Abstract
Accelerated long‐term forgetting (ALF) refers to a rapid loss of information over days or weeks despite normal acquisition/encoding. Notwithstanding its potential relevance as a presymptomatic marker of cognitive dysfunction, no study has addressed the relationship between ALF and Alzheimer’s disease (AD) biomarkers. We examined ALF in APOE ɛ4 carriers versus noncarriers, and its relationships with AD cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) biomarkers. We found ALF over three months in APOE ɛ4 carriers (F(1,19) = 5.60; P < 0.05; Cohen’s d = 1.08), and this performance was associated with abnormal levels of the CSF Aβ42/ptau ratio (r = −.614; P < 0.01). Our findings indicate that ALF is detectable in at‐risk individuals, and that there is a relationship between ALF and the pathophysiological processes underlying AD.
Introduction
Accelerated long‐term forgetting (ALF) has been defined as a rapid loss of information over days or weeks despite normal acquisition and encoding. Recently, the potential usefulness of ALF in the early detection of subtle cognitive difficulties in asymptomatic stages of familial 1 and sporadic 2 , 3 Alzheimer’s disease (AD) has attracted particular attention. Two recent studies showed larger ALF in autosomal dominant disease mutation carriers 1 and asymptomatic individuals with increased genetic risk (APOE ɛ4 carriers) 3 over a 1‐week retention period.
Despite the potential relevance of ALF as a presymptomatic marker of subtle cognitive dysfunction, no study has yet addressed the relationship between ALF and AD biomarkers, or even evaluated ALF over periods beyond one week. To fill this gap, we employed an effortful and cognitively demanding associative memory task to explore ALF during a 6‐month follow‐up in cognitively healthy carriers versus noncarriers of the APOE ɛ4 haplotype. Moreover we examined the relationship between ALF and AD cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) biomarker levels. The possible findings derived from the present work would be of particular relevance for AD prevention trials, as well as for the assessment and monitoring of cognitively unimpaired populations at risk of AD.
Participants and methods
The present participants represent a sub‐sample of Tort‐Merino et al., 2 and thus the methods description follows that paper. Among all the participants included in this previous work, 11 subjects were APOE ɛ4 carriers (heterozygous for ɛ3 and ɛ4). In the present study, these 11 carriers were age‐, sex‐, and education‐matched with 11 noncarriers (homozygous for ɛ3). The study was not pre‐registered, and thus the present analyses are exploratory. All subjects underwent APOE genotyping, a lumbar puncture to determine CSF amyloid‐ß (Aβ42), total tau (tau), and phosphorylated tau (ptau) levels, a standard neuropsychological assessment to ensure that they were cognitively normal, and the cognitively demanding Ancient Farming Equipment Test (AFE‐T) to tap learning and long‐term retention at 1 week, 3 and 6 months. Participants were blind to APOE status and CSF results. The mean time lapse between the lumbar puncture and the AFE‐T assessment was 2.09 (1.4) years. The neuropsychological battery encompassed four cognitive domains (memory, language, perception, and executive functions; see Tort‐Merino et al. 2 for tests details), and it included the Cognitive Reserve Questionnaire (CRQ) for assessing cognitive reserve 4 and the Subjective Cognitive Decline Questionnaire (SCD‐Q) 5 for measuring cognitive concerns.
The AFE‐T calls for learning novel object/name pairs: it contains 24 black‐and‐white images of unfamiliar ancient farming equipment taken from the AFE paradigm 6 which are paired with a pseudoword. The test consists of two initial learning sessions administered on two consecutive days. Each learning session include seven learning runs (range of performance score 0‐24 points per run). The AFE‐T provides two final learning outcomes: (1) the free learning score (FLS) and (2) the cued learning score (CLS). Long‐term recall is examined one week, three months and six months after the initial learning phase, including a visual recognition task, free recall, and cued recall (participants are not told in advance that they will be reassessed). Forgetting rates for free and cued recall scores at 1 week, 3 months, and 6 months were obtained with comparisons to the initial learning phase. The forgetting rate was defined as one minus the ratio between each delayed score (free or cued) and the score obtained on the FLS (for free forgetting rates) or CLS (for cued forgetting rates) [e.g., 1‐(one‐week free recall score/ FLS), for one‐week free forgetting rate; and 1‐(one‐week cued recall score/ CLS), for 1‐week cued forgetting rate]. Recall rates at 3 months and 6 months were also compared with the 1‐week recall session. At the end of the 6‐month session, a verbal recognition task was given. Concerning the APOE carriers, one subject did not obtain the CLS and three subjects refused to complete the 6‐month assessment.
In order to mathematically model the forgetting functions, we followed previous recommendations. 7 , 8 , 9 Forgetting curves are characterized by a curvilinear relation that shows a rapid initial decline of information that is followed by a slower and longer decay, thus showing that information is lost in a larger extent after initial encoding. Previous studies on forgetting have shown that these functions (power and logarithmic) are the most accurate ones to describe forgetting curves. Information decay with time in our sample was fitted better using a logarithmic function [(y = a–bln(time)] when compared to a power function. Thus, each data point (4‐time data points) for each subject and condition (free and cued recall conditions) was fitted using a nonlinear least‐squares regression. Fit parameters were calculated based on the residual sum of squares and showing the proportion of data variance accounted for (R 2). The slope (parameter b) and intercept (parameter a) were computed separately for each subject and condition. The slope captures the forgetting rate of encoded information while the intercept represents the estimated initial level of performance (immediately after the last learning run).
Demographical data, levels of CSF Aß42, CSF tau, and CSF ptau were compared using Student t‐tests for independent samples and Chi‐square analyses when appropriate. Analysis of variance (ANOVA) with post‐hoc Bonferroni corrections and effect sizes expressed as Cohen’s d were conducted to compare the between‐group scores on the AFE‐T and the standard neuropsychological tests. Magnitude scale for Cohen’s d has been suggested as: d = 0.2, small effect size, d = 0.5, medium effect size and d = 0.8, large effect size. 10 The AFE‐T scores included in the analyses were the two final learning scores (i.e., FLS and CLS) and the forgetting measures (raw scores and forgetting rates). Pearson’s bivariate correlations were used to assess the association between the forgetting measures (including the slope parameter from the modeled functions) with AD CSF biomarkers and subjective cognitive ratings.
Results
Group differences between APOE ɛ4 carriers and noncarriers
We observed no group differences in age, sex, years of education, or cognitive reserve. Regarding the AD biomarkers, there were no differences in CSF tau or p‐tau levels, but CSF Aβ42 levels were significantly lower (t(20) = 3.98; P < 0.01) in the APOE ɛ4 carrier group (Table 1). There were no differences in any of the standard neuropsychological tests. Neither did the AFE‐T exhibit group differences in the learning scores (Table 1).
Table 1.
Parameters | APOE ɛ4 non‐carriers | APOE ɛ4 carriers | F | P 1 |
---|---|---|---|---|
Demographics | ||||
Gender (% women) | 72.7% | 72.7% | 0.00 2 | 1.00 |
Age | 65.1 (6.5) [58‐75] | 65.8 (6.9) [56‐77] | −0.23 | 0.814 |
Years of education | 11.0 (3.3) [8‐18] | 12.2 (4.3) [8‐20] | −0.78 | 0.446 |
CRQ | 16.4 (3.9) [9‐22] | 15.4 (5.5) [6‐22] | 0.46 | 0.651 |
CSF levels | ||||
Aβ42 | 873.9 (294.1) pg/ml | 470.5 (163.1) pg/ml | 3.98 | 0.001** |
Tau | 247.4 (98.7) pg/ml | 307.1 (95.2) pg/ml | −1.44 | 0.165 |
Ptau | 53.8 (14.8) pg/ml | 59.1 (15.7) pg/ml | −0.81 | 0.423 |
AFE‐T | ||||
Free learning score (FLS) | 17.8 (6.2) | 14.2 (6.8) | 1.62 | 0.218 |
Cued learning score (CLS) | 21.0 (3.2) | 18.6 (4.8) | 1.87 | 0.187 |
1‐week free recall | 14.1 (6.5) | 9.8 (7.4) | 2.05 | 0.168 |
1‐week cued recall | 18.2 (4.6) | 15.1 (5.9) | 1.86 | 0.188 |
1‐week FFR | 0.15 (0.4) | 0.41 (0.3) | 2.54 | 0.127 |
1‐week CFR | 0.14 (0.1) | 0.21 (0.2) | 1.21 | 0.285 |
3‐month free recall | 7.1 (6.9) | 3.4 (4.6) | 2.18 | 0.155 |
3‐month cued recall | 13.3 (5.5) | 7.6 (6.4) | 4.99 | 0.037* |
3‐month FFR | 0.59 (0.3) | 0.82 (0.2) | 3.76 | 0.067 |
3‐month CFR | 0.37 (0.2) | 0.62 (0.3) | 5.60 | 0.029* |
6‐month free recall | 7.3 (6.2) | 4.8 (5.4) | 0.81 | 0.380 |
6‐month cued recall | 13.1 (5.9) | 10.2 (7.2) | 0.89 | 0.358 |
6‐month FFR | 0.58 (0.3) | 0.74 (0.2) | 1.61 | 0.222 |
6‐month CFR | 0.38 (0.2) | 0.48 (0.3) | 0.67 | 0.423 |
1‐week to 3‐month FFR | 0.51 (0.3) | 0.75 (0.2) | 3.52 | 0.076 |
1‐week to 3‐month CFR | 0.27 (0.2) | 0.57 (0.3) | 8.70 | 0.008** |
1‐week to 6‐month FFR | 0.49 (0.3) | 0.70 (0.2) | 2.50 | 0.132 |
1‐week to 6‐month CFR | 0.27 (0.2) | 0.46 (0.3) | 2.14 | 0.161 |
1‐week visual recognition | 48.0 (0.0) | 47.5 (1.5) | 1.44 | 0.244 |
3‐month visual recognition | 46.3 (1.8) | 45.7 (1.6) | 0.71 | 0.409 |
6‐month visual recognition | 46.0 (1.5) | 46.1 (0.8) | 0.04 | 0.839 |
6‐month verbal recognition | 21.9 (2.3) | 22.4 (1.6) | 0.26 | 0.616 |
Data are presented as means (SD; standard deviation) [range]. Key: CRQ, cognitive reserve questionnaire; CSF, cerebrospinal fluid; Aβ42, amyloid‐β isoform 42; Tau, total tau; ptau, phosphorylated tau; FFR, free forgetting rate; CFR, cued forgetting rate.
p values were determined by Student’s t‐test for demographics and CSF levels;
X2 statistic.
p < 0.05
p < 0.01.
Modeling of forgetting curves
Figures 1A,B show the forgetting curves as well as the decaying information function modeled for the whole group. Importantly, the fit of the logarithmic function to the group data was nearly perfect in both groups, slightly better for the noncarriers (R 2 = 99% of variance explained) compared to carriers (R 2 = 97%). Particularly for cued recall (Figure 1B), the slope (forgetting rate parameter) was steeper for the carriers (b = −9.1) compared to noncarriers (b = −6.6). Importantly for the convergent validity of the different forgetting measures used (information decay mathematical model and forgetting rates at each time point, see results below), the 3‐month cued forgetting rate and the cued slope were strongly correlated (r = −.796; P < 0.01).
Forgetting rates at 1‐week, 3‐ and 6‐months
We computed forgetting rates as in previous studies of ALF 1 , 3 (see Figure 1C,D). At 3 months, cued recall score was significantly lower (F(1,19) = 4.99; P < 0.05; Cohen’s d = 0.99) in the carriers than in the noncarriers (Table 1; Figure 1B). Besides, the APOE ɛ4 carriers evidenced ALF in their 3‐month cued forgetting rate (F(1,19) = 5.60; P < 0.05; Cohen’s d = 1.08), as well as in their cued forgetting rate between the 1‐week and 3‐month recall (F(1,19) = 8.70; P < 0.01; Cohen’s d = 1.32; Post hoc power = 0.81) (Table 1; Figure 1D). No significant group differences were found on any of the free forgetting rates (Table 1; Figure 1C) or on the recognition scores (Table 1).
Correlational analyses
Correlational analyses in the whole sample showed a significant negative correlation between the 3‐month cued forgetting rate and the ratio CSF Aβ42/ptau (r = −.614; P < 0.01; Figure 2A). This correlation remained significant after controlling for potential confounders such as age (r = −.481; P < 0.05), years of education (r = −.606; P < 0.01) or the delay between CSF AD biomarkers acquisition and AFE‐T testing (r = −.548; P < 0.05). Besides, a significant positive correlation between the cued slope (from the modeled function in each individual) and the ratio Aβ42/ptau (r = .458; P < 0.05; Figure 2B) was found, again indicating that higher forgetting was associated to lower (more pathologic) levels of CSF AD biomarkers.
A significant positive correlation was found between the 3‐month cued forgetting rate and the SCD‐Q score (r = .636; P < 0.01; Figure 2C), and a significant negative correlation was found between the cued slope and the SCD‐Q (r = −.508; P < 0.05; Figure 2D), showing that higher forgetting was associated with higher cognitive concerns.
Discussion
Using a new highly demanding associative memory test, AFE‐T, we examined ALF in cognitively healthy APOE ɛ4 carriers and its relationships with AD CSF biomarkers. The results revealed ALF in cued recall over three months in our well‐characterized sample of APOE ε3/ε4 heterozygotes. Moreover this performance was positively associated with core AD CSF biomarkers and subjective cognitive scores.
The effects of APOE ε4 on memory performance in clinically normal adults and its relationship with amyloid burden have been widely studied. 11 , 12 , 13 As expected, our results showed that APOE ε4 carriers presented lower CSF Aβ levels than noncarriers (Table 1). However, data on the link between ALF and AD is still limited. Recent evidence from animal research showed accelerated forgetting with intact learning performance in a study using a model of familial AD (pre‐pathological PDAPP mice). 14 In humans, ALF has been proposed as a potential cognitive marker for asymptomatic stages of AD. In an earlier report, our group provided evidence for subtle learning dysfunction and long‐term forgetting in preclinical AD subjects by employing the AFE‐T. 2 A more recent study on a cohort of autosomal dominant AD families showed one‐week ALF in presymptomatic mutation carriers by using three standard cognitive tests 1 . Moreover Zimmerman & Butler 3 revealed ALF in asymptomatic APOE ɛ4 carriers by assessing 60 participants (20 homozygous for ɛ3, 20 heterozygous for ɛ3 and ɛ4, and 20 homozygous for ɛ4) with a standard memory test. Participants were asked to learn a 15‐word list from the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT) 15 on four consecutive trials to an 80% accuracy criterion. In their analyses, APOE status had no effects on memory encoding or short‐term recall, but was associated with long‐term forgetting over one week.
In line with Zimmermann & Butler, 3 we found intact learning and ALF in APOE ε4 carriers. However, here we employed AFE‐T in order to explore participants’ cognition in a more comprehensive way. AFE‐T requires forming new associations or binding information without previous semantic knowledge, which is setting high demands on cognitive processing, 16 especially compared to other standard episodic memory tasks. Regarding the learning performance, AFE‐T allowed a deeper analysis of encoding processes by yielding free and cued recall scores. Another strength is that we did not predefine any learning accuracy criterion which limits scoring range and thereby the chances for finding between‐group differences. The setup with AFE‐T also allowed exploration of free and cued recall for a longer time period, tracking ALF over 6 months after the learning phase. While the one‐week session appeared to be too close to initial learning, the 3‐month session was the optimal time‐point to find ALF in our APOE ε4 carriers, and cued recall measures showed higher sensitivity than spontaneous naming. Although the APOE ε4 carriers also performed worse than noncarriers on the free recall, the lack of statistically significant between‐group differences in these measures might be related to task difficulty and the floor effects observed beyond the 1‐week session. Also, the lack of significant between‐group differences in the 6‐month session might be due to the missing data in the APOE carriers group, which would also explain the slight improvement of this group compared to its performance at 3 months.
Importantly, both groups fitted the logarithmic function nearly perfect and particularly for cued recall, the APOE ε4 carriers showed a steeper slope (comprehensive forgetting measure) compared to noncarriers, indicating an overall tendency to show larger information loss after the initial learning in the carrier group, especially in between the first week and the 3‐month period. It is important to note that the slope parameter from the employed function strongly correlated with the AFE‐T forgetting rate at 3 months, demonstrating that both measures are clearly associated and again pointing out to the idea that the information is lost to a larger extent during the initial period after encoding. These results may also explain why the correlations found are larger with the 3‐month forgetting rate and the slope. After three months the rate of information loss is very small and therefore it might be futile to evaluate ALF beyond this time point. Taken together, our findings speak for the use of more sensitive measures with longer follow‐up designs when studying cognitively healthy at‐risk subjects. ALF is emerging as a cognitive feature of presymptomatic AD, highlighting the utility of long‐term recall designs on monitoring cognitive changes in this population.
Similar to the previous studies, 1 , 3 we found a relationship between self‐perceived subjective cognitive decline and ALF. This is an important finding since subjective cognitive concerns have been suggested as a risk factor for cognitive decline and dementia. 17 , 18 Furthermore, the present results showed a strong correlation between ALF (measured by cued forgetting rates and the slope parameter from the AFE‐T) and the CSF Aβ42/ptau ratio. Importantly, CSF Aβ42 and CSF ptau levels are valid indicators of the abnormal protein deposits underlying AD pathophysiology (i.e., β‐amyloid plaques and neurofibrillary tangles, respectively) and define AD as a specific neurodegenerative disease amongst other cognitive disorders. 19 Our finding on the correlation between ALF as measured by AFE‐T and the ratio CSF Aβ42/ptau provides further evidence for ALF as a promising candidate for a specific marker of AD‐related subtle cognitive decline in presymptomatic stages of the disease. Finally, it is important to note that given our relatively small sample size, validation of the present results is called for in future studies including larger samples and more robust power analyses.
In sum, ALF is detectable in at‐risk individuals, and there is a relationship between this cognitive measure and the pathophysiological processes underlying AD. We strongly recommend the use of more demanding cognitive tests including long‐term forgetting measures for identifying and tracking the earliest cognitive manifestations in presymptomatic AD. The present results show that accelerated forgetting is able to capture cognitive dysfunction earlier in the AD continuum. Thus, they are particularly relevant for AD prevention trials, in which regulatory agencies require a change in cognition/functional status and not only a biomarker change.
Author Contributions
A.R.F, L.R., M.L., and A.T.M. contributed to the conception and design of the study. All authors contributed to acquisition and analysis of the data. A.T.M., A.R.F., M.L., and L.R. contributed to drafting a significant portion of the manuscript. All authors have reviewed the manuscript and approved the final version.
Conflicts of Interest
Nothing to report.
Acknowledgments
This work has been supported by the Carlos III Health Institute (projects PI043610 – AC14/00014 and PI19/00745), integrated in the State Plan of Scientific and Technical Research and Innovation, and co‐financed by the European Regional Development Fund (ERDF) “Una manera de hacer Europa”. This study has been funded by Ministerio de Ciencia, Innovación y Universidades (MCIU), which is part of Agencia Estatal de Investigación (AEI), through the grant "Generación del Conocimiento", number PGC2018‐099859‐B‐I00. (Co‐funded by European Regional Development Fund. ERDF, a way to build Europe). We thank CERCA Programme/Generalitat de Catalunya for institutional support. The authors thank all the volunteers for their participation in this study.
Funding Information
Ministerio de Ciencia, Innovación y Universidades Grant Number: PGC2018‐099859‐B‐I00. European Regional Development Fund (ERDF). Carlos III Health Institute Grant Numbers: AC14/00014, PI043610, PI19/00745
Funding Statement
This work was funded by Ministerio de Ciencia, Innovación y Universidades grant PGC2018‐099859‐B‐I00; European Regional Development Fund (ERDF) grant ; Carlos III Health Institute grants AC14/00014, PI043610, and PI19/00745; Ministerio de Ciencia, Innovación y Universidades (MCIU) grant PGC2018‐099859‐B‐I00.
References
- 1. Weston PSJ, Nicholas JM, Henley SMD, et al. Accelerated long‐term forgetting in presymptomatic autosomal dominant Alzheimer’s disease: a cross‐sectional study. Lancet Neurol 2018;17(2):123–132. 10.1016/S1474-4422(17)30434-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2. Tort‐Merino A, Valech N, Peñaloza C, et al. Early detection of learning difficulties when confronted with novel information in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease stage 1. J Alzheimer’s Dis 2017;58(3):855–870. 10.3233/JAD-161173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. Zimmermann JF, Butler CR. Accelerated long‐term forgetting in asymptomatic APOE ε4 carriers. Lancet Neurol 2018;17(5):394–395. 10.1016/S1474-4422(18)30078-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. Rami L, Valls‐Pedret C, Bartrés‐Faz D, et al. Cognitive reserve questionnaire. Scores obtained in a healthy elderly population and in one with Alzheimer's disease. Rev Neurol 2011;52(4):195–201. 10.33588/rn.5204.2010478. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. Rami L, Mollica MA, García‐Sanchez C, et al. The subjective cognitive decline questionnaire (SCD‐Q): a validation study. J Alzheimer’s Dis 2014;41(2):453–466. 10.3233/JAD-132027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6. Laine M, Salmelin R. Neurocognition of new word learning in the native tongue: lessons from the ancient farming equipment paradigm. Lang Learn 2010;60(2):25–44. 10.1111/j.1467-9922.2010.00599.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Rubin DC, Wenzel AE. One hundred years of forgetting: a quantitative description of retention. Psychol Rev 1996;103(4):734–760. [Google Scholar]
- 8. Wixted JT, Ebbesen EB. On the form of forgetting. Psychol. Sci. 1991;2(6):409–415. [Google Scholar]
- 9. Wixted JT, Ebbesen EB. Genuine power curves in forgetting: a quantitative analysis of individual subject forgetting functions. Mem Cogn 1997;25(5):731–739. 10.3758/bf03211316. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. USA: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1988. [Google Scholar]
- 11. Lim YY, Ellis KA, Ames D, et al. Aβ amyloid, cognition, and APOE genotype in healthy older adults. Alzheimer’s Dement 2013;9(5):538–545. 10.1016/j.jalz.2012.07.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Mormino EC, Betensky RA, Hedden T, et al. Amyloid and APOE e4 interact to influence short‐term decline in preclinical Alzheimer disease. Neurology 2014;82(20):1760–1767. 10.1212/WNL.0000000000000431. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Kantarci K, Lowe V, Przybelski SA, et al. APOE modifies the association between Aβ load and cognition in cognitively normal older adults. Neurology 2012;78(4):232–240. 10.1212/WNL.0b013e31824365ab. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Beglopoulos V, Tulloch J, Roe AD, et al. Early detection of cryptic memory and glucose uptake deficits in pre‐pathological APP mice. Nat Commun 2016;7:1–10. 10.1038/ncomms11761. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Rey A. L’examen clinique en psychologie. Paris: Presses Universitaire de France, 1964. [Google Scholar]
- 16. Rentz DM, Amariglio RE, Becker JA, et al. Face‐name associative memory performance is related to amyloid burden in normal elderly. Neuropsychologia 2011;49(9):2776–2783. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.06.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Jessen F, Wiese B, Bachmann C, et al. Prediction of dementia by subjective memory impairment effects of severity and temporal association with cognitive impairment. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2010;67(4):414–422. 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.30. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. Reisberg B, Shulman MB, Torossian C, et al. Outcome over seven years of healthy adults with and without subjective cognitive impairment. Alzheimer’s Dement 2010;6(1):11–24. 10.1016/j.jalz.2009.10.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Jack CR, Bennett DA, Blennow K, et al. NIA‐AA Research Framework: Toward a biological definition of Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimer’s Dement 2018;14(4):535–562. 10.1016/j.jalz.2018.02.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]