the sensillum, reach an OR, and induce a post-synaptic depolarization capable of inducing a measurable signal in the OSNs? By producing olfactory stimuli with high temporal resolution, Szyszka and colleagues measured the response latency from the odorant reaching the antenna to odor signal transduction in the OSNs to be as little as 2 ms (Szyszka et al. 2014). Also, electroantennogram responses to high-frequency stimulation showed that honeybee OSNs could follow a stimulus frequency up to 125 Hz. This phenomenon is odorant and concentration dependent, and the coherence between stimulus frequency and antennal nerve firing frequency improved during the stimulus train duration. This does not necessarily imply that a single OSN can follow such a high-frequency stimulation. Instead, it is likely that single neurons may respond intermittently to different pulses of the train of stimuli, and that only their combined activity can outperform the tracking power of a single neuron (Szyszka et al. 2014).
Antennal lobe
Olfactory sensory neurons
All OSNs’ axons converge onto the first processing center of the olfactory circuit, the antennal lobe (AL), functionally analogous and evolutionarily convergent to the vertebrate olfactory bulb (Fig. 2). The honeybee AL is a spheroid neuropil about 300 μm wide, organized in ~ 163 anatomical and functional units called glomeruli (Esslen and Kaissling 1976; Galizia et al. 1999a; Pareto 1972; Robertson and Wanner 2006). Glomeruli are stereotyped in size, shape, and relative position across individuals of the same caste (Sandoz et al. 2007), allowing to build a reference neuroanatomical atlas of the honeybee AL (Flanagan and Mercer 1989b; Galizia et al. 1999a). In the fruit fly, it has been shown that all OSNs expressing the same OR converge onto the same glomerulus, and that this wiring plan is genetically determined (Vosshall et al. 2000). An odorant interacting with different affinities across different ORs elicits neural activity in a subset of glomeruli, creating stereotypical maps of odorant-induced glomerular responses that are, to a large degree, conserved across animals (Fig. 1a). This allowed to complement the morphological atlas with the functional response properties of glomeruli in the honeybee antennal lobe (Galizia et al. 1999b; Sachse et al. 1999) (see also https://neuro.uni-konstanz.de/honeybeeALatlas), which has been used to investigate the principles of olfactory coding (Deisig et al. 2006; Deisig et al. 2010; Paoli et al. 2016b; Paoli et al. 2018; Sachse and Galizia 2003), the relationship between perceptual and chemical similarity of odorants (Carcaud et al. 2012; Guerrieri et al. 2005), as well as changes in olfactory representation upon learning (Chakroborty et al. 2016; Chen et al. 2015; Hourcade et al. 2009; Peele et al. 2006; Rath et al. 2011).
The approx. 60.000 OSN axons enter the AL via the antennal nerve. This nerve enters the brain in six separate subtracts: T1 to T4 are formed by OSNs’ axons and innervate the AL, whereas T5-T6 terminate in the dorsal lobe and the subesophageal ganglion, conveying mechanosensory and gustatory information from the antenna to the brain, and motor information in the opposite direction. Each antennal tract innervates a different subset of glomeruli: tracts T1 and T3 innervate two large groups of approx. 70 glomeruli in the dorsal and ventral AL, respectively, whereas T2 and T4 tracts innervate two minor subsets of 7 glomeruli each (Flanagan and Mercer 1989b). In the honeybee, each antenna innervates only the ipsilateral side (Pareto 1972), unlike Drosophila where many axons innervate both ALs. Axons in T4 arborize extensively throughout each innervated glomerulus, while axons from the other tracts innervate the external glomerular layers only, i.e. the glomerular cortex (Brockmann and Brückner 1995; Galizia et al. 1999a). Innervation is antennotopic: afferents originating in the distal flagellomers occupy the external margin of the glomerular cortex, proximal segments innervate the inner cortex (Nishino et al. 2009; Pareto 1972). Immunolabelling analysis highlighted the presence of acetylcholinesterase and acetylcholine receptors in the antennal nerve and within AL glomeruli, suggesting that OSN neurotransmission relies on acetylcholine (Kreissl and Bicker 1989).
Local interneurons in the antennal lobe
Within AL glomeruli, sensory afferents form synapses with local interneurons (LNs) and projection neurons (PNs). LNs arborize, as a population, throughout the whole glomerular volume and form synapses with both OSNs and PNs (Flanagan and Mercer 1989a; Fonta et al. 1993). Based on their morphology two groups can be classified: homogeneous LNs present a similar density of arborization among all innervated glomeruli, while heterogeneous LNs display a dense arborization in one particular glomerulus and a few sparse branches within the other units they innervate (Flanagan and Mercer 1989a; Fonta et al. 1993; Galizia and Kimmerle 2004; Sun et al. 1993). Based on electrophysiological recordings there are at least five different populations of LNs (Meyer and Galizia 2012; Meyer et al. 2013), an observation confirmed by different populations of peptides being expressed (Galizia and Kreissl 2012). Electrophysiological recordings coupled to cell labeling have shown that LNs’ activity is odor specific, with different stimuli leading to different degrees of excitation or inhibition. Moreover, heterogeneous LNs have a polarized physiology, i.e., they receive input from the densely innervated glomerulus and deliver an output (often inhibitory) to other glomeruli (Galizia and Kimmerle 2004; Sachse and Galizia 2002; Sachse and Galizia 2003). Chemical manipulation of the AL network during olfactory stimulation revealed that the inhibitory effect of LNs is not spatially concentric, but towards glomeruli in a spatially discontinuous pattern (Girardin et al. 2013). Although this may seem inefficient from the connectivity-length point of view, it must be considered that glomeruli are organized on a single layer around the core of the AL, and due to such radial organization, interglomerular distances as seen from the center of the AL are comparable independently of their proximity on the AL surface (Girardin et al. 2013). Alternatively, or in addition, this patchy solution might reflect the multidimensional nature of olfactory coding (Linster et al. 2005): it might not be possible to arrange glomeruli in a two-dimensional or three-dimensional configuration, where interconnectivity creates a logical pattern such that local neuron connectivity is limited to neighboring glomeruli. Honeybees have ~ 4000 LNs (Galizia and Rössler 2010; Witthöft 1967), a relatively large number as compared to Drosophila with ~ 200 LNs (Chou et al. 2010; Seki et al. 2010), despite the fact that the ratio of PN count and glomeruli remains somewhat comparable across species (e.g., 185/50 in Drosophila, 800/160 in honeybees). In bees, 750 LNs (less than 25% of all LNs) are GABAergic (Schäfer and Bicker 1986), whereas in the fruit fly and cockroaches, the vast majority of LNs are GABA-immunoreactive (Chou et al. 2010; Distler 1989; Leitch and Laurent 1996). Thirty GABAergic LNs constitute a thin commissure linking the two bilateral ALs (Schäfer and Bicker 1986). LNs were also found to be histamine immunoreactive (~ 35 LNs, (Sachse et al. 2006)) or to express different neuropeptides, e.g., allatostatin or tachykinin (Galizia and Kreissl 2012; Kreissl et al. 2010), showing a high functional diversity within the LN population.
Functional imaging analysis of AL input and output signals highlighted the role of the LN local network in olfactory information processing. Although input and output spatial response maps maintain a strong similarity and are dominated by the same odorant-responsive glomeruli, the transition from a compound input signal largely dominated by OSNs’ activity to the output signal of the sole PN population involves a reduction in overall response intensity and a spatial and temporal sharpening of the induced responses. This effect, probably achieved by the inhibitory activity of LNs, contributes to enhancing the contrast between the glomerular representation of different stimuli, and can be strongly reduced by selectively blocking the local inhibitory network (Carcaud et al. 2012; Carcaud et al. 2018; Deisig et al. 2006; Deisig et al. 2010; Sachse and Galizia 2002; Sachse and Galizia 2003).
PNs
Olfactory information processed in glomeruli is relayed to higher order brain centers by approx. 800 projection neurons (PNs) (Galizia 2008; Hammer 1997; Rybak 2012). Most of them are uniglomerular (uPNs), receiving input from individual glomeruli, whereas multiglomerular PNs (mPNs) extend their dendritic arborization across multiple glomeruli. The high convergence of ~ 60,000 OSNs onto ~ 800 PNs increases sensitivity and improves signal-to-noise ratio (Galizia 2014). Morphologically, PNs’ dendrites occupy principally the glomerular inner volume, i.e., the core, thus only partially overlapping with the OSNs pre-synaptic terminals which occupy the glomerular cortex (see above) (Galizia et al. 1999a; Pareto 1972). PN axons innervate the mushroom body (MB) and the lateral protocerebrum (LP), in particular the lateral horn (LH) (Fig. 2). Most insects have multiple axonal tracts connecting the ALs to higher order brain areas (Galizia and Rössler 2010). So does the honeybee: axons leave the AL in five AL tracts (ALTs). The lateral (l-ALT) and medial (m-ALT) tracts comprise the axon fibers of uPNs, while mPNs use three smaller mediolateral tracts (ml-ALTs) (Abel et al. 2001; Kirschner et al. 2006; Mobbs 1982). Both the l-ALT and the m-ALT innervate MB and LP, although in reverse order: the former innervates first the LP and then the MB, while the latter does the opposite. m-ALT PNs are strongly acetylcholinesterase immunoreactive and convey olfactory information from T2, almost all T3 and T4 glomeruli (Kirschner et al. 2006; Kreissl and Bicker 1989). l-ALT fibers receive input mainly from T1 (and from a few T2 and T3) glomeruli (Kirschner et al. 2006) within the dorsal half of the AL, and exhibit taurine-like (Schäfer et al. 1988) and occasionally GABA-like immunoreactivity (Zwaka et al. 2016). This wiring pattern via two separate parallel nerve bundles (l- and m-ALT, respectively), creates different latencies, and is mediated by different neurotransmitters (Krofczik et al. 2009; Rössler and Brill 2013). Simultaneous labelling of m-ALT and l-ALTs revealed that both pathways remain spatially segregated in the MB: the medial tract innervates the whole lip of the calyces with densely packed pre-synaptic boutons, whereas the lateral one innervates the central core of the lip region with a sparse innervation pattern (Kirschner et al. 2006; Zwaka et al. 2016). Similarly, they also segregate in the LP, showing a tract-specific compartmentalization (Kirschner et al. 2006). Multiglomerular PNs, instead, are mainly GABAergic and project to the LP only (Bicker 1999; Fonta et al. 1993; Schäfer and Bicker 1986).
Fibers of the two tracts differ in response latency, concentration coding, and odorant specificity, suggesting that they may encode different stimulus properties (Brill et al. 2013; Brill et al. 2015; Carcaud et al. 2012; Carcaud et al. 2018; Krofczik et al. 2009; Yamagata et al. 2009). Whereas m-ALT neurons were reported to be more narrowly tuned, prone to elemental mixture analysis, and not subject to mixture inhibition, l-ALT neurons displayed shorter latencies, responded to lower stimulus concentration, tend to encode mixtures synthetically rather than elementally, and were often subject to mixture inhibition (Krofczik et al. 2009; Rössler and Brill 2013). The two tracts may also have different sensitivity/specificity for different odorants, as shown for the processing of some pheromones. Calcium imaging analysis of either m- or l-ALT innervated glomeruli revealed that most odorants activate glomeruli in both areas, including aggregation and alarm pheromones, while queen and brood pheromones activate mostly l- or m-ALT neurons, respectively (Carcaud et al. 2015; Galizia et al. 2012; Müller et al. 2002). A fundamental aspect of a parallel processing system converging on the same neuropils is timing. Recordings of spontaneous and induced activity in both m-ALT and l-ALT simultaneously revealed that the rate of coincident activity was higher in presence of an odorant rather than during spontaneous firing (Brill et al. 2015). However, coincidence rates did not increase between tracts, but only within tracts, particularly among odorant-responsive fibers of m-ALT neurons, which show lower firing frequency but higher coincidence probability than l-ALT neurons (Rössler and Brill 2013).
Although the majority of olfactory processing occurs ipsilaterally, there is some cross-communications in the bee brain: apart from a LNs commissure interconnecting both ALs (see above), uPNs from the T4 glomeruli were shown to project bilaterally to the MB calyces (Abel et al. 2001), and one mPN was reported to innervate multiple glomeruli in both hemispheres and projecting olfactory information from both ALs to the ipsilateral protocerebral lobe (Rybak 2012).
Glomerular activity patterns
An odorant reaching the antennae activates a variable number of ORs, thus activating a subset of AL glomeruli, each at a different intensity. These activity patterns depend on ORs’ molecular response ranges, and on network connectivity within the AL. Each odorant elicits a dynamic across-glomeruli pattern, and this stimulus-elicited response creates a trajectory in the glomerular space that is highly reproducible across honeybees. Here, “glomerular space” is the mathematical construct of a multidimensional space where each OSN type represents a single dimension (Fdez Galán et al. 2004). Patterns are largely determined by genetic instructions, and therefore bilaterally symmetrical (Galizia et al. 1998) and comparable across individuals (Galizia et al. 1999b), allowing for the creation of a functional response atlas (see above). Nonetheless, individual variability due to plasticity within the neural network is prominent (Galizia et al. 1999b; Joerges et al. 1997; Sachse and Galizia 2002).
How is stimulus similarity coded in the olfactory circuit at this stage? By means of calcium imaging analysis of the AL, it is possible to record odorant representation across glomeruli, i.e., to determine the location of an odorant in the bee’s glomerular space. This analysis revealed that no single glomerulus represents a particular chemical functional group, and that chemically related odorants evoke partially overlapping response patterns. Considering odorants with the same functional group, their induced glomerular responses gradually shift with changing chain length, and thus their representation in the olfactory space is more similar for similar carbon chain length (Fig. 1). This provided the first evidence how chemical properties of olfactory stimuli were encoded in the brain, and suggested that similar neurophysiological representation could provide the basis for similar odor perception and generalization (Joerges et al. 1997; Sachse et al. 1999). This observation found further support by a direct comparison of the perceptual and neurophysiological distances among pairs of odorants (Carcaud et al. 2012; Carcaud et al. 2018; Guerrieri et al. 2005). A comparison of the behavioral generalization matrix (Guerrieri et al. 2005) and the matrix of neurophysiological distances (Sachse et al. 1999) shows a high correlation (Fig. 1). This observation is also valid for odorant mixtures: Fernandez et al. mixed two odorants varying their ratio, generating a series of binary mixtures. After conditioning honeybees to one of the mixtures, they tested their discrimination ability. They observed that a change in the ratio of the two components induces a proportional change in perceptual similarity, and found a similar gradual shift in the neural representation of the binary mixtures (Fernandez et al. 2009).
Odorant generalization is asymmetric, meaning that bees may generalize more to odorant B after learning odorant A than vice versa (Guerrieri et al. 2005; Sandoz et al. 2001; Smith and Menzel 1989). From the neurophysiological perspective, asymmetric generalization may arise when the pool of receptors activated by two stimuli—and consequently the respective glomerular response maps—are asymmetrically overlapping. In the extreme case where an odorant A activates a sub-group of the receptors activated by B, the activity pattern elicited by the latter encompasses the neural representation of the former. In this case, in a brain model of additive neural computation, A will generalize to B (as a sub-pattern), while B will not generalize to A (since glomeruli present in B do not belong to A).
A similar phenomenon is observed in odorant concentration coding: low concentration (as a sub-pattern) will generalize to high, while high will not generalize to low (since some glomeruli activated at high concentrations do not respond to lower concentrations). Indeed, in behavioral experiments, generalization from low to high concentration is higher than in the other way (Bhagavan and Smith 1997; Pelz et al. 1997). Physiologically, as a general rule, higher concentration leads to the recruitment of more glomeruli, and to signals of shorter latency, higher intensity, and longer duration, resulting in broader and less specific odor response maps (Akers and Getz 1993; Paoli et al. 2018; Sachse and Galizia 2003; Szyszka et al. 2014). The reason is that affinity of an odorant receptor for a ligand depends on their chemical and physical properties, and the likelihood of activation increases with increasing ligand availability. Simultaneous calcium imaging of AL input and output neurons revealed that the AL local network compensates parts of this effect, by modulating the input signal to improve discriminability across concentrations, and yielding a “sharper” odor representation across projection neurons (Sachse and Galizia 2003).
The temporal onset of glomerular responses also contributes to olfactory discrimination (Martin and Hildebrand 2010; Stopfer et al. 1997; Wehr and Laurent 1996) and may be experience and context dependent (Christensen et al. 2000). Fast multiphoton imaging in the honeybee AL showed that each odorant induces a specific latency map, with the ranking of glomerular activity onset highly conserved across individuals. Such ranking-based code has a prediction accuracy comparable to the response amplitude code, thus suggesting that response latencies may provide a first available information for odorant identification, later refined by other informative cues such as the number/type of total responsive units and extent of synchronously firing neurons (Paoli et al. 2018). Electrophysiological recordings investigating AL oscillatory patterns showed that odorous stimuli induce oscillatory synchronization of the projection neurons around 30 Hz detectable both in the AL and in the MB, and likely due to inhibitory feedback loop activity of the LN network (Stopfer et al. 1997). This phenomenon is consistent with an increase of coincident activity across uPNs during and after stimulation (Brill et al. 2015; Galán et al. 2006), as well as with the decrease of other oscillatory frequency powers in AL and MB (Paoli et al. 2016b; Popov and Szyszka 2020). Interestingly, odorant-induced decrease in low-frequency power in the AL does not concern only responsive glomeruli, but also some non-responsive ones (Paoli et al. 2016b), suggesting that this effect is mediated by elements of the local connectivity rather than from a direct interaction of AL input and output neurons.
Mushroom body
The MB comprises two cup-like structures, the medial and lateral calyx, which are the converging sites of multimodal sensory inputs, mostly olfactory, visual and gustatory. Each calyx can be subdivided in lip, the main olfactory input region, collar, which receives mainly visual input, and basal ring, which receives input from both sensory modalities (Gronenberg 2001; Mobbs 1982). Axons in the m-ALT innervate a larger volume with higher arborization and synaptic density in the lips than the l-ALT. While ALTs bulk labeling suggested a segregation of l-ALT terminals to the core of the lips, this was not confirmed by single-cell labelling (Kirschner et al. 2006; Zwaka et al. 2016).
MB structure is defined by the morphology of its approx. 184,000 intrinsic neurons, the Kenyon cells (KCs) (Mobbs 1982; Strausfeld 2002), named after F.C. Kenyon who first described them in 1896. Each KC has its cell body inside or around the calyx cup, extends its dendritic arborization within the calyx, and projects its axon into the pedunculus, where it bifurcates into the vertical (α and γ, forming a fused lobe) and the medial (β) lobes (Strausfeld 2002). Topology within the calyx compartments is maintained into the lobes, creating a consistent band pattern in the lobes reflecting the calyx regions (Mobbs 1982; Strausfeld 2002).
Synaptic transmission from PNs to KCs occurs in microcircuits, the microglomeruli (MG). Each MG comprises a PN pre-synaptic bouton surrounded by multiple KC post-synaptic profiles, GABAergic neuron terminals (Ganeshina and Menzel 2001; Groh and Rössler 2020; Grünewald 1999a), as well as modulatory input from octopaminergic and dopaminergic neurons (Blenau et al. 1999; Hammer 1993; Zwaka et al. 2018). The organization of pre- and post-synaptic terminals confers to the MG a spheroidal modular structure, that has been used to label them and quantify how their number and density in different areas of the MB calyces varies, e.g., with age, environmental factors, or after olfactory conditioning (Cabirol and Haase 2019; Groh et al. 2012; Hourcade et al. 2010; Scholl et al. 2014).
KCs are classified in two groups based on their morphology. The main group of approx. 170,000 class I (or spiny) KCs has densely packed somata located within the cups of the calyces. These KCs possess a spiny dendritic tree, each spine taking part in a different MG, thus receiving input from multiple PNs. A second group is composed by approx. 14,000 class II (or clawed) KCs. Their cell bodies are less packed and located just outside the cups of the calyces, and their dendrites extend multiple short claw-like protrusions, each enwrapping a single PN pre-synaptic bouton (Groh and Rössler 2020; Strausfeld 2002). These neuroanatomical differences suggest different functional roles for the two KC populations. While in class I cells a single KC receives inputs from numerous PN boutons and is likely to require coincident inputs to be activated, class II KCs receive multiple synapses from the same PN terminal suggesting that the input of a single neuron may be sufficient to cross its activation threshold. In addition, class I KCs bifurcate and project their axons to both the vertical and the medial lobe, whereas class II neurons innervate exclusively the anterior part of the vertical lobe, i.e., the γ-lobe (Mobbs 1982; Strausfeld 2002). KCs express receptors for acetylcholine, the major neurotransmitter of m-ALT PNs (Kreissl and Bicker 1989). Differently from the fruit fly, where KCs are cholinergic (Barnstedt et al. 2016), histochemical analysis and gene expression studies did not (yet) detect the presence of the cholinergic machinery in bees. Several peptides have been shown in KCs, which may act as co-transmitters, modulators, or “ordinary” transmitters (Kreissl and Bicker 1989; Suenami et al. 2018).
Different types of MB extrinsic (or output) neurons (MBONs) interconnect the mushroom body with other brain neuropils (i.e., unilateral or bilateral output neurons) or interconnect the lobes between them and with the calyces (i.e., recurrent neurons) (Rybak and Menzel 1993). The most well-characterized is a population of ~ 110 A3 GABAergic recurrent interneurons (Grünewald 1999b; Mobbs 1982; Rybak and Menzel 1993; Zwaka et al. 2018). One part of this population innervates a restricted portion of the medial lobe and the corresponding area of the vertical lobe, according to the band pattern innervation characteristic of KCs pre-synaptic innervation (Zwaka et al. 2018). Thus, class I KCs branching in circumscribed regions in both lobes are interconnected by one (or a few) A3 interneurons. The remaining A3 interneurons (also known as MB feedback neurons) receive inputs in the MB lobes and project their axons to the calyces delivering an internal feedback signal to the KCs. They have a stimulus specific activity and extend their pre-synaptic terminals to narrow sub-regions of the calyces, thus providing a stimulus specific pattern of inhibitory activity within the MB input region (Grünewald 1999a; Grünewald 1999b; Zwaka et al. 2018). This compartmentalized tuning system differs from, e.g., the MB feedback network of the fruit fly or the locust, where a single feedback neuron (the anterior paired lateral neuron) collects input from all KCs and enhances sparseness in KC activity patterns by inhibiting the whole KC population (Lin et al. 2014; Papadopoulou et al. 2011). Approximately 150 mushroom body output neurons (MBONs) relay information from the MB lobes to other areas of the protocerebrum (Rybak and Menzel 1993). The dendrites of these neurons branch in large areas of the MB lobes and in the LP, receiving input from different sensory modalities. One of the largest MBONs is the pedunculus-extrinsic neuron 1 (PE-1), an inhibitory neuron collecting mechanosensory, visual and olfactory information (Rybak and Menzel 1998). Neurophysiological studies showed that appetitive olfactory conditioning reduces its inhibitory activity on its target premotor centers (Haehnel and Menzel 2012; Mauelshagen 1993).
Lateral protocerebrum
The lateral protocerebrum (LP) and its lateral horn (LH) collect olfactory input both from AL projection neurons and the MB output neurons (Fig. 2). Direct inputs are conveyed by all AL tracts (Kirschner et al. 2006). uPNs forming the m-ALT and l-ALT provide excitatory input from each glomerulus individually, thus conveying the complete AL glomerular response pattern (Roussel et al. 2014). The LP is also targeted by GABAergic mPNs. Within the AL, these neurons collect information across several glomeruli, thus providing an inhibitory input from a glomerular ensemble that may correspond to specific glomerular patterns. Due to their multiglomerular innervation pattern in the AL, they have access to complex across-glomeruli information, which may reflect innate valence. Learned odor information converges on the same areas of the LP from the MB, via the MBONs. These neurons innervate—among other areas of the LP—the LH, and provide experience-related information about the olfactory input (Galizia 2014; Rybak and Menzel 1993).
Whereas within the AL and the MB it is possible to find a prominent neuroanatomical structure, the absence of an evident organization and of anatomical boundaries makes the LP more difficult to investigate. Calcium imaging of the PN dendritic arborizations in the AL and pre-synaptic terminals in the LP revealed that response intensity and intra-odor distances observed in the AL were conserved also in the LP (Roussel et al. 2014), suggesting that PNs innervate the LP in a stereotyped and genetically determined way.
Given that data from bees are limited, we might look at other insects, well aware of the phylogenetic distance between different insect species. Still, because of a similar olfactory coding logic, the fruit fly may help understanding the role of LP in honeybees, and may allow to design working hypotheses to further define its function. In both species, there are by far fewer neurons in the LP than there are KCs in the MB (Aso et al. 2009; Frechter et al. 2019). In flies, the innervation of LP neurons by PNs is not random: excitatory uPNs from the same glomerulus and from similarly tuned glomeruli tend to converge on the same LP neurons, while inhibitory input from broadly tuned mPNs contributes to enhance innate discrimination among similar odorants. PNs innervate the LP in a spatially organized way (Jefferis et al. 2007; Sachse and Beshel 2016; Strutz et al. 2014; Wong et al. 2002). Therefore, the wiring scheme from AL to LP is odorant-biased and has a strong component of genetic predetermination, providing the neural substrate for odorant classification according to their innate valence (Parnas et al. 2013; Strutz et al. 2014). An analysis of odorant coding using principal component analysis revealed that LP neurons are better than PNs in encoding higher-order odor features that are more likely to be behaviorally relevant for the fly (Dolan et al. 2019; Frechter et al. 2019; Jeanne et al. 2018). Hence, it appears that the LP, which does not have the computational power and the architecture to support discrimination and learning of a virtually infinite number of stimuli, may provide a good substrate for innate odorant classification according to behavioral significance. However, also odorants with innate meaning, such as pheromones, are prone to change their valence if experience is strong. For example, bees can be trained to associate the alarm pheromone isoamyl acetate with an appetitive reward (Becker et al. 2000; Sandoz et al. 2001), indicating that worker bees can override their innate aversive reaction. Thus, the innate circuitry within the LP is modulated by the plastic circuitry from the MB (via the connection provided by MBONs), whose input conveys experience-dependent odor valence (Galizia 2014; Okada et al. 2007; Rybak and Menzel 1993).
Learning and memory
Olfactory learning
Honeybees have a relatively short foraging life of only about two weeks, while flowers and other food sources are scattered in space and limited in time. They possess a general innate search image characterized by a clear preference for scented and colored feeders (Koethe et al. 2020), but can quickly associate a significant food reward with specific olfactory and visual cues to facilitate future foraging trips (Menzel 1993). Honeybees are polylectic, i.e., they collect nectar and pollen from many different flowering species, but at the same time bees are, to a large degree, flower-constant, i.e., when a flower species is in bloom, they will concentrate on that particular species until depleted, or until a better one comes into blossom. Since a bee cannot know a priori all odorants that it will encounter in her lifetime, learning capacity in the olfactory system is particularly important. Thus, they have evolved a strong learning capability, including an ability to solve a wide range of learning tasks, among which classical and operant learning, context learning, and non-associative learning (Giurfa 2015; Giurfa and Sandoz 2012; Menzel 1993). The possibility to study such behaviors under controlled conditions—both in the laboratory and in the field—allowed investigating the cellular and network-related mechanisms guiding the different phases of memory formation (Eisenhardt 2014; Giurfa 2015; Menzel 1999; Menzel 2012).
Bees learn to associate olfactory cues with information not only in the field, but also within the hive. Karl von Frisch observed that returning foragers were frequently engaging in trophallaxis with other foragers, and by this chemical information transfer they could communicate the nature of the target food source (von Frisch 1965). Analysis of frequency and duration of mandibular contacts during and after a bee dance, suggests that during the dance followers acquire gustatory and olfactory information about the target food source (Farina and Wainselboim 2005; Gil and De Marco 2005). Moreover, a direct contact with a fellow bee may act as a reinforcer even in the absence of nectar (Cholé et al. 2019). Foragers which had not visited the foraging site themselves responded to the odorant associated with the foraging site (Grüter et al. 2006), and feeding to a scented food source increased the preference bias towards the associated odorant (Arenas et al. 2008), suggesting that olfactory information about the food source is transferred between dancer and followers (Farina et al. 2005).
Classical and operant conditioning
An unconditioned stimulus (US) is a stimulus able to trigger an innate response. A (neutral) conditioned stimulus (CS) does not provide a specific valence per se. In classical conditioning, both stimuli are combined in a temporally organized way, and after the association, the CS is not neutral anymore and elicits the conditioned response (Pavlov 1927). In the honeybee, Pavlovian conditioning is most often used with the proboscis extension response (PER) paradigm upon appetitive association (Bitterman et al. 1983; Kuwabara 1957; Matsumoto et al. 2012; Takeda 1961), and a protocol exploiting the sting extension response upon the exposure to an unpleasant US was recently developed (Giurfa et al. 2009; Junca and Sandoz 2015; Roussel et al. 2010; Vergoz et al. 2007). Conditioned and unconditioned stimuli need to be delivered in temporal contiguity, with the US starting a few seconds later than the CS, so that the CS acquires a predictive value for the US (Bitterman et al. 1983; Szyszka et al. 2011). Conditioning is also possible when there is a temporal gap between the CS and the US, a procedure referred to as trace conditioning (Lüdke et al. 2018; Szyszka et al. 2011). Conditioning can be absolute, where the bee is exposed to a reward-associated stimulus only and learns its predictive value, or differential, where the animal experiences two stimuli, one of which is paired with a reward (CS+) (Bitterman et al. 1983; Matsumoto et al. 2012).
Although most experiments on classical conditioning have used harnessed honeybees, freely moving animals can also be conditioned in flying (Giurfa et al. 1999) or walking (Buatois et al. 2017; Kirkerud et al. 2013; Nouvian and Galizia 2019) paradigms. When the animal is free to move, the learning task might involve components of operant conditioning. In a classical conditioning paradigm, the association between CS and US occurs in a “passive” context, whereas in operant conditioning the animal’s movements adopt a goal-directed component, where learning leads to seeking a reward or avoiding a punishment. Hence, associative learning in an ‘operant’ context reflects the ability of the animal to learn from the consequences of the animal’s own behavior (Brembs 2003).
Phases of memory formation
Traditionally, memory has been classified by how long it lasts. A single conditioning trial leads to the formation of a short- (seconds to minutes, STM) to medium-term memory (up to 1 h, MTM), whereas multiple conditioning trials lead to the formation of a long-term memory (LTM), which can be retrieved the next day (early LTM) or up to 72 h later (late LTM) (Giurfa and Sandoz 2012; Giurfa et al. 2009; Menzel 2012; Schwärzel and Müller 2006). However, this classification based on time windows was found to be unreliable, with temporal phases being shorter or longer depending on many factors, both external and internal to the animal. A more reliable definition of STM and LTM, therefore, uses the molecular mechanisms involved: STM does not depend on protein synthesis but only on electrical activity or protein phosphorylation (and quickly fades over time), whereas LTM involves protein synthesis: mRNA translation is sufficient for MTM and early LTM formation, but late LTM is both transcription and translation-dependent (Wüstenberg et al. 1998). Honeybees can form late LTM even after a single conditioning trial (Pamir et al. 2014; Sandoz et al. 1995; Villar et al. 2020), and acute injection of transcription and translation blockers showed that transcription but not translation-dependent memory was present already at 4 h, whereas at 24-h single trial-induced memory was already transcription and translation-dependent (Villar et al. 2020).
Neurophysiology of olfactory learning
All three principal olfactory processing centers (AL, MB, and LP) receive first-order (the AL) or second-order (MB and LP) olfactory information. Also, all three neuropils are innervated by the ventral unpaired median neurons of the maxillary neuromere 1, of which VUMmx1 is the best known (Hammer 1993). At least three reasons suggest the implication of this octopaminergic neuron in appetitive memory formation: (1) it responds to a sucrose stimulation with a long burst of activity, which outlasts the stimulus; (2) it extends its dendritic arborization in the subesophageal ganglion, where it receives gustatory input from sucrose receptor cells, and innervates extensively all odor processing centers; (3) artificial VUMmx1 depolarization paired to an olfactory stimulus induces associative learning of the paired odorant (Hammer 1993). Immunohistochemical studies on the distribution of octopamine receptors in the bee brain confirmed its presence in the main odor processing centers. While OSNs do not express the OA receptor genes, antennal lobe PNs and LNs do. The presence of OA receptors in KCs is still unclear, whereas immunoreactive fibers have been found in the MB calyces, possibly belonging to extrinsic MB neurons (Kreissl et al. 1994; Sinakevitch et al. 2011). Compared to the AL and MB, a lower expression of OA receptors is found in the LH (Kreissl et al. 1994; Sinakevitch et al. 2011; Sinakevitch et al. 2013).
During conditioning, the appetitive US triggers octopamine (OA) release (Hammer 1993; Hammer and Menzel 1998). Consistently, injecting OA in the AL or MB can replace the US in appetitive association (Hammer and Menzel 1998), whereas silencing the OA receptor AmOA1 in the honeybee AL impairs memory formation (Farooqui et al. 2003). These experiments revealed that neural plasticity in the AL and in the MB promotes associative learning, and that it relies on the pairing of the olfactory input with OA release. Moreover, OA influences the network activity within the AL to a different degree in different glomeruli, suggesting that the distribution of AmOA1 receptors across glomeruli is not stereotyped, but experience-dependent itself (Rein et al. 2013). Experiments also showed that the system is very robust: while cooling of the entire AL or MB prejudices memory formation (Erber et al. 1980), ablating even large parts of the MBs does not significantly impair memory (Malun et al. 2002).
In the MB, glutamate uncaging experiments showed that glutamate release paired with an olfactory stimulation produces LTM comparable to a classical conditioning protocol (Locatelli et al. 2005). Conversely, silencing NMDA glutamate receptors during (or shortly after) olfactory conditioning impairs memory formation, but affects neither odorant discrimination nor memory retrieval (Müssig et al. 2010). Simultaneous depolarization of the dendritic spines and presence of glutamate are necessary for NMDA receptors to activate. Thus, we may consider a working model where NMDA receptors act as coincidence detectors between CS (i.e., KC depolarization) and US, possibly mediated by glutamatergic MB intrinsic neurons. The opening of NMDA receptor channels produces a sustained increase in intracellular calcium level and the consequent activation of calcium-dependent signaling cascades (Eisenhardt 2014; Jarome and Helmstetter 2013).
The sustained increase in intracellular calcium concentration following the activation of glutamate NMDA receptors or OA receptors leads to an increase in cAMP concentration, and to the activation of protein kinase A (PKA) - CREB signaling cascade (Bollen et al. 2014; Eisenhardt et al. 2003; Leboulle and Müller 2004; Matsumoto et al. 2014; Müller 2000). In addition, multiple CS/US learning trials promote a long-lasting activation of a constitutively active protein kinase C (PKC), a possible mediator of medium and long-term memory related plasticity (Grünbaum and Müller 1998) (for a more in depth review of cellular physiology of memory formation see (Eisenhardt 2014; Himmelreich and Grünewald 2012)). Finally, memory formation results in regulation of gene expression via DNA methylation and demethylation (Biergans et al. 2012; Lockett et al. 2010), with modulatory effects on memory-associated genes (Biergans et al. 2015) and LTM formation (Biergans et al. 2016).
Learning-related plasticity
Several studies found memory-related changes in glomerular response patterns in the AL (Faber et al. 1999; Locatelli et al. 2013; Rath et al. 2011; Sandoz et al. 2003). An initial analysis of the effects of different appetitive olfactory learning protocols on AL uPNs reported no significant change in odorant-related responses before and after training, thus suggesting that these neurons may be more relevant for consistent odor coding than for memory-related physiological (and possibly morphological) changes (Peele et al. 2006). Nonetheless, traces of non-associative memory could be localized to the AL by optical imaging experiments (Locatelli et al. 2013). A detailed analysis of learning-related physiological changes in across-glomeruli activity patterns allowed to elaborate a working model to explain the events behind associative learning in the AL. The model comprises two superimposed and glomerulus-specific learning effects: a non-associative effect in presence of a pre-synaptic activity not followed by a coincident post-synaptic one (as in the case of an inhibitory LN-to-OSN synapse), and an associative one that relies on coincident pre- and post-synaptic activity (as in the case of an excitatory OSN-to-PN synapse) in presence of an appetitive reinforcement (here, mediated by the octopaminergic VUMmx1 neuron) (Rath et al. 2011). The olfactory system is also modulated by developmental plasticity, where exposure to odors during development leads to morphological changes (Andrione et al. 2017; Devaud et al. 2001; Hourcade et al. 2009; Sachse et al. 2007), or influences their odorant sensitivity and discrimination capability (Jernigan et al. 2020).
Octopaminergic and dopaminergic neurons innervate the MB calyces, relaying appetitive and aversive input. Imaging studies showed that MB neural responses to an odorant increase within the first 30 minutes after conditioning (Faber and Menzel 2001). Associative learning promotes plastic changes in the activity of PN axon terminals: in differential conditioning, the intensity of the responses to both the rewarded (CS+) and the unrewarded stimulus (CS−) may increase or decrease after conditioning, but changes affect mainly CS+ responses, increasing the neurophysiological distance between the two stimuli and the bee’s learning performance. Response reduction appeared more frequently than increase, and the longer latency of inhibitory responses compared to the excitatory ones suggests that learning related plasticity in the MB calyces is, at least in part, driven by inhibitory mechanisms on PNs’ terminals (Haenicke et al. 2018). Simultaneous recordings of PNs and KCs during olfactory stimulation indicated that early-responsive KCs drive the activity of GABAergic feedback neurons, sharpening response dynamics and inhibiting KCs’ responses to late components of the PNs' input signal (Szyszka et al. 2005). Repetitive olfactory stimulation induces a decrease in KCs’ responsiveness (Szyszka et al. 2008)—but not PNs’ input intensity (Peele et al. 2006), suggesting that microglomerular circuits in the MB calyx provide the neural substrate for non-associative memory formation. However, pairing the repeated stimulus with a sugar reward evokes longer-lasting neural responses, promotes the recruitment of additional KCs, and, during the test phase, the response to the conditioned odorant returns to its initial intensity, while the response to the unpaired stimulus remains depressed (Szyszka et al. 2008). In a similar conditioning paradigm, MB feedback neurons—which are both target and input of the KCs—show a decrease of responsiveness, with the response to the CS+ decreasing less than the one to the CS− (Haehnel and Menzel 2010). These experiments suggest the existence of a neural network, where responsive KCs activate a population of MB feedback neurons that exert a general inhibitory activity on the KC population itself. However, the coincidence of CS and US inputs on a KC subpopulation may prevent a decrease in response intensity in those cells and in their downstream targets. LTM formation may also influence MB neuroanatomy: honeybees exposed to paired CS/US stimulation showed an increased density of microglomeruli in the lip, the calyx compartment innervated by olfactory projection neurons, but not in the collar, which receives visual input (Hourcade et al. 2010).
MB output/extrinsic neurons collect processed olfactory inputs from the KCs and relay it to other neuropils, including premotor areas (Rybak and Menzel 1993). Electrophysiology of the vertical lobe output neurons revealed that odor tuning and response strength of a large group MBONs changed after olfactory conditioning (Strube-Bloss et al. 2011). Such plastic changes could not be observed during conditioning itself—thus they are unlikely to be related to the acquisition of a short term memory—but were detected 3 h later, hence supporting MTM/LTM formation. Furthermore, the pedunculus extrinsic neuron 1 (PE-1) innervating the medial and lateral protocerebrum was shown to reduce response intensity to the CS+ specifically, while maintaining unchanged the response to the unpaired stimulus and to a control odorant (Mauelshagen 1993; Okada et al. 2007). These learning-related effects are visible five minutes after conditioning—thus providing a neural substrate for STM—and are possibly due to an increased inhibition of recurrent MB neurons. Olfactory information processed by MB neurons is also collected by the bilateral antennal lobe feedback neuron 1 (ALF-1) that innervates in the vertical lobe in correspondence of the output region of spiny KCs from the calyx lip and projects to the AL with arborizations spanning across the entire neuropil. This neuron has a broad odor tuning and provides a feedback signal to the AL, allowing to modulate olfactory coding at the periphery of the olfactory system (Iwama and Shibuya 1998; Kirschner et al. 2006; Rybak and Menzel 1993).
Outlook
In this review, we focused on three main aspects of honeybee olfactory coding: (1) the role different odorants play in the life of a honeybee; (2) the neuroanatomy and neurophysiology of the olfactory system; (3) the principal neurophysiological mechanisms leading to memory formation and learning related plasticity. Decades of neuroanatomy, neurophysiology and behavioral experiments provided us with a wealth of knowledge about the logic behind olfactory coding, and of the architecture of the global neuronal network of the olfactory system as well as of the local networks guiding olfactory coding within individual neuropils. Still, a lot remains to do. Luckily, research in honeybee olfaction is alive and vivid, supported by the recent development of large-scale flight tracking, higher resolution optical physiology, and the extensive application of computational tools for the analysis of large behavioral, morphological and neurophysiological datasets. In our view, two areas will be of prominent importance in the next few years, and both areas will strongly benefit from a comparative approach between insect species. The first one relates to understanding the pathway linking stimulus evaluation to decision making. Although we know that different neuropils provide the architecture for different cognitive processes (e.g., the modular MB network allows stimulus recognition and memory storage as well as multisensory integration, the LP may provide stimulus valence evaluation) we still do not fully grasp how learned and innate information are integrated to drive behavior. The second question relates to possible olfactory specialization associated to honeybee sociality. We have reviewed many pheromones that bees use to communicate among conspecifics. Did establishing such a specific mean of communication influence the fundamental neural architecture of the bee brain? As reviewed above, the similarities are plenty even with distantly related species such as the fruit fly, but so are the differences, and it is unclear which of them evolved with honeybee sociality. In this respect, a comparative analysis of social bees with solitary ones, as well as with more distantly related Hymenoptera such as wasps and ants is needed.
Abbreviations
- 9-ODA
9-Oxo-2-decenoic acid
- A3
Mushroom body extrinsic neuron of the A3 somata cluster
- AL
Antennal lobe
- ALF-1
Antennal lobe feedback neuron 1
- ALT
Antennal lobe tract
- AmOA1
Apis mellifera octopamine receptor 1
- AmOr2
Apis mellifera olfactory receptor 2 (the bee ortholog of the fly co-receptor Orco)
- cAMP
Cyclic adenosine monophosphate
- CS
Conditioned stimulus
- CS+
Reinforced stimulus
- CS-
Unreinforced stimulus
- DA
Dopamine
- DNA
Desoxyribonucleic acid
- IR
Ionotropic receptor (olfactory)
- KC
Kenyon cell
- l-ALT
Lateral antennal lobe tract
- LH
Lateral horn in the lateral protocerebrum
- LN
Local neuron (in the antennal lobe)
- LTM
Long-term memory
- LP
Lateral protocerebrum
- m-ALT
Medial antennal lobe tract
- MB
Mushroom body
- MBON
Mushroom body output neuron
- MG
Microglomerulus (in the mushroom body)
- ml-ALT
Mediolateral antennal lobe tract
- mPN
Multiglomerular projection neuron
- MTM
Medium-term memory
- NMDA
N-Methyl-D-aspartate
- OA
Octopamine
- OBP
Odorant-binding protein
- ODE
Odorant-degrading enzyme
- OR
Olfactory receptor
- Orco
Odorant receptor co-receptor
- OSN
Olfactory sensory neuron
- PE-1
Pedunculus-extrinsic neuron 1
- PER
Proboscis extension response
- PKA
Protein kinase A
- PKC
Protein kinase C
- PN
Projection neuron
- QMP
Queen mandibular pheromone
- STM
Short-term memory
- T1-T6
(Antennal nerve) Tract 1 to 6
- uPN
Uniglomerular projection neuron
- US
Unconditioned stimulus
Funding
Open Access funding enabled and organized by Projekt DEAL. This study is funded by the European Union’s Horizon 2020 research and innovation program under the Marie Sklodowska-Curie grant agreement no. 893382, and the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG, German Research Foundation) under Germany’s Excellence Strategy – EXC 2117 – 422037984.
Compliance with ethical standards
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.
Ethical approval
All applicable international, national, and institutional guidelines for the care and use of animals were followed.
Footnotes
Publisher’s Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Contributor Information
Marco Paoli, Email: mar.paoli@gmail.com, Email: marco.paoli@univ-tlse.fr.
Giovanni C. Galizia, Email: giovanni.galizia@uni-konstanz.de
References
- Abel R, Rybak J, Menzel R. Structure and response patterns of olfactory interneurons in the honeybee, Apis mellifera. J Comp Neurol. 2001;437:363–383. doi: 10.1002/cne.1289. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aguiar JMRBV, Roselino AC, Sazima M, Giurfa M (2018) Can honey bees discriminate between floral-fragrance isomers? J Exp Biol 221, jeb180844 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Akers RP, Getz WM. Response of olfactory receptor neurons in honeybees to odorants and their binary mixtures. J Comp Physiol A. 1993;173:169–185. [Google Scholar]
- Andrione M, Timberlake BF, Vallortigara G, Antolini R, Haase A. Morphofunctional experience-dependent plasticity in the honeybee brain. Learn Mem. 2017;24:622–629. doi: 10.1101/lm.046243.117. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arenas A, Fernández VM, Farina WM. Floral scents experienced within the colony affect long-term foraging preferences in honeybees. Apidologie. 2008;39:714–722. [Google Scholar]
- Aso Y, Grübel K, Busch S, Friedrich AB, Siwanowicz I, Tanimoto H, Grü K. The mushroom body of adult Drosophila characterized by GAL4 drivers. J Neurogenet. 2009;23:156–172. doi: 10.1080/01677060802471718. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Avitabile A, Morse RA, Boch R. Swarming Honey Bees Guided by Pheromones. Ann Entomol Soc Am. 1975;68:1079–1082. [Google Scholar]
- Barnstedt O, Owald D, Felsenberg J, Brain R, Moszynski JP, Talbot CB, Perrat PN, Waddell S. Memory-relevant mushroom body output synapses are cholinergic. Neuron. 2016;89:1237–1247. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.02.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bastin F, Cholé H, Lafon G, Sandoz JC. Virgin queen attraction toward males in honey bees. Sci Rep. 2017;7:1–11. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-06241-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Becker MM, Brückner D, Crewe R. Behavioural response of drone honey bees, Apis mellifera carnica and Apis mellifera scutellata, to worker-produced pheromone components. J Apic Res. 2000;39:149–154. [Google Scholar]
- Benton R, Sachse S, Michnick SW, Vosshall LB. Atypical membrane topology and heteromeric function of Drosophila odorant receptors in vivo. PLoS Biol. 2006;4:e20. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0040020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bhagavan S, Smith BH. Olfactory conditioning in the honey bee, Apis mellifera: Effects of odor intensity. Physiol Behav. 1997;61:107–117. doi: 10.1016/s0031-9384(96)00357-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bicker G. Histochemistry of classical neurotransmitters in antennal lobes and mushroom bodies of the honeybee. Microsc Res Tech. 1999;45:174–183. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0029(19990501)45:3<174::AID-JEMT5>3.0.CO;2-U. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Biergans SD, Jones JC, Treiber N, Galizia CG, Szyszka P. DNA methylation mediates the discriminatory power of associative long-term memory in honeybees. PLoS One. 2012;7:e39349. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0039349. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Biergans SD, Giovanni Galizia C, Reinhard J, Claudianos C. Dnmts and Tet target memory-associated genes after appetitive olfactory training in honey bees. Sci Rep. 2015;5:16223. doi: 10.1038/srep16223. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Biergans SD, Claudianos C, Reinhard J, Galizia CG. DNA methylation adjusts the specificity of memories depending on the learning context and promotes relearning in honeybees. Front Mol Neurosci. 2016;9:1–11. doi: 10.3389/fnmol.2016.00082. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bitterman ME, Menzel R, Fietz A, Schäfer S. Classical conditioning of proboscis extension in honeybees (Apis mellifera) J Comp Psychol. 1983;97:107–19. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blenau W, Schmidt M, Faensen D, Schürmann FW. Neurons with dopamine-like immunoreactivity target mushroom body Kenyon cell somata in the brain of some hymenopteran insects. Int J Insect Morphol Embryol. 1999;28:203–210. [Google Scholar]
- Boch R, Shearer DA, Stone BC. Identification of isoamyl acetate as an active component in the sting pheromone of the honey bee. Nature. 1962;195:1018. doi: 10.1038/1951018b0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bollen E, Puzzo D, Rutten K, Privitera L, De Vry J, Vanmierlo T, Kenis G, Palmeri A, D’Hooge R, Balschun D, et al. Improved long-term memory via enhancing cGMP-PKG signaling requires cAMP-PKA signaling. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2014;39:2497–2505. doi: 10.1038/npp.2014.106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bortolotti L, Costa C. Chemical communication in the honey bee society. In: Mucignat-Caretta Carla., editor. Neurobiology of Chemical Communication. Boca Raton (FL): CRC Press/Taylor & Francis; 2014. pp. 147–210. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brandstaetter AS, Bastin F, Sandoz JC. Honeybee drones are attracted by groups of consexuals in a walking simulator. J Exp Biol. 2014;217:1278–1285. doi: 10.1242/jeb.094292. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brembs B. Operant conditioning in invertebrates. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2003;13:710–717. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2003.10.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brill MF, Rosenbaum T, Reus I, Kleineidam CJ, Nawrot MP, Rössler W. Parallel processing via a dual olfactory pathway in the honeybee. J Neurosci. 2013;33:2443–56. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4268-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brill MF, Meyer A, Rössler W (2015) It takes two — coincidence coding within the dual olfactory pathway of the honeybee. 6, 1–14 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Brockmann A, Brückner D. Projection pattern of poreplate sensory neurones in honey bee worker, Apis mellifera L. (Hymenoptera : Apidae) Int J Insect Morphol Embryol. 1995;24:405–411. [Google Scholar]
- Buatois A, Pichot C, Schultheiss P, Sandoz JC, Lazzari CR, Chittka L, Avarguès-Weber A, Giurfa M. Associative visual learning by tethered bees in a controlled visual environment. Sci Rep. 2017;7:1–19. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-12631-w. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Butler CG, Callow RK, Johnston NC. The isolation and synthesis of queen substance, 9-oxodec-trans-2-enoic acid, a honeybee pheromone. Proc R Soc London Ser B Biol Sci. 1962;155:417–432. [Google Scholar]
- Cabirol A, Haase A. The neurophysiological bases of the impact of neonicotinoid pesticides on the behaviour of honeybees. Insects. 2019;10:344. doi: 10.3390/insects10100344. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carcaud J, Hill T, Giurfa M, Sandoz JC. Differential coding by two olfactory subsystems in the honeybee brain. J Neurophysiol. 2012;108:1106–1121. doi: 10.1152/jn.01034.2011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carcaud J, Giurfa M, Sandoz J-C. Differential combinatorial coding of pheromones in two olfactory subsystems of the honey bee brain. J Neurosci. 2015;35:4157–4167. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0734-14.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carcaud J, Giurfa M, Sandoz J-C. Differential processing by two olfactory subsystems in the honeybee brain. Neuroscience. 2018;374:33–48. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2018.01.029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Celani A, Villermaux E, Vergassola M. Odor landscapes in turbulent environments. Phys Rev X. 2014;4:041015. [Google Scholar]
- Chakroborty NK, Menzel R, Schubert M. Environment-specific modulation of odorant representations in the honeybee brain. Eur J Neurosci. 2016;44:3080–3093. doi: 10.1111/ejn.13438. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chen J-Y, Marachlian E, Assisi C, Huerta R, Smith BH, Locatelli F, Bazhenov M. Learning modifies odor mixture processing to improve detection of relevant components. J Neurosci. 2015;35:179–197. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2345-14.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chertemps T, Younus F, Steiner C, Durand N, Coppin CW, Pandey G, Oakeshott JG, Maïbèche M. An antennal carboxylesterase from Drosophila melanogaster, esterase 6, is a candidate odorant-degrading enzyme toward food odorants. Front Physiol. 2015;6:315. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2015.00315. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cholé H, Carcaud J, Mazeau H, Famié S, Arnold G, Sandoz JC. Social contact acts as appetitive reinforcement and supports associative learning in honeybees. Curr Biol. 2019;29:1407–1413.e3. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2019.03.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chou Y-H, Spletter ML, Yaksi E, Leong JCS, Wilson RI, Luo L. Diversity and wiring variability of olfactory local interneurons in the drosophila antennal lobe. Nat Neurosci. 2010;13:439–449. doi: 10.1038/nn.2489. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Christensen TA, Pawlowski VM, Lei H, Hildebrand JG. Multi-unit recordings reveal context-dependent modulation of synchrony in odor-specific neural ensembles. Nat Neurosci. 2000;3:927–931. doi: 10.1038/78840. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Claudianos C, Lim J, Young M, Yan S, Cristino AS, Newcomb RD, Gunasekaran N, Reinhard J. Odor memories regulate olfactory receptor expression in the sensory periphery. Eur J Neurosci. 2014;39:1642–1654. doi: 10.1111/ejn.12539. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deisig N, Giurfa M, Lachnit H, Sandoz J-C. Neural representation of olfactory mixtures in the honeybee antennal lobe. Eur J Neurosci. 2006;24:1161–1174. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.04959.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deisig N, Giurfa M, Sandoz JC. Antennal lobe processing increases separability of odor mixture representations in the honeybee. J Neurophysiol. 2010;103:2185–2194. doi: 10.1152/jn.00342.2009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Devaud JM, Acebes A, Ferrús A. Odor exposure causes central adaptation and morphological changes in selected olfactory glomeruli in Drosophila. J Neurosci. 2001;21:6274–6282. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-16-06274.2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Distler P. Histochemical demonstration of GABA-like immunoreactivity in cobalt labeled neuron individuals in the insect olfactory pathway. Histochemistry. 1989;91:245–249. doi: 10.1007/BF00490139. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ditzen M, Evers JF, Galizia CG. Odor similarity does not influence the time needed for odor processing. Chem Senses. 2003;28:781–789. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjg070. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dolan MJ, Frechter S, Bates AS, Dan C, Huoviala P, Roberts RJ, Schlegel P, Dhawan S, Tabano R, Dionne H, et al. Neurogenetic dissection of the drosophila lateral horn reveals major outputs, diverse behavioural functions, and interactions with the mushroom body. Elife. 2019;8:1–45. doi: 10.7554/eLife.43079. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eisenhardt D. Molecular mechanisms underlying formation of long-term reward memories and extinction memories in the honeybee (Apis mellifera) Learn Mem. 2014;21:534–542. doi: 10.1101/lm.033118.113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eisenhardt D, Friedrich A, Stollhoff N, Müller U, Kress H, Menzel R. The AmCREB gene is an ortholog of the mammalian CREB/CREM family of transcription factors and encodes several splice variants in the honeybee brain. Insect Mol Biol. 2003;12:373–82. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2583.2003.00421.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Erber J, Masuhr T, Menzel R. Localization of short-term memory in the brain of the bee, Apis mellifera. Physiol Entomol. 1980;5:343–358. [Google Scholar]
- Esslen J, Kaissling K-E. Zahl und Verteilung antennaler Sensillen bei der Honigbiene (Apis mellifera L.) Zoomorphologie. 1976;83:227–251. [Google Scholar]
- Faber T, Menzel R. Visualizing mushroom body response to a conditioned odor in honeybees. Naturwissenschaften. 2001;88:472–476. doi: 10.1007/s001140100263. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Faber T, Joerges J, Menzel R. Associative learning modifies neural representations of odors in the insect brain. Nat Neurosci. 1999;2:74–78. doi: 10.1038/4576. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Farina WM, Núñez JA. Trophallaxis in the honeybee, Apis mellifera (L.) as related to the profitability of food sources. Anim Behav. 1991;42:389–394. [Google Scholar]
- Farina WM, Wainselboim A. Trophallaxis within the dancing context: a behavioral and thermographic analysis in honeybees (Apis mellifera) Apidologie. 2005;36:43–47. [Google Scholar]
- Farina WM, Grüater C, Díaz PC. Social learning of floral odours inside the honeybee hive. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci. 2005;272:1923–1928. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2005.3172. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Farooqui T, Robinson K, Vaessin H, Smith BH. Modulation of early olfactory processing by an octopaminergic reinforcement pathway in the honeybee. J Neurosci. 2003;23:5370–5380. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-12-05370.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fdez Galán R, Sachse S, Galizia CG, Herz AVM. Odor-driven attractor dynamics in the antennal lobe allow for simple and rapid olfactory pattern classification. Neural Comput. 2004;16:999–1012. doi: 10.1162/089976604773135078. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fernandez PC, Locatelli FF, Person-Rennell N, Deleo G, Smith BH. Associative conditioning tunes transient dynamics of early olfactory processing. J Neurosci. 2009;29:10191–10202. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1874-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fernández PC, Gil M, Farina WM. Reward rate and forager activation in honeybees: recruiting mechanisms and temporal distribution of arrivals. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2003;54:80–87. [Google Scholar]
- Flanagan D, Mercer AR (1989a) Morphology and response characteristics of neurones in the deutocerebrum of the brain in the honeybee Apis mellifera. 483–494
- Flanagan D, Mercer AR. An atlas and 3-D reconstruction of the antennal lobes in the worker honey bee, Apis mellifera L. (Hymenoptera : Apidae) Int J Insect Morphol Embryol. 1989;18:145–159. [Google Scholar]
- Fonta C, Sun XJ, Masson C. Morphology and spatial distribution of bee antennal lobe interneurones responsive to odours. Chem Senses. 1993;18:101–119. [Google Scholar]
- Forêt S, Maleszka R. Function and evolution of a gene family encoding odorant binding-like proteins in a social insect, the honey bee (Apis mellifera) Genome Res. 2006;16:1404–1413. doi: 10.1101/gr.5075706. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frechter S, Bates AS, Tootoonian S, Dolan MJ, Manton J, Jamasb AR, Kohl J, Bock D, Jefferis G. Functional and anatomical specificity in a higher olfactory centre. Elife. 2019;8:1–39. doi: 10.7554/eLife.44590. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Free JB (1987) Pheromones of social bees. Chapman and Hall
- Free JB, Williams IH. Scent-marking of flowers by honeybees. J Apic Res. 1983;22:86–90. [Google Scholar]
- Galán RF, Weidert M, Menzel R, Herz AVM, Galizia CG. Sensory memory for odors is encoded in spontaneous correlated activity between olfactory glomeruli. Neural Comput. 2006;18:10–25. doi: 10.1162/089976606774841558. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Galizia GC (2008) Insect olfaction. In The senses: a comprehensive reference (ed. Basbaum, A. I.), Kaneko, A.), Shepherd, G. M.), and Westheimer, G.), pp. 725–770. San Diego: Academic Press
- Galizia CG. Olfactory coding in the insect brain: data and conjectures. Eur J Neurosci. 2014;39:1784–1795. doi: 10.1111/ejn.12558. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Galizia CG, Kimmerle B. Physiological and morphological characterization of honeybee olfactory neurons combining electrophysiology, calcium imaging and confocal microscopy. J. Comp. Physiol. A Neuroethol. Sensory, Neural. Behav Physiol. 2004;190:21–38. doi: 10.1007/s00359-003-0469-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Galizia GC, Kreissl S (2012) Neuropeptides in Honeybees. In Honeybee Neurobiology and Behavior - A tribute to Randolf Menzel (ed. Galizia, G. C.), Eisenhardt, D.), and Giurfa, M.), pp. 211–226. Springer
- Galizia CG, Rössler W. Parallel olfactory systems in insects: anatomy and function. Annu Rev Entomol. 2010;55:399–420. doi: 10.1146/annurev-ento-112408-085442. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Galizia CG, Nägler K, Hölldobler B, Menzel R. Odour coding is bilaterally symmetrical in the antennal lobes of honeybees (Apis mellifera) Eur J Neurosci. 1998;10:2964–2974. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.1998.00303.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Galizia CG, McIlwrath SL, Menzel R. A digital 3-dimensional atlas of the honeybee antennal lobe based on optical sections acquired using confocal micoscropy. Cell Tissue Res. 1999;295:383–394. doi: 10.1007/s004410051245. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Galizia CG, Sachse S, Rappert A, Menzel R. The glomerular code for odor representation is species specific in the honeybee Apis mellifera. Nat Neurosci. 1999;2:473–8. doi: 10.1038/8144. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Galizia CG, Franke T, Menzel R, Sandoz JC. Optical imaging of concealed brain activity using a gold mirror in honeybees. J Insect Physiol. 2012;58:743–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jinsphys.2012.02.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ganeshina O, Menzel R. GABA-immunoreactive neurons in the mushroom bodies of the honeybee: an electron microscopic study indexing terms: GABA; postembedding EM-immunocytochemistry; microcircuits. J Comp Neurol. 2001;437:335–349. doi: 10.1002/cne.1287. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gary NE. Chemical mating attractants in the queen honey bee. Science. 1962;136:773–774. doi: 10.1126/science.136.3518.773. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Getz WM, Akers RP. Olfactory response characteristics and tuning structure of placodes in the honey bee Apis mellifera L. Apidologie. 1993;24:195–217. [Google Scholar]
- Getz WM, Akers RP. Honeybee olfactory sensilla behave as integrated processing units. Behav Neural Biol. 1994;61:191–195. doi: 10.1016/s0163-1047(05)80075-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gil M, De Marco RJ. Olfactory learning by means of trophallaxis in Apis mellifera. J Exp Biol. 2005;208:671–680. doi: 10.1242/jeb.01474. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gilley DC. Hydrocarbons emitted by waggle-dancing honey bees increase forager recruitment by stimulating dancing. PLoS One. 2014;9:e105671. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0105671. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Girardin CC, Kreissl S, Galizia CG. Inhibitory connections in the honeybee antennal lobe are spatially patchy. J Neurophysiol. 2013;109:332–343. doi: 10.1152/jn.01085.2011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Giurfa M. Learning and cognition in insects. Wiley Interdiscip Rev Cogn Sci. 2015;6:383–395. doi: 10.1002/wcs.1348. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Giurfa M, Núñez JA. Honeybees mark with scent and reject recently visited flowers. Oecologia. 1992;89:113–117. doi: 10.1007/BF00319022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Giurfa M, Sandoz JC. Invertebrate learning and memory: Fifty years of olfactory conditioning of the proboscis extension response in honeybees. Learn Mem. 2012;19:54–66. doi: 10.1101/lm.024711.111. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Giurfa M, Hammer M, Stach S, Stollhoff N, MuLler-Deisig, Mizyrycki, CN (1999) Pattern learning by honeybees: conditioning procedure and recognition strategy. Anim Behav 57:315–324 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Giurfa M, Fabre E, Flaven-Pouchon J, Groll H, Oberwallner B, Vergoz V, Roussel E, Sandoz JC. Olfactory conditioning of the sting extension reflex in honeybees: memory dependence on trial number, interstimulus interval, intertrial interval, and protein synthesis. Learn Mem. 2009;16:761–5. doi: 10.1101/lm.1603009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gomez-Diaz C, Reina JH, Cambillau C, Benton R. Ligands for pheromone-sensing neurons are not conformationally activated odorant binding proteins. PLoS Biol. 2013;11:e1001546. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1001546. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Groh C, Rössler W. Analysis of synaptic microcircuits in the mushroom bodies of the honeybee. Insects. 2020;11:43. doi: 10.3390/insects11010043. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Groh C, Lu Z, Meinertzhagen I, a and Rössler, W. Age-related plasticity in the synaptic ultrastructure of neurons in the mushroom body calyx of the adult honeybee Apis mellifera. J Comp Neurol. 2012;520:3509–27. doi: 10.1002/cne.23102. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gronenberg W. Subdivisions of hymenopteran mushroom body calyces by their afferent supply. J Comp Neurol. 2001;435:474–489. doi: 10.1002/cne.1045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gronenberg W, Raikhelkar A, Abshire E, Stevens J, Epstein E, Loyola K, Rauscher M, Buchmann S. Honeybees (Apis mellifera) learn to discriminate the smell of organic compounds from their respective deuterated isotopomers. Proceedings Biol Sci. 2014;281:20133089. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2013.3089. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grünbaum L, Müller U. Induction of a specific olfactory memory leads to a long-lasting activation of protein kinase C in the antennal lobe of the honeybee. J Neurosci. 1998;18:4384–92. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-11-04384.1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grünewald B. Morphology of feedback neurons in the mushroom body of the honeybee, Apis mellifera. J Comp Neurol. 1999;404:114–26. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1096-9861(19990201)404:1<114::aid-cne9>3.3.co;2-r. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grünewald B. Physiological properties and response modulations of mushroom body feedback neurons during olfactory learning in the honeybee, Apis mellifera. J Comp Physiol - A Sensory, Neural Behav Physiol. 1999;185:565–576. [Google Scholar]
- Grüter C, Acosta LE, Farina WM. Propagation of olfactory information within the honeybee hive. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2006;60:707–715. [Google Scholar]
- Guerrieri F, Schubert M, Sandoz JC, Giurfa M. Perceptual and neural olfactory similarity in honeybees. PLoS Biol. 2005;3:0718–0732. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0030060. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haehnel M, Menzel R. Sensory representation and learning-related plasticity in mushroom body extrinsic feedback neurons of the protocerebral tract. Front Syst Neurosci. 2010;4:1–13. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2010.00161. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haehnel M, Menzel R. Long-term memory and response generalization in mushroom body extrinsic neurons in the honeybee Apis mellifera. J Exp Biol. 2012;215:559–565. doi: 10.1242/jeb.059626. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haenicke J, Yamagata N, Zwaka H, Nawrot M, Menzel R (2018) Neural correlates of odor learning in the presynaptic microglomerular circuitry in the honeybee mushroom body Calyx. eNeuro 5, 1–13 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Hammer M. An identified neuron mediates the unconditioned stimulus in associative olfactory learning in honeybees. Nature. 1993;366:59–63. doi: 10.1038/366059a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hammer M. The neural basis of associative reward learning in honeybees. Trends Neurosci. 1997;20:245–52. doi: 10.1016/s0166-2236(96)01019-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hammer M, Menzel R. Multiple sites of associative odor learning as revealed by local brain microinjections of octopamine in honeybees. Learn Mem. 1998;5:146–56. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- He XJ, Zhang XC, Jiang WJ, Barron AB, Zhang JH, Zeng ZJ. Starving honey bee (Apis mellifera) larvae signal pheromonally to worker bees. Sci Rep. 2016;6:22359. doi: 10.1038/srep22359. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Himmelreich S, Grünewald B. Cellular physiology of olfactory learning in the honeybee brain. Apidologie. 2012;43:308–321. [Google Scholar]
- Hoover SER, Keeling CI, Winston ML, Slessor KN. The effect of queen pheromones on worker honey bee ovary development. Naturwissenschaften. 2003;90:477–480. doi: 10.1007/s00114-003-0462-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hopfield JJ. Olfactory computation and object perception. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1991;88:6462–6. doi: 10.1073/pnas.88.15.6462. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hourcade B, Perisse E, Devaud J-M, Sandoz J-C. Long-term memory shapes the primary olfactory center of an insect brain. Learn Mem. 2009;16:607–15. doi: 10.1101/lm.1445609. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hourcade B, Muenz TS, Sandoz J-C, Rössler W, Devaud J-M. Long-term memory leads to synaptic reorganization in the mushroom bodies: a memory trace in the insect brain? J Neurosci. 2010;30:6461–5. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0841-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Iovinella I, Cappa F, Cini A, Petrocelli I, Cervo R, Turillazzi S, Dani FR. Antennal protein profile in honeybees: caste and task matter more than age. Front Physiol. 2018;9:1–16. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2018.00748. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Iwama A, Shibuya T. Physiology and morphology of olfactory neurons associating with the protocerebral lobe of the honeybee brain. J Insect Physiol. 1998;44:1191–1204. doi: 10.1016/s0022-1910(98)00084-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jarome TJ, Helmstetter FJ. The ubiquitin-proteasome system as a critical regulator of synaptic plasticity and long-term memory formation. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2013;105:107–16. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2013.03.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jarriault D, Mercer AR. Queen mandibular pheromone: questions that remain to be resolved. Apidologie. 2012;43:292–307. [Google Scholar]
- Jeanne JM, Fişek M, Wilson RI. The organization of projections from olfactory glomeruli onto higher-order neurons. Neuron. 2018;98:1198–1213.e6. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.05.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jefferis GSXE, Potter CJ, Chan AM, Marin EC, Rohlfing T, Maurer CR, Luo L. Comprehensive maps of Drosophila higher olfactory centers: spatially segregated fruit and pheromone representation. Cell. 2007;128:1187–1203. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2007.01.040. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jernigan CM, Halby R, Gerkin RC, Sinakevitch I, Locatelli F, Smith BH (2020) Experience-dependent tuning of early olfactory processing in the adult honey bee, Apis mellifera. J Exp Biol 223, jeb206748 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Joerges J, Küttner A, Galizia CG, Menzel R. Representations of odours and odour mixtures visualized in the honeybee brain. Nature. 1997;387:285–288. [Google Scholar]
- Junca P, Sandoz JC. Heat perception and aversive learning in honey bees: putative involvement of the thermal/chemical sensor AmHsTRPA. Front Physiol. 2015;6:1–15. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2015.00316. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Karlson P, Lüscher M. ‘Pheromones’: a new term for a class of biologically active substances. Nature. 1959;183:55–56. doi: 10.1038/183055a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kay LM, Crk T, Thorngate J. A redefinition of odor mixture quality. Behav Neurosci. 2005;119:726–733. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.119.3.726. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Keeling CI, Slessor KN, Higo HA, Winston ML. New components of the honey bee (Apis mellifera L.) queen retinue pheromone. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2003;100:4486–4491. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0836984100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kelber C, Rössler W, Kleineidam CJ. Multiple olfactory receptor neurons and their axonal projections in the antennal lobe of the honeybee Apis mellifera. J Comp Neurol. 2006;496:395–405. doi: 10.1002/cne.20930. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kirkerud NH, Wehmann HN, Giovanni Galizia C, Gustav D. APIS - A novel approach for conditioning honey bees. Front Behav Neurosci. 2013;7:1–19. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2013.00029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kirschner S, Kleineidam CJ, Zube C, Rybak J, Grünewald B, Rössler W. Dual olfactory pathway in the honeybee, Apis mellifera. J Comp Neurol. 2006;499:933–52. doi: 10.1002/cne.21158. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Knudsen JT, Tollsten L, Bergstrom LG. Floral scents-A checklist of volatile compounds by head-space techniques. Phytochemistry. 1993;33:253–280. [Google Scholar]
- Kocher SD, Richard FJ, Tarpy DR, Grozinger CM. Queen reproductive state modulates pheromone production and queen-worker interactions in honeybees. Behav. Ecol. 2009;20:1007–1014. doi: 10.1093/beheco/arp090. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koeniger N, Koeniger G. Mating behavior in honey bees (Genus Apis) Trop Agric Res Ext. 2004;7:13–28. [Google Scholar]
- Koethe S, Fischbach V, Banysch S, Reinartz L, Hrncir M, Lunau K. A comparative study of food source selection in stingless bees and honeybees : scent marks, location, or color. Front Plant Sci. 2020;11:1–10. doi: 10.3389/fpls.2020.00516. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koywiwattrakul P, Thompson GJ, Sitthipraneed S, Oldroyd BP, Maleszka R. Effects of carbon dioxide narcosis on ovary activation and gene expression in worker honeybees Apis mellifera. J Insect Sci. 2005;5:1–10. doi: 10.1093/jis/5.1.36. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kramer E. The orientation of walking honeybees in odour fields with small concentration gradients. Physiol Entomol. 1976;1:27–37. [Google Scholar]
- Kreissl S, Bicker G. Histochemistry of acetylcholinesterase and immunocytochemistry of an acetylcholine receptor-like antigen in the brain of the honeybee. J Comp Neurol. 1989;286:71–84. doi: 10.1002/cne.902860105. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kreissl S, Eichmüller S, Bicker G, Rapus J, Eckert M. Octopamine-like immunoreactivity in the brain and subesophageal ganglion of the honeybee. J Comp Neurol. 1994;348:583–595. doi: 10.1002/cne.903480408. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kreissl S, Strasser C, Galizia CG. Allatostatin immunoreactivity in the honeybee brain. J Comp Neurol. 2010;518:1391–1417. doi: 10.1002/cne.22343. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krieger J, Klink O, Mohl C, Raming K, Breer H. A candidate olfactory receptor subtype highly conserved across different insect orders. J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol. 2003;189:519–26. doi: 10.1007/s00359-003-0427-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krofczik S, Menzel R, Nawrot MP. Rapid odor processing in the honeybee antennal lobe network. Front Comput Neurosci. 2009;2:9. doi: 10.3389/neuro.10.009.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuwabara M. Bildung des bedingten Reflexes von Pavlovs Typus bei der Honigbiene, Apis mellifica. Jour Fac Sei Hokkaido Univ Sero VI, Zool. 1957;13:458–464. [Google Scholar]
- Lacher V. Elektrophysiologische Untersuchungen an einzelnen Rezeptoren für Geruch, Kohlendioxyd, Luftfeuchtigkeit und Tempratur auf den Antennen der Arbeitsbiene und der Drohne (Apis mellifica L.) Z Vgl Physiol. 1964;48:587–623. [Google Scholar]
- Laloi D, Bailez O, Blight MM, Roger B, Pham-Delegue M, Wadhams LJ. Recognition of complex odors by restrained and free-flying honeybees, Apis mellifera. J Chem Ecol. 2000;26:2307–2319. [Google Scholar]
- Larter NK, Sun JS, Carlson JR. Organization and function of Drosophila odorant binding proteins. Elife. 2016;5:1–22. doi: 10.7554/eLife.20242. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Laska M, Galizia CG. Enantioselectivity of odor perception in honeybees (Apis mellifera carnica) Behav Neurosci. 2001;115:632–9. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.115.3.632. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Laska M, Galizia CG, Giurfa M, Menzel R. Olfactory discrimination ability and odor structure-activity relationships in honeybees. Chem Senses. 1999;24:429–438. doi: 10.1093/chemse/24.4.429. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Laughlin JD, Ha TS, Jones DNM, Smith DP. Activation of pheromone-sensitive neurons is mediated by conformational activation of pheromone-binding protein. Cell. 2008;133:1255–1265. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2008.04.046. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Le Conte Y, Arnold G, Trouiller J, Masson C, Chappe B. Identification of a brood pheromone in honeybees. Naturwissenschaften. 1990;77:334–336. [Google Scholar]
- Le Conte Y, Mohammedi A, Robinson GE. Primer effects of a brood pheromone on honeybee behavioural development. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci. 2001;268:163–168. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2000.1345. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leal WS. Odorant reception in insects: roles of receptors, binding proteins, and degrading enzymes. Annu Rev Entomol. 2013;58:373–391. doi: 10.1146/annurev-ento-120811-153635. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leboulle G, Müller U. Synergistic activation of insect cAMP-dependent protein kinase A (type II) by cyclicAMP and cyclicGMP. FEBS Lett. 2004;576:216–20. doi: 10.1016/j.febslet.2004.08.079. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leitch B, Laurent G. GABAergic synapses in the antennal lobe and mushroom body of the locust olfactory system. J Comp Neurol. 1996;372:487–514. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1096-9861(19960902)372:4<487::AID-CNE1>3.0.CO;2-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lensky Y, Blum MS. Chirality in insect chemoreceptors. Life Sci. 1974;14:2045–2049. doi: 10.1016/0024-3205(74)90422-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leoncini I, Le Conte Y, Costagliola G, Plettner E, Toth AL, Wang M, Huang Z, Bécard JM, Crauser D, Slessor KN, et al. Regulation of behavioral maturation by a primer pheromone produced by adult worker honey bees. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2004;101:17559–17564. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0407652101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lin AC, Bygrave AM, De Calignon A, Lee T, Miesenböck G. Sparse, decorrelated odor coding in the mushroom body enhances learned odor discrimination. Nat Neurosci. 2014;17:559–568. doi: 10.1038/nn.3660. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Linster C, Sachse S, Galizia CG. Computational modeling suggests that response properties rather than spatial position determine connectivity between olfactory glomeruli. J Neurophysiol. 2005;93:3410–3417. doi: 10.1152/jn.01285.2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Locatelli F, Bundrock G, Müller U. Focal and temporal release of glutamate in the mushroom bodies improves olfactory memory in Apis mellifera. J Neurosci. 2005;25:11614–11618. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3180-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Locatelli FF, Fernandez PC, Villareal F, Muezzinoglu K, Huerta R, Galizia CG, Smith BH. Nonassociative plasticity alters competitive interactions among mixture components in early olfactory processing. Eur J Neurosci. 2013;37:63–79. doi: 10.1111/ejn.12021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Locatelli FF, Fernandez PC, Smith BH. Learning about natural variation of odor mixtures enhances categorization in early olfactory processing. J Exp Biol. 2016;219:2752–2762. doi: 10.1242/jeb.141465. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lockett GA, Helliwell P, Maleszka R. Involvement of DNA methylation in memory processing in the honey bee. Neuroreport. 2010;21:812–816. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e32833ce5be. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lüdke A, Raiser G, Nehrkorn J, Herz AVM, Galizia CG, Szyszka P. Calcium in Kenyon cell somata as a substrate for an olfactory sensory memory in Drosophila. Front Cell Neurosci. 2018;12:1–16. doi: 10.3389/fncel.2018.00128. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mackensen O. Effect of carbon dioxide on initial oviposition of artificially inseminated and virgin queen bees. J Econ Entomol. 1947;40:344–349. doi: 10.1093/jee/40.3.344. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mafra-Neto A, Cardé RT. Fine-scale structure of pheromone plumes modulates upwind orientation of flying moths. Nature. 1994;369:142–144. [Google Scholar]
- Maisonnasse A, Lenoir JC, Costagliola G, Beslay D, Choteau F, Crauser D, Becard JM, Plettner E, Conte Y. Le. A scientific note on E-β-ocimene, a new volatile primer pheromone that inhibits worker ovary development in honey bees. Apidologie. 2009;40:562–564. [Google Scholar]
- Malnic B, Hirono J, Sato T, Buck LB. Combinatorial receptor codes for odors. Cell. 1999;96:713–23. doi: 10.1016/s0092-8674(00)80581-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Malun D, Giurfa M, Galizia CG, Plath N, Brandt R, Gerber B, Eisermann B. Hydroxyurea-induced partial mushroom body ablation does not affect acquisition and retention of olfactory differential conditioning in honeybees. J Neurobiol. 2002;53:343–60. doi: 10.1002/neu.10119. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martin JP, Hildebrand JG. Innate recognition of pheromone and food odors in moths: a common mechanism in the antennal lobe? Front Behav Neurosci. 2010;4:159. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2010.00159. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Matsumoto Y, Menzel R, Sandoz JC, Giurfa M. Revisiting olfactory classical conditioning of the proboscis extension response in honey bees: A step toward standardized procedures. J Neurosci Methods. 2012;211:159–167. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2012.08.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Matsumoto Y, Sandoz JC, Devaud JM, Lormant F, Mizunami M, Giurfa M. Cyclic nucleotide-gated channels, calmodulin, adenylyl cyclase, and calcium/calmodulin-dependent protein kinase II are required for late, but not early, long-term memory formation in the honeybee. Learn Mem. 2014;21:272–286. doi: 10.1101/lm.032037.113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mauelshagen J. Neural correlates of olfactory learning paradigms in an identified neuron in the honeybee brain. J Neurophysiol. 1993;69:609–25. doi: 10.1152/jn.1993.69.2.609. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Menzel R. Associative learning in honey bees. Apidologie. 1993;24:157–168. [Google Scholar]
- Menzel R. Memory dynamics in the honeybee. J. Comp. Physiol. - A Sensory, Neural. Behav Physiol. 1999;185:323–340. [Google Scholar]
- Menzel R. The honeybee as a model for understanding the basis of cognition. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2012;13:758–768. doi: 10.1038/nrn3357. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meyer A, Galizia CG. Elemental and configural olfactory coding by antennal lobe neurons of the honeybee (Apis mellifera). J. Comp. Physiol. A Neuroethol. Sensory, Neural. Behav Physiol. 2012;198:159–171. doi: 10.1007/s00359-011-0696-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meyer A, Galizia CG, Nawrot MP. Local interneurons and projection neurons in the antennal lobe from a spiking point of view. J Neurophysiol. 2013;110:2465–2474. doi: 10.1152/jn.00260.2013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Michelsen A, Andersen BB, Storm J, Kirchner WH, Lindauer M. How honeybees perceive communication dances, studied by means of a mechanical model. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 1992;30:143–150. [Google Scholar]
- Millor J, Pham-Delegue M, Deneubourg JL, Camazine S. Self-organized defensive behavior in honeybees. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1999;96:12611–12615. doi: 10.1073/pnas.96.22.12611. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mobbs PG. The brain of the honeybee Apis mellifera. I. The connections and spatial organization of the mushroom bodies. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. London. B. Biol Sci. 1982;298:309–354. [Google Scholar]
- Mohammedi A, Paris A, Crauser D, Le Conte Y. Effect of aliphatic esters on ovary development of queenless bees (Apis melliera L.) Naturwissenschaften. 1998;85:455–458. [Google Scholar]
- Müller U. Prolonged activation of cAMP-dependent protein kinase during conditioning induces long-term memory in honeybees. Neuron. 2000;27:159–168. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)00017-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Müller D, Abel R, Brandt R, Zöckler M, Menzel R. Differential parallel processing of olfactory information in the honeybee, Apis mellifera L. J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sensory, Neural Behav Physiol. 2002;188:359–370. doi: 10.1007/s00359-002-0310-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Münch D, Galizia CG. DoOR 2.0 - Comprehensive mapping of Drosophilamelanogaster odorant responses. Sci Rep. 2016;6:1–14. doi: 10.1038/srep21841. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murlis J, Elkinton JS, Cardé RT. Odor plumes and how insects use them. Annu Rev Entomol. 1992;37:505–532. [Google Scholar]
- Müssig L, Richlitzki A, Rössler R, Eisenhardt D, Menzel R, Leboulle G. Acute disruption of the NMDA receptor subunit NR1 in the honeybee brain selectively impairs memory formation. J Neurosci. 2010;30:7817–25. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5543-09.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nishino H, Nishikawa M, Mizunami M, Yokohari F. Functional and topographic segregation of glomeruli revealed by local staining of antennal sensory neurons in the honeybee Apis mellifera. J Comp Neurol. 2009;515:161–180. doi: 10.1002/cne.22064. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nouvian M, Galizia CG. Aversive training of honey bees in an automated y-maze. Front Physiol. 2019;10:1–17. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2019.00678. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nouvian M, Hotier L, Claudianos C, Giurfa M, Reinhard J. Appetitive floral odours prevent aggression in honeybees. Nat Commun. 2015;6:10247. doi: 10.1038/ncomms10247. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nouvian M, Mandal S, Jamme C, Claudianos C, D’Ettorre P, Reinhard J, Barron AB, Giurfa M. Cooperative defence operates by social modulation of biogenic amine levels in the honey bee brain. Sci: Proc R Soc B Biol; 2018. p. 285. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nowotny T, Stierle JS, Galizia CG, Szyszka P. Data-driven honeybee antennal lobe model suggests how stimulus-onset asynchrony can aid odour segregation. Brain Res. 2013;1536:119–134. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2013.05.038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Okada R, Rybak J, Manz G, Menzel R. Learning-related plasticity in PE1 and other mushroom body-extrinsic neurons in the honeybee brain. J Neurosci. 2007;27:11736–11747. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2216-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pamir E, Szyszka P, Scheiner R, Nawrot MP. Rapid learning dynamics in individual honeybees during classical conditioning. Front Behav Neurosci. 2014;8:313. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00313. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paoli M, Anesi A, Antolini R, Guella G, Vallortigara G, Haase A. Differential odour coding of isotopomers in the honeybee brain. Sci Rep. 2016;6:21893. doi: 10.1038/srep21893. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paoli M, Weisz N, Antolini R, Haase A. Spatially resolved time-frequency analysis of odour coding in the insect antennal lobe. Eur J Neurosci. 2016;44:2387–2395. doi: 10.1111/ejn.13344. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paoli M, Münch D, Haase A, Skoulakis E, Turin L, Galizia CG (2017) Minute impurities contribute significantly to olfactory receptorl ligand studies: tales from testing the vibration theory. eNeuro 4, e0070-17.2017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Paoli M, Albi A, Zanon M, Zanini D, Antolini R, Haase A. Neuronal response latencies encode first odor identity information across subjects. J Neurosci. 2018;38:9240–9251. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0453-18.2018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Papadopoulou M, Cassenaer S, Nowotny T, Laurent G (2011) Normalization for sparse encoding of odours by a wide-field interneurons. Science (80-. ). 332, 721–725 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Pareto A. Spatial distribution of sensory antennal fibres in the central nervous system of worker bees. Zeitschrift für Zellforsch und mikroskopische Anat. 1972;131:109–40. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Parnas M, Lin AC, Huetteroth W, Miesenböck G. Odor discrimination in drosophila: from neural population codes to behavior. Neuron. 2013;79:932–944. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.08.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pavlov IP (1927) Conditioned Reflexes: an Investigation of the Physiological Activity of the Cerebral Cortex. (ed. Dr. G. V. Anrep) Oxford University Press [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Peele P, Ditzen M, Menzel R, Galizia CG. Appetitive odor learning does not change olfactory coding in a subpopulation of honeybee antennal lobe neurons. J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol. 2006;192:1083–103. doi: 10.1007/s00359-006-0152-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pelz C, Gerber B, Menzel R. Odorant intensity as a determinant for olfactory conditioning in honeybees: roles in discrimination, overshadowing and memory consolidation. J Exp Biol. 1997;200:837–847. doi: 10.1242/jeb.200.4.837. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pickett JA, Williams IH, Martin AP, Smith MC. Nasonov pheromone of the honey bee, Apis mellifera L. (Hymenoptera: Apidae) - Part I Chemical characterization. J Chem Ecol. 1980;6:425–434. doi: 10.1007/BF00987702. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Popov T, Szyszka P. Alpha oscillations govern interhemispheric spike timing coordination in the honey bee brain. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci. 2020;287:20200115. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2020.0115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rath L, Giovanni Galizia C, Szyszka P. Multiple memory traces after associative learning in the honey bee antennal lobe. Eur J Neurosci. 2011;34:352–360. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2011.07753.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rein J, Mustard JA, Strauch M, Smith BH, Galizia CG. Octopamine modulates activity of neural networks in the honey bee antennal lobe. J Comp Physiol A. 2013;199:947–962. doi: 10.1007/s00359-013-0805-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reinhard J, Claudianos C (2012) Molecular Insights into Honey Bee Brain Plasticity. In Honeybee Neurobiology and Behavior - A tribute to Randolf Menzel (ed. Galizia, G. C.), Eisenhardt, D.), and Giurfa, M.), pp. 359–372. Springer
- Reinhard J, Sinclair M, Srinivasan MV, Claudianos C (2010) Honeybees learn odour mixtures via a selection of key odorants. PLoS One 5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Riffell JA, Abrell L, Hildebrand JG. Physical processes and real-time chemical measurement of the insect olfactory environment. J Chem Ecol. 2008;34:837–53. doi: 10.1007/s10886-008-9490-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robertson HM. Molecular evolution of the major arthropod chemoreceptor gene families. Annu Rev Entomol. 2019;64:227–242. doi: 10.1146/annurev-ento-020117-043322. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robertson HM, Kent LB. Evolution of the gene lineage encoding the carbon dioxide receptor in insects. J Insect Sci. 2009;9:19. doi: 10.1673/031.009.1901. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robertson HM, Wanner KW. The chemoreceptor superfamily in the honey bee, Apis mellifera: expansion of the odorant, but not gustatory, receptor family. Genome Res. 2006;16:1395–1403. doi: 10.1101/gr.5057506. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rohrseitz K, Tautz J. Honey bee dance communication: Waggle run direction coded in antennal contacts? J Comp Physiol A Sensory, Neural Behav Physiol. 1999;184:463–470. [Google Scholar]
- Rössler W, Brill MF (2013) Parallel processing in the honeybee olfactory pathway: structure, function, and evolution. J Comp Physiol A 199 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Roussel E, Sandoz J-C, Giurfa M. Searching for learning-dependent changes in the antennal lobe: simultaneous recording of neural activity and aversive olfactory learning in honeybees. Front Behav Neurosci. 2010;4:1–12. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2010.00155. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roussel E, Carcaud J, Combe M, Giurfa M, Sandoz JC. Olfactory coding in the honeybee lateral horn. Curr Biol. 2014;24:561–567. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2014.01.063. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ruttner H, Ruttner F. Investigations on the flight activity and mating behavior of drones. Apidologie. 1972;3(203):232. [Google Scholar]
- Rybak J (2012) The Digital Honey Bee Brain Atlas. In Honeybee Neurobiology and Behavior - A tribute to Randolf Menzelavior (ed. Galizia, G. C.), Eisenhardt, D.), and Giurfa, M.), pp. 125–140. Springer
- Rybak J, Menzel R. Anatomy of the mushroom bodies in the honey bee brain: the neuronal connections of the alpha-lobe. J. Comp Neurol. 1993;334:444–465. doi: 10.1002/cne.903340309. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rybak J, Menzel R. Integrative properties of the Pe1 neuron, a unique mushroom body output neuron. Learn Mem. 1998;5:133–145. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sachse S, Beshel J. The good, the bad, and the hungry: how the central brain codes odor valence to facilitate food approach in Drosophila. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2016;40:53–58. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2016.06.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sachse S, Galizia CG. Role of inhibition for temporal and spatial odor representation in olfactory output neurons: a calcium imaging study. J Neurophysiol. 2002;87:1106–17. doi: 10.1152/jn.00325.2001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sachse S, Galizia CG. The coding of odour-intensity in the honeybee antennal lobe: local computation optimizes odour representation. Eur J Neurosci. 2003;18:2119–2132. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2003.02931.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sachse S, Rappert A, Galizia CG. The spatial representation of chemical structures in the antennal lobe of honeybees: Steps towards the olfactory code. Eur J Neurosci. 1999;11:3970–3982. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.1999.00826.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sachse S, Peele P, Silbering AF, Gühmann M, Galizia CG. Role of histamine as a putative inhibitory transmitter in the honeybee antennal lobe. Front Zool. 2006;3:1–7. doi: 10.1186/1742-9994-3-22. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sachse S, Rueckert E, Keller A, Okada R, Tanaka NK, Ito K, Vosshall LB. Activity-dependent plasticity in an olfactory circuit. Neuron. 2007;56:838–850. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.10.035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sagili RR, Pankiw T, Metz BN. Division of labor associated with brood rearing in the honey bee: how does it translate to colony fitness? PLoS One. 2011;6:1–7. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0016785. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sandoz JC. Behavioral and neurophysiological study of olfactory perception and learning in honeybees. Front Syst Neurosci. 2011;5:98. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2011.00098. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sandoz J-C, Roger B, Pham-Delégue MH. Olfactory learning and memory in the honeybee : comparison of different classical conditioning procedures of the proboscis extension response. C R Acad Sci Paris. 1995;318:749–755. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sandoz J, Pham-Delègue M, Renou M, Wadhams L. Asymmetrical generalisation between pheromonal and floral odours in appetitive olfactory conditioning of the honey bee (Apis mellifera L.) J Comp Physiol A. 2001;187:559–568. doi: 10.1007/s003590100228. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sandoz J, Galizia C, Menzel R. Side-specific olfactory conditioning leads to more specific odor representation between sides but not within sides in the honeybee antennal lobes. Neuroscience. 2003;120:1137–1148. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(03)00384-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sandoz JC, Deisig N, de Brito Sanchez MG, Giurfa M. Understanding the logics of pheromone processing in the honeybee brain: From labeled-lines to across-fiber patterns. Front Behav Neurosci. 2007;1:1–12. doi: 10.3389/neuro.08.005.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sato K, Pellegrino M, Nakagawa T, Nakagawa T, Vosshall LB, Touhara K. Insect olfactory receptors are heteromeric ligand-gated ion channels. Nature. 2008;452:1002–6. doi: 10.1038/nature06850. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schäfer S, Bicker G. Distribution of GABA-like immunoreactivity in the brain of the honeybee. Neuroscience. 1986;246:287–300. doi: 10.1002/cne.902460302. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schäfer S, Bicker G, Ottersen OP, Storm-Mathisen J. Taurine-like immunoreactivity in the brain of the honeybee. J Comp Neurol. 1988;268:60–70. doi: 10.1002/cne.902680107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmidt JO. Hierarchy of attractants for honey bee swarms. J Insect Behav. 2001;14:469–477. [Google Scholar]
- Schneider D, Steinbrecht R. Checklist of insect olfactory sensilla. Symp Zool Soc Lond. 1968;23:279–297. [Google Scholar]
- Scholl C, Wang Y, Krischke M, Mueller MJ, Amdam GV, Rössler W. Light exposure leads to reorganization of microglomeruli in the mushroom bodies and influences juvenile hormone levels in the honeybee. Dev Neurobiol. 2014;74:1141–1153. doi: 10.1002/dneu.22195. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schubert M, Sandoz J-C, Galizia G, Giurfa M. Odourant dominance in olfactory mixture processing: what makes a strong odourant? Proceedings Biol Sci. 2015;282:20142562. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2014.2562. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schwärzel M, Müller U. Dynamic memory networks: Dissecting molecular mechanisms underlying associative memory in the temporal domain. Cell Mol Life Sci. 2006;63:989–998. doi: 10.1007/s00018-006-6024-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seeley TD. Atmospheric carbon dioxide regulation in honey-bee (Apis mellifera) colonies. J Insect Physiol. 1974;20:2301–2305. doi: 10.1016/0022-1910(74)90052-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seki Y, Rybak J, Wicher D, Sachse S, Hansson BS. Physiological and morphological characterization of local interneurons in the Drosophila antennal lobe. J Neurophysiol. 2010;104:1007–19. doi: 10.1152/jn.00249.2010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shearer DA, Boch R. 2-heptanone in the mandibular gland secretion of the honeybee. Nature. 1965;206:530. [Google Scholar]
- Shepard RN. Toward a universal law of generalization for psychological science. Science. 1987;237:1317–1323. doi: 10.1126/science.3629243. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Silbering AF, Rytz R, Grosjean Y, Abuin L, Ramdya P, Jefferis GSXE, Benton R. Complementary function and integrated wiring of the evolutionarily distinct Drosophila olfactory subsystems. J Neurosci. 2011;31:13357–75. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2360-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sinakevitch I, Mustard JA, Smith BH. Distribution of the octopamine receptor AmOA1 in the honey bee brain. PLoS One. 2011;6:e14536. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0014536. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sinakevitch IT, Smith AN, Locatelli F, Huerta R, Bazhenov M, Smith BH. Apis mellifera octopamine receptor 1 (AmOA1) expression in antennal lobe networks of the honey bee (Apis mellifera) and fruit fly (Drosophila melanogaster) Front Syst Neurosci. 2013;7:70. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2013.00070. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Slessor KN, Kaminski L-A, King GGS, Borden JH, Winston ML. Semiochemical basis of the retinue response to queen honey bees. Nature. 1988;332:354–356. [Google Scholar]
- Slessor KN, Winston ML, Le Conte Y. Pheromone communication in the honeybee (Apis mellifera L.) J Chem Ecol. 2005;31:2731–2745. doi: 10.1007/s10886-005-7623-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith BH, Menzel R. The use of electromyogram recordings to quantify odourant discrimination in the honey bee Apis mellifera. J Insect Physiol. 1989;35:369–375. [Google Scholar]
- Song XM, Zhang LY, Fu X. Bin, Wu F, Tan J, Li HL. Various bee pheromones binding affinity, exclusive chemosensillar localization, and key amino acid sites reveal the distinctive characteristics of odorant-binding protein 11 in the Eastern honey bee Apis cerana. Front Physiol. 2018;9:422. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2018.00422. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stange G, Diesendorf M. The response of the honeybee antennal CO2-receptors to N2O and Xe. J Comp Physiol. 1973;86:139–158. [Google Scholar]
- Stierle JS, Galizia CG, Szyszka P. Millisecond stimulus onset-asynchrony enhances information about components in an odor mixture. J Neurosci. 2013;33:6060–6069. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5838-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stopfer M, Bhagavan S, Smith BH, Laurent G. Normalization for sparse encoding of odours by a wide-filed interneurons. Nature. 1997;390:70–74. doi: 10.1038/36335. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stout JC, Goulson D. The use of conspecific and interspecific scent marks by foraging bumblebees and honeybees. Anim Behav. 2001;62:183–189. [Google Scholar]
- Strauch M, Ditzen M, Galizia CG. Keeping their distance? Odor response patterns along the concentration range. Front Syst Neurosci. 2012;6:71. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2012.00071. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Strausfeld NJ. Organization of the honey bee mushroom body: Representation of the calyx within the vertical and gamma lobes. J Comp Neurol. 2002;450:4–33. doi: 10.1002/cne.10285. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Strube-Bloss MF, Nawrot MP, Menzel R. Mushroom body output neurons encode odor-reward associations. J Neurosci. 2011;31:3129–3140. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2583-10.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Strutz A, Soelter J, Baschwitz A, Farhan A, Grabe V, Rybak J, Knaden M, Schmuker M, Hansson BS, Sachse S. Decoding odor quality and intensity in the Drosophila brain. Elife. 2014;3:e04147. doi: 10.7554/eLife.04147. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Suenami S, Oya S, Kohno H, Kubo T. Kenyon cell subtypes/populations in the honeybee mushroom bodies: possible function based on their gene expression profiles, differentiation, possible evolution, and application of genome editing. Front Psychol. 2018;9:1–9. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01717. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Suh GSB, Wong AM, Hergarden AC, Wang JW, Simon AF, Benzer S, Axel R, Anderson DJ. A single population of olfactory sensory neurons mediates an innate avoidance behaviour in Drosophila. Nature. 2004;431:854–9. doi: 10.1038/nature02980. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sun X-J, Fonta C, Masson C. Odour quality processing by bee antennal lobe interneurones. Chem Senses. 1993;18:355–377. [Google Scholar]
- Szyszka P, Stierle JS. Mixture processing and odor-object segregation in insects. Prog Brain Res. 2014;208:63–85. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-444-63350-7.00003-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Szyszka P, Ditzen M, Galkin A, Galizia CG, Menzel R, Giovanni C, Sparsening RM. Sparsening and temporal sharpening of olfactory representations in the honeybee mushroom bodies. J Neurophysiol. 2005;94:3303–3313. doi: 10.1152/jn.00397.2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Szyszka P, Galkin A, Menzel R. Associative and non-associative plasticity in Kenyon cells of the honeybee mushroom body. Front Syst Neurosci. 2008;2:1–10. doi: 10.3389/neuro.06.003.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Szyszka P, Demmler C, Oemisch M, Sommer L, Biergans S, Birnbach B, Silbering AF, Galizia CG. Mind the gap: olfactory trace conditioning in honeybees. J Neurosci. 2011;31:7229–39. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6668-10.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Szyszka P, Stierle JS, Biergans S, Galizia CG. The speed of smell: odor-object segregation within milliseconds. PLoS One. 2012;7:4–7. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0036096. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Szyszka P, Gerkin RC, Galizia CG, Smith BH. High-speed odor transduction and pulse tracking by insect olfactory receptor neurons. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2014;111:16925–30. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1412051111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Takeda K. Classical conditioned response in the honey bee. J Insect Physiol. 1961;6:168–179. [Google Scholar]
- Tautz J. Honeybee waggle dance: recruitment success depends on the dance floor. J Exp Biol. 1996;199:1375–1381. doi: 10.1242/jeb.199.6.1375. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thom C, Gilley DC, Hooper J, Esch HE. The scent of the waggle dance. PLoS Biol. 2007;5:1862–1867. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0050228. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Turin L. A spectroscopic mechanism for primary olfactory reception. Chem Senses. 1996;21:773–791. doi: 10.1093/chemse/21.6.773. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Urlacher E, Frances B, Giurfa M, Devaud J-M. An alarm pheromone modulates appetitive olfactory learning in the honeybee (Apis mellifera) Front Behav Neurosci. 2010;4:1–10. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2010.00157. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vergoz V, Roussel E, Sandoz JC, Giurfa M. Aversive learning in honeybees revealed by the olfactory conditioning of the sting extension reflex. PLoS One. 2007;2:e288. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0000288. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Villar ME, Marchal P, Viola H, Giurfa M. Redefining single-trial memories in the honeybee. Cell Rep. 2020;30:2603–2613.e3. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2020.01.086. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vogt RG, Riddiford LM, Prestwich GD. Kinetic properties of a sex pheromone-degrading enzyme: the sensillar esterase of Antheraea polyphemus. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1985;82:8827–31. doi: 10.1073/pnas.82.24.8827. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vogt RG, Prestwich GD, Lerner MR. Odorant-binding-protein subfamilies associate with distinct classes of olfactory receptor neurons in insects. J Neurobiol. 1991;22:74–84. doi: 10.1002/neu.480220108. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- von Frisch K (1965) Die Tänze der Bienen. In Tanzsprache und Orientierung der Bienen, pp. 3–330. Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer Berlin Heidelberg
- von Frisch K. The dancing bees: an account of the life and sensing of the honey bee. London: Metheuen; 1966. [Google Scholar]
- Vosshall LB, Wong AM, Axel R. An olfactory sensory map in the fly brain. Cell. 2000;102:147–159. doi: 10.1016/s0092-8674(00)00021-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wanner KW, Nichols AS, Walden KKO, Brockmann A, Luetje CW, Robertson HM. A honey bee odorant receptor for the queen substance 9-oxo-2-decenoic acid. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2007;104:14383–14388. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0705459104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wehr M, Laurent G. Odour encoding by temporal sequences of firing in oscillating neural assemblies. Nature. 1996;384:162–6. doi: 10.1038/384162a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson EO, Bossert WH. Chemical communication among animals. Recent Progr Harm Res. 1963;19:673–716. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Witthöft W. Absolute anzahl und verteilung der zellen im him der honigbiene. Zeitschrift für Morphol der Tiere. 1967;61:160–184. [Google Scholar]
- Wong AM, Wang JW, Axel R. Spatial representation of the glomerular map in the Drosophila protocerebrum. Cell. 2002;109:229–41. doi: 10.1016/s0092-8674(02)00707-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wright GA, Schiestl FP. The evolution of floral scent: the influence of olfactory learning by insect pollinators on the honest signalling of floral rewards. Funct Ecol. 2009;23:841–851. [Google Scholar]
- Wright GA, Smith BH. Different thresholds for detection and discrimination of odors in the honey bee (Apis mellifera) Chem Senses. 2004;29:127–135. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjh016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wüstenberg D, Gerber B, Menzel R. Short communication: long- but not medium-term retention of olfactory memories in honeybees is impaired by actinomycin D and anisomycin. Eur J Neurosci. 1998;10:2742–5. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.1998.00319.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Xiao S, Sun JS, Carlson JR. Robust olfactory responses in the absence of odorant binding proteins. Elife. 2019;8:1–17. doi: 10.7554/eLife.51040. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yamagata N, Schmuker M, Szyszka P, Mizunami M, Menzel R. Differential odor processing in two olfactory pathways in the honeybee. Front Syst Neurosci. 2009;3:1–13. doi: 10.3389/neuro.06.016.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Younus F, Chertemps T, Pearce SL, Pandey G, Bozzolan F, Coppin CW, Russell RJ, Maïbèche-Coisne M, Oakeshott JG. Identification of candidate odorant degrading gene/enzyme systems in the antennal transcriptome of Drosophila melanogaster. Insect Biochem Mol Biol. 2014;53:30–43. doi: 10.1016/j.ibmb.2014.07.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Younus F, Fraser NJ, Coppin CW, Liu JW, Correy GJ, Chertemps T, Pandey G, Maïbèche M, Jackson CJ, Oakeshott JG. Molecular basis for the behavioral effects of the odorant degrading enzyme Esterase 6 in Drosophila. Sci Rep. 2017;7:1–12. doi: 10.1038/srep46188. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zwaka H, Münch D, Manz G, Menzel R, Rybak J. The circuitry of olfactory projection neurons in the brain of the honeybee Apis mellifera. Front Neuroanat. 2016;10:90. doi: 10.3389/fnana.2016.00090. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zwaka H, Bartels R, Grünewald B, Menzel R. Neural organization of A3 mushroom body extrinsic neurons in the honeybee brain. Front Neuroanat. 2018;12:1–11. doi: 10.3389/fnana.2018.00057. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]