Skip to main content
PLOS One logoLink to PLOS One
. 2021 Feb 19;16(2):e0236974. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0236974

Flower consumption, ambient temperature and rainfall modulate drinking behavior in a folivorous-frugivorous arboreal mammal

Óscar M Chaves 1,2,*, Vanessa B Fortes 3, Gabriela P Hass 1, Renata B Azevedo 4, Kathryn E Stoner 5, Júlio César Bicca-Marques 1
Editor: Julie Jeannette Gros-Louis6
PMCID: PMC7894884  PMID: 33606693

Abstract

Water is vital for the survival of any species because of its key role in most physiological processes. However, little is known about the non-food-related water sources exploited by arboreal mammals, the seasonality of their drinking behavior and its potential drivers, including diet composition, temperature, and rainfall. We investigated this subject in 14 wild groups of brown howler monkeys (Alouatta guariba clamitans) inhabiting small, medium, and large Atlantic Forest fragments in southern Brazil. We found a wide variation in the mean rate of drinking among groups (range = 0–16 records/day). Streams (44% of 1,258 records) and treeholes (26%) were the major types of water sources, followed by bromeliads in the canopy (16%), pools (11%), and rivers (3%). The type of source influenced whether howlers used a hand to access the water or not. Drinking tended to be evenly distributed throughout the year, except for a slightly lower number of records in the spring than in the other seasons, but it was unevenly distributed during the day. It increased in the afternoon in all groups, particularly during temperature peaks around 15:00 and 17:00. We found via generalized linear mixed modelling that the daily frequency of drinking was mainly influenced negatively by flower consumption and positively by weekly rainfall and ambient temperature, whereas fragment size and the consumption of fruit and leaves played negligible roles. Overall, we confirm the importance of preformed water in flowers to satisfy the howler’s water needs, whereas the influence of the climatic variables is compatible with the ‘thermoregulation/dehydration-avoiding hypothesis’. In sum, we found that irrespective of habitat characteristics, brown howlers seem to seek a positive water balance by complementing the water present in the diet with drinking water, even when it is associated with a high predation risk in terrestrial sources.

Introduction

Water is an essential chemical substance for all animals, not only because it represents a large percentage of whole-body mass, but because it is the medium within which the chemical reactions and physiological processes of the body take place [13]. This substance is involved in a myriad of vital processes, such as secretion, absorption, and transport of macromolecules (e.g. nutrients, hormones, metabolites, antibodies, and neurotransmitters), electrolyte homeostasis, transmission of light and sound, and thermoregulation [24]. Therefore, water intake is essential for animal health and survival, particularly in the case of terrestrial vertebrates [3, 57].

In all terrestrial mammals, water inputs come from three major sources—water ingested within consumed foods, metabolic water resulting from macronutrient oxidation, and water drunk. Water outputs result from excretion, egestion, or evaporation through the skin or the respiratory tract [4, 5, 8]. When water intake is appropriate, healthy animals maintain a physiological state in which water inputs and outputs are the same throughout the day (i.e. the ‘water balance’), an essential condition for the correct functioning of body cells [2, 5]. Animals reach this water and electrolyte homeostasis by applying a repertoire of behavioral and physiological strategies that depend on the organism’s complexity and the surrounding environment [2]. Whereas drinking increases water input, shade seeking, low metabolic rates, and the excretion of salt by the kidney reduce water loss [1, 2, 4]. Dehydration (i.e. a negative water balance) resulting from long periods of adverse dry conditions when water losses exceed water intake can seriously compromise health, being lethal when losses reach 15 to 25% of body weight (camels are an exception [2, 4]).

Given that plant items contain more water than animal bodies, herbivorous mammals are expected to obtain a larger volume of water from their diets than do omnivores and carnivores [8]. However, plant items can show wide intraspecific and seasonal variations in chemical composition that influence their importance and reliability as water sources, thereby influencing the animals’ need to drink [9]. Herbivorous mammals inhabiting dry environments, such as desert rodents and camelids, can reach water balance by relying on preformed (i.e. water in plant items) and oxidation (i.e. metabolic water resulting from macronutrient oxidation) water during dry periods [2, 10]. In addition to these water sources, animals inhabiting wetter environments also rely on another major source, drinking water [2, 7, 11]. Drinking is rare (e.g. giraffe, Giraffa camelopardalis) or presumably nonexistent in mammals that rely on succulent diets [2]. Arboreal folivores once believed to obtain all their water demands from food have been reported to drink either in captivity (sloth, Choloepus hoffmanii [12]) or in the wild (koala, Phascolarctos cinereus [13, 14]).

While ground-living species drink water from rarely-depletable sources (e.g. rivers, streams, and lagoons), highly arboreal mammals depend on depletable arboreal reservoirs, such as bromeliads and treeholes (primates [1518]), or on short lasting rain water on tree branches and leaves (koalas [14], sloths [19]). The exploitation of terrestrial water reservoirs by the latter tends to be rare because their vulnerability to predators likely increases when they descend to the forest floor, as has been observed for tropical primates [15, 2023].

Among the highly arboreal Neotropical primates, reports of drinking are restricted to a few social groups of the better-studied taxa, including howler monkeys (Alouatta spp. [6, 15, 17, 22, 2426], spider monkeys (Ateles geoffroyi [27]), capuchin monkeys (Cebus capucinus [28], Sapajus libidinosus [29]), and marmosets (Callithrix flaviceps [11]). These monkeys meet their water needs primarily via preformed water [15, 30], although they also drink from arboreal reservoirs or, to a lesser extent, terrestrial sources [1517, 20].

Two main non-exclusive hypotheses have been proposed to explain the drinking behavior of howler monkeys. The thermoregulatory/dehydration-avoidance hypothesis (TDH) relates drinking to a behavioral strategy for maintaining a positive water balance during the hottest and driest periods of the year [6, 17, 26]. The metabolite detoxification hypothesis (MDH) states that the consumption of large amounts of some plant parts (e.g. mature leaves, branches, and seeds) containing digestion inhibitors (fiber and secondary metabolites) ‘forces’ monkeys to drink more to help in their processing [15, 17, 20, 26]. The trend of anti-herbivory metabolites to increase in plants with increasing latitude [31] further supports the potential relevance of the MDH to howler monkeys living in southern latitudes (e.g. Alouatta guariba clamitans and A. caraya). The bacterial fermentation of the leaf-rich diet of howlers also requires an appropriate water supply [32], as does the excretion of the higher salt content of leaves [8].

Howlers’ low rates of digestion [32] together with the cumulative water loss via urine, lung evaporation, and sweat over the course of an activity period (i.e. daytime), especially during more active and hot times, and under low air humidity [33], can increase plasma osmolarity and cell dehydration to levels that cause thirst and create circadian rhythms of drinking [2, 30]. Similar drinking rhythms associated with feeding are found in squirrel monkeys (Saimiri sp. [34]) and owl monkeys (Aotus sp. [35]). Finally, forests inhabited by howler monkeys also show seasonal and site-related differences in thermal environment [36], food availability [6, 37], and the presence and reliability of water sources. Therefore, it is important to identify the factors that modulate their drinking behavior to better understand how habitat patch size and spatial restriction resulting from land use changes can affect their health and survival.

Here we investigate the drinking behavior in wild groups of brown howler monkeys (A. guariba clamitans) inhabiting Atlantic Forest fragments in southern Brazil as models of folivorous-frugivorous arboreal mammals. Specifically, we assess (i) the arboreal and terrestrial water sources that these monkeys exploit and how they drink, (ii) the daily frequency and seasonal distribution of drinking records, and (iii) the influence of fragment size, season, ambient temperature, rainfall, and the contribution of fruits, leaves, and flowers to the diet on drinking. We predicted that brown howlers would complement the preformed water obtained from their diet with water from arboreal and terrestrial reservoirs, if available, because the availability of fleshy fruits, flowers and young leaves vary seasonally in the study region [37]. We also predicted a within-day gradual increase in drinking in the afternoon in response to an increase in water demands resulting from temperature rise throughout the day and the daily water loss via digestion, excretion, breathing, and sweating [1, 8]. Finally, we predicted that diet composition, climatic variables, and fragment size influence the frequency of drinking. While the TDH will receive support if ambient temperature and rainfall are good predictors of the frequency of drinking, a positive influence of leaf ingestion on water consumption will support the MDH.

Methods

This investigation followed the ethical guidelines of the International Primatological Society and the legal requirements established by the Ethical Committee of the Zoological Society of London for research with nonhuman primates. All studies met all Brazilian animal care policies and were strictly observational. Furthermore, studies conducted from 2011 to 2019 were approved by the Scientific Committee of the Faculty of Biosciences of the Pontifical Catholic University of Rio Grande do Sul (projects SIPESQ #5933 and 7843).

Study fragments and groups

We collected data on 14 groups of wild brown howlers inhabiting Atlantic Forest fragments ranging from 1 to 977 ha in the municipalities of Porto Alegre, Viamão, and Santa Maria in the State of Rio Grande do Sul, southern Brazil (Table 1, Fig 1). We classified the fragments in three size categories: small (<1 to 10 ha), medium (>10 to 100 ha) and large (>100 to 1,000 ha; sensu [38]). Small and medium fragments in Porto Alegre (S1-S3 and M1) and Viamão (S4-S6; Fig 1) were surrounded by anthropogenic matrices comprised of small human settlements, pastures, subsistence orchards, and small parcels of cultivated land (<0.5 to 2 ha). None of them are officially protected. Conversely, the large fragments in Porto Alegre and Viamão (L1-L3) are found in legally protected areas (Fig 1, see [37] for further information on these fragments). The Atlantic Forest study fragments in Santa Maria (S7, M2, L4, and L5; <1 to 977 ha; Fig 1) compose a 5,876-ha mosaic of natural grasslands, extensive pastures devoted to cattle ranching, and other scattered forest fragments. This area, named Campo de Instrução de Santa Maria (CISM), belongs to the Brazilian Army. Therefore, although it is not officially protected by Brazilian laws, CISM is impacted by a lower human pressure than the unprotected study sites.

Table 1. Study fragments, brown howler group size, sampling effort, and number of drinking records.

Site Size Latitude Longitude WSa Group sizeb Sampling effort #rec.d
Months Daysc Hours
S1 1.6 S30°11’ 00.1” W51°06’ 06.6” P,B 7 (1,2) 21 67 (4) 492 4
S2 9.5 S30°12’ 18.4” W51°06’ 05.7” R,B 11 (1,3) 19 61 (13) 438 47
S3 2.3 S30°12’ 26.6” W51°05’ 54.0” S,P,B 10 (1,3) 20 65 (12) 539 28
S4 3.6 S30°12’ 27.0” W50°55’39.0” B,T 8 (1,2) 12 56 (33) 681 90
S5 5.2 S30°17’27.0” W50°57’36.0” B,T 4 (1,2) 12 55 (14) 663 27
S6 7.3 S30°17’39.0” W51°00’42.0” B,T 8 (1,2) 12 69 (9) 826 13
S7 1 S29°47’05.8" W53°53’12.0" S,B 7 (1,4) 12 59 (44) 654 322
M1 27 S30°12’ 00.0” W51°04’00.0” P,B 12 (2,2) 12 57 (34) 518 173
M2 17 S29°45’21.3" W53°52’32.2" S,B 6 (1,2) 12 58 (26) 623 99
L1 108 S30°10’ 39.5” W51°06’ 18.2” R,B,S 9 (2,3) 21 73 (6) 536 18
L2 93 S30°23’ 15.6” W51°02’ 43.3” L,P,B 12 (3,3) 18 81 (7) 460 10
L3 106 S30°20’ 56.8” W51°02’ 58.2” L,P,B 10 (2,3) 17 87 (27) 536 102
L4 977 S29°46’46.0" W53°51’52.0" S,B 5 (2,3) 12 54 (45) 577 184
L5 977 S29°47’05.9" W53°53’03.0" S,B 7 (2,3) 12 67 (39) 836 144
Sum 116 212 909 (313) 8379 1261

a Water sources detected during the study period: Bromeliads (B), treeholes (T), pools (P), rivers (R), streams (S), and lagoon (L).

b Group size and number of adult males and females (in parentheses).

c Number of study days with drinking events in parentheses.

d Total number of drinking records per study group.

Fig 1. Location of the 14 study sites in the municipalities of Santa Maria (SM, red polygon), Porto Alegre (PA, purple polygon), and Viamão (Vi, cyan polygon), southern Brazil.

Fig 1

Color markers indicate the exact location of the small (white), medium (rose), and large (cyan) Atlantic forest fragments inhabited by the study groups. Lansat7 open-access images (available at http://earthexplorer.usgs.gov/) from 2008 for SM and 2013 for PA and Vi.

The predominant vegetation in all study fragments is subtropical semideciduous forest. Despite differences in size, all fragments have a similar vegetation structure with a canopy often <30-m tall and a relatively open understory allowing clear observations of monkeys in the canopy. Given its latitude (30°-31°S), the region is characterized by marked climatic seasonality: summer (21 December-20 March), fall (20 March-21 June), winter (21 June-22 September), and spring (22 September-21 December). According to meteorological records of Porto Alegre, the average annual ambient temperature during the study period was 21°C [39]. The highest temperatures occurred in the summer (mean = 26°C, range = 19°-35°C), and the lowest in the winter (mean = 15°C, range = 3°-26°C; S1 and S2 Figs in S1 File). The average annual rainfall during the study years was 1,450 mm. There was no clear rainfall pattern between months or seasons in Porto Alegre, Viamão or Santa Maria (S1 and S2 Figs in S1 File).

Despite the variation in fragment size, all fragments contained fleshy fruit tree species (i.e. Ficus spp., Eugenia spp., and Syagrus romanzoffiana) intensively exploited by brown howlers [37, 40, 41]. All study fragments had arboreal and terrestrial water reservoirs, such as bromeliads, streams, and/or rivers (Table 1).

We followed brown howler groups ranging from 4 to 12 individuals in each fragment (n = 116 individuals, Table 1). All individuals of the study groups in small fragments were well-habituated to humans before the study, while we habituated the individuals of the study groups inhabiting medium and large fragments during two to three months prior to their respective monitoring. We followed Williamson & Feistner’s [42] recommendations for the habituation of free-ranging primates. Whenever possible, we kept a distance of 15 to 30 m from the projected position of the animals on the ground, and we wore green or olive clothes. Additionally, the same observer (OMC, VBF, GPH, or RBA) habituated and followed the study group(s) during each study period in each fragment. Whereas most groups inhabited a single forest fragment, S1, S2, and S7 (hereafter named by the acronym of their respective fragments) used more fragments. S1 ranged outside of its most used fragment for about 35% of the study days to feed in a neighboring 10-ha fragment. S2 regularly used three forest fragments distant about 70 to 850 m from each other (the home range of this group included the area of these three fragments). Lastly, S7 also used three forest remnants distant from 30 to 40 m from each other.

Behavioral data collection

We studied the diet and drinking behavior of the groups during periods ranging from 12 to 21 months (Table 1): (i) January to December 1996 (L5), (ii) June 2002 to August 2003 (M1), (iii) January to December 2005 (S7, M2, and L4), (iv) June 2011 to June 2014 (S1, S2, S3, L1, L2, and L3), and (v) June 2018 to July 2019 (S4, S5, and S6). We collected data for all groups from dawn to dusk using high-resolution 10 x 42 binoculars. We monitored the groups on a monthly basis during three to eight consecutive days in periods (i), (ii), and (iii), during four to five consecutive days on a bimonthly basis in period (iv), and during four to nine consecutive days on a monthly basis in period (v). We recorded the behavior (including feeding and drinking) of these groups using the instantaneous scan sampling method in periods (i) to (iv) and the ‘all occurrences’ method [43] to record all drinking events (i.e. when one or more members of the study group drank) that occurred outside scan sampling units. We collected 5-min scan samples at 15-min intervals. Finally, we used the focal-animal method [43] to record the behavior (including feeding and drinking) and complemented the recording of all events of drinking by any group member also using the ‘all occurrences’ method. We recorded the behavior of the focal howler instantaneously at 20-s intervals during 5-min focal sampling units every 15 min. A single focal adult was monitored per group during each study period in order to fulfill the primary goal of the respective research.

We recorded the feeding and drinking behavior of adults, subadults, and conspicuous juvenile individuals, except for S4, S5, and S6, of which we only recorded the behavior of adults. We assumed that our diet composition datasets based on two sampling methods are comparable because the within-group feeding behavior of howlers is very similar among age-sex classes [4446].

During feeding bouts we recorded the main plant items eaten (i.e. ripe and unripe fruits, adult and young leaves, and flowers) and, whenever possible, the plant species (see [37] for additional details). When a howler drank water, we recorded the individual’s identity, the type of arboreal or terrestrial water source (i.e. bromeliad, treehole, river, stream, or pool), and the water acquisition method (e.g. inserting the head in the treehole and drinking the water directly with the mouth or inserting a hand in the water and licking the dripping water). Whenever possible, we also identified the bromeliad source of water at the species level. We used the number of drinking records (i.e. the total number of records devoted to drinking per study day) and the number of feeding records devoted to each plant item in the analyses.

Climatic data

We obtained data on ambient temperature and rainfall for the three study municipalities from the meteorological database of the Instituto Nacional de Meteorologia do Brasil [39]. For each day with a record of drinking, we used the meteorological data for ambient temperature for that day and the weekly rainfall (i.e. the rainfall accumulated during the previous seven days) as proxies for the thermal environment and the amount of rainfall water potentially available for brown howlers. Furthermore, we recorded the hourly variation in in-site ambient temperature throughout the day during periods (iv) and (v) to determine daily peaks. We measured the temperature after each scan or focal sampling unit in the shade at a height of ca. 2 m above the ground using a pocket thermo-hygrometer (Yi Chun®, PTH 338) during period (iv) and a portable meteorological station (Nexus, model 351075) distant from 0.4 to 2.6 km from the study fragments during period (v). We used the data on ambient temperature as a predictor variable in our modelling and the hourly variations in temperature to prepare S5 Fig in S1 File.

Statistical analyses

We performed Chi-square tests for proportions at the group level to compare the proportions of drinking records per water source and season in each study group using the ‘prop.test’ function of R. We calculated these proportions by dividing the number of records for each water source or season (i.e. summer, fall, winter, spring) by the total number of records for each group during the entire study period. We did not perform between-fragment comparisons because of sampling effort differences (i.e. the number of sampling months, days, or days per month varied between the five study periods, Table 1). We used the same procedure above to calculate and compare the proportion of drinking records in each hour of the day in those fragments with >10 drinking records. When we found significant differences, we compared the proportion of records in each class using post-hoc proportion contrasts via the R function ‘pairwise.prop.test’ with a Bonferroni correction because of multiple comparisons of the same data sets.

We performed a generalized linear mixed-effects model (GLMM) to assess the influence of six predictor variables—contribution of fruits, leaves, and flowers to the diet, fragment size (i.e. fragment area in hectares), ambient temperature, and weekly rainfall—on the total number of drinking records recorded from dawn to dusk using the function ‘lmer’ of the R package lme4. Although the number of drinking records was not correlated with group size (rs = 0.03, P = 0.53), we used the latter as a covariable in the model to control for any potential influence of group size differences (Table 1) on the response variable. Similarly, we used ‘sampling method’ (instantaneous scan and focal animal) as a covariable. We set the Poisson error family for the response variable as recommended for count data [47, 48]. We performed the overdispersion diagnostic via the R package DHARMa. Even when data are overdispersed (overdispersion ratio ɷ = 1.4, P = 0.03), statistical adjustments (e.g. set a negative binomial distribution) are indispensable mainly when ɷ≥2 [48]. We specified group ID as a random factor to account for repeated-measures from the same groups. We did not consider interactions between predictor variables to minimize overparameterization and problems of convergence of the global model (i.e. the model containing all fixed and random factors [49]). We standardized variable scales using the ’stdize’ function of the R package MuMIn [50]. Additionally, we found no multicollinearity problem between variables using the ‘vifstep’ function of R package dplyr [51], as all of them had Variance Inflation Factor (VIF) <3 [52]. The correlation matrix between diet variables (rs ranged from 0.01 to -0.21) is available in S3 Fig in S1 File. Therefore, we included all variables in the global GLMM model.

We used the Akaike’s Information Criterion for small samples (AICc) to select the models that best explain the effects of the predictor variables on drinking behavior. According to this criterion, the model with the strongest empirical support is the one with the smallest difference in AICc [53]. However, given that all models with ΔAICc<2 are considered equally parsimonious, we used the full-model averaging framework to determine which parameters best predict the number of drinking records while accounting for model uncertainty [49]. We used the ’dredge’ function of the package MuMIn [50] to generate a full submodel set from the global model and the ‘model.avg’ function of the same package to determine the averaged model and the relative importance of each variable or predictor weight (∑wi). We used a likelihood ratio test over the function ‘anova’ to test the significance of the averaged model compared with the model including only the random factor (i.e. null model). We used the ’r.squaredGLMM’ function of the package MuMIn to estimate an equivalent of the coefficient of determination or pseudo-R2 for each competing best GLMM model. All statistical analyses were run in R v.3.6.3 [54] and the statistical significance threshold was set at P≤0.05.

Results

Water sources

We obtained a total of 1,261 individual drinking records (range = 4–322 records/group, Table 1) distributed in 917 drinking events and 313 observation days (range = 0–16 records/day, Table 1). We did not record drinking in 66% of the study days (i.e. 596 out of 909 days). The water sources were streams (44% of 1,258 records), followed by treeholes (26%), Vriesea, Aechmea, and Tillandsia bromeliads (16%), pools (11%), and rivers (3%) (Fig 2a). The proportion of drinking records per water source type differed in nine of the 14 groups (X2 tests, P<0.05 in all significant cases, Fig 2a). Arboreal sources were exploited by most groups (treeholes = 12, bromeliads = 11), whereas terrestrial ones were less common (streams = 6, pools = 4, rivers = 2; Fig 2a).

Fig 2. Percentage of drinking records in 14 brown howler groups per water source (a) and season (b).

Fig 2

Different lower-case letters in the middle of each bar indicate significant differences in the number of records. Asterisks on the bars indicate the significance level according to Chi-square tests for proportions: *P≤0.05, **P<0.01, ***P<0.001. The proportion of records at the forest size level (small = S, medium = M, and large = L) and in the pooled dataset (All) is indicated in the four bars to the right. Water sources—rivers: Permanent water currents >4-m in width and >1-m in depth; streams: Seasonal water currents <2-m in width and <1-m in depth; treeholes: 10-40-cm diameter holes in trunks or large branches; bromeliads: Water stored in the rosette of epiphytic bromeliads. Significant differences in the proportion of records between water sources or seasons within each fragment are indicated with different lower-case letters in the bars. When proportion contrasts tests did not detect differences, no letter is shown. N = 1,128 records in (Fig 2a) and 1,131 records in (Fig 2b).

The most common drinking behavior consisted of inserting their head and sipping water directly from bromeliads and treeholes. When the treehole had a small diameter, monkeys immersed a cupped hand into the hole, pulled it out, and placed the mouth under the fingers to lick the dripping water. Vigilance was negligible during these arboreal drinking events.

In contrast, when drinking from terrestrial sources (rivers, streams and pools) howlers scanned the surroundings very carefully and were highly vigilant when drinking. Terrestrial drinking events began with some group members moving slowly to the understory, where they remained vigilant for ca. 30 s to 5 min before one or two of them descended to the ground to drink directly from the terrestrial water source for 102 ± 66 s (mean ± S.D., n = 463) while the other individuals waited in vigilance in the understory. When the first individuals climbed back to the understory, the others descended slowly to the ground to drink, and the first remained in vigilance. Overall, a drinking event involved between one-fifth and four-fifths of the group members.

Seasonal and daily patterns in drinking behavior

We found no clear pattern in the proportion of drinking records documented between or within seasons (Fig 2b, S4 Fig in S1 File). We observed drinking in all seasons in seven fragments, in three seasons in six fragments, and in two seasons in the remaining fragment (Fig 2b). We found seasonal differences in the proportion of drinking records in 11 out of 14 groups (proportion contrasts, P<0.05 in all significant cases, Fig 2b). A greater proportion of records from these groups occurred in a single season (winter—n = 3 fragments: S3, S5, and L2; summer—n = 2 fragments: S7 and L1; fall—n = 2 fragments: M1, L3), in two seasons (n = 1 fragment: S4) or in three (n = 3 fragments: S2, M2, and L5; Fig 2b). We found a lower percentage of drinking records in the spring than in the other seasons when analyzing all groups together (X2 = 77, df = 3, P<0.0001; Fig 2b).

Finally, we found that the distribution of drinking during the day showed a unimodal pattern in most fragments. The higher percentages of records occurred in the afternoon, particularly from 15:00 to 17:00 (8 out of 12 analyzed fragments; proportion contrasts, P<0.05 in all significant cases, Fig 3). This peak of drinking occurred near times with higher ambient temperatures in the fragments for which we have in-site temperature data (n = 7; S5 Fig in S1 File).

Fig 3. Variation in percentage of drinking records by brown howler monkeys during the day in small, medium, and large Atlantic Forest fragments.

Fig 3

Different lower-case letters on the bars indicate significant differences after Bonferroni adjustment in P values. The absence of letters indicates that these hour intervals did not differ from the others. The number of degrees of freedom was 12 in all cases. Only fragments with >10 drinking records were considered in this analysis.

Factors driving the drinking behavior of brown howlers

We found nine models with substantial empirical support (i.e. ΔAICc<2). Together, these models included all predictor variables, except group size (Table 2). Flower consumption was the only predictor present in all models. The ‘best model’ for explaining the frequency of drinking also included ambient temperature, weekly rainfall, fruit consumption and sampling method, while the second ‘best model’ included the same variables, except fruit consumption and sampling method (Table 2).

Table 2. Best supported GLMM models (ΔAICc<2) and model-averaged that predict the variation in the number of drinking records in 14 brown howler groups in southern Brazil.

Predictor variables Parametersa
Best supported models
AICc ΔAICc wi R2c
1) Flower+Fruit+Rainfall+Temp+Method 1454.30 0.00 0.18 0.38
2) Flower+Rainfall+Temp 1454.34 0.04 0.18 0.40
3) Flower+Temp+Method 1455.18 0.89 0.12 0.39
4) Flower+Leaf+Rainfall+Temp +Method 1455.31 1.01 0.11 0.40
5) Flower+Fruit+Temp+Method 1455.50 1.21 0.10 0.37
6) Flower+Rainfall+Method 1455.74 1.44 0.09 0.40
7) Flower+Fruit+Leaf+Rainfall+Temp+Method 1455.91 1.61 0.08 0.38
8) Flower+Rainfall+Temp 1455.94 1.64 0.08 0.43
9) Flower+Rainfall+Temp+Fsize 1456.28 1.99 0.07 0.41
Averaged model (R2c = 0.40)
βi SE 95% CI wi
Flower consumption (Flower) -0.19 0.07 (-0.32, -0.05) 0.98
Ambient temperature (Temp) 0.12 0.07 (0.00, 0.25) 0.86
Weekly rainfall (Rainfall) -0.09 0.07 (-0.24, 0.01) 0.79
Feeding behavior sampling method (Method)
 Focal sampling -0.54 0.30 (-1.11, 0.06) 0.74
Fruit consumption (Fruit) -0.04 0.08 (-0.29, 0.05) 0.37
Leaf consumption (Leaf) 0.02 0.05 (-0.09, 0.25) 0.28
Fragment size (Fsize) 0.01 0.08 (-0.44, 0.65) 0.18

aAkaike’s Information Criterion for small samples (AICc), difference in AICc (ΔAICc), model probability Akaike weights (wi), Pseudo-R2 (R2c) indicating the percent of variance explained by the fixed and random factors, partial regression coefficients of the model-averaged (β), standard errors which incorporate model uncertainty (SE), 95% confidence intervals for the parameter estimates (95% CI), and relative importance of each predictor variable (∑wi) based in models with ΔAICc<4. The three predictor variables with higher contribution to the averaged model are enhanced in bold.

The averaged model differed from the null model (likelihood ratio test: X2 = 17, df = 6, P<0.01) and explained 40% of the variation in the number of drinking records. The three variables with higher predictive power in this model were flower consumption (β = -0.19, ∑wi = 0.97), ambient temperature (β = 0.12, ∑wi = 0.86), and weekly rainfall (β = -0.09, ∑wi = 0.79; Table 2). Finally, the focal-animal behavioral sampling method showed a negative influence on the number of drinking records, while the predicting importance of the other variables was minor (Table 2).

Discussion

We found that brown howlers drank water accumulated in bromeliads and treeholes in the canopy, and that they also descended to the ground to drink from streams, rivers, and pools. Drinking increased in the afternoon and was less frequent in the spring. Also, while flower consumption and climatic conditions were good predictors of drinking behavior, the relevance of fruit and leaf consumption and habitat size was negligible.

The exploitation of non-food arboreal and terrestrial water reservoirs supports our expectation that oxidation and preformed water are insufficient for permanently satisfying howlers’ water needs, as reported for many terrestrial mammals [13]. The finding that streams were the most used water sources by brown howlers differs from the greater importance of arboreal water reservoirs for other howler monkeys inhabiting both large (e.g. 1,564 ha in Barro Colorado Island, Panama [15, 30]) and small forest remnants (e.g. ≤10 ha [26, 55]).

This use of terrestrial water sources occurred despite the high risk of predation by domestic/stray dogs and small wild felids in the study region (e.g. Leopardus wiedii [56]; also OMC, personal observation). The fact that dog attacks represent a major cause of brown howler death in urban and suburban populations in southern Brazil [23, 57] explains the highly cautious behavior and vigilance displayed by brown howlers when descending to the ground to drink, a behavior also observed in other primates (e.g. Callithrix flaviceps [11]). This threat is believed to reduce (or even eliminate) howlers’ use of terrestrial water reservoirs in better-conserved large forests inhabited by wild carnivore populations in Central America (e.g. A. palliata [30, 58]). The frequency of brown howler remains in ocelot (Leopardus pardalis) scats in a ca. 950-ha Atlantic Forest reserve in southeastern Brazil highlights their vulnerability to wild felids [21].

The lower drinking in the spring and the overall positive influence of temperature on drinking behavior are likely explained, at least partially, by three main reasons. First, unlike at lower tropical latitudes where the hottest and driest times often coincide (i.e. dry season [59]), summer and spring are the hottest, but not the driest seasons in this subtropical study region (ca. 31°S, S1 Fig in S1 File). In fact, rainfall is relatively well distributed throughout the year in Rio Grande do Sul state ([39], see also S1 and S2 Figs in S1 File), where ‘rainy quarters’ occur at any time [60]. Second, the higher availability and consumption of flowers and other water-richer/lower-secondary metabolite food items, such as young leaves [61] and some brown howler’s preferred fruits (e.g. Ficus spp. and Allophylus edulis), and the consequent lower consumption of mature leaves, by the study groups also occurred in the hottest seasons [37]. This diet composition likely reduces the need for water to detoxify secondary metabolites in leafy material (hypothesis not supported in this study) while supplying water to counterbalance the losses of thermoregulation and many other vertebrate physiological processes (see [2, 4]). Therefore, the existence of a strong negative relationship between flower consumption and drinking behavior in brown howlers is not surprising. Flowers exploited by primates can have high water contents [62] that contribute to satisfy the animals’ daily requirements as has been reported in some Neotropical primates (e.g. Cebus imitator [63]).

Finally, brown howlers may lower the thermoregulatory demands for water by preventing body over-heating and dehydration via positional adjustments and shade-seeking [64] during the hottest times of the day (strategies also reported in other Neotropical primates: A. palliata [65], A. caraya [36], C. imitator [66], Callicebus bernhardi [67]). Despite these strategies, the peaks of drinking in the afternoon tended to occur around the warmer times of the day (S5 Fig in S1 File), which are likely triggered by the need of water in this period of intensified physiological thermoregulation together with the recovery of the water spent earlier in the day (particularly during early and middle morning, when brown howlers are more active: OMC and GPH, personal observation) that is required to reach the homeostasis of blood osmolarity [34, 35]. Therefore, we found support for the TDH hypothesis. Future studies noninvasively assessing individuals’ body temperature and water balance together with behavioral and climatological data are required to confirm that the drinking behavior of brown howlers is better explained by the thermoregulatory/dehydration-avoidance hypothesis than the metabolite detoxification hypothesis.

In sum, we found that the drinking behavior of brown howlers responded to changes in the consumption of flowers, rainfall and the thermal environment. Extrapolating from brown howlers to arboreal folivorous-frugivorous mammals in general that also lack adaptations to tolerate high levels of dehydration, we suggest that the higher the ambient temperature and lower the availability of water-rich plant items, the greater might be the challenges in fulfilling their water requirements, particularly in habitats where terrestrial water reservoirs are scarce, strongly seasonal or absent, such as small and medium forest fragments. Despite the higher availability of leaves than flowers and fruits in forests, highly folivorous mammals may also be more vulnerable to predators if they are forced to descend to the ground to drink from terrestrial reservoirs, particularly in forest fragments immersed in anthropogenic landscapes (a growing scenario in the tropics [68]), where dogs roam freely. In this respect, studies assessing how differences in land-use and human disturbance influence the abundance and distribution of arboreal and terrestrial water reservoirs and how they impact the drinking behavior, water balance, and health of arboreal folivorous-frugivorous mammals are critical for enabling us to design and implement appropriate management strategies for promoting their conservation in anthropogenic fragmented landscapes.

Supporting information

S1 File

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

We thank Danielle Camaratta and João Claudio Godoy for logistical support and field assistance. We thank the landowners of the study fragments in Porto Alegre and Viamão for giving us permission to conduct this research on their properties. We thank Commandant Aluísio S.R. Filho for giving us permission to work in the Campo de Instrução de Santa Maria (CISM).

Data Availability

All relevant data needed to replicate the analyses are within the paper and its Supporting information files. Additional datasets used to perform the main statistical analysis are available in Mendeley Data (http://dx.doi.org/10.17632/3gxy6vrsbf.1) and Dryad Digital Repository (https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.hdr7sqvh7).

Funding Statement

This study was supported by a grant from the Programa Nacional de Pós-Doutorado of the Brazilian Higher Education Authority/CAPES (PNPD grant # 2755/2010). OMC was supported by a PNPD postdoctoral fellowship. JCBM thanks the Brazilian National Research Council/CNPq for research fellowships (PQ#303306/2013-0 and 304475/2018-1). GPH was supported by a doctoral fellowship from CNPq (GD#140641/2016-5). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

  • 1.Nagy KA, Peterson CC. Scaling of water flux rate in animals. London: University of California Press, 1988. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Fitzsimons J. Evolution of physiological and behavioural mechanisms in vertebrate body fluid homeostasis In: Ramsay DJ, Booth D, editors. Thirst. New York: Springer, 1991. pp. 3–22. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.NRC. Nutrient requirements of nonhuman primates. Washington DC: National Resource Council, National Academies Press, 2003. [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Jéquier E, Constant F. Water as an essential nutrient: the physiological basis of hydration. Eur J Clin Nutr. 2010; 64:115–123. 10.1038/ejcn.2009.111 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Grandjean AC, Campbell SM. Hydration: fluids for fife A monograph by the North American Branch of the International Life Science Institute. Washington DC: ILSI North America, 2004. [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Dias PA, Rangel-Negrin A, Coyohua-Fuentes A, Canales-Espinosa D. Factors affecting the drinking behavior of black howler monkeys (Alouatta pigra). Primates. 2014; 55:1–5. 10.1007/s10329-013-0383-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Sharma N, Huffman MA, Gupta S, Nautiyal H, Mendonca R, Morino L, et al. Watering holes: the use of arboreal sources of drinking water by Old World monkeys and apes. Behav Proc. 2016; 129:18–26. 10.1016/j.beproc.2016.05.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.McNab BK. The physiological ecology of vertebrates: a view from energetics. London: Cornell University Press, 2002. [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Green B. Field energetics and water fluxes in marsupials In: Saunders NR, Hinds LA, editors. Marsupial Biology: recent research, new perspectives. Sydney: UNSW Press, 1997. pp. 143–159. [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Xu M.M., Wang D.H. Water deprivation up-regulates urine osmolality and renal aquaporin 2 in Mongolian gerbils (Meriones unguiculatus). Comp Biochem and Phys A. 2016; 194:37–44. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Ferrari SF, Hilário RR. Use of water sources by buffy-headed marmosets (Callithrix flaviceps) at two sites in the Brazilian Atlantic forest. Primates. 2012; 53:65–70. 10.1007/s10329-011-0277-z [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Gilmore DP, Da-Costa CP, Duarte DPF. An update on the physiology of two- and three-toed sloths. Braz J Med Biol Res. 2000; 33:129–146. 10.1590/s0100-879x2000000200001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Ellis W, Melzer A, Clifton ID, Carrick F. Climate change and the koala Phascolarctos cinereus: water and energy. Aust Zool. 2010; 35:369–377. [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Mella VSA, Orr C, Hall L, Velasco S, Madani G. An insight into natural koala drinking behavior. Ethology. 2020. https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/abs/10.1111/eth.13032. [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Glander KE. Drinking from arboreal water sources by mantled howling monkeys (Alouatta palliata-Gray). Folia Primatol. 1978a; 29:206–217. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Oates JF. Water-plant and soil consumption by guereza monkeys (Colobus guereza): a relationship with minerals and toxins in the diet? Biotropica. 1978; 10:241–253. [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Bicca-Marques JC. Drinking behavior in the black howler monkey (Alouatta caraya). Folia Primatol. 1992; 58:107–111. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Hillyer AP, Armstrong R, Korstjens AH. Dry season drinking from terrestrial man-made watering holes in arboreal wild Temminck’s red colobus, the Gambia. Primate Biol. 2015; 2:21–24. [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Martínez N, Antelo C, Rumiz DE. Rehabilitación de perezosos (Bradypus variegatus) urbanos en reservas privadas aledañas a Santa Cruz de La Sierra: una iniciativa multipropósito de investigación, manejo y educación. Rev Bol Ecol. 2004; 16:1–10. [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Glander KE. Howling monkey feeding behavior and plant secondary compounds: a study of strategies In: Montgomery GG, editor. The ecology of arboreal folivores, Washington DC: Smithsonian Institution Press, 1978b. pp. 561–574. [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Bianchi RC, Mendes SL. Ocelot (Leopardus pardalis) predation on primates in Caratinga Biological Station, southeast Brazil. Am J Primatol. 2007; 69:1173–1178. 10.1002/ajp.20415 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Pozo‐Montuy G, Serio‐Silva JC. Movement and resource use by a group of Alouatta pigra in a forest fragment in Balancán, México. Primates. 2007; 48:102–107. 10.1007/s10329-006-0026-x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Bicca-Marques JC, Chaves ÓM, Hass GP. Howler monkey tolerance to habitat shrinking: lifetime warranty or death sentence? Am J Primatol. 2020; 82:e23089 10.1002/ajp.23089 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Bonvicino CR. Observações sobre a ecologia e o comportamento de Alouatta belzebul (Primates: Cebidae) na Mata Atlântica. M.Sc.Thesis, Universidade Federal da Paraíba. 1988.
  • 25.Steinmetz S. Drinking by howler monkeys (Alouatta fusca) and its seasonality at the Intervales State Park, São Paulo, Brazil. Neotrop Primates. 2001; 9:111–112. [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Moro-Rios RF, Serur-Santos CS, Miranda JMD, Passos FC. Water intake by a group of Alouatta clamitans (Primates: Atelidae), on an Araucaria Pine Forest: seasonal, sex-age and circadian variations. Rev Bras Zool. 2008; 25:558–562. [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Campbell CJ, Aureli F, Chapman CA, Ramos-Fernandez G, Matthews K, Russo SE, et al. Terrestrial behavior of Ateles spp. Int J Primatol. 2005; 26:1039–1051. [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Freese CH. The behavior of white-faced capucins (Cebus capucinus) at a dry-season waterhole. Primates. 1978; 19:275–286. [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Castro SCN, Souto AS, Schiel N, Biondi LM, Caselli CB. Techniques used by bearded capuchin monkeys (Sapajus libidinosus) to access water in a semi-arid environment of North-Eastern Brazil. Folia Primatol. 2017; 88:267–273. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Nagy KA, Milton K. Aspects of dietary quality, nutrient assimilation and water-balance in wild howler monkeys (Alouatta palliata). Oecologia. 1979; 39:249–258. 10.1007/BF00345437 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Moles AT, Bonser SP, Poore AG, Wallis IR, Foley WJ. Assessing the evidence for latitudinal gradients in plant defense and herbivory. Func Ecol. 2011; 25:380–388. [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Milton K. Food choice and digestive strategies of two sympatric primate species. Am Nat. 1981; 117:476–495. [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Thompson CL, Williams SH, Glander KE, Teaford MF, Vinyard CJ. Body temperature and thermal environment in a generalized arboreal anthropoid, wild mantled howling monkeys (Alouatta palliata). Am J Phys Anthropol. 2014; 154:1–10. 10.1002/ajpa.22505 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Sulzman FM, Fuller CA, Moore-Ede M. Feeding time synchronizes primate circadian rhythms. Physiol Behav. 1977; 18:775–779. 10.1016/0031-9384(77)90182-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Hoban TM, Levine AH, Shane RB, Sulzman FM. Circadian rhythms of drinking and body temperature of the owl monkey (Aotus trivirgatus). Physiol Behav. 1985; 34:513–518. 10.1016/0031-9384(85)90042-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Bicca-Marques JC, Calegaro-Marques C. Behavioral thermoregulation in a sexually and developmentally dichromatic neotropical primate, the black-and-gold howling monkey (Alouatta caraya). Am J Phys Anthropol. 1998; 106:533–546. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Chaves ÓM, Bicca-Marques JC. Feeding strategies of brown howler monkeys in response to variations in food availability. Plos One. 2016; 11:e0145819 10.1371/journal.pone.0145819 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Marsh LK, Chapman CA, Norconk MA, Ferrari SF, Gilbert KA, Bicca-Marques JC, et al. Fragmentation: specter of the future or the spirit of conservation? In: Marsh LK, editor. Primates in fragments: ecology and conservation. New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers, 2003. pp. 381–398. [Google Scholar]
  • 39.INMET. Banco de Dados meteorológicos para ensino e pesquisa: registros metereologicos para Rio Grande do Sul.Instituto Nacional de Metereologia do Brasil, Porto Alegre, 2019. https://www.inmet.gov.br/portal/index.php?r=bdmep/bdmep.
  • 40.Chaves ÓM, Bicca-Marques JC. Dietary flexibility of the brown howler monkey throughout its geographic distribution. Am J of Primatol. 2013; 75:16–29. 10.1002/ajp.22075 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Fortes VB. Ecologia e comportamento do bugio-ruivo (Alouatta guariba clamitans Cabrera, 1940) em fragmentos florestais na depressão central do Rio Grande do Sul, Brasil. Ph.D. Thesis, Pontifícia Universidade Católica do Rio Grando do Sul. 2008. http://repositorio.pucrs.br/dspace/bitstream/10923/5421/1/000400984-Texto%2BCompleto-0.pdf.
  • 42.Williamson EA, Feistner ATC. Habituating primates: processes, techniques, variables and ethics In: Setchell JM, Curtis DJ, editors. Field and laboratory methods in primatology: a practical guide. New York: Cambridge University Press, 2011. pp. 33–50. [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Altmann J. Observational study of behavior: sampling methods. Behavior. 1974; 49:227–267. 10.1163/156853974x00534 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Bicca-Marques JC, Calegaro-Marques C. Activity budget and diet of Alouatta caraya: an age-sex analysis. Folia Primatol. 1994; 63:216–220. 10.1159/000156823 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Koch F, Bicca-Marques JC. Padrão de atividades e dieta de Alouatta guariba clamitans Cabrera, 1940: uma análise sexo-etária In: Bicca-Marques JC, editor. A primatologia no Brasil—10. Porto Alegre: Sociedade Brasileira de Primatologia, 2007. pp. 353–361. [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Prates HM, Bicca-Marques JC. Age-sex analysis of activity budget, diet, and positional behavior in Alouatta caraya in an orchard forest. Int J Primatol. 2008; 29:703–715. [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Crawley MJ. The R book. New Jersey: John Wiley & Sons, 2012. [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Payne EH, Gebregziabher M, Hardin JW, Ramakrishnan V, Egede LE. An empirical approach to determine a threshold for assessing overdispersion in Poisson and negative binomial models for count data. Commun Stat Simul Comput. 2018; 47:1722–1738. 10.1080/03610918.2017.1323223 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Grueber CE, Nakagawa S, Laws RJ, Jamieson IG. Multimodel inference in ecology and evolution: challenges and solutions. J Evol Biol. 2011; 24:699–711. 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2010.02210.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Barton K. MuMIn: multi-model Inference. R package version 1.15.6. http://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/MuMIn/index.html. 2016.
  • 51.Wickham H, François R, Henry L, Müller K. dplyr: a grammar of data manipulation. R package version 0.7.8. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=dplyr. 2018.
  • 52.Zuur AF, Ieno EN, Elphick CS. A protocol for data exploration to avoid common statistical problems. Methods Ecol Evol. 2010; 1:3–14. [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Burnham KP, Anderson D. Model selection and multi-model inference: a practical information-theoretic approach. New York: Springer Press; 2003. [Google Scholar]
  • 54.R Core Team. R: a language and environment for statistical computing. Version 3.6.3. https://www.R-project.org. 2020.
  • 55.Silver SC, Ostro LET, Yeager CP, Horwich R. Feeding ecology of the black howler monkey (Alouatta pigra) in northern Belize. Am J Primatol. 1998; 45:263–279. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Gonçalves AS. Uso de hábitat de mamíferos terrestres em fragmentos de floresta estacional decidual. M.Sc. Thesis, Universidade do Vale do Rio dos Sinos. 2006. http://www.repositorio.jesuita.org.br/bitstream/handle/UNISINOS/2300/uso%20de%20habitat.pdf?sequence=1.
  • 57.Buss G. Conservação do bugio-ruivo (Alouatta guariba clamitans) (PRIMATES, ATELIDAE) no entorno do Parque Estadual de Itapuã, Viamão, RS. Ph.D. Thesis, Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Sul. 2012. https://www.lume.ufrgs.br/bitstream/handle/10183/69918/000867969.pdf?sequence=1.
  • 58.Chapman CA. Patterns of foraging and range use by three species of Neotropical primates. Primates. 1988; 29:177–194. [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Allaby M. Tropical forests. New York: Chelsea House Publishers, 2006. [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Nimer E. Climatologia do Brasil. 2.ed Rio de Janeiro, Brazil: IBGE/Departamento de Recursos Naturais e Estudos Ambientais, 1989. [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Coley PD, Barone JA. Herbivory and plant defenses in tropical forests. Ann Rev Ecol Syst. 1996; 27:305–335. [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Richard A. Primates in nature. New York: WH Freeman, 1985. [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Hogan JD, Melin AD, Mosdossy KN, Fedigan LM. Seasonal importance of flowers to Costa Rican capuchins (Cebus capucinus imitator): implications for plant and primate. Am J Phys Anthropol. 2016; 161:591–602. 10.1002/ajpa.23059 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Bicca-Marques JC, Azevedo RB. The ’thermoregulation hypothesis’ does not explain the evolution of sexual dichromatism in the brown howler monkey (Alouatta guariba clamitans). Folia Primatol. 2004; 75:236. [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Paterson JD. Postural-positional thermoregulatory behaviour and ecological factors in primates. Can Rev Phys Anthropol. 1981; 3:3–11. [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Campos FA, Fedigan LM. Behavioral adaptations to heat stress and water scarcity in white-faced capuchins (Cebus capucinus) in Santa Rosa National Park, Costa Rica. Am J Phys Anthropol. 2009; 138:101–111. 10.1002/ajpa.20908 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Lopes KG, Bicca-Marques JC. Ambient temperature and humidity modulate the behavioural thermoregulation of a small arboreal mammal (Callicebus bernhardi). J Thermal Biol. 2017; 69:104–109. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Estrada A, Garber PA, Rylands AB, Roos C, Fernandez-Duque E, et al. Impending extinction crisis of the world’s primates: why primates matter. Sci Adv. 2017; 3:e1600946 10.1126/sciadv.1600946 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

Julie Jeannette Gros-Louis

19 Nov 2020

PONE-D-20-21970

Leaf and flower consumption modulate the drinking behavior in a folivorous-frugivorous arboreal mammal

PLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Chaves,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

I agree with the reviewer's recommendation of a major revision. 

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

Please submit your revised manuscript by Jan 03 2021 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Julie Jeannette Gros-Louis, PhD

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

2. We note that you have stated that you will provide repository information for your data at acceptance. Should your manuscript be accepted for publication, we will hold it until you provide the relevant accession numbers or DOIs necessary to access your data. If you wish to make changes to your Data Availability statement, please describe these changes in your cover letter and we will update your Data Availability statement to reflect the information you provide.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: N/A

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: This manuscript presents data on water ingestion by a tropical primate, and compares the demonstrated drinking patterns to two hypotheses that explain variation in the urge for animals to drink. The manuscript is relatively straightforward and contains valuable information. Most research on drinking in primates is sparse and anecdotal, so this thorough and long-term report will be a welcome addition to the literature. I have a few suggestions and considerations for the authors that I believe may benefit the paper.

First, for the GLMM analysis (l. 233), I recommend that the authors standardize the number of drinking events by group size. The dependent variable in this analysis is the number of daily drinking records. However, these data come from 14 howler groups, with differing sizes ranging from 6-12 animals. It seems logical to me that a group of 12 animals will have more observed drinking events than a group half that size. Dividing the daily drinking records by group size would be an easy way to fix this confounding factor without adding another predictor variable to the analysis. While group ID is a factor in the model, this nominal category will not control for effects of increasing or decreasing size between groups.

I think that more details and clarity on the relationship between fruit and leaf (and to a lesser extent, flower) consumption would be helpful. The methods indicate that leaf, fruit, and flower consumption are not multicollinear (l. 241-242). However, this is quite surprising for howlers, whose diet is essentially 100% comprised of these three foods. Are the howlers in this study eating some other items that decrease the correlation between these dietary items? For howlers, fruit and leaf consumption are generally inversely related to one another. If the percentage of leaf, flower, and fruit eaten in a day totals ~100%, then shouldn’t these predictor variables be correlated? Presenting a correlation matrix of these variables for readers may be helpful.

Furthermore, this relationship has important implications for the results. The authors find a significant effect of leaf consumption, but not fruit consumption. If fruit and leaf consumption are correlated, then this may simply be a statistical artifact rather than a biological pattern—leaf consumption is already explaining the variation in drinking records, so no variance is attributed to fruit. This is how multicollinear variables generally behave. Was a step function used so that leaf consumption was always entered into the model first? Again, knowing the correlation matrix between these dietary variables would help the authors establish this as a relevant biological finding vs. a statistic effect of correlated predictor variables.

Lastly, the interpretation of these data seems to blur the lines of this relationship. L. 342-346 uses fruit (and flower) consumption to explain the significant reduction in leaf consumption, which is interpreted as both increasing the amount of water ingested and reducing the need for water to detoxify secondary metabolites. In short, fruit are the most water rich food being consumed, which in turn drives lower leaf consumption, which then drives drinking behavior….despite that fruit was not a significant predictor of drinking behavior. Clearly, this relationship is family complex, however the conclusion that fruit plays a role in the observed drinking patterns, despite the results indicating otherwise, seems somewhat circular and/or inconsistent. Overall, a more detailed portrayal of how these variables are interrelated would help readers better understand these patterns.

Line by line comments

l. 3 Recommend deleting “the”

l. 33-34 Could some of this variation be due to group size rather than true drinking differences?

l. 47 ‘preformed’ is not defined until later in the ms. This may be unclear for abstract readers.

l. 79 I think the double use of ‘foods’ here is a bit awkward.

l. 133-135 A bit more information about the time lag of this process would be helpful. If hotter temperatures are experienced in the afternoon, would the drinking response be that quick to initiate?

l. 179 Is there a possibility for differential visibility between fragments? This could impact observed drinking.

l. 204-206 How are these feeding data summarized into the GLMM data? It looks like the GLMM had daily summaries...was the food item per each bout (and if so how were bouts defined)? Per minute? Was there ever averaging of a day's eating across more than one focal animal? Data were also collected via both instantaneous scan and focal animal sampling (l. 198-199)...how were feeding data from these different collection methods merged? Or were feeding data only from one of these methods?

l. 206-207 Does ‘individual records’ refer to individual drinks or individual monkeys?

l. 211-219 Temperature data were collected two different ways. Unless I am mistaken, the ms does not clarify which temperature measurements were used for which analyses.

l. 222-223 It would help to clarify in this text that a chi square test was done separately for each group.

l. 233-236 As per above, I recommend standardizing group drink records by the number of group members. Also, which ambient temperature measurement was used here?

l. 241-242 As above, it is surprising that VIF<3 if leaf, fruit, and flower consumption cumulatively make up ~100% of howler diet….shouldn’t there be a strong correlation? A correlation matrix would be helpful. This could be included at supplemental material, if needed.

l. 262-263 What is an 'event of group drinking' compared to 'individual drinking records?' The methods only defines "all drinking events."

l. 281-282 Same here—a ‘single drinking event’ is unclear. Is this the same as ‘individual drinking record’ earlier? Where is the ‘out of five’ coming from…particularly as some groups had >5 members?

l. 285 It would be helpful for readers to define the analyzed seasons earlier in the methods.

l. 289-291 I am not entirely sure what this sentence refers to. Is it that one, two, or three seasons were significantly higher than the remaining season(s)?

l. 293 Recommend specifying that pooling is of fragments.

l. 302 It is slightly unclear what ‘all’ refers to. I think this means that all predictor variables are cumulatively covered across the six different models, but this sentence could be confusing for readers.

l. 308 I don’t think ‘direct’ is the best word here. A direct relationship could still be either negative or positive.

l. 309 Recommend ‘nonsignificant’

l. 315 A comparison somewhere to overall activity patterns by time of day would be helpful. Are howlers simply more active in the afternoon?

l. 337-341 This would be helpful to know earlier. Recommend moving to the methods.

l. 342-346 As per above, the reasoning here seems slightly circular/inconsistent. Fruit consumption drives leaf consumption, which drives drinking behavior, but fruit consumption is not a significant predictor of drinking? Despite it being the most water-rich food ingested?

l. 348 It is unclear how this is a pers obs. How do you observe dehydration and overheating? I recommend deleting or being more specific about the observed behavior.

l. 351-355 Information on biological lags in the mammalian thirst response would be helpful, if known. Information on the overall timing of activities across the day would help as well.

l. 357 It is unclear what data this refers to...is this from the time of day analysis? I don’t see a direct analysis of temperature and within-day patterns. If so, recommend using the exact variables here so that readers can refer back to the results more easily.

l. 359 It is unclear what ‘broader temporal scale’ refers to. The GLMM was based on daily drinking…is that what this means?

Table 2: Recommend providing exact p values.

l. 603 The stated number does not appear in the middle of each bar, only a letter.

l. 612 (c) and (d) are not defined here. Also, this (a) and (b) may be confusing with those in l. 602-603.

Reviewer #2: A very nice study on the drinking patterns of a primate species, the brown howler monkeys. The study is based on an impressive sample size, in terms of study sites (fragments), individuals, and observation effort. Authors characterize the drinking sources, seasonal variation in drinking, and model drinking in relation to several dietary and environmental factors. The manuscript is well framed in the introduction; methods are generally complete and clearly described (but see specific comments); results are well-presented and statistical reporting is exhaustive (supplemental results are also very useful); and the discussion is synthetic and stimulates further research. The manuscript is also well organized and easy to read. I believe that this is an important contribution to all of those interested in drinking behavior. I only have a few comments detailed below. I especially call your attention to my questions concerning data organization and statistical analysis.

L131-133: as stated, it is not clear why you have two different predictions pertaining to the putative influence of diet composition on drinking behavior.

L181-182: I believe that habituation refers to individuals, not groups. How did you habituate individuals?

L199: which recording technique was used with the focal animal sampling?

L205: mature leaves?

L207: given that two different sampling methods were used to collect dietary data, how was “number of feeding records” calculated?

L214: “better” than what?

L217: meaning of “after each behavioral sampling unit”?

L223: you should describe before (behavioral data collection?) which water sources are you referring to.

L225: I apologize, but it is unclear to me how were these “total number of records” calculated given that two different sampling methods were used.

L225-226: the same argument could be used against the model you describe in the next paragraph so, am I missing something? Additionally, isn’t the calculation of proportions an actual way to account for such variation in sampling effort? Finally, please acknowledge that Figure 1 depicts data per group and per season, which you state here that could/should not be done.

L233: a single model was built (the complete model).

L234: about “contribution of fruits, leaves, and flowers to the diet”, how were these data organized? Which feeding records did you use to match drinking events? For instance, if in a particular day a single drinking event was recorded and it happened before noon, did you use only morning records as predictor values? If so, how was variation in the number of feeding records associated with drinking events accounted for? If not, please offer more information on data handling.

L234: perhaps add “(categorical variable with, three levels)” after “fragment size” to help us remembering your classification of fragment sizes.

Please report results of overdispersion diagnosis.

L307-308 & Table 2: the information theoretic framework is not based on significance testing. I refer you to Burnham and Anderson (2002), which you cite and, for instance, Mundry (2011; https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-010-1040-y). The combination of “frequentist” and IT approaches is associated with Type I errors and is incorrect. You must decide which approach best suits your study. SE and CI shown in Table 2 are sufficient to assess the reliability of effects of predictors on drinking behavior.

Merge paragraphs starting at L336, L342 & L347.

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: Yes: Pedro Dias

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2021 Feb 19;16(2):e0236974. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0236974.r002

Author response to Decision Letter 0


15 Jan 2021

*The responses to the reviewers and Editor are included in the Cover Letter. We also included the same responses below:

RESPONSES TO COMMENTS OF THE EDITOR AND REVIEWERS

Below we list our answers (italics) after the comments of the academic editor and the reviewer (bold)

Leaf and flower consumption modulate the drinking behavior in a folivorous-frugivorous arboreal mammal

PLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Chaves,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

I agree with the reviewer's recommendation of a major revision.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

• A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

• A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

• An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Julie Jeannette Gros-Louis, PhD

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

2. We note that you have stated that you will provide repository information for your data at acceptance. Should your manuscript be accepted for publication, we will hold it until you provide the relevant accession numbers or DOIs necessary to access your data. If you wish to make changes to your Data Availability statement, please describe these changes in your cover letter and we will update your Data Availability statement to reflect the information you provide.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Authors: Ok, thank you Julie. We have checked all technical details you have mentioned. Below we have included detailed responses to the comments and suggestions of the two reviewers.

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

________________________________________

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: N/A

________________________________________

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

________________________________________

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

________________________________________

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1:

This manuscript presents data on water ingestion by a tropical primate, and compares the demonstrated drinking patterns to two hypotheses that explain variation in the urge for animals to drink. The manuscript is relatively straightforward and contains valuable information. Most research on drinking in primates is sparse and anecdotal, so this thorough and long-term report will be a welcome addition to the literature. I have a few suggestions and considerations for the authors that I believe may benefit the paper.

Authors: Thank you for the comments.

First, for the GLMM analysis (l. 233), I recommend that the authors standardize the number of drinking events by group size. The dependent variable in this analysis is the number of daily drinking records. However, these data come from 14 howler groups, with differing sizes ranging from 6-12 animals. It seems logical to me that a group of 12 animals will have more observed drinking events than a group half that size. Dividing the daily drinking records by group size would be an easy way to fix this confounding factor without adding another predictor variable to the analysis. While group ID is a factor in the model, this nominal category will not control for effects of increasing or decreasing size between groups.

Authors: We understand the concern of the reviewer. However, we believe that this standardization is not necessary. Despite the differences in group size, there was no correlation between group size and the number of drinking records per day (rs=0.03, P=0.54) probably, at least partially, because not all group members used to take part of the drinking sessions/events as we have mentioned in Results (p.13, line 306). Furthermore, the proposed standardization would transform the count data into proportions and, then, the transformed data would not fit the Poisson (nor negative binomial distribution or Gaussian even after several transformations) distribution family.

Furthermore, we have included group size as a covariable in the GLMM model to control for any potential influence of differences in this variable on the response variable. We have clarified this point in the Methods (please see p.11, lines 253-255).

I think that more details and clarity on the relationship between fruit and leaf (and to a lesser extent, flower) consumption would be helpful. The methods indicate that leaf, fruit, and flower consumption are not multicollinear (l. 241-242). However, this is quite surprising for howlers, whose diet is essentially 100% comprised of these three foods. Are the howlers in this study eating some other items that decrease the correlation between these dietary items? For howlers, fruit and leaf consumption are generally inversely related to one another. If the percentage of leaf, flower, and fruit eaten in a day totals ~100%, then shouldn’t these predictor variables be correlated? Presenting a correlation matrix of these variables for readers may be helpful.

Authors: We agree that the lack of multicollinearity between the consumption of leaves, fruits and flowers could sound counterintuitive. However, as we disclosed in the Methods, we analyzed “the number of feeding records devoted to each plant item” instead of the percentage of records devoted to them. The collinearity would be expected in the latter case, not necessarily in the former because the howlers can simultaneously increase or decrease the number of feeding records with both leaves and fruits, for example. This difference explains why we found a negligible collinearity between these variables using the Variation Inflation Factor (p.12, line 266). We have cited a graph with the correlation matrix between the dietary variables (Fig S3 in the Supplementary Material) in the Methods (p.12, line 267). Overall, the correlation coefficients between the consumption of fruits, leaves and flowers expressed by the number of feeding records ranged from 0.01 to -0.21.

Furthermore, this relationship has important implications for the results. The authors find a significant effect of leaf consumption, but not fruit consumption. If fruit and leaf consumption are correlated, then this may simply be a statistical artifact rather than a biological pattern—leaf consumption is already explaining the variation in drinking records, so no variance is attributed to fruit. This is how multicollinear variables generally behave.

Authors: We are confident the pattern we report is not a statistical artifact. Please see our explanation in our response above.

Was a step function used so that leaf consumption was always entered into the model first? Again, knowing the correlation matrix between these dietary variables would help the authors establish this as a relevant biological finding vs. a statistic effect of correlated predictor variables.

Authors: Yes, we used a step function and the correlation matrix support our findings as can be seen in the correlation matrix in the Supplementary Fig S3:

Fig. S3. Matrix of Spearman and Pearson (in parentheses) correlation coefficients between the consumption of fruits, leaves and flowers by14 brown howler monkey (Alouatta guariba clamitans) groups in the State of Rio Grande do Sul, Brazil.

Lastly, the interpretation of these data seems to blur the lines of this relationship. L. 342-346 uses fruit (and flower) consumption to explain the significant reduction in leaf consumption, which is interpreted as both increasing the amount of water ingested and reducing the need for water to detoxify secondary metabolites. In short, fruit are the most water rich food being consumed, which in turn drives lower leaf consumption, which then drives drinking behavior….despite that fruit was not a significant predictor of drinking behavior. Clearly, this relationship is family complex, however the conclusion that fruit plays a role in the observed drinking patterns, despite the results indicating otherwise, seems somewhat circular and/or inconsistent. Overall, a more detailed portrayal of how these variables are interrelated would help readers better understand these patterns.

Authors: We agree that this paragraph may sound circular. However, we are not referring here to all fruits, but particularly to some preferred fleshy fruit species abundant in this season and to other water-richer items other than adult leaves, such as young leaves. It is reasonable to expect that the consumption of all these water-richer plant items together contributes to reduce the need of drinking. We rewrote the paragraph in an attempt of making our rationale clearer. Please see p. 16, lines 370-372.

Line by line comments

l. 3 Recommend deleting “the”

Authors: Done.

l. 33-34 Could some of this variation be due to group size rather than true drinking differences?

Authors: No, we do not believe so. As we explained above (p. 5), there was no correlation between group size and the number of drinking records.

We suspect that the differences were partially influenced by the availability of water sources (i.e. streams, rivers, tree holes, and bromeliads) in each study site, in addition to the relationship with leaf and flower consumption. Unfortunately, we do not have reliable information on this issue, particularly on the availability of hard-to-detect water sources in the canopy, such as tree holes and bromeliad rosettes.

l. 47 ‘preformed’ is not defined until later in the ms. This may be unclear for abstract readers.

l. 79 I think the double use of ‘foods’ here is a bit awkward.

Authors: The text was corrected.

l. 133-135 A bit more information about the time lag of this process would be helpful. If hotter temperatures are experienced in the afternoon, would the drinking response be that quick to initiate?

Authors: In fact, the temperature in the study sites increased gradually throughout the day, reaching a peak often later in the afternoon (please see Fig. S4). Because of this, we predicted a similar pattern in the drinking. We have edited the text in this section for clarity.

l. 179 Is there a possibility for differential visibility between fragments? This could impact observed drinking.

Authors: Despite differences (not measured, but probably subtle) in visibility both between sites as well as within each site, we do not believe this biased our ability to record events of drinking across studies. The canopy of all Atlantic Forest study sites was sufficiently open to enable us to monitor most group members throughout the day. We have clarified this issue in the Methods (see p. 7, lines 160-161).

l. 204-206 How are these feeding data summarized into the GLMM data? It looks like the GLMM had daily summaries...was the food item per each bout (and if so how were bouts defined)? Per minute?

Was there ever averaging of a day's eating across more than one focal animal? Data were also collected via both instantaneous scan and focal animal sampling (l. 198-199)...how were feeding data from these different collection methods merged? Or were feeding data only from one of these methods?

Authors: We have improved the description of the methods used to record diet and drinking behavior data (please see p. 9). The data on feeding behavior were recorded via instantaneous scan sampling at 15-min intervals in most study periods. The exception was the period (v) of groups S4, S5 and S6, whose data were collected via 5-min focal samples at 10-min intervals. We recorded information on the plant species exploited, the food item (s) consumed, and the time devoted to each species/item per study group (scan) or the focal monkey (focal-animal).

We organized the data on diet composition and drinking behavior based on the number of records per study day with at least one drinking record to run the analyses (available in Mendeley Data: http://dx.doi.org/10.17632/3gxy6vrsbf.1).

l. 206-207 Does ‘individual records’ refer to individual drinks or individual monkeys?

Authors: We refer here to individual drinking events or drinking records, not to individual monkeys. We removed the word ‘individual’ to avoid confusion.

l. 211-219 Temperature data were collected two different ways. Unless I am mistaken, the ms does not clarify which temperature measurements were used for which analyses.

Authors: The temperature data that we used to run the GLMM were obtained from the same source: Instituto Nacional de Meteorologia do Brasil. However, we used hourly in situ variations in temperature to prepare Fig S5. We clarified this point in the text (please see p. 10).

l. 222-223 It would help to clarify in this text that a chi square test was done separately for each group.

Authors: Done (see p. 10, line 239)

l. 233-236 As per above, I recommend standardizing group drink records by the number of group members.

Authors: Please see our response on this issue above (p. 5).

Also, which ambient temperature measurement was used here?

Authors: As we mentioned above, we used the ambient temperature available from the Instituto de Meteorologia do Brasil (see p. 10, lines 223-224).

l. 241-242 As above, it is surprising that VIF<3 if leaf, fruit, and flower consumption cumulatively make up ~100% of howler diet….shouldn’t there be a strong correlation? A correlation matrix would be helpful. This could be included at supplemental material, if needed.

Authors: Please see our response on this issue in p. 5 above and the correlation matrix in Fig. S3.

l. 262-263 What is an 'event of group drinking' compared to 'individual drinking records?' The methods only defines "all drinking events."

Authors: We are referring to ‘drinking events’ (i.e. when one or more members of the study group drank). Corrected (please see p. 9, lines 199-200).

l. 281-282 Same here—a ‘single drinking event’ is unclear. Is this the same as ‘individual drinking record’ earlier? Where is the ‘out of five’ coming from…particularly as some groups had >5 members?

Authors: Corrected. Here we are referring to the number of monkeys participating in a ‘drinking event’, not to an ‘individual drinking record’. Then we observed that a drinking event involved between one-fifth (or 20%) to four-fifths (or 80%) of group members. Please see p. 13, line 306.

l. 285 It would be helpful for readers to define the analyzed seasons earlier in the methods.

Authors: Done. Please p. 10, line 241.

l. 289-291 I am not entirely sure what this sentence refers to. Is it that one, two, or three seasons were significantly higher than the remaining season(s)?

Authors: Corrected. See p. 14, lines 314-317

l. 293 Recommend specifying that pooling is of fragments.

Authors: The ‘pooled dataset’ refers to the comparison of the number records in each season considering all groups together. We improved the sentence for clarity (please see p. 14, lines 317-318).

l. 302 It is slightly unclear what ‘all’ refers to. I think this means that all predictor variables are cumulatively covered across the six different models, but this sentence could be confusing for readers.

Authors: We are referring to the six study predictor variables mentioned in Methods. In fact, now there are eight variables after the addition of ‘group size’ and ‘sampling method’ as covariables to address the recommendations of reviewer #2 (please see p. 11). We found nine models with ΔAICc<2 in the new GLMM analyses (please see the new Table 2). We edited the sentence to avoid confusion (please see p. 14, line 328).

l. 308 I don’t think ‘direct’ is the best word here. A direct relationship could still be either negative or positive.

Authors: We believe the word ‘direct’ is not statistically incorrect, particularly because we previously mention the word ‘inverse’. However, we rewrote this sentence following reviewer 2’s suggestion about the incorrect use of the Null Hypotheses Significance Testing NHST combined with IT-models (please see p. 15 below and the manuscript’s p. 15).

l. 309 Recommend ‘nonsignificant’

Authors: We removed this term and any reference to p-values in this paragraph in light of reviewer 2’s recommendation about the Null Hypothesis Testing approach (see p. 15).

l. 315 A comparison somewhere to overall activity patterns by time of day would be helpful. Are howlers simply more active in the afternoon?

Authors: We have no data on the activity budget of all 14 study groups and this analysis was not an objective of this manuscript (it will be part of another manuscript with a subset of six study groups). Nevertheless, we have the impression based on our field observations that brown howlers are more active early in the morning than in the afternoon. They often spend more time feeding in the morning (particularly from 6:00 to 11:00) and resting in the shade from 11:30 to 14:00-15:00 (depending on ambient temperature). However, some individuals occasionally drank from arboreal or terrestrial water sources during the afternoon resting periods. We added this information in the new version (please see p. 17, lines 387-388).

l. 337-341 This would be helpful to know earlier. Recommend moving to the methods.

Authors: We disagree with this recommendation because this sentence is a potential explanation for our finding, not a simple methodological detail. Furthermore, we provided a detailed description of the climatic conditions of the study sites in Methods (p. 7), where we highlighted that there is not a clear rainfall difference between months.

l. 342-346 As per above, the reasoning here seems slightly circular/inconsistent. Fruit consumption drives leaf consumption, which drives drinking behavior, but fruit consumption is not a significant predictor of drinking? Despite it being the most water-rich food ingested?

Authors: As we explained above (p. 6), we improved this sentence to avoid this circular argument (please also see the corrected sentence in p. 16, lines 370-376).

l. 348 It is unclear how this is a pers obs. How do you observe dehydration and overheating? I recommend deleting or being more specific about the observed behavior.

Authors: Here we are referring to a frequent behavioral pattern observed in some study groups (specifically groups S2, S3, S7, M2, L1, L4, and L5): these groups used to select a riparian-shaded site to rest during the hottest hours of the day (partially midday). We considered it an important thermoregulatory behavior because according to our meteorological field records for six of these groups (S1, S2, S3, L1, L2, and L3), the temperature in the riparian forest can be up to 3ºC lower than in forest edges and other open forested areas. The other groups did not show this behavior because their habitats had no riparian forest.

However, we removed this sentence from the text (see p. 17, lines 381-382) because we do not have data on thermoregulation or dehydration as highlighted by the reviewer and because this idea is also implicit in the ‘shade-seeking adjustments’ mentioned in the next sentence.

l. 351-355 Information on biological lags in the mammalian thirst response would be helpful, if known. Information on the overall timing of activities across the day would help as well.

Authors: Unfortunately, we did not find information on biological lags in the mammalian thirst response in our literature review. However, as we mentioned above (p. 9), we improved the sentence by including additional information on the activity periods of brown howlers (please see p. 17, lines 387-388).

l. 357 It is unclear what data this refers to...is this from the time of day analysis? I don’t see a direct analysis of temperature and within-day patterns. If so, recommend using the exact variables here so that readers can refer back to the results more easily.

Authors: Yes, here we are referring to the fact that in some groups the peaks of drinking records (Fig 3) trended to occur in the afternoon (Fig S5). We further explain that we did perform a specific analysis on this issue. We rewrote the paragraph to avoid further confusions (please see p. 17).

l. 359 It is unclear what ‘broader temporal scale’ refers to. The GLMM was based on daily drinking…is that what this means?

Authors: As mentioned in the previous response, this sentence was improved to avoid confusion.

Table 2: Recommend providing exact p values.

Authors: We removed the P-values in light of reviewer 2’s recommendations (please see p. 15 below). We followed this recommendation because many statisticians have shown that the combination of IT-based inference and Null Hypotheses Significance Testing NHST is not statistically appropriate given that it significantly increases Type I error. In fact, as reviewer 2 mentioned, the C.I. and the S.E. provide sufficient evidence to assess the relevance of predictor variables. Please see Mundry (2011, https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-010-1040-y) for an excellent explanation on this issue.

l. 603 The stated number does not appear in the middle of each bar, only a letter.

Authors: Corrected.

l. 612 (c) and (d) are not defined here. Also, this (a) and (b) may be confusing with those in l. 602-603.

Authors: Here we are referring to Figs 2a and 2b, not to the letters into the bars. We edited the Fig. 2. legend to avoid confusing readers.

Reviewer #2:

A very nice study on the drinking patterns of a primate species, the brown howler monkeys. The study is based on an impressive sample size, in terms of study sites (fragments), individuals, and observation effort. Authors characterize the drinking sources, seasonal variation in drinking, and model drinking in relation to several dietary and environmental factors. The manuscript is well framed in the introduction; methods are generally complete and clearly described (but see specific comments); results are well-presented and statistical reporting is exhaustive (supplemental results are also very useful); and the discussion is synthetic and stimulates further research. The manuscript is also well organized and easy to read. I believe that this is an important contribution to all of those interested in drinking behavior. I only have a few comments detailed below. I especially call your attention to my questions concerning data organization and statistical analysis.

Authors: Thank you very much for your evaluation and your well-justified comments and suggestions. In this revised version of the manuscript we have corrected most of the issues mentioned by you and the other reviewer.

L131-133: as stated, it is not clear why you have two different predictions pertaining to the putative influence of diet composition on drinking behavior.

Authors: We did not understand why the reviewer considered that we have two predictions about the influence of diet composition… In this first prediction we simply expect that howlers will complement the preformed water in the diet with drinking water as a response to the highly seasonal pattern of availability of water-rich food items in the region. This prediction is based on previous research that found that howlers often supplement the preformed water in their diet with water from arboreal or terrestrial sources. We found support for this prediction as you can see in Fig. 2. In the second prediction we refer to the within-day gradual increase in drinking. Then, our third prediction addresses the main drivers of drinking behavior. In sum, we believe that our hypotheses are not overlapping and complement each other to improve our understanding of the factors affecting the drinking behavior of howler monkeys.

L181-182: I believe that habituation refers to individuals, not groups. How did you habituate individuals?

Authors: Yes, we refer to the habituation of the individuals of each study group. To habituate the howlers in medium and large fragments we followed most recommendations of Williamson & Feistner (2011). We improved the paragraph by including additional details on the habituation process (please see p. 8, lines 174-182).

Williamson EA, Feistner ATC. Habituating primates: processes, techniques, variables and ethics. In: Setchell JM, Curtis DJ, editors. Field and laboratory methods in primatology: a practical guide. New York: Cambridge University Press, 2011. pp. 33–50.

L199: which recording technique was used with the focal animal sampling?

Authors: We applied the focal-animal method in a single research – period (v). The method consisted in recording the behavior, plant species exploited and food item(s) ingested by the focal howler instantaneously at 20-s intervals during 5-min focal sampling units every 15 min. A single focal adult was monitored per group during each study period in order to fulfill the major goals of the respective research. We identified individuals according to their sex-age category, and based on their size, pelage color, skin pigmentation, face form, genital’s size and color, scars, etc. We added some of these details in the revised text (please see p. 9).

L205: mature leaves?

Authors: Yes, we collected information on mature (i.e. old or adult) and young leaves. We changed ‘old’ to ‘adult’ (as is frequently used by plant physiologists) to avoid confusion.

L207: given that two different sampling methods were used to collect dietary data, how was “number of feeding records” calculated?

Authors: We clarified these methodological details in the Methods section (please see p. 9, lines 207-217). The number of feeding records on each plant item by the focal animal was the sum of the instantaneous records (once every 20 s during the 5-min focal sampling units), whereas the number of instantaneous scan sampling feeding records was the sum of all records of feeding on a given food item by all recorded group members.

L214: “better” than what?

Authors: Here we are explaining that we used the mean ambient temperature and weekly rainfall as proxies or indicators of thermal ambient temperature and water availability for brown howlers…To avoid confusion, we have replaced the words ‘they represent better’ by ‘proxies of’ (see p. 10, line 224-227).

L217: meaning of “after each behavioral sampling unit”?

Authors: Corrected. We replaced ‘behavioral sampling unit’ with ‘scan or focal sampling unit’.

L223: you should describe before (behavioral data collection?) which water sources are you referring to.

Authors: Done (please see p. 9, line 215).

L225: I apologize, but it is unclear to me how were these “total number of records” calculated given that two different sampling methods were used.

Authors: Please see our previous comment on this issue (L207) above. Furthermore, we have clarified these details in the Methods (p. 9).

L225-226: the same argument could be used against the model you describe in the next paragraph so, am I missing something? Additionally, isn’t the calculation of proportions an actual way to account for such variation in sampling effort?

Authors: We feel that the reviewer misinterpreted the meaning of this sentence. In fact, our point is that the inter-fragment comparisons may be inappropriate because of differences in sampling effort (see Table 1). For this reason, we only compared the proportion of records devoted to each water source or season within each study fragment. We tried to clarify this issue in the Methods of the new version (please see p. 11).

Finally, please acknowledge that Figure 1 depicts data per group and per season, which you state here that could/should not be done.

Authors: We believe that the reviewer is referring to Figure 2, instead of Figure 1, which is the map. As we have explained in the previous comment, inter-fragment comparisons may be inadequate because of differences in sampling effort. For this reason we performed the comparisons shown in Figure 2 only within-fragments. We also have clarified this point in the Methods (p.11, line 242-243).

L233: a single model was built (the complete model).

Authors: Corrected.

L234: about “contribution of fruits, leaves, and flowers to the diet”, how were these data organized? Which feeding records did you use to match drinking events?

Authors: We used the information on the total number of drinking records, the weekly rainfall, the ambient temperature, and the number of feeding records devoted to fruits, leaves, and flowers (calculated as mentioned in the Methods, see p. 9) for each sampling day with at least one drinking record. Our datasets and all details of data organization are available in Mendeley Data (http://dx.doi.org/10.17632/3gxy6vrsbf.1).

For instance, if in a particular day a single drinking event was recorded and it happened before noon, did you use only morning records as predictor values? If so, how was variation in the number of feeding records associated with drinking events accounted for? If not, please offer more information on data handling.

Authors: No, we always used all feeding records of the day, irrespective if the hypothetic single drinking event happened before noon or near the end of the day. Therefore, our predictor variable was the total number of feeding records as we mentioned in the Methods (please see p. 11, line 252).

L234: perhaps add “(categorical variable with, three levels)” after “fragment size” to help us remembering your classification of fragment sizes.

Authors: In fact, we used the total area of each study fragment, instead of size category, in this analysis as we mentioned in Table 1. We clarified this detail in the new version (please see p. 11, line 251).

Please report results of overdispersion diagnosis.

Authors: Done. We now report this parameter in the Methods. Despite the fact that our data are overdispersed (overdispersion ratio=1.4, P=0.03) as is often the case for most count data, we believe that the use of an alternative family distribution is not indispensable in our case (please see more details in p. 11, lines 257-260)

L307-308 & Table 2: the information theoretic framework is not based on significance testing. I refer you to Burnham and Anderson (2002), which you cite and, for instance, Mundry (2011; https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-010-1040-y). The combination of “frequentist” and IT approaches is associated with Type I errors and is incorrect. You must decide which approach best suits your study. SE and CI shown in Table 2 are sufficient to assess the reliability of effects of predictors on drinking behavior.

Authors: This is a very good point! We acknowledge that the use of both statistical approaches (IT-based inference and NHST-Null Hypotheses Significance Testing) is not statistically appropriate despite its frequent use in the scientific literature as mentioned by Mundry (2011). We believe that the IT-based inference is the most appropriate approach for our data and concur that SE and CI (and the sum of the Akaike weights) are sufficient to assess the reliability and relevance of our predictor variables. Therefore, we improved this paragraph following the suggestion of both reviewers (please see p. 15). We also removed the z-values and the significance from Table 2 and the text. We only show the parameter values and the relative importance of the main predictors (∑wi) in the new version.

Merge paragraphs starting at L336, L342 & L347.

Authors: Done.

Decision Letter 1

Julie Jeannette Gros-Louis

28 Jan 2021

Flower consumption, ambient temperature and rainfall modulate drinking behavior in a folivorous-frugivorous arboreal mammal

PONE-D-20-21970R1

Dear Dr. Chaves,

We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication.

An invoice for payment will follow shortly after the formal acceptance. To ensure an efficient process, please log into Editorial Manager at http://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the 'Update My Information' link at the top of the page, and double check that your user information is up-to-date. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

Kind regards,

Julie Jeannette Gros-Louis, PhD

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments (optional):

Reviewers' comments:

Acceptance letter

Julie Jeannette Gros-Louis

8 Feb 2021

PONE-D-20-21970R1

Flower consumption, ambient temperature and rainfall modulate drinking behavior in a folivorous-frugivorous arboreal mammal

Dear Dr. Chaves:

I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact onepress@plos.org.

If we can help with anything else, please email us at plosone@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access.

Kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Dr. Julie Jeannette Gros-Louis

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    S1 File

    (DOCX)

    Data Availability Statement

    All relevant data needed to replicate the analyses are within the paper and its Supporting information files. Additional datasets used to perform the main statistical analysis are available in Mendeley Data (http://dx.doi.org/10.17632/3gxy6vrsbf.1) and Dryad Digital Repository (https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.hdr7sqvh7).


    Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES