Abstract
Patients with Parkinson’s disease (PD) can have significant cognitive dysfunction; however, the mechanisms for these cognitive symptoms are unknown. Here, we used scalp electroencephalography (EEG) to investigate the cortical basis for PD-related cognitive impairments during interval timing, which requires participants to estimate temporal intervals of several seconds. Time estimation is an ideal task demand for investigating cognition in PD because it is simple, requires medial frontal cortical areas, and recruits basic executive processes such as working memory and attention. However, interval timing has never been systematically studied in PD patients with cognitive impairments. We report three main findings. First, 71 PD patients had increased temporal variability compared to 37 demographically matched controls, and this variability correlated with cognitive dysfunction as measured by the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MOCA). Second, PD patients had attenuated ~4 Hz EEG oscillatory activity at midfrontal electrodes in response to the interval-onset cue, which was also predictive of MOCA. Finally, trial-by-trial linear mixed-effects modeling demonstrated that cue-triggered ~4 Hz power predicted subsequent temporal estimates as a function of PD and MOCA. Our data suggest that impaired cue-evoked midfrontal ~4 Hz activity predicts increased timing variability that is indicative of cognitive dysfunction in PD. These findings link PD-related cognitive dysfunction with cortical mechanisms of cognitive control, which could advance novel biomarkers and neuromodulation for PD.
Subject terms: Neuroscience, Neurophysiology
Introduction
Parkinson’s disease (PD) is a devastating neurodegenerative disease that involves motor as well as cognitive symptoms1–4. For motor symptoms of PD, detailed neurophysiological studies led to highly targeted and effective therapies such as deep-brain stimulation (DBS)5–8. Compared to motor symptoms, neurobehavioral symptoms are less treatable and have a stronger impact on the quality of life9,10. There are no therapies that improve cognitive symptoms of PD, in part because the neural mechanisms are unknown.
Cognitive deficits in PD are diverse, affecting a range of executive processes such as working memory, attention, reasoning, visuospatial dysfunction, inhibitory control, and flexibility11,12. One process that is ideal for studying PD-related cognitive dysfunction is interval timing, which requires participants to estimate temporal intervals of several seconds13. Interval timing involves cortical areas that are dysfunctional in PD, such as the medial frontal cortex14,15. Moreover, interval timing depends on executive functions such as working memory for temporal rules and attention to the passage of time16. PD patients have impairments in interval timing17,18 and temporal processing in general19,20. Since this prior work has focused on basal ganglia mechanisms in cognitively high-functioning PD patients, it remains unknown how interval timing is affected by a broader range of cognitive impairments.
Findings from scalp electroencephalography (EEG) have provided convergent evidence of impaired midfrontal ~4 Hz rhythms in PD patients, particularly during interval timing21,22. These cortical rhythms originate from medial frontal brain structures and are highly similar to midfrontal ~4–8 Hz rhythms that have been advanced as a mechanism of cognitive control23–25. Animal studies have shown that ~4 Hz oscillations are coherent with single neurons involved in the intricacies of cognitive operations21,22,25,26. Midfrontal ~4 Hz rhythms are consistently impaired in PD patients, particularly around imperative cues signifying surprise, conflict, or the start of time estimation during interval-timing tasks27,28. Together, this line of evidence suggests that PD patients have dysfunctional cognitive-control processes that can be indexed by ~4 Hz midfrontal EEG signals. This hypothesis specifically predicts that interval-timing performance should be related to midfrontal ~4 Hz rhythms, and to cognitive dysfunction in PD. We directly tested this hypothesis by collecting EEG data from a large number of PD patients who demonstrated a range of cognitive abilities during the performance of an interval-timing task. We used a time-production version of interval timing that we could relate to previous studies in PD and prior animal work13,17,18,21,22,29. We found that the average temporal estimates of PD patients and demographically matched controls were similar, yet variance was increased in PD. Critically, we discovered that this variability correlated with cognitive function in PD patients. We also found that PD patients had attenuated midfrontal cue-triggered ~4 Hz power relative to controls, and this neural measure also correlated with cognitive dysfunction in PD. Trial-by-trial analyses demonstrated that cue-related ~4 Hz power predicted timing variability. These findings support a model that cognitive failures in PD are related to attenuated cortical cognitive-control mechanisms, and that the integrity of this process can be monitored by EEG assessment of ~4 Hz rhythms.
Results
Interval-timing variability correlates with cognitive dysfunction in PD
We tested the hypothesis that interval-timing performance is related to cognitive dysfunction in PD. We recruited PD patients and demographically matched controls to perform a two-interval-timing task with 3- and 7-s intervals. Participants started estimating when an imperative “Go” cue appeared in the center of their computer screen (Fig. 1a). Participants were instructed to press the keyboard spacebar just before they estimated the interval to have elapsed; we focused on this keypress as the start of their temporal estimate (Fig. 1b, c). Consistent with past work, average keypress time was similar for PD patients and demographically matched controls for 3-s and 7-s intervals (mean + /-SEM keypress: control 3-s: 3.2 + /−0.1 s, control 7-s: 6.1 + /−0.2 s; PD 3-s: 3.2 + /−0.1 s, PD 7-s: 6.4 + /−0.2 s; Fig. 1d, e)17,18,30. PD patients had more variance in keypress time as measured by the coefficient of variation (CV) for both the 3-s (control: 0.19 + /−0.01; PD: 0.23 + /−0.01 s; t(81) = −2.30, P = 0.02; d = −0.52; Fig. 1f) and 7-s (control: 0.15 + /−0.01 s; PD: 0.22 + /−0.01 s; t(106) = −3.24, P = 0.002; d = −0.66; Fig. 1g) intervals. Notably, 3-s and 7-s CVs were similar (rho = 0.79; P < 0.0000001). Linear mixed-effects modeling revealed that there were main effects of interval-type (F(1,110) = 5.4, P = 0.02) and group (F(1,157) = 28.2, P = 0.0000004), but no significant interactions. Because keypress CV is a metric of temporal variability, these data provide evidence that PD affects the variability of temporal estimates31,32.
Critically, we wanted to understand whether temporal variability was related to cognitive function as measured by MOCA, a widely used clinical assessment of cognitive function that does not involve temporal judgment. In line with our hypothesis, we found that for 7-s intervals, cognitive dysfunction predicted keypress CV for PD patients (Spearman’s rho = −0.39, P = 0.001; Fig. 1i). For 3-s intervals, there was no reliable correlation between MOCA and CV for PD patients (rho = −0.22; P = 0.12; Fig. 1h) although Fisher’s z comparisons with 7-s intervals were not statistically significant. However, 7-s CV correlations with MOCA were stronger in PD patients than controls, in whom there was no relationship of CV with MOCA (CV and MOCA; 3-s: rho = 0.12, P = 0.51; 7-s: rho = 0.18, P = 0.27; Supplementary Fig. 1. Fisher’s z comparing CV and MOCA correlation between PD and controls, P < 0.004). For 3-s intervals in PD patients, there was no significant relationship of keypress CV with mUPDRS (rho = −0.08, P = 0.59; Supplementary Fig. 2a). In PD patients, keypress CV for 7-s intervals was also correlated with mUPDRS (rho = 0.28; P = 0.02; Supplementary Fig. 2b), though MOCA and mUPDRS were not correlated (rho = −0.09, P = 0.47; Supplementary Fig. 2c). Hierarchical multiple linear regression analysis supported the relationship between CV and mUPDRS (F(1,69) = 4.3; P = 0.04). However, the model became much stronger when MOCA was added after mUPDRS in the regression analysis (F(2,68) = 7.6; P = 0.001), and regression coefficients differed significantly between the two models (F(2,68) = 10.3; P = 0.002). These results suggest that there was a significant effect of MOCA above and beyond the mUPDRS in PD patients during the 7-s interval-timing task. Taken together, these data support our hypothesis that temporal variability as measured by keypress CV was linked to cognitive dysfunction in PD.
Midfrontal ~4 Hz activity correlates with cognitive dysfunction in PD
To explore the neural basis of these deficits, we analyzed EEG data. We focused on two key events: the imperative “Go” cue and the keypress. In addition, we focused on the midfrontal electrode Cz in line with our prior work21,22,27–29,33. Power-spectral density (PSD) analyses of resting-state data is shown in Supplementary Fig. 3. A comparison of ERPs or CNV did not reveal consistent differences between control and PD patients (Supplementary Figs 4–6). In our time-frequency analysis, we found that PD patients had attenuated midfrontal delta rhythms triggered by the imperative cue (300–400 ms after cue, 1–4 Hz control: 0.7 + /−0.2 dB, PD: 0.0 + /−0.1 dB; t(106) = 2.31, P = 0.02; d = 0.47 for 7-s trials, Fig. 2a–d; 3-s trials in Supplementary Fig. 7; midfrontal cluster of electrodes FCz/Cz/CPz in Supplementary Fig. 8). We also found that PD patients had attenuated theta rhythms (300–400 ms after cue; 4–7 Hz control: 0.3 + /−0.2 dB, PD: −0.3 + /−0.1 dB; t(106) = 2.24, P = 0.03; d = 0.45; Fig. 2a–c, f). Phase-locking analyses revealed that PD patients had marked differences in phase-locking around ~4 Hz on 7-s trials (Supplementary Fig. 9). Attenuated cue-triggered delta activity replicates prior work from our group, showing that PD patients had disrupted ~4 Hz activity around events that engage cognitive control such as novelty, conflict, and imperative timing cues21,22,27–29. As in our prior work, distinctions between ~4 Hz activity were more reliable for the longer 7-s intervals than for 3-s intervals (Supplementary Fig. 7) or for keypress (Supplementary Fig. 10)21,22.
Strikingly, we found that midfrontal cue-triggered delta rhythms in the 7-s task correlated with cognitive dysfunction in PD (rho = 0.31, P = 0.01; Fig. 2e; Supplementary Fig. 11). This was not observed for theta rhythms (rho = 0.15, P = 0.21; Fig. 2g), although Fisher’s z test did not reveal reliable differences between delta and theta correlations with MOCA. Delta power was correlated with CV (rho = −0.4, P = 0.006), and mediation analysis confirmed that CV significantly mediated the relationship between delta power and MOCA (average causal mediation effects: P < 0.05), but mUPDRS did not (P > 0.05). This finding was directly supportive of our hypothesis that deficits in midfrontal cue-triggered ~4 Hz rhythms underlie cognitive deficits in PD.
These data suggest that PD patients have attenuated cue-triggered delta rhythms and that individuals with smaller delta activity have a larger CV and lower MOCA scores. To test the hypothesis that cue-triggered delta-dysfunction mechanistically affects time estimation ~7 s later, we turned to linear mixed-effects models at a trial-by-trial level. We used a model where the outcome variable was keypress as an index of time estimation, and predictor variables were EEG delta power (1–4 Hz, 300–400 ms after cue), interval (short vs. long), disease status (control vs. PD), and cognitive status (as measured by MOCA). As previously shown, this analysis revealed the main effect of short vs. long intervals (Table 1) and a significant interaction between delta power and disease status (F(1,6421) = 4.48, P = 0.03; Table 1). In support of our trial-to-trial hypothesis, there was an interaction between delta power, disease status, and MOCA (F(1,6421) = 4.71, P = 0.03; Table 1). There were no higher interactions with interval.
Table 1.
Contrast | DF | F | P |
---|---|---|---|
Delta | 6421 | 1.06 | 0.30 |
CTL/PD | 102 | 0.12 | 0.73 |
MOCA | 102 | 0.79 | 0.38 |
Interval | 6446 | 134.29 | <0.0000001** |
Delta:CTL/PD | 6421 | 4.48 | 0.03* |
Delta:MOCA | 6421 | 0.88 | 0.35 |
CTL/PD:MOCA | 102 | 0.08 | 0.77 |
Delta:interval | 6416 | 1.42 | 0.23 |
CTL/PD:interval | 6446 | 1.20 | 0.27 |
MOCA:interval | 6447 | 19.11 | 0.00001** |
Delta:CTL/PD:MOCA | 6421 | 4.71 | 0.03* |
Delta:CTL/PD:interval | 6416 | 0.06 | 0.81 |
Delta:MOCA:interval | 6416 | 1.40 | 0.24 |
CTL/PD:MOCA:interval | 6447 | 1.08 | 0.30 |
Delta:CTL/PD:MOCA:interval | 6416 | 0.14 | 0.71 |
DF degrees of freedom, CTL control, PD Parkinson’s disease, MOCA Montreal Cognitive Assessment.
Significant effects in bold via an F test in R. **P < 0.01, *P < 0.05.
Visualization of these relationships revealed that for controls, delta power was inversely related to keypress variance at a trial-specific level, such that lower delta power was associated with more variable keypress around the 7-s interval (Fig. 3a). However, for PD patients who had lower overall delta power, there was no clear relationship between delta power and keypress (Fig. 3b; see Supplementary Fig. 12 for 3-s intervals). These data linking cue-triggered midfrontal delta power and keypress support a model whereby in control participants, normative cue-triggered delta activity relates to better precision of time estimates, yet this relationship is absent in the PD group.
Effects of levodopa on midfrontal ~4 Hz activity
Finally, we examined the effects of levodopa on ~4 Hz rhythms during interval timing. We found that levodopa did not reliably alter keypress time (Supplementary Fig. 13a). However, it did tighten CVs for 3-s intervals (ON: 0.20 + /−0.03, OFF: 0.16 + /−0.02; paired t(8) = 2.75, P = 0.03; Supplementary Fig. 13b) though not for 7-s intervals (ON: 0.18 + /−0.03, OFF: 0.15 + /−0.02; paired t(8) = 1.83, P = 0.11; Supplementary Fig. 13b). Critically, there were no reliable effects of levodopa on cue-triggered midfrontal delta/theta rhythms (Supplementary Fig. 13c–e). This finding is in line with our past work and consistent with the lack of levodopa’s effects on cortical circuits28,34.
Discussion
This study revealed that deficits in midfrontal cognitive-control processes contribute to increased variability in interval timing and to cognitive dysfunction in PD. First, we found that PD patients had increased timing variability that correlated with cognitive function. Second, we replicated our earlier work demonstrating that PD patients had deficits in cue-triggered midfrontal delta activity21,22,27,28 and discovered that these deficits also correlated with cognitive dysfunction in PD. Finally, we found that delta activity was linked with cognitive function during the interval-timing performance in PD at a trial-by-trial level. Our data illuminate the nature of timing deficits in PD and PD-related cognitive dysfunction. Indeed, these results suggest that increased timing variability is a manifest expression of dysfunctional midfrontal ~4 Hz rhythms which contribute to cognitive dysfunction in PD. Together, these interindividual and intraindividual findings suggest a candidate mechanism and biomarker of cognitive dysfunction PD.
Complex cellular factors such as alpha-synuclein and deficits in ascending neurotransmitter systems contribute to PD11,35. Despite this complexity, these cellular processes lead to neurophysiological phenomena observable by scalp EEG and intracortical recordings, which have been enormously helpful in both designing medical therapies and targeting brain stimulation sites to address the motor symptoms of PD33,36–38. Our results are significant in advancing our neurophysiological understanding of cognitive function in PD because we link PD-related cognitive dysfunction (as measured by MOCA) to midfrontal ~4 Hz rhythms, which are a putative cognitive-control mechanism23. These findings imply that cognitive dysfunction in PD patients can result in part from their inability to recruit cognitive-control processes. Our data support a model in which deficits in ~4 Hz cognitive-control signals contribute to cognitive dysfunction in PD patients.
Our work also provides insight into the nature of timing deficits in PD patients. Seminal work in cognitively normal PD patients described deficits in working-memory processes during interval timing as a function of dopamine signaling17,18. Past work has linked PD-related timing deficits to basal ganglia dysfunction13,19; by contrast, our work strongly implicates cortical cognitive-control mechanisms. In our study, some cognitively impaired PD patients did not reliably release the spacebar. Thus, our primary behavioral measure was keypress or the start of when participants had estimated intervals to have elapsed. Hence our results are not directly comparable to the peak-time metrics in past studies. Of note, prior work on interval timing in PD has focused on cognitively high-functioning patients19,20. We included the full-range of cognitive function in PD, which is likely more reflective of real-world PD but may limit behavioral assessments1,3,4. We found that PD patients have a high rate of variability in keypress that was proportional to their cognitive impairment. Correlations between timing variability and cognition and differences in delta activity for PD patients were more reliable at the 7-s interval compared to the 3-s interval, although we note that fewer patients performed the 3-s interval22,29. This may be a result of distinct motor mechanisms affecting keypress at 3-s or increased working-memory/cognitive load for 7-s intervals. Furthermore, our interval-timing task included a strong motor component in keypress, and some studies of interval timing that use non-motor tasks have failed to find differences39. These data, combined with our differences at 3 vs. 7 s, may support a view where sustained attention to the passage of time is altered in PD. Sustained attention is necessary for time estimation and may reflect facets of cognitive-control processes that malfunction in PD patients with cognitive dysfunction. Low-frequency rhythms are common to the effortful exertion underlying cognitive-control processes related to error-processing, conflict resolution, novelty detection, as well as working memory23,40. Impaired cognitive control in PD patients may be linked to decreased 4 Hz rhythms and greater interval-timing variability23. Additionally, our task included distractors that involve additional executive function, and it is possible that this preferentially affected PD patients with cognitive dysfunction. We have found that cortical neurons involved in temporal processing can be coherent at ~4 Hz21,25,41,42. Indeed, our data show that a cue-triggered burst of ~4 Hz activity engages neurons that are directly involved in temporal processing22,25,41,42. Such low-frequency oscillations can engage not only cortical neurons but also neurons in the subthalamic nucleus or other brain areas29,43,44. Our data suggest that in cognitively limited PD patients, delta power is decreased and results in inefficient engagement of temporal processing by frontostriatal networks45–47. If neuronal ensembles encoding time are not precisely engaged, they might lead to decreased precision in time estimates several seconds later47. Our findings that delta power predicts keypress variability supports this model.
These findings are in line with recent evidence that ~4 Hz stimulation improves cognitive function in PD29,48. Animal studies support this idea; indeed, highly specific stimulation at low frequencies can improve interval timing in animal PD models21,41,49 and other cognitive disorders such as schizophrenia49. Strikingly, human studies show that ~4 Hz subthalamic nucleus DBS can improve interval timing and conflict tasks, yet it remains unknown what effect cortical stimulation might have. Low-frequency subthalamic nucleus DBS has the potential to mitigate cognitive-control deficits in PD, but precise and extensive circuit-mapping will be required to determine whether low-frequency brain stimulation can improve real-world function in PD patients. This effort will also require systematic clinical studies using EEG-based biomarkers to assess the usefulness of ~4 Hz rhythms in diagnosing cognitive dysfunction, which is often under-recognized in the clinical setting50. Of note, we focus on ~4 Hz based on extensive human work in PD and cognitive control as well as our animal work21–23,25,28,33. While ~4 Hz deficits may not be highly predictive of PD, our work suggests that interval-timing variability and ~4 Hz deficits are predictive of cognition in PD.
Midfrontal electrodes are reflective of medial frontal generators29, yet many studies of timing thus far have focused on basal ganglia processes19. Levodopa strongly affects nigrostriatal dopamine, resulting in shifts in time estimation17,18. However, levodopa does not reliably predict cortical dysfunction or cognitive deficits in PD35,51,52. PD can affect dopamine signaling in the cortex, and our work clearly links increased timing variability to cortical ~4 Hz rhythms. Although levodopa can affect frontal circuits in complex ways52,53, our data here are consistent with prior findings that delta/theta rhythms are not sensitive to levodopa28,54,55. Of note, our sample size was small, and practice effects may be relevant as patients performed “OFF” sessions after “ON”, and a prior study showed that levodopa alone can affect interval timing28. However, we are not aware of any work demonstrating that levodopa can affect cortical 4 Hz rhythms20,56. Identifying the defects in cognitive-control mechanisms that occur in the midfrontal cortex and are defective in PD may be helpful in designing therapies for PD which target these circuits.
Our work has several limitations. First, scalp EEG has poor spatial resolution, and more advanced MEG, EEG/fMRI, or intraoperative recording studies might make it possible to draw more detailed inferences about the sources of altered temporal processing in PD. Second, it is possible that neuropsychological assays more advanced than MOCA will further clarify advanced cognitive or interval-timing deficits in PD. One promising feature of interval timing is that it is highly translatable to animal models, enabling detailed insights into cortical mechanisms of temporal control of the action. Third, our task involves a strong motor component. However, mediation analyses suggested that motor function in PD as measured by the mUPDRS was not related to timing variability or cognition (Supplementary Fig. 2) and neither was response-related activity (Supplementary Fig. 10). Fourth, we presented distractors, but we lack vocal recordings to analyze these events. Fifth, we did not observe strong beta-rhythm distinctions in PD patients (Supplementary Figs 3 and 10); however, our PD population is considerably more diverse than prior studies, and beta rhythms can be sensitive to movement, which may have undermined our ability to detect these differences. Finally, although our work here is correlative, it will guide future studies aimed at establishing the cellular and network mechanisms contributing to cognitive dysfunction in PD.
In summary, this study provides novel evidence that PD patients have increased variability in interval-timing performance. Both increased timing variability and midfrontal delta activity correlated with cognitive dysfunction in PD, and greater delta activity corresponded to more precise time estimates. This work suggests that impaired cognitive-control mechanisms contribute to cognitive dysfunction in PD.
Methods
Participants
Eighty-nine PD patients (62 men and 27 women; Table 2) were recruited from the movement-disorders clinic at the University of Iowa. All patients were examined by a movement-disorders physician to verify that they met the diagnostic criteria recommended by the United Kingdom PD Society Brain Bank criteria. Forty-one control participants were recruited from the Iowa City community and matched for age, sex, and education. All PD and control participants were determined to have the decisional capacity to provide informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and the Ethics Committee on Human Research. We obtained written informed consent from each participant. Eighty PD patients participated in the study while taking their prescribed medications, including levodopa, as usual. Because levodopa can affect interval timing17,18, we also recruited nine additional PD patients for comparisons of interval timing and EEG signals on and off dopaminergic medications. We first tested these PD patients when they were taking medications as usual (ON sessions), and then they were asked to withhold dopaminergic medications for 12 h (OFF sessions) prior to repeating behavioral and EEG testing. All research protocols were approved by the University of Iowa Human Subjects Review Board (IRB# 201707828).
Table 2.
Control (N = 41) | PD (n = 80) | ON (n = 9) | OFF (n = 9) | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Demographics and disease | ||||
Gender, M/F | 23/18 | 56/24 | 6/3 | 6/3 |
Age, years | 71.3 (1.2) | 68.7 (0.9) | 67.2 (1.8) | 67.2 (1.8) |
Disease duration, years | – | 5.2 (0.4) | 3.7 (1.2) | 3.7 (1.2) |
LED, mg/day | – | 860.5 (50.3) | 727.8 (143.9) | – |
Cognition characteristics | ||||
MOCA (0–30) | 26.6 (0.3) | 24.3 (0.4)** | 27.1 (0.6) | 27.7 (0.7) |
Non-motor characteristics | ||||
PDSS (0–60) | 7.2 (0.7) | 17.5 (1.1)** | 12.8 (2.3) | 12.8 (2.3) |
BAI (0–63) | 1.8 (0.3) | 14.7 (1.2)** | 9.4 (2.8) | 9.4 (2.8) |
GDS (0–15) | 0.7 (0.2) | 3.7 (0.4)** | 2.8 (0.7) | 2.8 (0.7) |
Motor characteristics | ||||
mUPDRS (0–56) | – | 13.4 (0.8) | 9.1 (1.6) | 11.6 (1.7)* |
M male, F female, MOCA Montreal Cognitive Assessment, PDSS Parkinson’s Disease Sleep Scale, BAI Beck Anxiety Index, GDS Geriatric Depression Scale, mUPDRS motor Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale.
Values are expressed as mean (standard error of mean). A two-sample t test was used for comparison between PD vs control subjects. Paired t test was used for comparison between ON vs OFF dataset. Significant effects in bold. **P < 0.01, *P < 0.05.
We analyzed data from those patients who completed our experimental protocol and whose midfrontal EEG channels did not produce immediately observable artifacts or noise. Of 130 recruited participants, 6 PD patients and 4 controls were excluded from further analysis because they did not meet these criteria. Thus, EEG data from a total of 83 PD (74 ON + 9 ON/OFF) patients and 37 controls were available. Demographics of patients and control subjects are summarized in Table 2.
Interval-timing task
All testing was done in a laboratory setting in the University of Iowa’s Department of Neurology. All stimuli were presented via Psychtoolbox v3.0, a MATLAB toolbox running on a Dell XPS workstation, and a 19-inch monitor. We performed a peak-interval-timing task with 3-s and 7-s randomly intermixed intervals17,18,57. In this task, participants estimated the duration of either a 3- or 7-s interval after reading instructions displayed as text at the center of a video screen. White instructional text on the center of a black screen was presented that read “Short interval” on 3-s interval trials and “Long interval” on 7-s interval trials. The actual interval durations were never communicated to the patient. After the instructions were displayed for 1 s, the start of the interval was indicated by an imperative “Go” cue: the appearance of an image of a solid box in the center of the computer screen (Fig. 1a). The cue was displayed on the screen for the entire trial, which lasted 8–10 s for 3-s intervals and 18–20 s for 7-s intervals. Participants were instructed to estimate the duration of the intervals without counting. As a distraction to discourage counting, a vowel appeared sporadically at random intervals in the center of the screen. Participants were instructed to press the keyboard spacebar at the start of when they judged the target interval to have elapsed and to release the spacebar after they judged the target interval to have elapsed (Fig. 1a). Of note, our population included many patients with advanced motor dysfunction as well as cognitive impairment and dementia, and some patients did not reliably release the spacebar once they had pressed it. Consequently, for all participants we focused our analyses only on the time participants pressed the spacebar; this time is referred to as the “keypress.” On 15% of the trials, participants were then given nonnumerical/nonverbal performance feedback on the screen indicating their response relative to the actual target interval; feedback was a horizontal green line indicating how close their keypress was to the target time. Because feedback was relatively infrequent, we did not analyze this event. The time between trials varied from 3–8 s. All participants performed 6 training trials prior to testing trials and verbalized understanding of the task. Testing sets consisted of 40 trials of each interval length, for a total of 80 trials. Trials were presented in a random order in blocks of 20 trials. Four blocks were presented. Participants took a self-paced break between each block and moved to the next block by pressing any key. Only data from patients with >20 trials of each type of trial were analyzed; 71 PD patients and 37 controls had sufficient data on 7-s trials; of these, 52 PD patients and 31 controls had sufficient data on 3-s trials. No trials were removed based on EEG characteristics. We analyzed the mean keypress time to capture timing accuracy for PD patients. To analyze timing precision, we computed the coefficient of variation (CV) by dividing the standard deviation by the mean keypress time.
EEG recording and analysis
Scalp EEG signals were collected from 64 channels of EEG actiCAP (Brain Products GmbH) with a 0.1 Hz high-pass filter and a sampling frequency of 500 Hz. Electrode Pz was used as a reference, and electrode Fpz was used as the ground. We used recording methods as described previously28,33. We used a customized EEG cap with I1 and I2 leads and without PO3, and PO4, allowing for placement of cerebellar leads. Briefly, an additional channel was recorded at the mid-inion region (Iz) and we removed unreliable Fp1, Fp2, FT9, FT10, TP9, and TP10 channels, resulting in 59 channels for pre- and post-processing. Data were epoched around the onset of instructional text (−2 s to 10 s for 3-s intervals and −2 s to 20 s for 7-s intervals), from which the associated imperative Go-cue-locked and corresponding responses-locked epochs were isolated.
EEG activity at the reference electrode Pz was recovered by computing the average reference. Bad channels and bad epochs were identified using the FASTER algorithm and pop_rejchan from EEGLAB, and were then interpolated and rejected, respectively58. On average 1.6 + /− 0.9 channels were removed, and Cz was never removed during preprocessing. Eye blinks were removed using independent component analysis (ICA). Since our a priori hypothesis was focused on midfrontal ~4 Hz activity, we based our analyses on the Cz vertex electrode in this report21,22,27–29,33.
Event-related potentials (ERPs)
ERPs were low-pass-filtered at 20 Hz. We quantified ERP differences by focusing on canonical events of negative or positive deflections in voltage. These ERP differences were then analyzed as the peak-to-trough difference in the dominant canonical cue- or response-locked morphological feature (e.g., P2 and N2 for cue, pre-response peak and error-related negativity (ERN) for response). Topographic maps were created to highlight the sensitivity of midfrontal areas to PD-related differences59. Contingent negative variation (CNV, which can be influenced by dopamine60), was calculated by computing mean amplitude from 500–3000 ms and 500–7000 ms around the “Go” cue during 3 s and 7 s interval-timing tasks, respectively.
Time-frequency analyses
We computed spectral measures by multiplying the fast Fourier transformed (FFT) power spectrum of single-trial EEG data with the FFT power spectrum of a set of complex Morlet wavelets (defined as a Gaussian-windowed complex sine wave: ei2πtfe-t^2/(2xσ^2), where t = time and f = frequency). These wavelets increased from 1–50 Hz in 50 logarithmically-spaced steps. Steps defined the width or “cycles” of each frequency band, increasing from three to ten cycles between 1 and 50 Hz and taking the inverse FFT61. The end result of this process was identical to time-domain signal convolution, and it resulted in estimates of instantaneous power (the magnitude of the analytic signal) and phase angle (the arctangent of the analytic signal). We then cut the length of each signal accordingly for each trial. As in our past work, cue-locked epochs lasted −500 – +7500 ms in the case of 7-s intervals, and −500 – +3500 in the case of 3-s intervals; note that these analyses were focused primarily on cue-locked events. For response-locked trials, they were −500 to +1000 ms. These short temporal epochs reflect the wavelet-weighted influence of longer time and frequency periods. Power was normalized by converting to a decibel (dB) scale (10*log10(powert/powerbaseline)), allowing us to directly compare the effects across frequency bands. As in our past work, the baseline for each frequency was calculated by averaging power from −300 to −200 ms prior to the onset of the imperative “Go” cue27,28. We note that specific frequencies can greatly vary between subjects, and frequency-band analyses can be diverse. Given that our primary hypothesis pertained to 1–4 Hz delta power following imperative cues, the time-frequency region-of-interest (tf-ROI) analysis was constrained to pre-defined frequency bands with the potential to be relevant (as determined by extensive past research). These bands included delta: 1–4 Hz, theta: 4–7 Hz, alpha: 8–12 Hz, and beta: 13–30 Hz21–25,27–29,33. We selected the cue-triggered 300–400 ms ROI a priori based on this prior work. In addition to this well-motivated tf-ROI, we used a cluster size of 500 pixels and a t test as in our past work27,33. We restricted all analyses to electrode Cz to be consistent with our prior work.
Clinical metrics
All study participants completed the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MOCA), a cognitive screening tool for the evaluation of a variety of rudimentary cognitive assessments that do not involve temporal judgment (e.g., visuospatial ability, naming, short-term/working memory, etc.). Each item on the test is tabulated to give a total score ranging from 0 to 30, with lower scores indicating poorer cognitive function62,63. Of our sample, 5 patients had dementia, 26 patients had mild cognitive impairment, and 40 patients were normal63.
We assessed motor function using Part 3 of the Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale (mUPDRS)64. In addition, we performed other evaluations such as the PD sleep scale, the Geriatric Depression Scale, the Beck Anxiety Inventory, elements from the NIH Toolbox, the full UPDRS, and gait analyses in some patients, although these were not related to our hypothesis and were not analyzed.
Statistical analyses
Since our hypothesis pertained to interval-timing performance and midfrontal ~4 Hz power between two groups (PD vs. control), we used t tests to compare behavioral data between groups and for EEG analyses between groups, in line with our prior work21,22,27–29. Effect sizes were calculated via Cohen’s d, and relationships among behavioral measures, EEG features, and clinical metrics (such as MOCA and mUPDRS) were calculated using the nonparametric Spearman’s rho correlation. Correlations were compared by Fisher’s r to z transformation. EEG preprocessing was performed in the Matlab toolbox EEGLAB. Behavioral and EEG analyses were performed using custom-written scripts in MATLAB that were consistent with prior work28.
We performed hierarchical multiple linear regression in order to compare successive regression models in R statistical software, using the CV of response time. We tested both mUPDRS and MOCA score variables to determine whether or not adding MOCA after mUPDRS enhances predictive capability. These predictor models were compared using an ANOVA. We performed mediation analysis using the mediation package in R, which provides an estimate of the average causal mediation effects (ACME). P values for these analyses were derived from 1000 iterations of bootstrapping, and they were expressed relative to the alpha of P = 0.05. Variables included in mediation analyses included “Go”-cue-locked power values, CV of response time, and MOCA score.
To construct a comprehensive model of time estimation on a trial-by-trial basis, we used linear mixed-effects modeling of every trial from all participants using the lmer package in R. We quantified trial-by-trial effects of cue-related EEG power on time estimation by constructing a linear mixed-effects model, where response time was the outcome variable. EEG power, disease status, MOCA score, and interval-type were predictor variables. The trial-by-trial relationship between response time and cue-related EEG power was computed and participant was included as a random effect to reduce bias in our modeling results by accounting for the inherent correlation that exists between repeated measurements on the same participant. Our model was built based on our hypothesis; model-comparison via Akaikie Information Criteria (functions AIC and step in R) revealed that our model had less prediction error than individual covariates (AIC for our model: 111639; AIC for delta only: 117168; PD only: 117157; Interval only: 111745; and MOCA only: 117155).
All data and code are available at narayanan.lab.uiowa.edu and PRED + CT (predict.cs.unm.edu)65.
Supplementary information
Acknowledgements
We would like to thank Matthew Mattell for his feedback on the drafts of this paper. This work was funded by R01NS100849-01A1 and the Roy J. Carver Charitable Trust.
Author contributions
A.S., A.I.E., R.C., and N.N. participated in the design of the study. A.S., R.C., S.C., A.I.E., and A.E. collected the data for this paper. A.S. and N.N. analyzed the data, while R.C. independently checked the analysis code. A.S., R.C., J.C., and N.N. wrote the paper.
Data availability
Data will be available on publication at PRED + CT and narayanan.lab.uiowa.edu.
Code availability
All codes will be available on publication at PRED + CT and narayanan.lab.uiowa.edu.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary information
The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1038/s41531-021-00158-x.
References
- 1.Aarsland D, Brønnick K, Larsen JP, Tysnes OB, Alves G. Cognitive impairment in incident, untreated Parkinson disease: the Norwegian ParkWest study. Neurology. 2009;72:1121–1126. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000338632.00552.cb. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Alberico SL, Cassell MD, Narayanan NS. The vulnerable ventral tegmental area in Parkinson’s disease. Basal Ganglia. 2015;5:51–55. doi: 10.1016/j.baga.2015.06.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Hely MA, Reid WGJ, Adena MA, Halliday GM, Morris JGL. The Sydney multicenter study of Parkinson’s disease: the inevitability of dementia at 20 years. Mov. Disord. 2008;23:837–844. doi: 10.1002/mds.21956. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Williams-Gray CH, Foltynie T, Brayne CEG, Robbins TW, Barker RA. Evolution of cognitive dysfunction in an incident Parkinson’s disease cohort. Brain. 2007;130:1787–1798. doi: 10.1093/brain/awm111. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Deuschl G, et al. A randomized trial of deep-brain stimulation for Parkinson’s disease. N. Engl. J. Med. 2006;355:896–908. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa060281. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Follett, K. A. Comparison of pallidal and subthalamic deep brain stimulation for the treatment of levodopa-induced dyskinesias. Neurosurg. Focus17, 14–19 (2004). [DOI] [PubMed]
- 7.Limousin P, et al. Electrical stimulation of the subthalamic nucleus in advanced Parkinson’s disease. N. Engl. J. Med. 1998;339:1105–1111. doi: 10.1056/NEJM199810153391603. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Little S, et al. Adaptive deep brain stimulation in advanced Parkinson disease. Ann. Neurol. 2013;74:449–457. doi: 10.1002/ana.23951. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Chaudhuri KR, Odin P. The challenge of non-motor symptoms in Parkinson’s disease. Prog. Brain Res. 2010;184:325–341. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(10)84017-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Forsaa EB, Larsen JP, Wentzel-Larsen T, Alves G. What predicts mortality in Parkinson disease?: a prospective population-based long-term study. Neurology. 2010;75:1270–1276. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181f61311. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Aldridge GM, Birnschein A, Denburg NL, Narayanan NS. Parkinson’s disease dementia and dementia with Lewy bodies have similar neuropsychological profiles. Front. Neurol. 2018;9:123. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2018.00123. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Owen AM. Cognitive dysfunction in Parkinson’s disease: the role of frontostriatal circuitry. Neuroscientist. 2004;10:525–537. doi: 10.1177/1073858404266776. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Buhusi CV, Meck WH. What makes us tick? Functional and neural mechanisms of interval timing. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2005;6:755–765. doi: 10.1038/nrn1764. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Brück A, et al. Positron emission tomography shows that impaired frontal lobe functioning in Parkinson’s disease is related to dopaminergic hypofunction in the caudate nucleus. Neurosci. Lett. 2001;311:81–84. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3940(01)02124-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Coull JT, Cheng R-K, Meck WH. Neuroanatomical and neurochemical substrates of timing. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2011;36:3–25. doi: 10.1038/npp.2010.113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Parker KL, Lamichhane D, Caetano MS, Narayanan NS. Executive dysfunction in Parkinson’s disease and timing deficits. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 2013;7:75. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2013.00075. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Malapani C, Deweer B, Gibbon J. Separating storage from retrieval dysfunction of temporal memory in Parkinson’s disease. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2002;14:311–322. doi: 10.1162/089892902317236920. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Malapani C, et al. Coupled temporal memories in Parkinson’s disease: a dopamine-related dysfunction. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 1998;10:316–331. doi: 10.1162/089892998562762. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Jones CR, Jahanshahi M. Contributions of the basal ganglia to temporal processing: evidence from Parkinson’s disease. Timing Time Percept. 2013;1:1–41. doi: 10.1163/22134468-00002014. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Merchant H, Luciana M, Hooper C, Majestic S, Tuite P. Interval timing and Parkinson’s disease: heterogeneity in temporal performance. Exp. Brain Res. 2008;184:233–248. doi: 10.1007/s00221-007-1097-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Kim Y-C, et al. Optogenetic stimulation of frontal D1 neurons compensates for impaired temporal control of action in dopamine-depleted mice. Curr. Biol. 2017;27:39–47. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2016.11.029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Parker KL, Chen K-H, Kingyon JR, Cavanagh JF, Narayanan NS. Medial frontal ∼4-Hz activity in humans and rodents is attenuated in PD patients and in rodents with cortical dopamine depletion. J. Neurophysiol. 2015;114:1310–1320. doi: 10.1152/jn.00412.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Cavanagh JF, Frank MJ. Frontal theta as a mechanism for cognitive control. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2014;18:414–421. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2014.04.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Cavanagh JF, Zambrano-Vazquez L, Allen JJB. Theta lingua franca: a common mid-frontal substrate for action monitoring processes. Psychophysiology. 2012;49:220–238. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2011.01293.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Narayanan NS, Cavanagh JF, Frank MJ, Laubach M. Common medial frontal mechanisms of adaptive control in humans and rodents. Nat. Neurosci. 2013;16:1888–1897. doi: 10.1038/nn.3549. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Cosman JD, Lowe KA, Zinke W, Woodman GF, Schall JD. Prefrontal control of visual distraction. Curr. Biol. 2018;28:414–420.e413. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.12.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Chen, K.-H. et al. Startle habituation and midfrontal theta activity in Parkinson’s disease. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 28, 1923–1932 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 28.Singh A, Richardson SP, Narayanan N, Cavanagh JF. Mid-frontal theta activity is diminished during cognitive control in Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychologia. 2018;117:113–122. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.05.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Kelley R, et al. A human prefrontal-subthalamic circuit for cognitive control. Brain. 2018;141:205–216. doi: 10.1093/brain/awx300. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Wojtecki, L. et al. Modulation of human time processing by subthalamic deep brain stimulation. PLoS ONE6, e24589 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 31.Church RM. Properties of the internal clock. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1984;423:566–582. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1984.tb23459.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Gibbon J, Church RM, Meck WH. Scalar timing in memory. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1984;423:52–77. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1984.tb23417.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Singh A, et al. Frontal theta and beta oscillations during lower-limb movement in Parkinson’s disease. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2020;131:694–702. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2019.12.399. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Martinu K, Degroot C, Madjar C, Strafella AP, Monchi O. Levodopa influences striatal activity but does not affect cortical hyper-activity in Parkinson’s disease. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2012;35:572–583. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2011.07979.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Narayanan NS, Rodnitzky RL, Uc EY. Prefrontal dopamine signaling and cognitive symptoms of Parkinson’s disease. Rev. Neurosci. 2013;24:267–278. doi: 10.1515/revneuro-2013-0004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Wingeier B, et al. Intra-operative STN DBS attenuates the prominent beta rhythm in the STN in Parkinson’s disease. Exp. Neurol. 2006;197:244–251. doi: 10.1016/j.expneurol.2005.09.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Okun MS. Deep-brain stimulation for Parkinson’s disease. N. Engl. J. Med. 2012;367:1529–1538. doi: 10.1056/NEJMct1208070. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Singh A. Oscillatory activity in the cortico-basal ganglia-thalamic neural circuits in Parkinson’s disease. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2018;48:2869–2878. doi: 10.1111/ejn.13853. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Wearden JH, et al. Stimulus timing by people with Parkinson’s disease. Brain Cogn. 2008;67:264–279. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2008.01.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Jensen O, Tesche CD. Frontal theta activity in humans increases with memory load in a working memory task. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2002;15:1395–1399. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2002.01975.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Kim, Y.-C. & Narayanan, N. S. Prefrontal D1 dopamine-receptor neurons and delta resonance in interval timing. Cereb. Cortex29, 2051–2060 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 42.Parker KL, Chen K-H, Kingyon JR, Cavanagh JF, Narayanan NS. D1-dependent 4 Hz oscillations and ramping activity in rodent medial frontal cortex during interval timing. J. Neurosci. 2014;34:16774–16783. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2772-14.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Herz DM, Zavala BA, Bogacz R, Brown P. Neural correlates of decision thresholds in the human subthalamic nucleus. Curr. Biol. 2016;26:916–920. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2016.01.051. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Fujisawa S, Buzsáki G. A 4 Hz oscillation adaptively synchronizes prefrontal, VTA, and hippocampal activities. Neuron. 2011;72:153–165. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.08.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Emmons EB, et al. Rodent medial frontal control of temporal processing in the dorsomedial striatum. J. Neurosci. 2017;37:8718–8733. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1376-17.2017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Wang J, Narain D, Hosseini EA, Jazayeri M. Flexible timing by temporal scaling of cortical responses. Nat. Neurosci. 2018;21:102. doi: 10.1038/s41593-017-0028-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Paton JJ, Buonomano DV. The neural basis of timing: distributed mechanisms for diverse functions. Neuron. 2018;98:687–705. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.03.045. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Scangos KW, Carter CS, Gurkoff G, Zhang L, Shahlaie K. A pilot study of subthalamic theta frequency deep brain stimulation for cognitive dysfunction in Parkinson’s disease. Brain Stimul. 2018;11:456–458. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2017.11.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Parker KL, et al. Delta-frequency stimulation of cerebellar projections can compensate for schizophrenia-related medial frontal dysfunction. Mol. Psychiatry. 2017;22:647–655. doi: 10.1038/mp.2017.50. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Aarsland D, Kurz MW. The epidemiology of dementia associated with Parkinson’s disease. Brain Pathol. 2010;20:633–639. doi: 10.1111/j.1750-3639.2009.00369.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Morrison CE, Borod JC, Brin MF, Hälbig TD, Olanow CW. Effects of levodopa on cognitive functioning in moderate-to-severe Parkinson’s disease (MSPD) J. Neural Transm. 2004;111:1333–1341. doi: 10.1007/s00702-004-0145-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Cools R. Dopaminergic modulation of cognitive function-implications for L-DOPA treatment in Parkinson’s disease. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2006;30:1–23. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.03.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Cools R, D’Esposito M. Inverted-U-shaped dopamine actions on human working memory and cognitive control. Biol. Psychiatry. 2011;69:e113–e125. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.03.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Cavanagh JF, et al. habituation to novel events effectively classifies Parkinson’s patients. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2018;129:409–418. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2017.11.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Brown DR, Richardson SP, Cavanagh JF. An EEG marker of reward processing is diminished in Parkinson’s disease. Brain Res. 2020;1727:146541. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2019.146541. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Rakitin BC, Scarmeas N, Li T, Malapani C, Stern Y. Single-dose levodopa administration and aging independently disrupt time production. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2006;18:376–387. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2006.18.3.376. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Rakitin BC, et al. Scalar expectancy theory and peak-interval timing in humans. J. Exp. Psychol. 1998;24:15–33. doi: 10.1037//0097-7403.24.1.15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Delorme A, Makeig S. EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. J. Neurosci. Methods. 2004;134:9–21. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.10.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Seer C, Lange F, Georgiev D, Jahanshahi M, Kopp B. Event-related potentials and cognition in Parkinson’s disease: an integrative review. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2016;71:691–714. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.08.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Linssen AM, et al. Contingent negative variation as a dopaminergic biomarker: evidence from dose-related effects of methylphenidate. Psychopharmacology. 2011;218:533–542. doi: 10.1007/s00213-011-2345-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Cohen, M. X. Analyzing neural time series data: theory and practice. Clin. Cogn. Neuropsychol. (2014).
- 62.Biundo R, et al. MMSE and MoCA in Parkinson’s disease and dementia with Lewy bodies: a multicenter 1-year follow-up study. J. Neural Transm. 2016;123:431–438. doi: 10.1007/s00702-016-1517-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Dalrymple-Alford JC, et al. The MoCA: well-suited screen for cognitive impairment in Parkinson disease. Neurology. 2010;75:1717–1725. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181fc29c9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Movement Disorder Society Task Force on Rating Scales for Parkinson’s, D. The unified Parkinson’s disease rating scale (UPDRS): status and recommendations. Mov. Disord. 2003;18:738–750. doi: 10.1002/mds.10473. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Cavanagh JF, Napolitano A, Wu C, Mueen A. The patient repository for EEG data + computational tools (PRED+CT) Front. Neuroinform. 2017;11:67. doi: 10.3389/fninf.2017.00067. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
Data will be available on publication at PRED + CT and narayanan.lab.uiowa.edu.
All codes will be available on publication at PRED + CT and narayanan.lab.uiowa.edu.