Skip to main content
British Journal of Pain logoLink to British Journal of Pain
. 2020 Mar 10;15(1):82–90. doi: 10.1177/2049463720911399

Should musicians play in pain?

Jessica Stanhope 1,, Philip Weinstein 2
PMCID: PMC7882775  PMID: 33633855

Abstract

Musculoskeletal symptoms, including pain, are often experienced by musicians at all levels. These symptoms may have a detrimental impact on musicians’ personal and work lives, and may also impact upon the ensembles they work within. Providing musicians with appropriate, evidence-based advice regarding pain management is therefore paramount. In this review, we aim to improve the advice given to musicians regarding playing when in pain, by answering the question ‘should musicians play in pain?’. This multidisciplinary narrative review draws upon contemporary pain science, including factors associated with poorer prognoses for those in pain, as well as the reported experiences of musicians with pain (including those who have taken time off from playing). Our current understanding of pain science provides further support for the potential for consequences related to avoiding activities due to pain. Pain is modulated by a number of neuro-immunological processes and is influenced by a range of psychosocial factors. Taking time off from playing might therefore not have any benefit. Importantly, one of the leading causes of a transition from acute to chronic pain is fear-avoidance behaviour (e.g. not playing when in pain); hence, encouraging such behaviour cannot be supported. Musicians who have taken time off from playing due to pain have experienced a range of consequences, including emotional and financial consequences. These experiences indicate that there are potential negative consequences related to taking time off from playing which need to be weighed against any benefits. We conclude that musicians should not necessarily be advised to take time off from playing to manage their pain, in keeping with current best practice for pain management. Instead, we recommend that musicians be educated on contemporary pain science and when to seek treatment from a health professional for individualised advice to reduce the burden of musicians’ pain.

Keywords: Pain, musculoskeletal, neuroscience, musician, performing, advice

Background

Musculoskeletal symptoms (MSSs) are common among musicians at all levels,16 with a 12-month prevalence of 85–89% and point prevalence of 57–68% among professional musicians and university music students.4 University music students and professional musicians have a higher prevalence of MSSs compared with reference groups,710 while university music students with MSSs have reported higher ratings of impairment (i.e. impact on daily life) and emotional impact of MSSs compared with medical students,11 as well as higher ratings of functional impairment specific to neck/upper limb MSSs.12 Musculoskeletal disorders are the most common and costly type of workers’ compensation claim made by musicians;13 hence, strategies to reduce the burden of musicians’ MSSs, including pain, are required. A wide range of musicians’ MSSs have been investigated, and include pain, soreness, discomfort, ache, tension, clicking, popping, crepitus, tightness, stiffness, reduced range of motion, loss of motor control, cramp, muscle fatigue, loss of speed, loss of endurance, swelling, redness, burning, numbness and tingling.14 Our review focuses specifically on pain because pain is the most commonly reported15,16 and investigated14 type of MSS among musicians. Musicians with MSSs, including pain, have reported experiencing a range of consequences in both musical and non-musical aspects of their lives.9,1721 Providing musicians with appropriate advice regarding pain management is therefore paramount.

It has been suggested that musicians should not play when in pain,22,23 with evidence to suggest that some musicians adopt this recommendation.24 There appears, however, to be a lack of evidence supporting this advice, and there are potential negative consequences from doing so. In keeping with the ideals of beneficence and non-maleficence, integral to the work of health professionals,2528 the evidence regarding the potential negative consequences of this advice should be explored.

We therefore ask the question ‘should musicians play in pain?’ to provide evidence-based recommendations to musicians, clinicians and public health practitioners to improve pain outcomes for musicians, and ultimately reduce the burden of musicians’ pain.

Methods

A multidisciplinary, narrative review approach was adopted to answer our research question. First, we review the literature for pain mechanisms as well as risk factors for the transition from acute to chronic pain, and how this may relate to musicians. Second, we review the negative consequences for musicians who have taken time off from playing due to pain, and other MSSs (as the consequences of MSSs are likely the same as for pain). The latter is important as their experiences provide insight into some of the consequences faced by musicians who take time off from playing, and therefore should be considered when deciding whether musicians should or should not play in pain. Studies specific to musicians’ MSSs were primarily identified through a systematic mapping review of the recent peer-reviewed literature,29 while the broader pain science literature was also reviewed, including key comprehensive reviews.3033

Results

Pain

In this section, we briefly describe what pain is, the types of pain and pain mechanisms, so as to contextualise our argument. Readers who are interested in more comprehensive reviews on the topic are referred to Hainline et al.,30 Bushnell et al.,31 Guo et al.32 and Fregoso et al.33 for further information.

Pain has been defined by the International Association for the Study of Pain as ‘an unpleasant sensory and emotional experience associated with actual or potential tissue damage, or described in terms of such damage’.34 There are three main types of pain: nociceptive, neuropathic and nociplastic/ algopathic/ nocipathic pain,30 as summarised in Table 1. These pain types are not necessarily discrete and may occur in combination.30 Musicians’ pain may therefore be related to their musical activities through nociception (e.g. repetitive or ongoing load) or neuropathic processes (e.g. inflammatory irritation or repetitive mechanical load leading to peripheral nerve damage), non-musical exposures (nociceptive or neuropathic), or be the result of nociplastic, algopathic or nocipathic processes. The experience of pain is complex, and has inter- and intra-individual variability, that is, it is driven by the context, social, environmental, immunological and neurophysiological factors, psychological state, and cognitive and emotional aspects of pain.31,3941

Table 1.

Brief description of the characteristics of each pain type.

Pain type Characteristics
Nociceptive pain Involves the activation of nociceptors (the peripheral nerve terminals that detect noxious stimuli, i.e. mechanical, thermal and chemical stimuli).35
Nociceptors can be triggered following an injury (e.g. sprained ankle) where the tissue damage is sudden, or via ongoing repetitive load (e.g. tendinosis).
Nociception (the encoding and processing of noxious stimuli)35 does not necessarily lead to pain, because other processes are involved.30
Nociceptive pain is a type of protective mechanism, and nociceptors have been described as the ‘first detection’ system for body tissue.30
The association between the amount of nociceptor activation and the experience of pain is not necessarily linear,30 and pain does not necessarily mean the tissue is threatened.
Inflammatory pain is a type of nociceptive pain, where the physiological changes involved in inflammation trigger nociceptors.35
Neuropathic pain Relates to a lesion in the somatosensory nervous system.34,35,36
Lesions may occur as the result of trauma (including surgery) or disease (e.g. diabetes mellitus, or stroke) in the spinal cord, nerve roots or peripheral nerves.37
Damage to peripheral nerves may also occur through inflammatory irritation or repetitive mechanical load;30 hence some musical activities may lead to neuropathic pain.
Neuropathic pain does not require nociceptive activation.30
Nociplastic, algopathic & nocipathic pain Sometimes called ‘dysfunctional pain’38
Describes pain that relates to altered nociception, without evidence of a threat or damage to tissue, nor any lesion of the somatosensory nervous system.36
Nociplastic pain describes pain driven by altered nociceptive pathway function.30
Nocipathic pain, is a pathological state of nociception.30
Algoplastic pathological pain, is a pain that has not been generated by injury.30
Fibromyalgia, visceral pain disorders, and Complex Regional Pain Syndrome Type 1 are examples of conditions that appear to be driven by these pain types.36
Mechanisms may include central sensitisation,36 a neurophysiological construct whereby the nociceptors in the central nervous system become hypersensitive.35

Note: see Hainline et al.30 for a description of the clinical patterns.

It is now well established that pain is a conscious event, not a sensory signal,30 and that the relationship between tissue damage and the experience of pain is not linear.30 Pain can occur without tissue damage, and tissue damage without pain.4245 For instance, abnormal anatomical findings on imaging scans occur in asymptomatic individuals,4345 while symptomatic individuals may have no detected abnormalities.42 There is also evidence of a poor correlation between physical test findings and ‘soreness, pain or discomfort’ in musicians specifically.4648 As pain is not necessarily the result of tissue damage nor threatened tissue, the value of musicians taking time off from their musical activities must be questioned. Pain is therefore a complex experience, and the drivers of pain modulation (in both directions) need to be explored in order to develop effective management strategies.

Pain modulation may explain why there is not a simple relationship between tissue damage (and potential damage) and the experience of pain. Modulation occurs through various processes in the peripheral and central nervous systems,30 and relevant neurological changes include both the anatomy and function of the nervous system31 (see Bushnell et al.31 for a comprehensive review). Several of the brain regions involved in the processing of pain (or the stimuli resulting in pain, e.g. the somatosensory,49 prefrontal,49 and anterior cingulate cortices,49 the insula,49 amygdala,50,51 nucleus accumbens,50,51 periaqueductal grey,52 thalamus,49 and the cerebellum)49 are also involved in a range of other processes (including sensory processing,5356 executive functioning (e.g. attention),54,56,57 memory,54,56,58 emotion (including fear),5962 motivation,58,59 motor control,53,57,59 and descending pain modulation).63 The multiple functions of these brain regions may explain the role that contextual cues,64 non-nociceptive sensory input,64 and affective and cognitive factors31 play in the modulation of pain. Furthermore, associations between changes in some of these brain regions and emotional and cognitive representations of pain have been identified in those with chronic pain,65 providing further support for the important role that psychosocial factors may play in the pain experience, particularly the transition from acute to chronic pain.

There are several differences between the brains of musicians and non-musicians,6669 with some of these differences including having a larger somatosensory cortex68 and cerebellum,69 as well as greater insula connectivity70 – changes associated with pain processing.49 Musicians also have sensory processes which have been described as maladaptive, as musicians without chronic pain have sensory processing similar to those of non-musicians experiencing chronic pain.71 The neurological differences between musicians and non-musicians may contribute to the higher prevalence of MSSs among musicians compared with reference groups.710

Musicians may be more likely to experience pain of greater intensity than any tissue damage indicates, compared with the general population, owing both to neurological differences, and to musicians having a higher prevalence of sleep problems72 and psychological distress73 (both pain moderators). If musicians’ pain is driven by psychosocial factors, such as distress, then avoiding musical activities is arguably unlikely to lead to an improvement in pain outcomes.

Distraction also diminishes the intensity of pain and has been used for both acute (e.g. during medical procedures) and chronic pain.74,75 Indeed, playing a musical instrument may serve as a pain-reducing distraction. Musicians, for instance, have reported that they did not experience pain while playing, but experienced pain soon after stopping.76 Playing a musical instrument therefore may have analgesic effects.

Another process of particular relevance to musicians that may alter an individual’s response to mechanical loading is the up-regulation of nociception.30 This up-regulation may occur due to low-level inflammation whereby the threshold of mechanical nociception is reduced; hence, mechanical demands which would previously not have triggered nociceptors now do, which may lead to an increase in the level of pain experienced with a previously unpainful experience.30 Low-level inflammation may be due to load exceeding the tissue’s capacity, but also other factors such as ongoing stress and sleep deprivation.30 Another issue with ongoing, repetitive load that exceeds the tissue’s capacity is the establishment of a cycle of inflammation-repair-remodelling-inflammation.30 This cycle can alter the mechanical properties of tissues which may in turn alter nociceptive activation,30 and may therefore be of particular relevance to musicians.

Fear-avoidance behaviour is of particular importance to whether musicians should play in pain, given this behaviour relates directly to not playing when in pain. While nociceptive pain is a protective mechanism, the lack of direct relationship between tissue damage and pain indicates that it is not a valid indicator of damage or potential damage. Individuals may however inaccurately believe that certain activities or actions may result in injury, and fear the sensation of pain.77 These inaccurate beliefs may lead to the avoidance of the ‘dangerous’ activity, which may in turn contribute to deconditioning and disability.77 While the impact of fear-avoidance on musicians’ pain has not been examined,29 fear-avoidance behaviour is a driver of pain modulation, and is associated with the transition from acute to chronic pain in other populations.78 Furthermore, fear-avoidance behaviour is associated with a poorer prognosis79 and treatment outcomes in the general population, particularly when the fear-avoidance behaviour is not addressed as part of the treatment.80 Advice to musicians regarding not playing while experiencing pain may reinforce the individual’s inaccurate beliefs, and lead to fear-avoidance behaviour, and ultimately poorer pain outcomes, including chronic pain.

Consequences of having time off due to pain

Musicians typically do not want to take time off from playing, and fear being given such advice by health professionals.81 Musicians work in a competitive industry,82 characterised by unstable employment83 and constant criticism,84 with a stigma regarding pain.22 There are several consequences for musicians who disclose their pain experiences to others, particularly when they take time off from playing.

Musicians who have taken time off from playing have described feeling a loss of identity and self-worth, as well as feelings of ‘letting down the team’, isolation, and depression.22 These consequences are potentially more influential in musicians than other occupational groups for several reasons. For example, musicians begin their training in their childhoods85,86 and being a musician becomes a strong part of their identities18,22,81,87 – arguably more so than most other occupational groups. Playing music is not only a career but also a recreational activity for musicians that provides a form of communication and socialisation.19 Furthermore, musicians have been described as ‘subjective careerists’,84 for they are driven by internal factors or a ‘calling’ to participate in their career.88 There are therefore clear potential negative consequences involved in advising musicians to take time off from playing, which must be weighed against the benefits, especially when provided as generic public health advice.

Taking time off from playing may threaten musicians’ employability and financial security, given the competitive nature of the industry82 and the stigma related to pain.22 Furthermore, musicians typically have precarious employment. For example, in Australia 86% of professional musicians work in a self-employed or freelance capacity,83 so many are unlikely to be able to access sick leave and/or workers’ compensation. These entitlements are not available to those who are self-employed, and while they are available for freelance musicians who are employed by another entity, the fear of damaging their reputations and future employability may prevent musicians from accessing these entitlements. Even musicians working for a reliable employer have reported such concerns.20 Without accessing sick leave and/or workers’ compensation, musicians may struggle financially if not playing. They typically have low incomes, for instance in Australia musicians’ total weekly incomes are lower than for other artists and the general population (including those who are not working).89 Taking even a short break from work may therefore have significant financial consequences. Recommending that musicians take time off from playing when in pain may therefore have both financial and career consequences.

For musicians employed in ensemble work, there may be additional negative consequences from taking leave for pain and/or other MSSs, particularly given the point prevalence of MSSs among professional musicians and university music students is as high as 57–68%.4 With such a high prevalence of MSS (including pain), it would be impractical for all musicians with pain to take time off from playing, especially considering the complex musical relationships in ensemble playing (e.g. orchestras, that do not have understudies or replacements trained up to fill in for musicians on leave.29,90 Although impractical for these ensembles, this problem in itself is not sufficient to recommend that musicians keep playing when in pain, but should be included with other considerations when determining whether musicians should play in pain.

Discussion

The current evidence regarding pain mechanisms and predictors of chronicity, as well as musicians’ experiences with having time off from playing due to pain, indicate that musicians, in most cases, should continue to play in pain. Our findings are in conflict with some of the present advice given to musicians.22,23 This evidence-practice gap is not only present in the care of musicians, but also the general population.91

Both current pain science and evidence regarding musicians’ pain indicate that public health advice directed towards musicians should not include suggestions that they do not play when experiencing pain. This recommendation is in keeping with best practice for managing musculoskeletal pain, where the maintenance of normal physical activity should be encouraged.92 Musicians are more likely to experience pain of greater intensity than any tissue damage indicates, compared with the general population, owing to neurological differences, and musicians having a higher prevalence of sleep problems72 and psychological distress73 (both pain moderators). If this is the case, taking time off from playing would not be anticipated to alter the musicians’ pain. Indeed, because a range of negative consequences of having time off have been reported, their pain may increase with time off, thereby further exacerbating pain through the encouragement of fear-avoidance behaviour. Public health advice suggesting that musicians should not play while in pain is therefore potentially damaging.

We are not suggesting that musicians be expected to play while in pain, but rather that this advice should be provided on an individual level. In a clinical setting, health professionals can determine the extent (or presence) of any tissue damage and/or inflammation, the intensity and degree of chronicity of a musicians’ pain, as well as any other contributing factors (e.g. pain beliefs, distress, sleep problems). Based on an individual assessment, a health professional may deem it appropriate to suggest that a musician stop playing for a period of time, or reduce the amount that they are playing. This decision should involve the musician, and clinicians should be aware of the potential consequences of this advice. In addition to the concerns raised in this review, clinicians should be aware that some musicians may try to ‘make up’ lost practice time after being granted permission to commence playing again. A plan to pace back into playing should therefore also be discussed.

As previously mentioned, there is a range of non-biomechanical factors that may be contributing to a musicians’ pain, including pain beliefs, stress, distress, and sleep problems. If present, these factors should be addressed by a health professional, potentially within a multidisciplinary team; an approach that should not be unique to musicians. Musicians do have unique concerns, however, and addressing other contributing factors provides a more effective approach than focusing on these unique concerns.

Rather than suggesting that musicians stop playing when in pain, we recommend that musicians be educated about what pain is and how best to manage it (including when to see a health professional) as a public health strategy. To date, no study of musicians has investigated the effect of this kind of education on musicians’ pain.93 Pain education has, however, been investigated in workplace settings and reportedly improved work ability,94 reduced sick leave9496 and number of visits to healthcare professionals,94 resulted in lower ratings of the ‘bothersomeness’ of pain,94 and reduced feelings of sadness/depression.94 The evidence regarding pain education for those with chronic pain is mixed regarding pain and disability, with pooled estimates indicating no clinically significant benefit; however, significant reductions were reported in catastrophisation and kinesiophobia.97 These findings may indicate that pain education may be more useful as a public health strategy, particularly before the individual experiences chronic pain. Pain education is in line with current best practice for musculoskeletal pain management,92 and public health recommendations.98 We therefore suggest that public health pain education interventions for musicians be developed, evaluated, and effectively implemented, to reduce the burden of musicians’ pain.

As highlighted here, musicians experience a range of negative consequences when they have pain, particularly if they take time off. Many of these consequences appear to be driven by stigma and a fear of ‘retribution’. Some musicians will have to take time off, and others may fear that they will have to do so in the future. By changing the culture of silence around musicians’ pain, and by providing a supportive environment whereby musicians with pain are not simply seen as being unreliable and having poor technique, some of the factors that may increase a musicians’ risk of transitioning from acute to chronic pain may be reduced. Such factors are likely to include emotional distress,65 and fear-avoidance behaviour,78 and ameliorating these adverse influences would also likely improve quality of life generally.

The findings of our review may be generalisable to other groups. As outlined above, encouraging people to maintain normal physical activity is in line with current best practice for musculoskeletal pain management.92 The additional consequences musicians experience when taking time off from playing may also be experienced by other groups. For instance, occupational groups characterised by precarious employment, particularly in competitive industries, are likely to experience similar issues to musicians should they take time off due to their pain. Furthermore, musicians share many similarities with sportspeople (including dancers), particularly regarding the close ties between their identity and career.99 Nevertheless, there are several differences which may influence musicians’ experience and management of pain.100 For instance, even though dancers have reported similar negative emotional consequences to musicians regarding having time off due to pain and/or injury,99,101 a key difference is that dancers, like sportspeople,100 normalise pain; they expect and work through pain.99,101 Dancers are also arguably more likely to experience injuries with tissue damage than musicians, given the physical loads encountered, which may limit the applicability of our recommendations to some occupational groups, even when there are similarities in career and emotional consequences.

Conclusion

While some musicians may require time off from their playing due to pain, this should not be provided as generic public health advice. Contemporary multidisciplinary evidence indicates that such advice is not appropriate. Not only may taking time off from playing be unnecessary, it may encourage fear-avoidance behaviour, and result in a range of negative consequences for musicians. By not playing, the pain experience may be exacerbated and the risk of transitioning from acute to chronic pain increased. Educating musicians regarding contemporary pain science and when to seek treatment from a health professional for individualised advice is recommended to reduce the burden of musicians’ pain.

Footnotes

Conflict of interest: The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship and/or publication of this article.

Contributorship: JS and PW conceptualised and drafted the review.

Funding: The author(s) disclosed receipt of the following financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article: JS was the recipient of an Australian Government Research Training Program Scholarship, and a SafeWork SA WHS Supplementary Scholarship (funded by the South Australian Government). SafeWork SA and the South Australian Government do not endorse the content of this material, and the views expressed herein do not represent the views of SafeWork SA or the South Australian Government.

Guarantor: J.S. is the guarantor of this article.

ORCID iD: Jessica Stanhope Inline graphic https://orcid.org/0000-0002-6657-3317

References

  • 1. Kochem FB, Silva JG. Prevalence of playing-related musculoskeletal disorders in string players: a systematic review. J Manipulative Physiol Ther 2018; 41(6): 540–549. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Stanhope J, Milanese S. The prevalence and incidence of musculoskeletal symptoms experienced by flautists. Occup Med 2016; 66(2): 156–163. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Silva AG, Lã FM, Afreixo V. Pain prevalence in instrumental musicians: a systematic review. Med Probl Perform Art 2015; 30(1): 8–19. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Kok LM, Huisstede BM, Voorn VM, et al. The occurrence of musculoskeletal complaints among professional musicians: a systematic review. Int Arch Occup Environ Health 2016; 89(3): 373–396. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Steinmetz A, Scheffer I, Esmer E, et al. Frequency, severity and predictors of playing-related musculoskeletal pain in professional orchestral musicians in Germany. Clin Rheumatol 2015; 34(5): 965–973. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Amaral Corrêa L, Teixeira Dos Santos L, Nogueira Paranhos EN, Jr, et al. Prevalence and risk factors for musculoskeletal pain in keyboard musicians: a systematic review. PM R 2018; 10(9): 942–950. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Joseph C, Walters AUC, Lawrence WL, et al. An ergonomic evaluation of pannists. Int J Occup Saf Ergon 2020; 26: 129–139. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Kok LM, Vlieland TP, Fiocco M, et al. A comparative study on the prevalence of musculoskeletal complaints among musicians and non-musicians. BMC Musculoskelet Disord 2013; 14: 9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Paarup HM, Baelum J, Holm JW, et al. Prevalence and consequences of musculoskeletal symptoms in symphony orchestra musicians vary by gender: a cross-sectional study. BMC Musculoskelet Disord 2011; 12: 223. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10. Ginsborg J, Kreutz G, Thomas M, et al. Healthy behaviours in music and non-music performance students. Health Educ 2009; 109: 242–258. [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Kok LM, Vliet Vlieland TP, Fiocco M, et al. Musicians’ illness perceptions of musculoskeletal complaints. Clin Rheumatol 2013; 32(4): 487–492. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Kok LM, Nelissen RG, Huisstede BM. Prevalence and consequences of arm, neck, and/or shoulder complaints among music academy students: a comparative study. Med Probl Perform Art 2015; 30(3): 163–168. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Stanhope J, Weinstein P, Pisaniello D. What can musicians’ claims data reveal about their musculoskeletal conditions? Arch Environ Occup Health. Epub ahead of print 7 May 2019. DOI: 10.1080/19338244.2019.1605968. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14. Stanhope J, Pisaniello D, Tooher R, et al. How do we assess musicians’ musculoskeletal symptoms?: a review of outcomes and tools used. Ind Health 2019; 57(4): 454–494. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Porter M, Wilson IM, Doherty L, et al. Extent of playing-related musculoskeletal problems in the Irish traditional music community: a survey. Med Probl Perform Art 2018; 33(1): 47–55. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16. Topoğlu O, Karagülle D, Keskin TU, et al. General health status, music performance anxiety, and coping methods of musicians working in Turkish state symphony orchestras: a cross-sectional study. Med Probl Perform Art 2018; 33(2): 118–123. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17. Stanhope J, Milanese S, Grimmer K. University woodwind students’ experiences with playing-related injuries and their management: a pilot study. J Pain Res 2014; 7: 133–148. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Bragge P, Bialocerkowski A, McMeeken J. Understanding playing-related musculoskeletal disorders in elite pianists: a grounded theory study. Med Probl Perform Art 2006; 21: 71–79. [Google Scholar]
  • 19. Guptill C. The lived experience of professional musicians with playing-related injuries: a phenomenological inquiry. Med Probl Perform Art 2011; 26(2): 84–95. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20. Chimenti RL, Van Dillen LR, Prather H, et al. Underutilization of worker’s compensation insurance among professional orchestral musicians. Med Probl Perform Art 2013; 28(1): 54–60. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Abréu-Ramos AM, Micheo WF. Lifetime prevalence of upper-body musculoskeletal problems in a professional-level symphony orchestra: age, gender, and instrument-specific results. Med Probl Perform Art 2007; 22: 97–104. [Google Scholar]
  • 22. Rickert DLL, Barrett MS, Ackermann BJ. Injury and the orchestral environment: Part III. The role of psychosocial factors in the experience of musicians undertaking rehabilitation. Med Probl Perform Art 2014; 29(3): 125–135. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23. Ackermann B, Kenny DT, Driscoll T, et al. Sound practice health handbook for orchestral musicians. Sydney, NSW, Australia: The University of Sydney, 2015. [Google Scholar]
  • 24. Stanhope J. University woodwind students’ playing-related injuries: a pilot study investigating beliefs, attitudes and prevention strategies. Aust J Music Educ 2018; 52: 29–42. [Google Scholar]
  • 25. Exercise & Sports Science Australia. Code of professional conduct and ethical practice. Hamilton, QLD, Australia: Exercise & Sports Science Australia, 2017. [Google Scholar]
  • 26. Australian Physiotherapy Association. The Australian Physiotherapy Association code of conduct. North Ryde, NSW, Australia: Australian Physiotherapy Association, 2017. [Google Scholar]
  • 27. Physiotherapy Board of New Zealand. Physiotherapy Standards framework. Wellington, New Zealand: Physiotherapy Board of New Zealand, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  • 28. Delany C, Fryer C, van Kessel G. An ethical approach to health promotion in physiotherapy practice. Health Promot J Austr 2015; 26(3): 255–262. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29. Stanhope J, Tooher R, Pisaniello D, et al. Have musicians’ musculoskeletal symptoms been thoroughly addressed? A systematic mapping review. Int J Occup Med Environ Health 2019; 32(3): 291–331. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30. Hainline B, Turner JA, Caneiro JP, et al. Pain in elite athletes – neurophysiological, biomechanical and psychosocial considerations: a narrative review. Br J Sports Med 2017; 51(17): 1259–1264. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31. Bushnell MC, Čeko M, Low LA. Cognitive and emotional control of pain and its disruption in chronic pain. Nat Rev Neurosci 2013; 14(7): 502–511. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32. Guo R, Chen LH, Xing C, et al. Pain regulation by gut microbiota: molecular mechanisms and therapeutic potential. Br J Anaesth 2019; 123(5): 637–654. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33. Fregoso G, Wang A, Tseng K, et al. Transition from acute to chronic pain: evaluating risk for chronic postsurgical pain. Pain Physician 2019; 22(5): 479–488. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34. International Association for the Study of Pain (IASP). IASP taxonomy, https://www.iasp-pain.org/Education/Content.aspx?ItemNumber=2051&navItemNumber=576 (2017, accessed 22 May 2019).
  • 35. Loeser JD, Treede RD. The Kyoto protocol of IASP basic pain terminology. Pain 2008; 137(3): 473–477. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36. Kosek E, Cohen M, Baron R, et al. Do we need a third mechanistic descriptor for chronic pain states? Pain 2016; 157: 1382–1386. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37. Vardeh D, Mannion RJ, Woolf CJ. Toward a mechanism-based approach to pain diagnosis. J Pain 2016; 17(9 Suppl.): T50–T69. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38. Nagakura Y. Challenges in drug discovery for overcoming ‘dysfunctional pain’: an emerging category of chronic pain. Expert Opin Drug Discov 2015; 10(10): 1043–1045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39. Gatchel RJ, Peng YB, Peters ML, et al. The biopsychosocial approach to chronic pain: scientific advances and future directions. Psychol Bull 2007; 133(4): 581–624. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40. Turk DC, Okifuji A. Psychological factors in chronic pain: evolution and revolution. J Consult Clin Psychol 2002; 70(3): 678–690. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41. Villemure C, Bushnell MC. Cognitive modulation of pain: how do attention and emotion influence pain processing? Pain 2002; 95(3): 195–199. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42. Jarvik JG, Hollingworth W, Heagerty PJ, et al. Three-year incidence of low back pain in an initially asymptomatic cohort: clinical and imaging risk factors. Spine 2005; 30: 1541–1548. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43. Comin J, Cook JL, Malliaras P, et al. The prevalence and clinical significance of sonographic tendon abnormalities in asymptomatic ballet dancers: a 24-month longitudinal study. Br J Sports Med 2013; 47(2): 89–92. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44. Guermazi A, Niu J, Hayashi D, et al. Prevalence of abnormalities in knees detected by MRI in adults without knee osteoarthritis: population based observational study (Framingham Osteoarthritis Study). BMJ 2012; 345: e5339. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45. Brinjikji W, Luetmer PH, Comstock B, et al. Systematic literature review of imaging features of spinal degeneration in asymptomatic populations. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol 2015; 36(4): 811–816. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46. Paarup HM, Baelum J, Manniche C, et al. Occurrence and co-existence of localized musculoskeletal symptoms and findings in work-attending orchestra musicians – an exploratory cross-sectional study. BMC Res Notes 2012; 5: 541. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47. Cheng Y, Chen C, Lin CP, et al. Love hurts: an fMRI study. NeuroImage 2010; 51(2): 923–929. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48. Lamm C, Decety J, Singer T. Meta-analytic evidence for common and distinct neural networks associated with directly experienced pain and empathy for pain. NeuroImage 2011; 54(3): 2492–2502. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49. Apkarian AV, Bushnell MC, Treede RD, et al. Human brain mechanisms of pain perception and regulation in health and disease. Eur J Pain 2012; 9: 463–484. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50. Baliki MN, Geha PY, Fields HL, et al. Predicting value of pain and analgesia: nucleus accumbens response to noxious stimuli changes in the presence of chronic pain. Neuron 2010; 66(1): 149–160. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51. Becerra L, Breiter HC, Wise R, et al. Reward circuitry activation by noxious thermal stimuli. Neuron 2001; 32(5): 927–946. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52. Dunckley P, Wise RG, Fairhurst M, et al. A comparison of visceral and somatic pain processing in the human brainstem using functional magnetic resonance imaging. J Neurosci 2005; 25(32): 7333–7341. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53. Borich MR, Brodie SM, Gray WA, et al. Understanding the role of the primary somatosensory cortex: opportunities for rehabilitation. Neuropsychologia 2015; 79(Pt. B): 246–255. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54. Fama R, Sullivan EV. Thalamic structure and associated cognitive functions: relations with age and aging. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2015; 54: 29–37. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55. Uddin LQ. Salience processing and insular cortical function and dysfunction. Nat Rev Neurosci 2015; 16(1): 55–61. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56. Menon V, Uddin LQ. Saliency, switching, attention and control: a network model of insula function. Brain Struct Funct 2010; 214(5–6): 655–667. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57. Klein AP, Ulmer JL, Quinet SA, et al. Nonmotor functions of the cerebellum: an introduction. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol 2016; 37(6): 1005–1009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58. Sullivan EV. Cognitive functions of the cerebellum. Neuropsychol Rev 2010; 20: 227–228. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59. Salgado S, Kaplitt MG. The nucleus accumbens: a comprehensive review. Stereotact Funct Neurosurg 2015; 93(2): 75–93. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60. Stevens FL, Hurley RA, Taber KH. Anterior cingulate cortex: unique role in cognition and emotion. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 2011; 23(2): 121–125. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61. Ressler KJ. Amygdala activity, fear, and anxiety: modulation by stress. Biol Psychiatry 2010; 67(12): 1117–1119. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62. Arnsten AFT. Stress signalling pathways that impair prefrontal cortex structure and function. Nat Rev Neurosci 2009; 10(6): 410–422. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63. Loyd DR, Murphy AZ. The role of the periaqueductal gray in the modulation of pain in males and females: are the anatomy and physiology really that different? Neural Plast 2009; 2009: 462879. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64. Moseley GL, Arntz A. The context of a noxious stimulus affects the pain it evokes. Pain 2007; 133(1–3): 64–71. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65. Malfliet A, Coppieters I, Van Wilgen P, et al. Brain changes associated with cognitive and emotional factors in chronic pain: a systematic review. Eur J Pain 2017; 21(5): 769–786. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66. Dawson WJ. How and why musicians are different from nonmusicians: a bibliographic review. Med Probl Perform Art 2011; 26(2): 65–78. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67. Barrett KC, Ashley R, Strait DL, et al. Art and science: how musical training shapes the brain. Front Psychol 2013; 4: 713. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68. Gaser C, Schlaug G. Brain structures differ between musicians and non-musicians. J Neurosci 2003; 23(27): 9240–9245. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69. Hutchinson S, Lee LHL, Gaab N, et al. Cerebellar volume of musicians. Cereb Cortex 2003; 13: 943–949. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70. Zamorano AM, Cifre I, Montoya P, et al. Insula-based networks in professional musicians: Evidence for increased functional connectivity during resting state fMRI. Hum Brain Mapp 2017; 38(10): 4834–4849. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71. Zamorano AM, Riquelme I, Kleber B, et al. Pain sensitivity and tactile spatial acuity are altered in healthy musicians as in chronic pain patients. Front Hum Neurosci 2015; 8: 1016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72. Vaag J, Saksvik-Lehouillier I, Bjørngaard JH, et al. Sleep difficulties and insomnia symptoms in Norwegian musicians compared to the general population and workforce. Behav Sleep Med 2016; 14(3): 325–342. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73. Vaag J, Håkon Bjørngaard J, Bjerkeset O. Symptoms of anxiety and depression among Norwegian musicians compared to the general workforce. Psychol Music 2016; 44: 234–248. [Google Scholar]
  • 74. Bukola IM, Paula D. The effectiveness of distraction as procedural pain management technique in pediatric oncology patients: a meta-analysis and systematic review. J Pain Symptom Manage 2017; 54(4): 589–600. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75. Mallari B, Spaeth EK, Goh H, et al. Virtual reality as an analgesic for acute and chronic pain in adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Pain Res 2019; 12: 2053–2085. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76. Guptill C. Injured professional musicians and the complex relationship between occupation and health. J Occup Sci 2012; 19: 258–270. [Google Scholar]
  • 77. Hodges PW, Smeets RJ. Interaction between pain, movement, and physical activity: short-term benefits, long-term consequences, and targets for treatment. Clin J Pain 2015; 31(2): 97–107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78. Hruschak V, Cochran G. Psychosocial predictors in the transition from acute to chronic pain: a systematic review. Psychol Health Med 2018; 23(10): 1151–1167. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79. Wertli MM, Rasmussen-Barr E, Weiser S, et al. The role of fear avoidance beliefs as a prognostic factor for outcome in patients with nonspecific low back pain: a systematic review. Spine J 2014; 14(5): 816–836. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80. Wertli MM, Rasmussen-Barr E, Held U, et al. Fear-avoidance beliefs – a moderator of treatment efficacy in patients with low back pain: a systematic review. Spine J 2014; 14(11): 2658–2678. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81. Zaza C, Charles C, Muszynski A. The meaning of playing-related musculoskeletal disorders to classical musicians. Soc Sci Med 1998; 47(12): 2013–2023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82. Dick RW, Berning JR, Dawson W, et al. Athletes and the arts – the role of sports medicine in the performing arts. Curr Sports Med Rep 2013; 12(6): 397–403. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83. Throsby D, Petetskaya K. Making art work: an economic study of professional artists in Australia. Sydney, NSW, Australia: Australia Council for the Arts, 2017. [Google Scholar]
  • 84. van den Eydne J, Fisher A, Sonn C. Working in the Australian entertainment industry: final report. Melbourne, VIC, Australia: Victoria University, 2016. [Google Scholar]
  • 85. Creech A, Papageorgi I, Duffy C, et al. Investigating musical performance: commonality and diversity among classical and non-classical musicians. Music Educ Res 2008; 10: 215–234. [Google Scholar]
  • 86. de Bezenac C, Swindells R. No pain, no gain? Motivation and self-regulation in music learning. Int J Educ Arts 2009; 10: 1–34. [Google Scholar]
  • 87. McCready S, Reid D. The experience of occupational disruption among student musicians. Med Probl Perform Art 2007; 22: 140–146. [Google Scholar]
  • 88. Hall DT, Chandler DE. Psychological success: when the career is a calling. J Organ Behav 2005; 26: 155–176. [Google Scholar]
  • 89. Australian Bureau of Statistics. Employment in culture, Australia, 2011. Report no. 6273.0. Canberra, ACT, Australia: Australian Bureau of Statistics, 2011. [Google Scholar]
  • 90. Stanhope J, Weinstein P. Why do we need to investigate non-classical musicians to reduce the burden of musicians’ musculoskeletal symptoms? Ind Health. Epub ahead of print 5 October 2019. DOI: 10.2486/indhealth.2019-0094. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91. Foster NE, Anema JR, Cherkin D, et al. Prevention and treatment of low back pain: evidence, challenges, and promising directions. Lancet 2018; 391(10137): 2368–2383. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92. Lin I, Wiles L, Waller R, et al. What does best practice care for musculoskeletal pain look like? Eleven consistent recommendations from high-quality clinical practice guidelines: systematic review. Br J Sports Med 2020; 54: 79–86. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93. Stanhope J. The preventable burden of musicians’ musculoskeletal conditions: a case study of Australian university music students and professional musicians. PhD Thesis, The University of Adelaide, 2019. [Google Scholar]
  • 94. Frederiksen P, Indahl A, Andersen LL, et al. Can group-based reassuring information alter low back pain behavior? A cluster-randomized controlled trial. PLoS ONE 2017; 12(3): e0172003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95. Ree E, Lie SA, Eriksen HR, et al. Reduction in sick leave by a workplace educational low back pain intervention: a cluster randomized controlled trial. Scand J Public Health 2016; 44(6): 571–579. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 96. Odeen M, Ihlebaek C, Indahl A, et al. Effect of peer-based low back pain information and reassurance at the workplace on sick leave: a cluster randomized trial. J Occup Rehabil 2013; 23(2): 209–219. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 97. Watson JA, Ryan CG, Cooper L, et al. Pain neuroscience education for adults with chronic musculoskeletal pain: a mixed-methods systematic review and meta-analysis. J Pain 2019; 20(10): 1140.e1–1140.e22. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 98. Buchbinder R, van Tulder M, Öberg B, et al. Low back pain: a call for action. Lancet 2018; 391(10137): 2384–2388. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 99. McEwen K, Young K. Ballet and pain: reflections on a risk-dance culture. Qual Res Sport Exerc Health 2011; 3: 152–173. [Google Scholar]
  • 100. Stanhope J. Physical performance and musculoskeletal disorders: are musicians and sportspeople on a level playing field? Perform Enhanc Health 2016; 4: 18–26. [Google Scholar]
  • 101. Markula P. (Im)Mobile bodies: contemporary semi-professional dancers’ experiences with injuries. Int Rev Sociol Sport 2015; 50: 840–864. [Google Scholar]

Articles from British Journal of Pain are provided here courtesy of SAGE Publications

RESOURCES