Skip to main content
Scientific Reports logoLink to Scientific Reports
. 2021 Feb 18;11:4085. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-83125-z

Early protein intake predicts functional connectivity and neurocognition in preterm born children

Emma G Duerden 1,2,, Benjamin Thompson 3,4, Tanya Poppe 5,6,8, Jane Alsweiler 6, Greg Gamble 5, Yannan Jiang 5, Myra Leung 5,6, Anna C Tottman 5, Trecia Wouldes 7, Steven P Miller 2, Jane E Harding 5; PIANO study group
PMCID: PMC7892564  PMID: 33602973

Abstract

Nutritional intake can promote early neonatal brain development in very preterm born neonates (< 32 weeks’ gestation). In a group of 7-year-old very preterm born children followed since birth, we examined whether early nutrient intake in the first weeks of life would be associated with long-term brain function and neurocognitive skills at school age. Children underwent resting-state functional MRI (fMRI), intelligence testing (Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, 5th Ed) and visual-motor processing (Beery-Buktenica, 5th Ed) at 7 years. Relationships were assessed between neonatal macronutrient intakes, functional connectivity strength between thalamic and default mode networks (DMN), and neuro-cognitive function using multivariable regression. Greater functional connectivity strength between thalamic networks and DMN was associated with greater intake of protein in the first week (β = 0.17; 95% CI 0.11, 0.23, p < 0.001) but lower intakes of fat (β = − 0.06; 95% CI − 0.09, − 0.02, p = 0.001) and carbohydrates (β = − 0.03; 95% CI − 0.04, − 0.01, p = 0.003). Connectivity strength was also associated with protein intake during the first month (β = 0.22; 95% CI 0.06, 0.37, p = 0.006). Importantly, greater thalamic-DMN connectivity strength was associated with higher processing speed indices (β = 26.9; 95% CI 4.21, 49.49, p = 0.02) and visual processing scores (β = 9.03; 95% CI 2.27, 15.79, p = 0.009). Optimizing early protein intake may contribute to promoting long-term brain health in preterm-born children.

Subject terms: Outcomes research, Paediatric research, Computational neuroscience, Cognitive neuroscience, Development of the nervous system

Introduction

Very preterm birth (< 32 weeks’ gestational age) remains a significant predictor of morbidity, with a sizable proportion of survivors developing motor difficulties1,2. Neonates born at younger gestational ages are at a higher risk of adverse outcomes3, and the risk is highest in those born < 30 weeks’ gestation4,5. Those neonates who escape major deficits may also go on to have subtle deficits in cognitive ability that are revealed at preschool and school age6,7. Cognitive and motor impairments in children born preterm have been associated with brain dysmaturation, particularly in the prefrontal cortices, which are essential for higher order cognitive functions. Further, longitudinal and cross-sectional neuroimaging studies have reported alterations in brain structural and functional development in infants, children and adolescents born preterm, including alterations in white matter8, decreases in brain volumes and cortical thickness in regionally specific locations in the thalamus, amygdala, hippocampus, frontal, parietal and temporal cortices912, with greater degree of prematurity associated with larger changes13.

Resting-state functional MRI has provided insight into functional connectivity patterns in infants and children born preterm1416. Alterations in structural and intrinsic functional connectivity affecting networks associated with higher-order cognitive functions including default mode networks (DMN) have been reported in school-age children, adolescents and adults born preterm14,17,18. The DMN develops in the third trimester, which is marked by substantial increases in both functional connectivity and development of white matter supporting a globally integrated network of brain regions1921. The DMN is separated into anterior and posterior subdivisions22. The anterior DMN includes portions of the medial and lateral prefrontal cortices as well as the anterior cingulate cortex, and anterior temporal lobe. It is involved in higher order cognitive functions21 and early disruptions in this network may underlie cognitive deficits in children born preterm23,24.

Some evidence suggests that enhanced nutrition may protect brain structural and functional development in children born preterm25,26. For example, protein and energy intake during the first week after birth positively predicted cognitive outcomes at 18 months in a cohort of extremely low birth weight infants27. In another cohort of very preterm born infants who underwent serial MRI scans from birth, total energy, carbohydrates, lipids and protein intake in the first few weeks positively predicted larger brain volumes at term equivalent age as well as cognitive outcomes at 18 months28. Further, compared to preterm infants randomized to a standard diet, those randomized to a high nutrient diet had larger caudate nucleus volumes in adolescence which correlated with higher verbal IQs, particularly in boys29. However, there have been no studies of the relationship between very early macronutrient intakes in very preterm infants and brain connectivity and neurodevelopmental outcomes at school age, when different aspects of brain function can be more reliably assessed.

We examined brain functional connectivity and cognitive and visuomotor ability at 7 years of age in a group of children born very preterm in whom daily actual macronutrient intakes and clinical variables were collected prospectively. During the course of the study, the amount of dietary intake of protein was increased as part of a planned change in clinical care to better meet international recommendations30, as previously described31. We focused on frontal resting state networks, given previous research suggesting structural and functional alterations in the prefrontal cortices underlying cognitive32 and visual perceptual abilities33 in children born preterm. Our a-priori hypotheses were that early neonatal macronutrient intakes would be associated with functional connectivity between the thalamic and default mode networks (DMN) at 7 years, and that functional connectivity would be associated with cognitive ability and visual-motor function.

Methods

Participants

The PIANO Study cohort has been reported previously34. In brief, all infants born at < 30 weeks’ gestation or < 1500 g between July 2005 and October 2008 inclusive and admitted to the neonatal intensive care unit at National Women’s Hospital, Auckland, New Zealand for at least the first postnatal week were eligible for recruitment into a follow-up study at 7 years of age. Of the 128 children assessed at 7 years (28% of original cohort), 113 consented to MRI, 102 completed at least some MRI sequences, and 56 successfully completed resting-state functional MRI sequences. The study was approved by the Northern B ethics committee (NTY/12/05/035) and the Auckland District Health Board (ADHB 5486). A statement to confirm that all methods were carried out in accordance with relevant guidelines and regulations. Informed consent was obtained from a parent and/or legal guardian.

Clinical data were collected from the medical records by trained clinical research staff. All daily actual parenteral and enteral intakes (excluding blood products) were recorded for the first 28 days. Energy intake was computed with proteins and carbohydrates providing 4 kilocalories (kcal) per gram and lipids providing 9 kcal per gram. Breast milk and formula composition was based on commercial notifications34. Intakes per kg were calculated for each day using the most recent highest weight, and averaged for the first 7 days and first 28 days (first month).

Magnetic resonance imaging and analysis

Children underwent anatomical and functional MRI on a 3 T scanner (Siemens, Skyra, Erlangen, Germany). Anatomical images were acquired using an MPRAGE pulse sequence ([repetition time]TR, 2000 ms, [echo time]TE, 3510 ms, [inversion time]TI, 1010 ms, slice thickness, 0.85 mm, FOV, 210 × 210 mm). Resting-state fMRI was acquired using an echo-planar imaging sequence (TR, 3200 ms, TE, 40 ms, FOV, 147 × 147 mm, matrix, 64 × 64, slice thickness, 3 mm).

Resting-state fMRI data were preprocessed using the FMRIB Software Library (FSL; version 5.0.6). Preprocessing included: slice timing and motion correction, spatial smoothing, band-pass filtering (suppressing physiological noise) and whole-brain tissue extraction. Data were nonlinearly registered to a standardized MRI template in MNI (Montreal Neurological Institute) space. Whole-brain resting state connectivity was examined using a group temporal concatenation independent components analysis (ICA), available in FSL using MELODIC (Multivariate Exploratory Linear Decomposition into Independent Components, version 4.0). This produced a set of independent components common to the whole group. All resting-state components were visually inspected. Resulting ICA maps were thresholded at Z ≥ 4. The time courses from the independent components were extracted.

Neuropsychological testing

Children underwent neuropsychological testing by a trained assessor. Full scale IQ (FSIQ) was assessed using the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children 5th Ed (WISC-V) (standard mean score 100, standard deviation [SD] 15)29. Subtests include the perceptual reasoning index (PRI), processing speed index (PSI), working memory index (WMI), and the verbal comprehension index (VCI).

Visuomotor integration was assessed using Beery–Buktenica Developmental Test of Visual–Motor Integration (Beery), 5th edition35, which assesses motor and perceptual skills. The maximum raw score for each of the three sub-tests is 30. The scores were converted to standardized scores.

Statistical analysis

All analyses were carried out using Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (v.25 SPSS, Chicago, IL). The time courses from the thalamic and DMN were extracted. Correlation coefficients describing the connectivity strength between the thalamus and the DMN time courses were calculated for each participant. To assess the relationship between early macronutrient intakes and functional connectivity strength between the subcortical and cortical resting-state networks, we used a general linear model, with the correlation coefficients describing thalamocortical connectivity strength entered as the dependent variables and macronutrient intakes (i.e., protein, carbohydrates, fats, total energy) as independent variables. Analyses were adjusted for gestational age (GA) at birth, birth weight z score, age at scan and biological sex. Birthweight z score reflects growth before birth, and is expressed in standard deviation scores, which are independent of absolute weight. Since we had one a-priori hypothesis (that neonatal macronutrient intake would be associated with functional connectivity between the thalamic and cortical networks at 7 years of age), we set the alpha level at 0.05.

To assess the relationships between functional connectivity strength and 7-year cognitive and visuomotor abilities, we used general linear models, with the correlation coefficients describing the connectivity strength between the thalamic and cortical networks entered as independent variables and the scores on the WISC-V subtests and the Beery entered as dependent variables. Analyses were adjusted for GA at birth, birth weight z score, age at scan and biological sex. Since we had two a-priori hypotheses (that both cognitive and visuomotor scores would be associated with functional connectivity at 7 years), we set the alpha level at 0.025.

Results

Of the total 113 children, 102 children underwent MRI, of whom 13 were unable to complete the resting-state scans, and an additional 33 children moved excessively during the scans. One of these children had grade III/IV IVH as a neonate. Analyzable data were available for 56 children (Table 1). The neonatal macronutrient intake for the cohort is available in Table 2. No significant differences in sex (p = 0.30), adjusted age (t = − 0.9, p = 0.35), GA at birth (t = 0.22, p = 0.08), or birth weight (t = 0.91, p = 0.37) were evident between the children with useable MRI data compared to the children in the full cohort. There were also no differences in neonatal macronutrient intakes between the children with included MRI data and the other children in the full cohort (all p > 0.05).

Table 1.

Participant demographics.

n = 56
Males (%) 26 (46)
Birth GA, weeks, median (IQR) 26 (25–28)
Birth weight, grams, median (IQR) 853 g (770–1030)
IVH grade III/IV (%) 2 (4)
Age, years, median (IQR) 7.5 (7.4–7.6)

Demographics and clinical variables of participants who completed the MRI scans.

GA gestational age, IQR interquartile range, IVH intraventricular hemorrhage.

Table 2.

Neonatal macronutrient intake.

Medians [interquartile ranges]
Macronutrients—first 7 days
Protein, g/kg/day 2.38 [2.1–2.7]
Carbohydrates, g/kg/day 10.71 [9.24–12.19]
Fats, g/kg/day 3.74 [3.02–4.21]
Energy, kcal/kg/day 83.25 [77.49–91.71]
Macronutrients—first 28 days
Protein, g/kg/day 3.41 [3.14–3.53]
Carbohydrates, g/kg/day 14.89 [13.88–15.90]
Fats, g/kg/day 6.19 [5.66–6.52]
Energy, kcal/kg/day 128.94 [119.97–133.82]

Cognitive outcomes

The majority of children performed within the typical range (i.e., 70–130) on the WISC-V (Table 3). GA at birth was related to FSIQ, VCI, PRI, and PSI scores (all, p < 0.05), but sex was not related to any of the WISC-V scores (p > 0.33). No significant differences were noted between the children whose MRI data were or were not included on the WISC-V subtests (all, p > 0.05), except for the PSI (p = 0.04).

Table 3.

Medians and interquartile ranges for the cognitive and visuomotor outcomes.

WISC-V
FSIQ 95 (86–104)
VCI 98 (90–106)
WMI 94 (84–102)
PSI 97 (88–106)
PRI 90 (80–102)
Beery
Visuomotor integration 94 (88–99)
Visual perception 101 (89–115)
Motor ability 92 (83–102)

WISC-V Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, fifth edition, FSIQ full scale intelligence quotient, VCI verbal comprehension index, WMI working memory index, PSI processing speed index, PRI perceptual reasoning index, Beery Beery-Buktenica developmental test of visual-motor integration, 5th edition.

Visuomotor outcomes

Most scores on the Beery were also in the typical range (Table 3). GA was not associated with any of the visuomotor outcome scores (all, p > 0.06), but girls had higher scores than boys (B = 0.21 [females], p = 0.003). No significant differences were noted between the children whose MRI data were or were not included on the Beery (all, p > 0.05).

Resting-state networks

Independent components analysis produced whole-brain functional connectivity maps, with robust resting state activity in anterior default mode (aDMN) and thalamic networks (Fig. 1).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Resting state networks overlaid on a template MRI in the space of the Montreal Neurological Institute.

Greater functional connectivity strength between the thalamic networks and aDMN (range: 0–1) was predicted by greater intake of protein in the first week (β = 0.17; 95% CI 0.11, 0.23, p < 0.001, Fig. 2) but lower intakes of fat (β = − 0.06; 95% CI − 0.09, − 0.02, p = 0.001) and carbohydrates (β = − 0.03; 95% CI − 0.04, − 0.01, p = 0.003). Total energy intake in the first week did not predict thalamocortical connectivity strength (β = -0.002; 95% CI − 0.006, 0.001, p = 0.1). Thalamocortical connectivity strength was also positively associated with increased intake of protein in the first 28 days (β = 0.22; 95% CI 0.06, 0.37, p = 0.006) but not with intakes of fats, carbohydrates or total energy (all p > 0.1).

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Relationship between functional connectivity strength in the thalamic and aDM resting state networks at 7 years (y-axis) and protein intake in the first week after birth (x-axis).

In turn, thalamocortical connectivity strength predicted higher scores for the PSI subtest of the WISC (β = 26.9; 95% CI 4.21, 49.49, p = 0.02, Fig. 3) but not for the WMI (β = 23.46; 95% CI 0.001, 46.9, p = 0.0.5), PRI (β = 23.25; 95% CI − 3.14, 49.64, p = 0.0.8), or VCI subtest scores (β = − 3.1; 95% CI − 29.91, 20.67, p = 0.8) or full-scale IQ (β = 20.2; 95% CI − 3.79, 44.18, p = 0.1).

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Relationship between functional connectivity strength in the thalamic and aDM resting state networks (x-axis) and processing speed index (y-axis) at 7 years.

Thalamocortical connectivity strength was also related to visual perceptual scores on the Beery (β = 9.03; 95% CI 2.27, 15.79, p = 0.009), but not the Beery visuomotor index (β = − 2.39; 95% CI − 6.47, 1.7, p = 0.3) or motor scores (β = 2.52; 95% CI − 2.62, 7.66, p = 0.3).

Discussion

Seven-year-old children born very preterm, who had higher protein intake in the first week and first 28 days after birth positively predicted the strength of the connectivity between subcortical and cortical resting state networks at 7 years of age, and in turn, that this connectivity is associated with specific neurocognitive skills such as processing speed index. Our findings are in line with a growing body of evidence indicating that very early macronutrient intake has long-term effects on preterm brain development and functional outcomes at school age.

Early nutrients and outcomes in preterm infants

Our observation of an association between early protein intake and functional connectivity between the thalamic and the anterior default mode networks at 7 years of age is consistent with previous reports that nutrition is an important predictor of growth and outcome in preterm neonates36. Greater energy and lipid intake during the first two weeks after birth predicted larger subcortical structures, cerebellum, and total brain, and accelerated white matter microstructural maturation over the course of neonatal intensive care to term age28.

Protein intake in the first two weeks also predicted 18-month cognitive and motor outcomes in very low birth weight neonates born preterm37. Stephens et al. reported that a 1 g/kg/day increase in protein intake up to 2.5–3.5 g/kg/day was independently associated with an ~ 8 point increase in cognitive scores at 18 months27. Protein intakes in our study cohort were largely within this range. In contrast to these findings, another report noted that high protein intake (4 g/kg/day) was associated with neurodevelopmental impairments at 2 years in very low birth weight children38. These findings suggest that adequate early protein intake is essential for optimal brain growth, although adverse effects of protein overfeeding cannot be excluded. The specific mechanisms regarding early protein intake and the association with functional brain connectivity may be attributed to regional increases in brain growth, promotion of synaptic connectivity or a combination of macrostructural and functional changes that are supportive of neuroplasticity25.

Previous studies have reported that early intake of fat and energy rather than protein were important predictors of brain maturation16 and later cognitive function28,39. However, other nutrients including protein, choline and zinc are also essential for healthy brain development40. In our study only protein intake positively predicted 7-year thalamocortical connectivity. Protein is essential for cell proliferation and synaptogenesis, which may support developing thalamocortical connectivity41, perhaps by supporting myelination of anterior white matter pathways in the preterm neonatal brain and hence connectivity between thalamic networks and the anterior DMN. Compared with previous studies, our cohort had on average greater energy and fat intakes, and similar or greater protein intakes. Thus, it is possible that protein intake has an additional effect on brain development only if energy intake is adequate. Remarkably, these relationships are seen following nutritional differences even in the first week after birth, emphasising the importance of very early nutrition for optimising long-term outcomes.

Functional connectivity, cognitive and visuomotor abilities

Resting-state networks have been reliably detected in neonates, infants and children born preterm during the first few weeks to years of life42,43. Previous research has demonstrated alterations in thalamic networks and thalamocortical connectivity strength in preterm infants16,44,45 and that thalamocortical connectivity predicted cognitive scores in the second year of life45. Our findings suggest that alterations in thalamocortical connectivity persist beyond the neonatal and early childhood period and have long lasting effects on brain function and specific cognitive skills. Indeed, the association of functional connectivity with visual perceptual abilities is consistent with previous functional neuroimaging studies of school-age children born preterm showing that altered functional connectivity in the prefrontal cortices is associated with visual-perceptual impairments33,46. Our results suggest that alterations in corticothalamic connectivity may contribute to higher order vision deficits associated with preterm birth.

Conclusions

In a cohort of school aged children born very preterm, greater protein intake during the first week and the first month of life were positively associated with more robust functional connectivity strength between thalamic networks and anterior DMN essential for higher order cognitive functioning. Further, greater strength between the thalamocortical networks was associated with processing speed and visual perceptual abilities at 7 years of age. However, the generalizability of findings from this small study requires confirmation. The full cohort recruited at birth were not assessed with MRI at 7 years. The subset of children included in the current study may not be entirely be representative of the full sample. Future studies with larger samples will permit the examination of early nutrient intake on the development of functional connectivity networks, and also the possible effects of social adversity. Early neonatal nutrition, particularly increasing early protein intake in very preterm neonates, may help mitigate the adverse effects of prematurity and neonatal intensive care on brain development.

Acknowledgements

We would like to sincerely thank the families that participated in this research. We would also like to thank the nursing staff and the MRI technologists for making this research possible. Funding for this research was provided by the Health Research Council of New Zealand, Gravida: National Centre for Growth and Development, and the University of Auckland.

Author contributions

B.T., T.P., J.A., Y.J., M.L., A.T., T.W., J.E.H. and G.G. were involved in the study design, database variable creation, test material selection, and data acquisition design and execution of the data analytic strategy, reviewed and/or revised the final version of the manuscript. S.P.M. conceptualized the execution of the data analytic strategy, contributed to the data analysis and reviewed/revised the final draft of the manuscript. E.G.D. contributed to the execution of the data analytic strategy, analyzed the data, prepared all figures and wrote the initial draft of the manuscript. All authors approved the final manuscript as submitted and agree to be accountable for all aspects of the work.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Footnotes

Publisher's note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

A list of authors and their affiliations appears at the end of the paper.

Contributor Information

Emma G. Duerden, Email: eduerden@uwo.ca

PIANO study group:

Jane M. Alsweiler, Janene B. Biggs, Coila Bevan, Joanna M. Black, Frank H. Bloomfield, Kelly Fredell, Greg D. Gamble, Jane E. Harding, Sabine Huth, Yannan Jiang, Christine Kevan, Myra Leung, Geraint Phillips, Tanya Poppe, Jennifer A. Rogers, Heather Stewart, Benjamin S. Thompson, Anna C. Tottman, Kathryn A. Williamson, and Trecia A. Wouldes

References

  • 1.Barfield WD. Public health implications of very preterm birth. Clin. Perinatol. 2018;45:565–577. doi: 10.1016/j.clp.2018.05.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Frey HA, Klebanoff MA. The epidemiology, etiology, and costs of preterm birth. Semin. Fetal Neonatal Med. 2016;21:68–73. doi: 10.1016/j.siny.2015.12.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Glass HC, et al. Outcomes for extremely premature infants. Anesth. Analg. 2015;120:1337–1351. doi: 10.1213/ANE.0000000000000705. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Isaacs EB, Morley R, Lucas A. Early diet and general cognitive outcome at adolescence in children born at or below 30 weeks gestation. J. Pediatr. 2009;155:229–234. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2009.02.030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Wocadlo C, Rieger I. Developmental outcome at 12 months corrected age for infants born less than 30 weeks gestation: Influence of reduced intrauterine and postnatal growth. Early Hum. Dev. 1994;39:127–137. doi: 10.1016/0378-3782(94)90161-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Nosarti C, Froudist-Walsh S. Alterations in development of hippocampal and cortical memory mechanisms following very preterm birth. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 2016;58(Suppl 4):35–45. doi: 10.1111/dmcn.13042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Loe IM, Heller NA, Chatav M. Behavior problems and executive function impairments in preterm compared to full term preschoolers. Early Hum. Dev. 2019;130:87–95. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2019.01.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Cainelli E, Arrigoni F, Vedovelli L. White matter injury and neurodevelopmental disabilities: A cross-disease (dis)connection. Prog. Neurobiol. 2020;193:101845. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2020.101845. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Chau CMY, et al. Hippocampus, amygdala, and thalamus volumes in very preterm children at 8 years: Neonatal pain and genetic variation. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2019 doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2019.00051. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Kelly CE, et al. Brain structural and microstructural alterations associated with cerebral palsy and motor impairments in adolescents born extremely preterm and/or extremely low birthweight. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 2015;57:1168–1175. doi: 10.1111/dmcn.12854. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Bjuland KJ, Løhaugen GC, Martinussen M, Skranes J. Cortical thickness and cognition in very-low-birth-weight late teenagers. Early Hum. Dev. 2013;89:371–380. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2012.12.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Duerden EG, et al. Early procedural pain is associated with regionally-specific alterations in thalamic development in preterm neonates. J. Neurosci. 2018;38:878–886. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0867-17.2017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Lax ID, et al. Neuroanatomical consequences of very preterm birth in middle childhood. Brain Struct. Funct. 2013;218:575–585. doi: 10.1007/s00429-012-0417-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Wehrle FM, et al. Altered resting-state functional connectivity in children and adolescents born very preterm short title. Neuroimage Clin. 2018;20:1148–1156. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2018.10.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Schneider J, et al. Procedural pain and oral glucose in preterm neonates: brain development and sex-specific effects. Pain. 2018;159:515–525. doi: 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000001123. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Duerden EG, et al. White matter injury predicts disrupted functional connectivity and microstructure in very preterm born neonates. Neuroimage Clin. 2019;21:101596. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2018.11.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.White TP, et al. Dysconnectivity of neurocognitive networks at rest in very-preterm born adults. Neuroimage Clin. 2014;4:352–365. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2014.01.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Cui J, et al. Microstructure of the default mode network in preterm infants. AJNR Am. J. Neuroradiol. 2017;38:343–348. doi: 10.3174/ajnr.A4997. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Arichi T, et al. Somatosensory cortical activation identified by functional MRI in preterm and term infants. Neuroimage. 2010;49:2063–2071. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.10.038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Cao M, et al. Early development of functional network segregation revealed by connectomic analysis of the preterm human brain. Cereb. Cortex. 2017;27:1949–1963. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhw038. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Xu Y, et al. Development and emergence of individual variability in the functional connectivity architecture of the preterm human brain. Cereb. Cortex. 2019;29:4208–4222. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhy302. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Damoiseaux JS, et al. Reduced resting-state brain activity in the "default network" in normal aging. Cereb. Cortex. 2008;18:1856–1864. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Gozdas E, et al. Altered functional network connectivity in preterm infants: Antecedents of cognitive and motor impairments? Brain Struct. Funct. 2018;223:3665–3680. doi: 10.1007/s00429-018-1707-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Bäuml JG, et al. The association of children's mathematic abilities with both adults' cognitive abilities and intrinsic fronto-parietal networks is altered in preterm-born individuals. Brain Struct. Funct. 2017;222:799–812. doi: 10.1007/s00429-016-1247-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Keunen K, van Elburg RM, van Bel F, Benders MJ. Impact of nutrition on brain development and its neuroprotective implications following preterm birth. Pediatr. Res. 2015;77:148–155. doi: 10.1038/pr.2014.171. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Coviello C, et al. Effects of early nutrition and growth on brain volumes, white matter microstructure, and neurodevelopmental outcome in preterm newborns. Pediatr. Res. 2018;83:102–110. doi: 10.1038/pr.2017.227. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Stephens BE, et al. First-week protein and energy intakes are associated with 18-month developmental outcomes in extremely low birth weight infants. Pediatrics. 2009;123:1337–1343. doi: 10.1542/peds.2008-0211. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Schneider J, et al. Nutrient intake in the first two weeks of life and brain growth in preterm neonates. Pediatrics. 2018 doi: 10.1542/peds.2017-2169. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Isaacs EB, et al. The effect of early human diet on caudate volumes and IQ. Pediatr. Res. 2008;63:308–314. doi: 10.1203/PDR.0b013e318163a271. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Tsang, R., Uauy, R., Koletzko, B. & Zlotkin, S.E. Nutrition of the Preterm Infant. Scientific Basis and Practical Application, 2 edn. (Digital Publishing Inc, 2005).
  • 31.Tottman AC, et al. Relationships between early neonatal nutrition and neurodevelopment at school age in children born very preterm. J. Pediatr. Gastroenterol. Nutr. 2020;70:72–78. doi: 10.1097/MPG.0000000000002471. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Moiseev A, Doesburg SM, Herdman AT, Ribary U, Grunau RE. Altered network oscillations and functional connectivity dynamics in children born very preterm. Brain Topogr. 2015;28:726–745. doi: 10.1007/s10548-014-0416-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Doesburg SM, Moiseev A, Herdman AT, Ribary U, Grunau RE. Region-specific slowing of alpha oscillations is associated with visual-perceptual abilities in children born very preterm. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2013;7:791. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00791. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Tottman AC, et al. Relationships between early neonatal nutrition and neurodevelopment at school age in children born very preterm. J. Pediatr. Gastroenterol. Nutr. 2019 doi: 10.1097/MPG.0000000000002471. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Beery, K. E., Buktenica, N. A. & Beery, N. A. The Beery–Buktenica Developmental Test of Visual–Motor Integration: Administration, Scoring and Teaching Manual, 5th edn. (NCS Pearson, 2004).
  • 36.Power VA, et al. Nutrition, growth, brain volume, and neurodevelopment in very preterm children. J. Pediatr. 2019 doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2019.08.031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Cormack BE, Bloomfield FH, Dezoete A, Kuschel CA. Does more protein in the first week of life change outcomes for very low birthweight babies? J. Paediatr. Child Health. 2011;47:898–903. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1754.2011.02106.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Barreault S, et al. Impact of early protein and energy intakes on neurodevelopment at 2 years of corrected age in very low birth weight infants: A single-center observational study. PLoS ONE. 2019;14:e0218887. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0218887. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Sammallahti S, et al. Nutrition after preterm birth and adult neurocognitive outcomes. PLoS ONE. 2017;12:e0185632. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0185632. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Cusick SE, Georgieff MK. The role of nutrition in brain development: The golden opportunity of the "first 1000 days". J. Pediatr. 2016;175:16–21. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2016.05.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Georgieff MK. Nutrition and the developing brain: Nutrient priorities and measurement. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2007;85:614S–620S. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/85.2.614S. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Lee W, Morgan BR, Shroff MM, Sled JG, Taylor MJ. The development of regional functional connectivity in preterm infants into early childhood. Neuroradiology. 2013;55(Suppl 2):105–111. doi: 10.1007/s00234-013-1232-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Doria V, Arichi T, Edwards DA. Magnetic resonance imaging of the preterm infant brain. Curr. Pediatr. Rev. 2014;10:48–55. doi: 10.2174/157339631001140408120821. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Toulmin H, et al. Specialization and integration of functional thalamocortical connectivity in the human infant. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A. 2015;112:6485–6490. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1422638112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Ball G, et al. Thalamocortical connectivity predicts cognition in children born preterm. Cereb. Cortex. 2015;25:4310–4318. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhu331. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Hunt, B. A. E. et al. Disrupted visual cortex neurophysiology following very preterm birth. Biol. Psychiatry Cognit. Neurosci. Neuroimaging. 10.1016/j.bpsc.2019.08.012. [DOI] [PubMed]

Articles from Scientific Reports are provided here courtesy of Nature Publishing Group

RESOURCES