Abstract
Inherited color vision deficiencies typically result from a loss or alteration of the visual photopigments absorbing light and thus impact the very first step of seeing. There is growing interest in how subsequent steps in the visual pathway might be calibrated to compensate for the altered receptor signals, with the possibility that color coding and color percepts might be less severely impacted than the receptor differences predict. These compensatory adjustments provide important insights into general questions about sensory plasticity and the sensory and cognitive processes underlying how we experience color.
Keywords: Neural plasticity, adaptation, vision, color vision, color deficiencies
Introduction
Plasticity is a hallmark of sensory processing. Environments vary widely over space and time, and sensory systems must continuously recalibrate to optimize coding for the current context1,2. Importantly, the sensory apparatus also varies widely among individuals, and even within an individual there are dramatic changes over time (e.g. development and aging) and space (e.g. between central and peripheral vision). Plasticity must therefore correct for the properties not only of the world but also of the observer. These adjustments can arise at many structural levels, from large-scale cortical reorganization (e.g. when cortical areas normally devoted to one sense are recruited by other modalities when that sense is lost)3 to local synaptic dynamics4 and sensitivity regulation within cells5–7 and networks8,9. They can also occur over many timescales10, from rapid response changes within milliseconds11 to experience-dependent development12,13. Finally, they occur throughout the processing hierarchy, from adjusting receptor sensitivity to tuning cognition and learning. A major unresolved issue in neuroscience is defining the actual number of distinct mechanisms underlying neural plasticity, how they are manifest, and how they interact.
Here we discuss general issues and insights about plasticity within the specific context of common inherited color vision deficiencies. Recent studies have highlighted the potential for compensating for a color vision deficiency so that the world is described as richer in color than the sensory losses would predict. However, the nature of these compensations and whether they reflect perceptual or conceptual adjustments14 remain poorly understood. For example, a perceptual adaptation could reflect changes in the sensory signals encoding color, while a conceptual adjustment might be learning to label the percept with the same color name that others use15–18. Exploring plasticity through the lens of color vision provides a rich and unique opportunity for revealing how and how far neural coding and perceptual experience can be calibrated to discount natural physiological variations within and between observers and one that can also provide new insights into longstanding questions and controversies about the processes mediating human color vision.
Human color vision
Most animal species have the capacity for color vison, and in all known cases it is based on detecting light with two or more photoreceptor classes that differ in the wavelength sensitivity of their photopigments19–22. However, having more types of receptors does not necessarily confer a higher dimensionality of color vision23. Most humans have three classes of cone receptors maximally sensitive to short (S), medium (M), or long (L) wavelengths, and thus normal (or, more aptly, routine) color vision is trichromatic. Encoding color further depends on the neural machinery for comparing the relative cone responses, for example to determine whether the L cones or M cones are more excited by a light spectrum. These comparisons begin in the retina, in post-receptoral neurons that receive inputs of the same or opposite sign from different receptor types, and are carried within three “cardinal” mechanisms24 with distinct cell types and pathways, named for their projections to different layers of the lateral geniculate nucleus25–28. Cells in the magnocellular (M) pathway sum the L and M cones’ signals and are the substrate of our luminance sensitivity (L+M)29. Chromatic information is instead carried by two cone-opponent cell types that receive opposing signals from the L and M cones (L-M, the parvocellular or P pathway) or from S cones opposed by both L and M (S-LM, the koniocellular or K pathway). Figure 1 illustrates the colors as defined by these two chromatic dimensions. However, these mechanisms describe only the initial steps of color coding. There are major further transformations of the cone-opponent signals in the cortex, and different transformations may arise at several different cortical stages30–32. Moreover, even within the retina, there is a possibility that color percepts are carried within pathways that combine the cones in different ways than the cardinal mechanisms33,34.
Inherited color vision deficiencies
Inherited color vision deficiencies primarily reflect alterations in the genes coding the cone photopigment opsins. The genes coding the L and M opsins lie in tandem on the X chromosome35. Errors in the L or M genes can lead to a loss of one receptor class (dichromacy) or to shifts in the spectral sensitivity (anomalous trichromacy) (Figure 1). The latter reduces the spectral separation between the L and M cones and thus reduces the difference signals (L-M) conveying chromatic information, resulting in reduced discrimination for this chromatic dimension. As an X-linked recessive trait, color vision deficiencies of this type are the most common and affect about 6% of males but are rare in females. Instead, female carriers for color vision deficiencies (with a normal gene on one X chromosome and modified gene on the other) might express both to develop a fourth dimension for color36. However, whether and when this leads to functional tetrachromacy has been challenging to resolve37.
Simulations of color vision deficiencies are common and work by filtering the image to remove or alter the chromatic information that should be lost or weakened by their altered sensors (Figure 1). These illustrations are often closer to depicting what a trichromat would experience if they suddenly lost one class of receptors but have also incorporated comparisons from rare individuals with a color loss in only one eye to better simulate a dichromat’s experience38. However, few simulations have tried to capture the impact of compensation for the receptor losses39.
Characterizing compensatory processes for inherited color vision deficiencies provides an ideal natural experiment for exploring the types and limits of plasticity in neural processing and perception. First, the deficit arises from only a single discrete change in the visual system that is restricted to the first step of vision, when the light is captured by the receptor35. This allows characterizing how the rest of the system can reorganize given a simple and highly stable change in its inputs. Second, each individual has had a lifetime experiencing the world through their receptor complement. Thus, their perceptual capacities can reveal adaptations over timescales that are orders of magnitude longer than are typically studied in a lab, potentially revealing different mechanisms as well as the ultimate limits of sensory plasticity. Third, the spectral sensitivity differences between observers can be very precisely quantified, allowing precise predictions about their color perception and sensitive tests for any adaptive changes. Finally, a wide variety of techniques have been developed to probe color vision and to try to isolate different stages of processing, from the photopigments and early sensory coding to cognition and consciousness. Thus, different measurements can be deployed to target different levels or properties of the system in order to assess at what stages or in what tasks a compensation for the color vision deficiency occurs and what this implies about the underlying mechanisms.
Compensation for color vision deficiencies
Because dichromats completely lose one cone class, in principle there is no signal that the visual system could restore. Yet dichromats can reliably use a wide range of color terms that map onto color naming patterns for trichromats, and these abilities have been attributed to both sensory processing and learning how colors are communicated14,40–42. Dichromats also can become trichromatic for large fields by taking advantage of the changes in spectral sensitivity across the retina or information provided by rod receptors43. The rods are a fourth receptor class specialized for sensing under dim light levels. The rod system is typically described as “color blind”, yet there are many examples of rod contributions to color vision44 and color can be experienced even at low light levels at which only rods are active, perhaps because of learned associations with cone-mediated percepts45. Another intriguing possibility is the recruitment of signals arising from the intrinsically photosensitive retinal ganglion cells (ipRGCs). These recently discovered cells are in the output neural layer of the eye but directly absorb light using melanopsin as the photopigment46,47. The ipRGC pathway may primarily serve non-image forming, or non-perceptual, visual function, including regulating circadian rhythms, sleep, mood, and cognition48. However, ipRGCs also receive inputs from cones and rods and project to visual pathways49,50. Their contributions to color perception remain difficult to isolate, but excitation of this pathway has been found to influence both brightness and color percepts51–55. To our knowledge, the possible role of these signals in color vision deficiencies remains unexplored. However, dichromats may offer a more powerful test case for probing color coding and ipRGCs because these observers may be more dependent on their signals and also because the signals themselves may be easier to isolate from the cone responses.
Recently, several theoretical and empirical studies have also explored compensation for anomalous trichromacy56. In this case, the smaller differences in the L and M cone sensitivities could in principle be discounted simply by amplifying the gain in the post-receptoral neurons that compare these signals39,57. As noted, this comparison is initially carried out in cone-opponent cells that receive opposite inputs from the L and M cones and is carried through the parvocellular pathway25,26. The rationale for amplifying their responses when the inputs are weaker is grounded in information theory, which predicts that each cell should center and scale its limited operating range for the distribution of inputs in order to maximize the information it can transmit58–61. Rescaling responses to the magnitude of luminance or chromatic variation (or contrast) is a well-established form of visual adaptation documented in countless behavioral and physiological studies6,7,62,63. Typically, these involve stimulating the system with high contrast and measuring the resulting sensitivity losses. Yet a small number of studies have also shown that when observers are exposed to weaker-than-normal luminance contrasts, the responses instead increase64–66, suggesting that perceived saturation should also increase in an observer habitually exposed to low L-M contrasts.
There are a number of unsolved puzzles about the impact of such post-receptoral gain adjustments. For example, adaptation could amplify not only the signal but also the noise, and thus the consequences for perception depend on where in the system sensitivity regulation and the limiting noise occur39,58,67. In addition, while color contrast adaptation is readily observed in the visual cortex, color-opponent cells at the level of the retina and LGN show substantially less adaptation68,69 (but see 70). This raises the possibility that the system could restore optimal coding in the cortex even though the chromatic signals in the retina and LGN remain weak and thus poorly calibrated to the natural environment. One potential rationale for this is that P cells encode both color and lightness information (because the weights of the L and M cone inputs vary substantially25 and because the cone inputs are segregated into different spatial subregions of the cell’s receptive field71). This means that gain changes at this level not only would amplify the weakened chromatic signal but also could “over-amplify” the better preserved lightness signals. In the cortex, lightness and chromatic information may become decoupled72,73, and it may therefore be more advantageous to adapt at this stage in order to separately calibrate these signals. A still further possibility is that the retina and LGN can adjust in different ways or over longer timescales than are revealed by short-term adaptation experiments. Adaptation to color can occur over many timescales74–77. However, it remains uncertain whether adjustments with different time constants reflect similar processes and consequences but tracking different rates of change or if they reflect fundamentally different mechanisms that calibrate different aspects of the system.
Empirically, the primary evidence for compensation has come from studies comparing the perceptual reports of anomalous trichromats. For example, in a perceptual grouping task, some anomalous observers perceived L-M color differences as more salient than expected78. Similarly, estimates of the color responses from multidimensional scaling79,80 or contrast scaling81 have found that the L-M percepts are stronger than predicted from the observer’s detection thresholds and can come close to the responses for routine trichromatic observers.
However, here again there are many unresolved questions. First, these studies rely on observers’ subjective reports, and thus it is difficult to infer from these reports what they actually “see.” For example, individuals could learn to interpret or label the same stimuli with the same scale or terms that others use even if they perceive them very differently, and they might base their reports on very different stimulus cues14,41,82. As noted, even dichromats can exhibit nearly normal patterns of basic color naming but might rely on different cues for this, such as luminance variations or rod signals. As such, some putative measures of color appearance and compensation may reflect more cognitive stages (such as how the sensory signals are interpreted or categorized) rather than the strength of the sensory signals themselves. Objective measures of the chromatic contrast responses in anomalous trichromacy remain lacking. However, recent studies have begun to probe compensation for color vision deficiencies with neuroimaging. For example, an fMRI study83 found that the BOLD response to L-M contrast was amplified within but not before early visual cortical areas, implicating V1 or V2 as the earliest site of the gain compensation. Another study using visual evoked potentials (VEPs) also found amplified signals, but only when the stimuli were viewed binocularly, again implicating a cortical locus for the response gains84. For monocular viewing, VEP amplitudes are substantially reduced and diagnostic of the deficiency85.
A second question is under what conditions the compensation is manifest. Clearly, the sensitivity losses are not completely discounted because anomalous trichromats fail color screening tests, which measure the ability to discriminate small color differences. Similarly, the simple loss hypothesis closely predicts their color matching behavior (i.e. when two lights with different physical spectra will look the same), and the color matches of dichromats have played a central role in deriving the cone spectral sensitivities86. The presumed amplification instead occurs above the detection threshold, when the system “knows” a signal is there. A related example is the phenomenon of contrast constancy. Thresholds for detecting spatial contrast (e.g. in spatial sinewave gratings) are lowest for medium spatial scales, forming the characteristic bandpass contrast sensitivity function. However, when the gratings are slightly above threshold, the perceived contrast is nearly independent of spatial frequency87,88. With regard to color, greater constancy or compensation may also be manifest when the task reflects cognitive factors (e.g. learning color categories) rather than early sensory signals14.
A further challenge for interpreting these studies is that anomalous trichromacy is itself a very heterogeneous condition56. Observers can vary widely in the separation of their longer wave cones, as well as which cone class is altered, and this separation does not always predict even their performance at threshold89,90. For example, some anomalous observers who are revealed by their altered color matches nevertheless have fine color discrimination. How these individual differences impact the form and extent of compensatory adjustments remains an important question to address.
Finally, it should be emphasized that even in measures of color appearance or salience, the degree of compensation is rarely complete. That is, suprathreshold percepts may be amplified relative to threshold sensitivity but, in most anomalous observers, still fall short of the L-M responses of routine trichromats78,79,81,91. Understanding why compensation is incomplete is as important as understanding why it occurs at all. In the case of color, there may again be many factors limiting plasticity, from the problems of amplifying noise to the possibility that anomalous observers are basing their judgments on different cues (e.g. lightness variations) that may be inherently weaker. Another potential limit is that the L and M cones are thought to differ only in their opsins and thus do not have a unique physiological signature. The visual system must therefore learn the identity of each cone from the patterns of their responses to the spectra the observer experiences92,93. Misclassifications are more likely the smaller the differences between these responses, and this could introduce errors in post-receptoral coding that cannot be undone by simply amplifying those differences.
Restoring color vision
We have emphasized the advantage of assessing plasticity when the adjustment is to a stable and permanent property of the visual system. But what if that property suddenly changes? There is now the prospect for viral vector-mediated gene therapy to introduce the missing photopigment in the retina33. Dichromatic adult monkeys whose eyes were injected with a missing L pigment gene expressed the pigment over time and became trichromatic in their color discriminations94. This suggests that even in the adult, the post-receptoral machinery can exploit the added input signals, though the extent to which this involves tapping into existing pathways versus recalibration remains uncertain. Beyond the expanded capacity to detect colors, it is also unknown how the added receptor type will impact the perceptual experience of color95 and what forms of plasticity might adjust for these.
Techniques have also been developed to optically enhance reddish-greenish contrasts for color-anomalous observers by using notch filters. These cannot restore normal color vision96 but can alter the relative salience of different colors. A recent study found that after wearing these glasses for just a few days, the participants reported experiencing stronger L-M contrasts, even with the glasses removed97. This is surprising given that the observers had a lifetime to adjust to their cone sensitivities and because the changes are in the opposite direction to the effects predicted by adaptation (since adapting to stronger colors should have resulted in reduced sensitivity). However, similar effects have been found in perceptual training for other visual deficits (e.g. amblyopia)98–100, where requiring the observer to attend to the weakened visual signals allows them to process them more effectively. An implication of such results is that the visual system is capable of even greater plasticity when the stimulus or task demands it, and these effects have been widely studied in the context of perceptual learning101–104. How this learning interacts with other forms of plasticity like adaptation remains poorly understood but is being actively investigated105–109.
Implications for routine trichromatic color vision
Importantly, the processes compensating for a color vision deficiency are also at play in calibrating the sensitivity and perception of individuals with routine trichromacy. For example, many authors have noted that because of the spectral overlap in the L and M cones (and, to a lesser extent, the S cones), their responses are very similar and for natural spectra are very highly correlated110–112. This means that the L-M chromatic signal is many times weaker than the L+M signal, which underlies our luminance sensitivity. Based on these cone contrasts, the world should appear to vary much more in brightness than color. However, post-receptoral sensitivity is correspondingly much higher for the chromatic cone contrasts57,113, to an extent that roughly matches the gamut of color signals we are exposed to in our environment114. From this perspective, then, the gain adjustments in the anomalous observers are simply the same adjustments that operate in every observer.
A further important point is that color vision deficiencies are just a more extreme case of the enormous variability inherent in human color vision. Individuals who are all classified as “normal” trichromats nevertheless differ widely in their spectral sensitivities because of differences in the lens and macular screening pigments, the specific absorption spectra of the cones, and the relative number of the L and M cones115–117. Spectral sensitivity differences influence an observer’s color matches (i.e. which physically different spectra appear identical) but have surprisingly little impact on measurements of color appearance118,119. For example, what looks gray does not vary with age despite the yellowing lens120,121 and does not vary with space despite the drop in macular screening pigment outside the central fovea122,123, and the relative sensitivities of the different cone classes also show little change with age124,125. Similarly, the L to M cone ratios strongly impact luminance sensitivity but have little influence on color percepts126–128. Again, this is because each individual’s visual system is calibrated for their environment63,118. Thus, what looks gray is as much a property of the environment (e.g. the average spectral stimulus) as the visual system representing it129,130.
Comparing different stages of color coding in routine and color-deficient observers could continue to help resolve some of the ongoing challenges of understanding the bases for human color vision and what different measurements of color perception actually reflect. Traditional models of color vision assume that subjective color appearance depends on two chromatic opponent processes signaling red vs. green or blue vs. yellow sensations. Yet, while opponency is well established by both behavioral and physiological measures131, the red-green and blue-yellow axes of color appearance do not agree with the early precortical dimensions along which color is encoded (i.e. the L-M and S-LM mechanisms)24. This has suggested that the red-green and blue-yellow processes arise at later stages132 or are carried along different pathways33,34. However, there is also growing uncertainty around whether these canonical hue processes exist at all118. A clear neural signature for these perceptual processes has yet to be identified, and the special colors they predict (e.g. pure red-green or blue-yellow) do not appear special in many behavioral tasks133–135. Thus, the visual representation of color—at stages closer to our conscious percepts—is, in reality, very poorly understood. Measurements with color-anomalous observers can be used to test which aspects of these percepts can be equated across observers with fundamentally different receptor constraints, and this may in turn elucidate the basis for this equivalence. For example, neuroimaging studies have attempted to identify different stages of color coding and the cortical sites at which the neural representation of color begins to resemble the perceptual organization of color136–138. If these representations look different for routine and anomalous observers, yet their color reports agree, then we would need to look elsewhere to understand the nature of these reports. More generally, tests of plasticity and compensation in color vision deficiencies may reveal which types of measurements uncover intrinsic signals within our senses versus the cognitive strategies involved in their readout.
Conclusion
While color vision deficiencies are often modeled as a reduced form of standard trichromatic color vision, many lines of evidence point to compensatory adjustments that can partially discount the consequences of the receptor loss or change. Understanding these adjustments—their number, nature, and limits—can shed important light on general principles of adaptation and plasticity in sensory systems and on the still poorly understood basis of human color experience.
The peer reviewers who approve this article are:
Kenneth Knoblauch, Univ Lyon, Université, Claude Bernard Lyon 1, Inserm, Stem Cell and Brain Research Institute, Bron, France
Anya Hurlbert, Neuroscience, Institute of Biosciences, Newcastle University, Newcastle upon Tyne, UK
Funding Statement
This work was supported by National Institutes of Health (NIH) grant EY-010834.
The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
References
- 1. Olshausen BA, Field DJ: Natural image statistics and efficient coding. Network. 1996; 7(2): 333–9. 10.1088/0954-898X/7/2/014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2. Geisler WS: Visual perception and the statistical properties of natural scenes. Annu Rev Psychol. 2008; 59: 167–92. 10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085632 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. Fine I, Park JM: Blindness and Human Brain Plasticity. Annu Rev Vis Sci. 2018; 4: 337–56. 10.1146/annurev-vision-102016-061241 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. Berry KP, Nedivi E: Experience-Dependent Structural Plasticity in the Visual System. Annu Rev Vis Sci. 2016; 2: 17–35. 10.1146/annurev-vision-111815-114638 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. Cameron MA, Morley JW, Pérez-Fernández V: Seeing the light: Different photoreceptor classes work together to drive adaptation in the mammalian retina. Curr Opin Physiol. 2020; 16: 43–9. 10.1016/j.cophys.2020.05.003 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 6. Clifford CWG, Webster MA, Stanley GB, et al. : Visual adaptation: Neural, psychological and computational aspects. Vision Res. 2007; 47(25): 3125–31. 10.1016/j.visres.2007.08.023 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Kohn A: Visual adaptation: Physiology, mechanisms, and functional benefits. J Neurophysiol. 2007; 97(5): 3155–64. 10.1152/jn.00086.2007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Solomon SG, Kohn A: Moving sensory adaptation beyond suppressive effects in single neurons. Curr Biol. 2014; 24(20): R1012–22. 10.1016/j.cub.2014.09.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; Faculty Opinions Recommendation
- 9. Carandini M, Heeger DJ: Normalization as a canonical neural computation. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2011; 13(1): 51–62. 10.1038/nrn3136 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; Faculty Opinions Recommendation
- 10. Kording KP, Tenenbaum JB, Shadmehr R: The dynamics of memory as a consequence of optimal adaptation to a changing body. Nat Neurosci. 2007; 10(6): 779–86. 10.1038/nn1901 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; Faculty Opinions Recommendation
- 11. Glasser DM, Tsui JMG, Pack CC, et al. : Perceptual and neural consequences of rapid motion adaptation. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2011; 108(45): E1080–8. 10.1073/pnas.1101141108 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Sugita Y: Experience in early infancy is indispensable for color perception. Curr Biol. 2004; 14(14): 1267–71. 10.1016/j.cub.2004.07.020 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; Faculty Opinions Recommendation
- 13. Brown AM: Development of visual sensitivity to light and color vision in human infants: A critical review. Vision Res. 1990; 30(8): 1159–88. 10.1016/0042-6989(90)90173-i [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Shepard RN, Cooper LA: Representation of Colors in the Blind, Color-Blind, and Normally Sighted. Psychol Sci. 1992; 3(2): 97–104. 10.1111/j.1467-9280.1992.tb00006.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Witzel C, Gegenfurtner KR: Color Perception: Objects, Constancy, and Categories. Annu Rev Vis Sci. 2018; 4: 475–99. 10.1146/annurev-vision-091517-034231 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Park J, Tauber S, Jameson KA, et al. : The Evolution of Shared Concepts in Changing Populations. Rev Phil Psych. 2019; 10: 479–98. 10.1007/s13164-018-0420-5 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Gibson E, Futrell R, Jara-Ettinger J, et al. : Color naming across languages reflects color use. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2017; 114(40): 10785–90. 10.1073/pnas.1619666114 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; Faculty Opinions Recommendation
- 18. Lindsey DT, Brown AM, Brainard DH, et al. : Hunter-Gatherer Color Naming Provides New Insight into the Evolution of Color Terms. Curr Biol. 2015; 25(18): 2441–6. 10.1016/j.cub.2015.08.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; Faculty Opinions Recommendation
- 19. Jacobs GH: The evolution of vertebrate color vision. Adv Exp Med Biol. 2012; 739: 156–72. 10.1007/978-1-4614-1704-0_10 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. Baden T, Osorio D: The Retinal Basis of Vertebrate Color Vision. Annu Rev Vis Sci. 2019; 5: 177–200. 10.1146/annurev-vision-091718-014926 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; Faculty Opinions Recommendation
- 21. Osorio D, Vorobyev M: Photoreceptor spectral sensitivities in terrestrial animals: Adaptations for luminance and colour vision. Proc Biol Sci. 2005; 272(1574): 1745–52. 10.1098/rspb.2005.3156 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22. Cuthill IC, Allen WL, Arbuckle K, et al. : The biology of color. Science. 2017; 357(6350): eaan0221. 10.1126/science.aan0221 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Jacobs GH: Photopigments and the dimensionality of animal color vision. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2018; 86: 108–30. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.12.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; Faculty Opinions Recommendation
- 24. Krauskopf J, Williams DR, Heeley DW: Cardinal directions of color space. Vision Res. 1982; 22(9): 1123–31. 10.1016/0042-6989(82)90077-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Derrington AM, Krauskopf J, Lennie P: Chromatic mechanisms in lateral geniculate nucleus of macaque. J Physiol. 1984; 357: 241–65. 10.1113/jphysiol.1984.sp015499 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26. Dacey DM: Parallel pathways for spectral coding in primate retina. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2000; 23: 743–75. 10.1146/annurev.neuro.23.1.743 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27. Martin PR, White AJ, Goodchild AK, et al. : Evidence that blue-on cells are part of the third geniculocortical pathway in primates. Eur J Neurosci. 1997; 9(7): 1536–41. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.1997.tb01509.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. Lee BB: Color coding in the primate visual pathway: A historical view. J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis. 2014; 31(4): A103–12. 10.1364/JOSAA.31.00A103 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29. Lennie P, Pokorny J, Smith VC: Luminance. J Opt Soc Am A. 1993; 10(6): 1283–93. 10.1364/josaa.10.001283 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30. Conway BR, Eskew RT, Martin PR, et al. : A tour of contemporary color vision research. Vision Res. 2018; 151: 2–6. 10.1016/j.visres.2018.06.009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31. Gegenfurtner KR: Cortical mechanisms of colour vision. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2003; 4(7): 563–72. 10.1038/nrn1138 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32. Conway BR: The Organization and Operation of Inferior Temporal Cortex. Annu Rev Vis Sci. 2018; 4: 381–402. 10.1146/annurev-vision-091517-034202 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33. Neitz J, Neitz M: Evolution of the circuitry for conscious color vision in primates. Eye (Lond). 2017; 31(2): 286–300. 10.1038/eye.2016.257 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34. Patterson SS, Neitz M, Neitz J: Reconciling Color Vision Models With Midget Ganglion Cell Receptive Fields. Front Neurosci. 2019; 13: 865. 10.3389/fnins.2019.00865 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; Faculty Opinions Recommendation
- 35. Neitz J, Neitz M: The genetics of normal and defective color vision. Vision Res. 2011; 51(7): 633–51. 10.1016/j.visres.2010.12.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36. Jameson KA, Satalich TA, Joe KC, et al. : Human Color Vision and Tetrachromacy (Elements in Perception). Cambridge University Press. 2020. Reference Source [Google Scholar]
- 37. Jordan G, Mollon J: Tetrachromacy: The mysterious case of extra-ordinary color vision. Curr Opin Behav Sci. 2019; 30: 130–134. 10.1016/j.cobeha.2019.08.002 [DOI] [Google Scholar]; Faculty Opinions Recommendation
- 38. Viénot F, Brettel H, Ott L, et al. : What do colour-blind people see? Nature. 1995; 376(6536): 127–8. 10.1038/376127a0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39. Webster MA, Juricevic I, McDermott KC: Simulations of adaptation and color appearance in observers with varying spectral sensitivity. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt. 2010; 30(5): 602–10. 10.1111/j.1475-1313.2010.00759.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40. Wachtler T, Dohrmann U, Hertel R: Modeling color percepts of dichromats. Vision Res. 2004; 44(24): 2843–55. 10.1016/j.visres.2004.06.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41. Bonnardel V: Color naming and categorization in inherited color vision deficiencies. Vis Neurosci. 2006; 23(3–4): 637–43. 10.1017/S0952523806233558 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42. Jameson D, Hurvich LM: Dichromatic color language: "reds" and "greens" don't look alike but their colors do. Sens Processes. 1978; 2(2): 146–55. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43. Smith VC, Pokorny J: Large-field trichromacy in protanopes and deuteranopes. J Opt Soc Am. 1977; 67(2): 213–20. 10.1364/josa.67.000213 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44. Buck SL: The interaction of rod and cone signals: pathways and psychophysics. In: The New Visual Neurosciences 2014; 485–497. [Google Scholar]
- 45. Elliott SL, Cao D: Scotopic hue percepts in natural scenes. J Vis. 2013; 13(13): 15. 10.1167/13.13.15 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46. Provencio I, Rollag MD, Castrucci AM: Photoreceptive net in the mammalian retina. This mesh of cells may explain how some blind mice can still tell day from night. Nature. 2002; 415(6871): 493. 10.1038/415493a [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47. Berson DM, Dunn FA, Takao M: Phototransduction by retinal ganglion cells that set the circadian clock. Science. 2002; 295(5557): 1070–3. 10.1126/science.1067262 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; Faculty Opinions Recommendation
- 48. Schmidt TM, Chen SK, Hattar S: Intrinsically photosensitive retinal ganglion cells: Many subtypes, diverse functions. Trends Neurosci. 2011; 34(11): 572–80. 10.1016/j.tins.2011.07.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49. Dacey DM, Liao HW, Peterson BB, et al. : Melanopsin-expressing ganglion cells in primate retina signal colour and irradiance and project to the LGN. Nature. 2005; 433(7027): 749–54. 10.1038/nature03387 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; Faculty Opinions Recommendation
- 50. Patterson SS, Kuchenbecker JA, Anderson JR, et al. : A Color Vision Circuit for Non-Image-Forming Vision in the Primate Retina. Curr Biol. 2020; 30(7): 1269–1274.e2. 10.1016/j.cub.2020.01.040 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; Faculty Opinions Recommendation
- 51. Cao D, Chang A, Gai S: Evidence for an impact of melanopsin activation on unique white perception. J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis. 2018; 35(4): B287–B291. 10.1364/JOSAA.35.00B287 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52. Horiguchi H, Winawer J, Dougherty RF, et al. : Human trichromacy revisited. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2013; 110(3): E260–9. 10.1073/pnas.1214240110 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53. Zele AJ, Feigl B, Adhikari P, et al. : Melanopsin photoreception contributes to human visual detection, temporal and colour processing. Sci Rep. 2018; 8(1): 3842. 10.1038/s41598-018-22197-w [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54. DeLawyer T, Tsujimura SI, Shinomori K: Relative contributions of melanopsin to brightness discrimination when hue and luminance also vary. J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis. 2020; 37(4): A81–A88. 10.1364/JOSAA.382349 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55. Lucas RJ, Allen AE, Milosavljevic N, et al. : Can We See with Melanopsin? Annu Rev Vis Sci. 2020; 6: 453–468. 10.1146/annurev-vision-030320-041239 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56. Bosten J: The known unknowns of anomalous trichromacy. Curr Opin Behav Sci. 2019; 30: 228–37. 10.1016/j.cobeha.2019.10.015 [DOI] [Google Scholar]; Faculty Opinions Recommendation
- 57. MacLeod DIA: The Verriest Lecture: Colour discrimination, colour constancy and natural scene statistics. In: Normal and Defective Colour Vision J.D.M. Mollon, J. Pokorny, and K. Knoblauch, Editors. Oxford University Press: Oxford. 2003; 189–217. [Google Scholar]
- 58. von der Twer T, MacLeod DIA: Optimal nonlinear codes for the perception of natural colours. Network. 2001; 12(3): 395–407. 10.1080/net.12.3.395.407 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59. Laughlin SA: A simple coding procedure enhances a neuron's information capacity. Z Naturforsch C Biosci. 1981; 36(9–10): 910–2. 10.1515/znc-1981-9-1040 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; Faculty Opinions Recommendation
- 60. Buchsbaum G, Gottschalk A: Trichromacy, opponent colours coding and optimum colour information transmission in the retina. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 1983; 220(1218): 89–113. 10.1098/rspb.1983.0090 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61. Atick JJ, Li Z, Redlich AN: Understanding Retinal Color Coding from First Principles. Neural Comput. 1992; 4(4): 559–72. 10.1162/neco.1992.4.4.559 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 62. Webster MA: Adaptation and visual coding. J Vis. 2011; 11(5): 1–23. 10.1167/11.5.3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63. Webster MA: Visual Adaptation. Annu Rev Vis Sci. 2015; 1: 547–67. 10.1146/annurev-vision-082114-035509 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; Faculty Opinions Recommendation
- 64. Kwon M, Legge GE, Fang F, et al. : Adaptive changes in visual cortex following prolonged contrast reduction. J Vis. 2009; 9(2): 20.1–16. 10.1167/9.2.20 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; Faculty Opinions Recommendation
- 65. Zhang P, Bao M, Kwon M, et al. : Effects of orientation-specific visual deprivation induced with altered reality. Curr Biol. 2009; 19(22): 1956–60. 10.1016/j.cub.2009.10.018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; Faculty Opinions Recommendation
- 66. Hata W, Motoyoshi I: Bidirectional aftereffects in perceived contrast. J Vis. 2018; 18(9): 12. 10.1167/18.9.12 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67. Rieke F, Rudd ME: The challenges natural images pose for visual adaptation. Neuron. 2009; 64(5): 605–16. 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.11.028 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68. Solomon SG, Peirce JW, Dhruv NT, et al. : Profound Contrast Adaptation Early in the Visual Pathway. Neuron. 2004; 42(1): 155–62. 10.1016/s0896-6273(04)00178-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69. Lutze M, Pokorny J, Smith VC: Achromatic parvocellular contrast gain in normal and color defective observers: Implications for the evolution of color vision. Vis Neurosci. 2006; 23(3–4): 611–6. 10.1017/S0952523806233078 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70. Chang DHF, Hess RF, Mullen KT: Color responses and their adaptation in human superior colliculus and lateral geniculate nucleus. NeuroImage. 2016; 138: 211–20. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.04.067 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71. De Valois RL, De Valois KK: Neural coding of color. In Readings on color, The science of color, A. Byrne and D.R. Hilbert, Editors., MIT Press. 1997; 2: 93–140. Reference Source [Google Scholar]
- 72. Shapley R, Hawken MJ: Color in the cortex: Single- and double-opponent cells. Vision Res. 2011; 51(7): 701–17. 10.1016/j.visres.2011.02.012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73. Kingdom FA, Mullen KT: Separating colour and luminance information in the visual system. Spat Vis. 1995; 9(2): 191–219. 10.1163/156856895x00188 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74. Tregillus KEM, Engel SA: Long-term adaptation to color. Curr Opin Behav Sci. 2019; 30: 116–21. 10.1016/j.cobeha.2019.07.005 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 75. Delahunt PB, Webster MA, Ma L, et al. : Long-term renormalization of chromatic mechanisms following cataract surgery. Vis Neurosci. 2004; 21(3): 301–7. 10.1017/s0952523804213025 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76. Neitz J, Carroll J, Yamauchi Y, et al. : Color Perception Is Mediated by a Plastic Neural Mechanism that Is Adjustable in Adults. Neuron. 2002; 35(4): 783–92. 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)00818-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77. Belmore SC, Shevell SK: Very-long-term and short-term chromatic adaptation: Are their influences cumulative? Vision Res. 2011; 51(3): 362–6. 10.1016/j.visres.2010.11.011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78. Regan BC, Mollon JD: The relative salience of the cardinal axes of colour space in normal and anomalous trichromats. In Colour Vision Deficiencies C.R. Cavonius, Editor, Dordrecht: Kluwer. 1997; 261–270. 10.1007/978-94-011-5408-6_27 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 79. Boehm AE MacLeod DIA, Bosten JM: Compensation for red-green contrast loss in anomalous trichromats. J Vis. 2014; 14(13): 19. 10.1167/14.13.19 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; Faculty Opinions Recommendation
- 80. Paramei GV: Color Space of Normally Sighted and Color-Deficient Observers Reconstructed from Color Naming. Psychol Sci. 1996; 7(5): 311–7. 10.1111/j.1467-9280.1996.tb00380.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 81. Knoblauch K, Marsh-Armstrong B, Werner JS: Suprathreshold contrast response in normal and anomalous trichromats. J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis. 2020; 37(4): A133–A144. 10.1364/JOSAA.380088 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; Faculty Opinions Recommendation
- 82. Mollon JD, Jordan G: On the nature of unique hues. In John Dalton's Colour Vision Legacy, C. Dickenson, I. Maurray, and D. Carden, Editors., Taylor and Francis: London. 1997. Reference Source [Google Scholar]
- 83. Tregillus KE: Color perception in anomalous trichromats: Neuroimaging investigations of neural compensation for losses in spectral sensitivity. (doctoral dissertation). 2017. Reference Source [Google Scholar]
- 84. Rabin J, Kryder A, Lam D: Binocular facilitation of cone-specific visual evoked potentials in colour deficiency. Clin Exp Optom. 2018; 101(1): 69–72. 10.1111/cxo.12567 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; Faculty Opinions Recommendation
- 85. Crognale MA, Switkes E, Rabin J, et al. : Application of the spatiochromatic visual evoked potential to detection of congenital and acquired color-vision deficiencies. J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis. 1993; 10(8): 1818–25. 10.1364/josaa.10.001818 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86. Stockman A: Cone fundamentals and CIE standards. Curr Opin Behav Sci. 2019; 30: 87–93. 10.1016/j.cobeha.2019.06.005 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 87. Georgeson MA, Sullivan GD: Contrast constancy: Deblurring in human vision by spatial frequency channels. J Physiol. 1975; 252(3): 627–56. 10.1113/jphysiol.1975.sp011162 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88. Brady N, Field DJ: What's constant in contrast constancy? The effects of scaling on the perceived contrast of bandpass patterns. Vision Res. 1995; 35(6): 739–56. 10.1016/0042-6989(94)00172-i [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89. Shevell SK, He JC, Kainz P, et al. : Relating color discrimination to photopigment genes in deutan observers. Vision Res. 1998; 38(21): 3371–6. 10.1016/s0042-6989(97)00434-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90. Barbur JL, Rodriguez-Carmona M, Harlow JA, et al. : A study of unusual Rayleigh matches in deutan deficiency. Vis Neurosci. 2008; 25(3): 507–16. 10.1017/S0952523808080619 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91. Romeskie M: Chromatic opponent-response functions of anomalous trichromats. Vision Res. 1978; 18(11): 1521–32. 10.1016/0042-6989(78)90007-x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92. Wachtler T, Doi E, Lee TW, et al. : Cone selectivity derived from the responses of the retinal cone mosaic to natural scenes. J Vis. 2007; 7(8): 6. 10.1167/7.8.6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93. Benson NC, Manning JR, Brainard DH: Unsupervised learning of cone spectral classes from natural images. PLoS Comput Biol. 2014; 10(6): e1003652. 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1003652 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94. Mancuso K, Hauswirth WW, Li Q, et al. : Gene therapy for red-green colour blindness in adult primates. Nature. 2009; 461(7265): 784–7. 10.1038/nature08401 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; Faculty Opinions Recommendation
- 95. Cornelissen FW, Brenner E: Is adding a new class of cones to the retina sufficient to cure color-blindness? J Vis. 2015; 15(13): 22. 10.1167/15.13.22 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96. Gómez-Robledo L, Valero EM, Huertas R, et al. : Do EnChroma glasses improve color vision for colorblind subjects? Opt Express. 2018; 26(22): 28693–703. 10.1364/OE.26.028693 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97. Werner JS, Marsh-Armstrong B, Knoblauch K: Adaptive Changes in Color Vision from Long-Term Filter Usage in Anomalous but Not Normal Trichromacy. Curr Biol. 2020; 20(15): 3011–3015. 10.2139/ssrn.3545957 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; Faculty Opinions Recommendation
- 98. Bavelier D, Green CS, Pouget A, et al. : Brain plasticity through the life span: learning to learn and action video games. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2012; 35: 391–416. 10.1146/annurev-neuro-060909-152832 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99. Levi DM, Li RW: Perceptual learning as a potential treatment for amblyopia: a mini-review. Vision Res. 2009; 49(21): 2535–49. 10.1016/j.visres.2009.02.010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 100. Lu ZL, Yu C, Watanabe T, et al. : Perceptual learning: functions, mechanisms, and applications. Vision Res. 2010; 50(4): 365–7. 10.1016/j.visres.2010.01.010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101. Dosher B, Lu ZL: Visual Perceptual Learning and Models. Annu Rev Vis Sci. 2017; 3: 343–63. 10.1146/annurev-vision-102016-061249 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 102. Lu ZL, Lin Z, Dosher BA: Translating Perceptual Learning from the Laboratory to Applications. Trends Cogn Sci. 2016; 20(8): 561–3. 10.1016/j.tics.2016.05.007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 103. Watanabe T, Sasaki Y: Perceptual learning: toward a comprehensive theory. Annu Rev Psychol. 2015; 66: 197–221. 10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015214 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 104. Li W: Perceptual Learning: Use-Dependent Cortical Plasticity. Annu Rev Vis Sci. 2016; 2: 109–30. 10.1146/annurev-vision-111815-114351 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 105. McGovern DP, Roach NW, Webb BS: Perceptual learning reconfigures the effects of visual adaptation. J Neurosci. 2012; 32(39): 13621–9. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1363-12.2012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 106. Yehezkel O, Sagi D, Sterkin A, et al. : Learning to adapt: Dynamics of readaptation to geometrical distortions. Vision Res. 2010; 50(16): 1550–8. 10.1016/j.visres.2010.05.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; Faculty Opinions Recommendation
- 107. Censor N, Harris H, Sagi D: A dissociation between consolidated perceptual learning and sensory adaptation in vision. Sci Rep. 2016; 6: 38819. 10.1038/srep38819 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 108. Dong X, Bao M: Direction selective habituation of motion adaptation. J Vis. 2019; 19(4): 6. 10.1167/19.4.6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 109. Dong X, Gao Y, Lv L, et al. : Habituation of visual adaptation. Sci Rep. 2016; 6: 19152. 10.1038/srep19152 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 110. Foster DH, Nascimento SM: Relational colour constancy from invariant cone-excitation ratios. Proc Biol Sci. 1994; 257(1349): 115–21. 10.1098/rspb.1994.0103 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 111. Ruderman DL, Cronin TW, Chiao CC: Statistics of cone responses to natural images: Implications for visual coding. J Opt Soc Am A. 1998; 15(8): 2036–2045. 10.1364/JOSAA.15.002036 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 112. Lee TW, Wachtler T, Sejnowski TJ: Color opponency is an efficient representation of spectral properties in natural scenes. Vision Res. 2002; 42(17): 2095–103. 10.1016/s0042-6989(02)00122-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 113. Chaparro A, Stromeyer CF, 3rd, Huang EP, et al. : Colour is what the eye sees best. Nature. 1993; 361(6410): 348–50. 10.1038/361348a0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 114. McDermott KC, Webster MA: The perceptual balance of color. J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis. 2012; 29(2): A108–17. 10.1364/JOSAA.29.00A108 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 115. Asano Y, Fairchild MD, Blondé L: Individual Colorimetric Observer Model. PLoS One. 2016; 11(2): e0145671. 10.1371/journal.pone.0145671 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 116. Webster MA, MacLeod DI: Factors underlying individual differences in the color matches of normal observers. J Opt Soc Am A. 1988; 5(10): 1722–35. 10.1364/josaa.5.001722 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 117. Hofer H, Carroll J, Neitz J, et al. : Organization of the human trichromatic cone mosaic. J Neurosci. 2005; 25(42): 9669–79. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2414-05.2005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; Faculty Opinions Recommendation
- 118. Webster MA: The Verriest Lecture: Adventures in blue and yellow. J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis. 2020; 37(4): V1–V14. 10.1364/JOSAA.383625 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 119. Emery KJ, Webster MA: Individual differences and their implications for color perception. Curr Opin Behav Sci. 2019; 30: 28–33. 10.1016/j.cobeha.2019.05.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 120. Wuerger S: Colour constancy across the life span: evidence for compensatory mechanisms. PLoS One. 2013; 8(5): e63921. 10.1371/journal.pone.0063921 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 121. Werner JS, Schefrin BE: Loci of achromatic points throughout the life span. J Opt Soc Am A. 1993; 10(7): 1509–16. 10.1364/josaa.10.001509 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 122. Webster MA, Leonard D: Adaptation and perceptual norms in color vision. J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis. 2008; 25(11): 2817–25. 10.1364/josaa.25.002817 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 123. Bompas A, Powell G, Sumner P: Systematic biases in adult color perception persist despite lifelong information sufficient to calibrate them. J Vis. 2013; 13(1): 19. 10.1167/13.1.19 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 124. Werner JS, Steele VG: Sensitivity of human foveal color mechanisms throughout the life span. J Opt Soc Am A. 1988; 5(12): 2122–30. 10.1364/josaa.5.002122 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 125. Knoblauch K, Vital-Durand F, Barbur JL: Variation of chromatic sensitivity across the life span. Vision Res. 2001; 41(1): 23–36. 10.1016/s0042-6989(00)00205-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 126. Brainard DH, Roorda A, Yamauchi Y, et al. : Functional consequences of the relative numbers of L and M cones. J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis. 2000; 17(3): 607–14. 10.1364/josaa.17.000607 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 127. Miyahara E, Pokorny J, Smith VC, et al. : Color vision in two observers with highly biased LWS/MWS cone ratios. Vision Res. 1998; 38(4): 601–12. 10.1016/s0042-6989(97)88334-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 128. Jordan G. Mollon JD: Unique hues in heterozygotes for protan and deutan deficiencies. In: Colour Vision Deficiencies XIII, C.R. Cavonius, Editor, Kluwer: Dordrecht. 1997; 67–76. 10.1007/978-94-011-5408-6_6 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 129. Bosten JM, Beer RD, MacLeod DIA: What is white? J Vis. 2015; 15: 5. 10.1167/15.16.5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 130. Walraven J, Werner JS: The invariance of unique white; a possible implication for normalizing cone action spectra. Vision Res. 1991; 31(12): 2185–93. 10.1016/0042-6989(91)90171-z [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 131. Shevell SK, Martin PR: Color opponency: Tutorial. J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis. 2017; 34(7): 1099–108. 10.1364/JOSAA.34.001099 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 132. de Valois RL, de Valois KK: A multi-stage color model. Vision Res. 1993; 33: 1053–65. 10.1016/0042-6989(93)90240-W [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 133. Emery KJ, Volbrecht VJ, Peterzell DH, et al. : Variations in normal color vision. VI. Factors underlying individual differences in hue scaling and their implications for models of color appearance. Vision Res. 2017; 141: 51–65. 10.1016/j.visres.2016.12.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 134. Bosten JM, Boehm AE: Empirical evidence for unique hues? J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis. 2014; 31(4): A385–93. 10.1364/JOSAA.31.00A385 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 135. Wool LE, Komban SJ, Kremkow J, et al. : Salience of unique hues and implications for color theory. J Vis. 2015; 15(2): 10. 10.1167/15.2.10 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 136. Brouwer GJ, Heeger DJ: Decoding and reconstructing color from responses in human visual cortex. J Neurosci. 2009; 29(44): 13992–4003. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3577-09.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; Faculty Opinions Recommendation
- 137. Kim I, Hong SW, Shevell SK, et al. : Neural representations of perceptual color experience in the human ventral visual pathway. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2020; 117(23): 13145–50. 10.1073/pnas.1911041117 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; Faculty Opinions Recommendation
- 138. Lafer-Sousa R, Conway BR: Parallel, multi-stage processing of colors, faces and shapes in macaque inferior temporal cortex. Nat Neurosci. 2013; 16(12): 1870–8. 10.1038/nn.3555 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; Faculty Opinions Recommendation