Skip to main content
Journal of Child & Adolescent Trauma logoLink to Journal of Child & Adolescent Trauma
. 2020 Apr 29;14(1):73–83. doi: 10.1007/s40653-020-00307-z

Self-Blame Mediates the Link between Childhood Neglect Experiences and Internalizing Symptoms in Low-Risk Adolescents

Michal Tanzer 1,, George Salaminios 1, Larisa Morosan 2, Chloe Campbell 1, Martin Debbané 1,2
PMCID: PMC7900265  PMID: 33692873

Abstract

Childhood neglect is the most common type of maltreatment, ranging from minor isolated incidents to consistent failures in emotional/physical caregiving. It has been associated with developmental impairments and considered a risk factor for the emergence of psychopathology, particularly internalizing disorders. This study aimed to explore individual differences in response to the continuum of severity of neglect in community adolescents, as well as the role of specific cognitive emotion regulation strategies (CERSs) as mediators between childhood neglect and current internalizing symptoms. Low-risk adolescents (12–19 years old; M age 15.88 years; N = 123; 64 Females) completed questionnaires assessing these experiences. We employed a regression model and a simple mediation analyses. Findings indicate a positive association between childhood neglect, internalizing behaviors, and the adoption of self-blame as CERS. Moreover, the use of self-blame in response to everyday stress partially mediated the relationship between neglect and internalizing behaviors (effect size: .28). Findings support the hypothesis that even in a low risk sample, neglect is associated with internalizing symptoms, and highlight the importance of assessing individual differences in the experience of neglect. Moreover, the mediation effect of the CERSs of self-blame might serve as a potential target for psychotherapeutic interventions aimed at reducing internalizing symptoms.

Keywords: Neglect, Cognitive emotion regulation, Self-blame, Internalizing symptoms, Adolescence


Child neglect is commonly understood as the failure of caregivers (even without intent) to meet their child’s basic physical, emotional, medical, or educational needs (Gilbert et al. 2009). It can include minor isolated incidents as well as more severe or consistent patterns of failure. Experiences of childhood neglect can therefore be seen as existing on a continuum (Dubowitz 2013) and can have diverse harmful effects on a child’s outcomes (Fonagy et al. 2015) enduring into adulthood (Anda et al. 2006). Child neglect often co-occurs with child abuse (Kim et al. 2017), which involves physical or persistent emotional harm and can have severe adverse effects on the child’s development (Krug et al. 2002).

While extensive research has focused on the impairments associated with childhood neglect in comparison with control groups, there remains a need for studies that examine these associations in community individuals reporting incidents of neglect, some of which are below the “clinical cut-off” but which might nevertheless have negative sequelae. In the present study, we focus on individual differences in response to childhood experiences of neglect in low-risk adolescents, and examine how and whether these experiences relate to internalizing behaviors and the use of different cognitive emotion regulation strategies (CERSs).

Neglect and Internalizing Symptoms

Neglect is a major risk factor for the emergence of various forms of psychopathology (Egeland et al. 1983; Hecker et al. 2018; Norman et al. 2012; Sturge-Apple et al. 2006; Widom et al. 2007), enduring into adulthood. Prospective and retrospective research has linked neglect with the emergence of internalizing symptoms such as depression or emotional withdrawal (Bolger and Patterson 2001; Hecker et al. 2018; Kim and Cicchetti 2010; Manly et al. 2001; Spratt et al. 2012). For example, it has been shown that emotional neglect in early childhood is positively associated with social withdrawal in middle childhood and negatively with adolescents’ social competence (Shaffer et al. 2009).

The lack of attuned emotional or physical responses from the caregiver, in the context of childhood neglect, may limit or disrupt the child’s emotional and cognitive development (Cook et al. 2005; Dvir et al. 2014; Gould et al. 2012; Hildyard and Wolfe 2002, 2007; Kendall et al. 1998; Maguire et al. 2015; Mills et al. 2011). For example, a comparison between children with no history of neglect, children with a history of neglect, and children with a history of institutional rearing (i.e., having had the experience of early neglect) who were adopted into families of high socioeconomic status showed that the two groups of children with a history of neglect reported significantly more internalizing symptoms than the control group. In the adopted group, there was a positive correlation between internalizing behaviors and the duration of their stay in the neglectful environment. Moreover, both neglect groups showed lower cognitive and language scores compared with nonneglected controls, highlighting the effect of early neglect on cognitive and verbal development (Spratt et al. 2012).

Children who have suffered from neglect, as compared with abuse, have been found to have more limited social relations and higher rates of cognitive deficit and internalizing symptoms, suggesting that, above and beyond the effect of abuse, neglect significantly impacts on individuals’ developmental trajectory (see review by Hildyard and Wolfe 2002; Pollak et al. 2000). In addition, when comparing children who have experienced neglect with children who have experienced both abuse and neglect, the neglect-only group scored significantly lower on vocabulary tests (O’Hara et al. 2015). This reduced verbal performance score suggests a specific pathological outcome relating to neglect: a deficit or absence of sustained social interactions that may have a developmental effect on language skills (O’Hara et al. 2015). In addition, brain imaging analysis has shown a different neural response to threat reactivity, with more distributed cortical reactivity in individuals who have suffered from neglect as compared to a more localized activation among individuals who have experienced abuse (Puetz et al. 2019). However, recent findings also highlight the particularly deleterious effect of emotional abuse on mental health (Cecil et al. 2017).

Despite the consistently powerful findings on the negative effects of neglect, there is still a need for studies that explore its effects on a continuum. Studies that explore individual differences in relation to maltreatment using the childhood trauma questionnaire (CTQ) as a continuous score, have reported a positive association between internalizing symptoms (e.g., Herringa et al. 2013; Huh et al. 2017), emotion dysregulation and psychopathology (Jennissen et al. 2016; Peh et al. 2017). In addition, a study examining the effect of emotional abuse and neglect on a continuum has found a positive relation with internalizing symptoms (Hamilton et al. 2013) and another exploring the continuous effect of the combined score of emotional abuse and neglect found that self-criticism mediated the link between emotional abuse and neglect and romantic satisfaction (Lassri et al. 2016).

Focusing on neglect indices, especially in adolescence, may further our understanding of the underlying mechanisms linking it to the development of psychopathology in later life. As will be discussed further below, adolescence is an especially significant period for the emergence of the negative sequelae of neglect given its importance as a period of biological, cognitive, and psychosocial development (Blakemore 2012).

Adolescence, Cognitive Emotion Stress Regulation and Internalizing Behaviors

Adolescence is a time of change (Fuhrmann et al. 2015). These significant changes take place in almost every arena (Steinberg and Morris 2001), and the experiences that accompany them generate an emotional, cognitive, and interpersonal stress load that confers vulnerability to mental health problems. During adolescence, a shift in social focus towards peer relations takes place: adolescents experience what their peers think of them extremely powerfully, and are hypersensitive to social rejection, elevating the risk of suffering from internalizing disorders such as social withdrawal and depression (Blakemore 2012). These social changes, taking place alongside neurobiological development, appear to create a particular window of vulnerability for the emergence of mental health disorder. Indeed, most psychopathologies (e.g., depression, anxiety, psychosis, substance use disorders, and eating disorders) first manifest during adolescence (Kessler et al. 2005; Paus et al. 2008).

Critically, biopsychosocial changes during adolescence create heightened demands in relation to self-monitoring and self-regulation capacities, making the ability to regulate stress a potential risk or resilience mechanism in this developmental period (Blakemore and Mills 2014; Garnefski et al. 2002). Adolescence further represents a period of growth in cognitive abilities (Aldwin 2007) and self-regulation, notably through the use and improvement of coping skills that sustain the management of emotion activation and stress (Garnefski et al. 2001). Individuals develop different cognitive approaches, or strategies, to regulate emotions in response to stress: the establishment of such strategies typically settles through the opportunities afforded by adolescent biopsychosocial development (Garnefski et al. 2001).

Empirical studies on cognitive coping have put forward a model of four maladaptive strategies – self-blame, other-blame, rumination, and catastrophizing – and five adaptive strategies – putting into perspective, positive refocusing, positive reappraisal, acceptance, and planning – that adolescents appear to use in response to stressful situations (Garnefski et al. 2005). The maladaptive or adaptive nature of the strategies adopted by an individual can have a long-term impact. For example, the use of strategies such as catastrophizing, self- and other-blame may, in the short term, help regulate distress to a level that is familiar and tolerable, but the entrenched use of this strategy will lead to biased self-appraisals and reduced social functioning psychopathology (Rudrauf and Debbané 2018).

Empirical work on cognitive coping has evidenced the association between maladaptive cognitive emotion regulation strategies (CERSs) and internalizing problems in adolescents (Garnefski et al. 2001, 2002, 2005). For example, individuals who displayed internalizing behaviors reported greater self-blame than individuals with externalizing behaviors (Garnefski et al. 2005). Furthermore, rumination and catastrophizing appear to moderate the link between bullying and anxiety symptoms in adolescence (Garnefski and Kraaij 2014). Research on adult patients experiencing clinical anxiety/depressive symptoms shows that maladaptive CERSs partially mediate the link between childhood maltreatment and the severity of symptoms (Huh et al. 2017).

In addition, studies have consistently found that the relationship between childhood maltreatment and the development of psychopathology symptoms is mediated by difficulties in emotion regulation (Alink et al. 2009; Langevin et al. 2015). For example, emotion dysregulation has been shown to mediate the relationship between adverse childhood experiences, including neglect, and psychopathology (Jennissen et al. 2016), as well as interpersonal difficulties in adulthood (Poole et al. 2018).

From a developmental perspective, the adoption of adaptive emotion regulation strategies is supported through secure interactions with significant others (Eisenberg et al. 2010). Through interactions with responsive caregivers as a source of comfort, infants and children develop their own abilities to self-regulate. Childhood maltreatment in all its forms disrupts the typical development of these skills, either by causing the employment of maladaptive psychological strategies or through adverse effects on neurobiological development (or both) (Dvir et al. 2014). For example, children who have experienced significant physical neglect demonstrate impaired emotion recognition, use fewer adaptive regulation skills, and show more attempts to suppress their emotions compared with control groups (Pollak et al. 2000; Shipman et al. 2005).

Taken together, the developmental framework of emotion regulation and its significance in adolescence highlights the importance of understanding the impact of child maltreatment, and specifically child neglect, on mental health problems in adolescence through the prism of CERSs. However, to our knowledge, there is limited research examining these relationships in low-risk adolescents who have lived through a range of neglectful experiences, from fairly minor to significant in severity. Such an approach may be increasingly important in the context of the ongoing shift in psychological research away from categorical diagnoses toward conceptualizing disorder(s) on continuous or spectral dimensions (Krueger et al. 2018).

The Present Study

The present study examines the relationship between childhood neglect, CERSs, and internalizing symptoms in a sample of low-risk adolescents. The study attempts to circumvent three issues: first, despite the frequent co-occurrence of multiple types of maltreatment (Kim et al. 2017), most extant studies of neglect do not control for the presence of abuse. Second, despite findings on the effect of neglect on cognitive abilities (O’Hara et al. 2015), to our knowledge, no study to date has focused on CERSs while controlling for cognitive abilities such as verbal functioning. Third, most studies have undertaken group comparisons between maltreated and nonmaltreated individuals, leaving intact the opportunity to assess individual differences across the entire continuum, without being confounded by factors related to clinical populations (e.g., effects of medication, social rupture and isolation linked to psychopathology, comorbidity, etc.).

We hypothesized that: (1) similar to clinical groups, adolescents’ experiences of childhood neglect even below the clinical “cut-off” would be associated with higher reported levels of internalizing problems; (2) replicating previous findings, internalizing problems would be positively correlated with maladaptive CERSs, and negatively with adaptive ones; (3) based on the reported previous mediation effect of CERSs in adults (Huh et al. 2017), CERSs would mediate the link between neglect and internalizing behavior.

Methods

Participants and Procedure

A total of 132 community adolescents were recruited through written advertisements and by word of mouth in local schools and youth community centers in Geneva, Switzerland. Inclusion criteria were age between 12 and 19 years, attendance at an age-appropriate school, and no past or current psychiatric treatment and/or neurological conditions, as assessed by a self-report questionnaire. Information concerning the socio-economic status of the participants’ parents was available for 129 adolescents: 6.8% were low socio-economic status, 20.9% were medium–low, 16.3% medium, 17.8% medium–high, 38% high socio-economic status. Information about ethnic origin was available for 105 adolescents: 39% were Swiss, 8.6% French, 7.6% Southern European (Portugal, Spain, Italy), 4.8% Eastern European and Balkan (Russia, Moldavia, Kosovo), 3.8% South American (Brazil, Colombia), 3.15% African (Ghana, Guinea, Ethiopia, Eritrea), 33.3% were of mixed ethnic origin (such as 54.3% European mixed, 22.8% European-South America, 11.4% European-African, 2.8% European-Middle East, 8.5% European-South Asia). Participants received financial compensation, and written consent was obtained from participants or their parents (if under 18 years of age), following a protocol approved by the university ethics committee. Each participant was individually tested in the university lab.

As we were interested in studying typical development, exclusion criteria included verbal and cognitive performance corresponding to IQ <70 (n = 9) in the vocabulary and block design subtests of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale (Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children – Fourth edition; Wechsler 2003 and for participants over the age of 18, the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale –Third edition; Wechsler 1997). The final sample consisted of 123 adolescents. The descriptive statistics for the different variables included in our analysis are presented in Table 1.

Table 1.

Participants’ characteristics

Measure M (SD) (N = 123) Range
Females/Males 64/59
Age in years 15.88 (1.86) 12.01–18.99
Vocabulary subset 11.48 (2.99) 1–19
Internalizing T-score 52.84 (10.53) 32–82
Internalizing subscales T-score
  Anxiety/Depression 56.48 (7.85) 50–94
  Withdrawal 55.30 (7.55) 50–91
  Somatic complaints 55.51 (6.96) 50–79
CTQ subscales
  Neglect sum score 17.29 (5.64) 10–35
  Abuse sum score 18.38 (5.10) 15–43
CERQ subscales
  Self-blame 9.54 (3.14) 4–19
  Other-blame 7.80 (2.81) 4–20
  Rumination 11.55 (3.55) 5–20
  Catastrophizing 8.42 (3.59) 4–19
  Acceptance 13.70 (3.44) 6–20
  Positive refocusing 11.52 (3.97) 4–20
  Positive reappraisal 13.47 (3.73) 4–20
  Putting into perspective 13.72 (3.90) 5–20
  Refocus on planning 13.95 (3.51) 5–20

CERQ Cognitive emotion regulation questionnaire, CTQ Childhood trauma questionnaire

Instruments

Behavioral and Emotional Problems

To evaluate participants’ psychopathology, we used the Youth Self-Report (YSR; for individuals aged 11–17; Achenbach 1991) and its adult equivalent, the Adult Self Report (ASR; for individuals above 17 years; Achenbach and Rescorla 2003) questionnaire. Both are designed to assess symptoms in the past 6 months using a 3-point scale (0 = not true to 2 = very true). The YSR and the ASR were validated in French. We focused on the internalizing scale reflecting three subscales, which evaluate the presence of the following difficulties: Anxious/Depressed, Withdrawn/Depressed and Somatic Complaints (Cronbach’s α: Internalizing ASR = .88; YSR = .82). For each subscale, a total T-score is computed. A T-score of 65 or 70 in the specific subscales represents the cut-off for clinically significant symptomatology. Of the total sample, 12% fell into the clinical range of internalizing symptoms (T score > 64), 3% fell into the clinical range of withdrawal symptoms or somatic complaints and 5% into the clinical range for anxiety and depression (T scores >70).

Childhood Neglect and Abuse

To evaluate individuals’ experience of childhood trauma, we used the French version of the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire (CTQ; Bernstein et al. 1994; Paquette et al. 2004), a 28-item self-report questionnaire validated for use in clinical and nonclinical populations. Individuals are asked to indicate on a 5-point Likert scale how often (1 = never, 5 = frequently) they experienced emotional, physical, and sexual abuse, and emotional or physical neglect in childhood. The French version has been validated with French-speaking populations (Paquette et al. 2004). In this study, the emotional and physical neglect subscales were summed to provide a total score of neglect and the emotional, physical, and sexual abuse subscales were summed to give a total score of abuse (Cronbach’s α = .70 and .72, respectively). Within our sample, 16% reported moderate to severe emotional neglect (scale score > 15), 9% moderate to severe physical neglect (scale score > 10), 5% moderate to severe physical abuse (scale score > 10), 4% moderate to severe emotional abuse (scale score > 13), and 3% moderate to severe sexual abuse (scale score > 8). These percentages are lower than those reported in international and European surveys (Moody et al. 2018).

Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire

To evaluate the cognitive emotion regulation strategies (CERSs) used in response to a negative or traumatic event (Garnefski et al. 2001), we used the French version of the Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (CERQ; Jermann et al. 2006). This version was validated in a community sample of adolescents and is comparable with the original English questionnaire (d’Acremont and Van der Linden 2007). The CERQ is a 36-item questionnaire that uses a 5-point Likert scale to assess nine strategy subscales: self-blame, rumination, catastrophizing, blaming others, acceptance, positive refocusing, refocus on planning, positive reappraisal, and putting into perspective (Cronbach’s α = .74, .64, .72, .83, .66, .80, .75, .80, and .78, respectively). Subscale scores can range from 4 to 20, with higher subscale scores indicating greater frequency of use of the specific cognitive strategy. Comparing the present sample scores on the CERQ sub-scales with those reported in d’Acremont and Van der Linden (2007), revealed that all fell within the average score.

Verbal Cognitive Functioning

To control for cognitive verbal functioning, we used the French version of the vocabulary subtest of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children – Fourth edition (WISC; Wechsler 2003). For participants over the age of 18, the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale – Third edition (WAIS-III; Wechsler 1997) was used. The vocabulary subtest measures word knowledge, language development, and concept understanding.

Data Analysis

We first ran descriptive statistics and as the variables of interest (i.e., neglect, internalizing behaviors and CERQ subscales) violated normality, we used nonparametric tests. First, we used the nonparametric Mann–Whitney U test to compare male and female participants on variables of interest (internalizing behaviors, experience of neglect and abuse, CERSs). Second, we ran a partial Spearman correlation analysis using R, conducted between neglect and internalizing behaviors, controlling for age, sex, abuse, and Wechsler’s WISC/WAIS vocabulary subtest score. Third, Spearman’s rho correlation analyses were performed to assess the associations between neglect, CERQ results, and internalizing behaviors. Results were corrected for multiple comparisons using a 5% false discovery rate (FDR), based on the sequential Benjamini–Hochberg FDR correction algorithm (Benjamini and Hochberg 1995). Finally, based on the correlation analysis, and in order to test the mediation hypothesis, we used SPSS version 25 combined with Hayes’s PROCESS macro (Hayes 2018). This macro uses bootstrapping (10,000 resamples) for testing mediation effects and is considered one of the most valid and powerful simulation methods (Hayes 2009).

Results

Descriptive and Correlation Analysis

The nonparametric Mann–Whitney U test for two independent groups revealed that males and females did not significantly differ in most of the variables [(i.e., internalizing behaviors (p = .79), neglect (p = .10) and abuse experience (p = .46), or the use of most of the CERSs (ps > .36) except the use of rumination as a strategy: our findings suggested that females used this strategy more than males (male M = 10.56, SD = 3.25; female M = 12.43, SD = 3.22, p = .001)]. Partial correlation analysis between neglect and internalizing behaviors, controlling for abuse, sex, age, and verbal cognitive functioning, yielded a positive correlation (r = .27, p = .002). In addition, when examining the Spearman correlation coefficients (Table 2; FDR corrected) between neglect, internalizing behaviors and CERSs, there were positive correlations between neglect and self-blame (r = .30, p = .003, 95% CI [0.13, 0.45]), and neglect and catastrophizing (r = .25, p = 0.02, 95% CI [0.07, 0.40]). There were no significant correlations between neglect and any of the adaptive CERSs. There were also positive correlations between internalizing behaviors and the CERSs of self-blame (r = .38, p = .00002, 95% CI [0.21, 0.52]), catastrophizing (r = .22, p = .01, 95% CI [0.04, 0.38]), rumination (r = .26, p = .003, 95% CI[0.08, 0.42]), and a positive significant trend with the tendency to blame others (r = .20, p = .07, 95% CI [0.02, 0.30]). There were no significant correlations between internalizing and any of the adaptive CERSs.

Table 2.

Summary of Spearman intercorrelations of neglect, internalizing behaviors and CERQs (N = 123, FDR corrected)

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
1. Internalizing behavior .40** .38** .20 .26* .22* .10 .00 .03 .04 .03
2. Neglect .30** .13 .18 .25* .15 .07 .04 .04 .05
3. CERQ – self-blame .38** .45** .35** .23* .00 .17 .17 .20*
4. CERQ – other-blame .22* .37** .03 .16 .11 .06 .14
5. CERQ–rumination .44** .38** .26** .35* .32** .30**
6. CERQ – catastrophizing .09 .18 .05 −.01 .00
7. CERQ – acceptance .40** .45** .53** .27*
8. CERQ – positive refocusing .49** .41** .32**
9. CERQ – positive reappraisal .61** .44**
10. CERQ – putting into perspective .20
11. CERQ – refocus on planning

CERQ Cognitive emotion regulation questionnaire, FDR False discovery rate

*p < 0.05

**p < 0.001

Mediation

Based on the correlation results, we tested whether the CERS self-blame score mediated the effect of neglect on internalizing symptoms. Results indicated that when controlling for age, sex, abuse, and verbal cognitive score, neglect was a significant predictor of self-blame (path a in Fig. 1; b = .32, SE = .10, p = .002, 95% CI [0.12, 0.52]), and self-blame was a significant predictor of internalizing behaviors (path b in Fig. 1; b = 0.36, SE = .09, p = .00001, 95% CI [0.19, 0.53]). The total effect of neglect on internalizing behaviors (b = .40, SE = .10, p = .001, 95% CI [0.21, 0.61]) was reduced by about 11% when self-blame was accounted for (path c’ in Fig. 1; b = .29, SE = .09, p = .003, 95% CI [0.09, 0.48]), indicating partial mediation. The indirect coefficients were significant (b = .12, 95% CI [0.03, 0.23]) and the mediation effect size, calculated as the ratio between the indirect and the total effect (Wen and Fan 2015), was found to be .28.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Mediation model. With neglect as independent variable, self-blame as mediator, and internalizing problems as dependent variable controlling for age, sex, abuse and Wechsler’s WISC/WAIS-III vocabulary subtest score

Using the same method, we also examined whether the CERS of catastrophizing mediated the effect of neglect on internalizing behaviors, as the use of this strategy was also positively correlated with both neglect and internalizing behaviors. However, indirect coefficients were not significant (b = .04, 95% CI [−0.005, 0.10]), indicating no mediation.

Post-hoc Analysis

Subscales of the Internalizing Dimension

To further examine the mediation effect of self-blame on the relationship between neglect and internalizing behaviors, we conducted similar mediation analysis using the subscales of the internalizing dimension (i.e., withdrawal, depression/anxiety, and somatic complaints). This analysis yielded the finding that self-blame fully mediated the link between neglect and withdrawal and partially mediated the link with depression/anxiety (Table 3). However, there was no effect in relation to somatic complaints.

Table 3.

Post-Hoc tests of a. indirect effects in the model of neglect and internalizing dimension, mediated by self-blame, and b. subtypes of neglect and internalizing behaviours. In both models age, sex, abuse, and Wechsler’s WISC/WAIS-IV vocabulary subtest score were controlled

Path b SE 95% CI
Lower Upper
a.
  Neglect → SB → Withdrawal .11 .06 0.03 0.25
  Neglect → SB → Anxiety/Depression .14 .06 0.02 0.28
  Neglect → SB → Somatic complaints .06 .04 −0.07 0.15
b.
  EN → SB → Internalizing behaviours 0.08 0.04 0.001 0.19
  PN → SB → Internalizing behaviours 0.12 0.05 0.04 0.23

CI Confidence intervals; EN Emotional neglect; PN Physical neglect, SB Self-blame

Subtypes of Neglect

To further examine the mediation effect of self-blame on the relationship between subtypes of neglect and internalizing behaviors, we conducted a similar mediation analysis using the subtypes of neglect before aggregating them to a total score (i.e., emotional and physical neglect). This analysis yielded the finding that self-blame fully mediated the link between physical neglect and internalizing, and partially mediated the link with emotional neglect (Table 3).

Discussion

In the present study, we examined in a cross-sectional design the relationships between childhood neglect, CERSs, and internalizing symptoms in low-risk adolescents. We observed that the association between childhood neglect and internalizing behaviors is specifically mediated by the CERS self-blame. Our findings support our first hypothesis that, similarly to clinical samples (Bolger and Patterson 2001; Hildyard and Wolfe 2002; Kim and Cicchetti 2010; Manly et al. 2001; Spratt et al. 2012), low-risk adolescents’ reports of perceived childhood neglect experiences are associated with current internalizing symptoms. Given that experiences of childhood abuse were controlled for in this study, this finding highlights the specific effect of neglect.

The second hypothesis, concerning the relationship between internalizing symptoms and CERSs, was partially supported. Findings showed a positive relationship between internalizing symptoms and all maladaptive CERSs (e.g., rumination, catastrophizing, self-blame, and other-blame) but not with adaptive ones. This finding is inconsistent with previous reports of negative associations between depression severity and adaptive CERSs in adults with depressive disorders (Huh et al. 2017). However, it is consistent with previous studies reporting that adolescents with internalizing symptoms score higher on maladaptive CERSs compared with a control group, but do not differ in adaptive ones (Garnefski et al. 2005). While it is necessary to explore further the null relation between internalizing symptoms and adaptive CERSs, the pronounced recourse to maladaptive strategies to regulate stress might suggests that psychological interventions targeting emotion-regulation skills may be indicated for vulnerable adolescents (Loevaas et al. 2019; Rossouw and Fonagy 2012; Sharp et al. 2009; Sukhodolsky et al. 2004; Young et al. 2019).

In addition, only the CERSs related to self-blame and catastrophizing were positively associated with both neglect and internalizing symptoms, allowing us to test the mediation hypothesis. Interestingly, the association between neglect and internalizing behavior is mediated by the CERS of self-blame and not by catastrophizing. This is consistent with previous findings that self-blame partially mediates the effect of childhood neglect on nonsuicidal self-injury in adulthood (Swannell et al. 2012), as well as the link between childhood traumatic experiences and symptom severity in adults with depression (Huh et al. 2017). However, it further suggests that this relationship is present at a normative level and not uniquely present in the context of clinical symptoms. This finding may be attributable to the particular nature of childhood neglect, which implies a lack of emotional or physical care by attachment figures (Leeb et al. 2011; Mennen et al. 2010). The child in a neglectful environment experiences reduced exposure to the caregiver responses and interactions; as a consequence, the child may be left with little option other than to adopt self-blame as there is no “other” to take the blame or to be blamed for these experiences.

Further post-hoc analysis within the internalizing dimension found that self-blame fully mediates the link between neglect and the withdrawn subscale, and partially mediates the relation between neglect and depression/anxiety, but not somatic complaints. These specific paths are intriguing and consistent with a different but related conceptualization of self-criticism as a stance augmented in neglectful familial environments (Shahar 2015): the experience of neglect might lead to the use of strategies that are directed inward (e.g., self-blame) at moments of heightened stress, although this path would require longitudinal investigations. The increased impact and value of social relations in adolescence (Blakemore 2008) may heighten the intensity of the affect associated with self-blame, generating greater distress and potentially leading to increased social impairment.

Contrary to findings that adaptive CERSs may mediate the link between neglect and adult anxiety/depression in a group of clinical adults (Huh et al. 2017), we did not find any association between neglect and adaptive CERS dimensions (e.g., positive refocusing or reappraisal). The difference could be accounted for by our use of a general population sample rather than a clinical one, as well as the difference in age in the samples studied, and should be further investigated in future research focusing on adaptive resilience mechanisms using longitudinal designs.

From a developmental perspective, the absence of attuned emotional responses in the context of childhood neglect in early relationships may limit the child’s capacity to regulate emotions and to develop adaptive mental representations of themselves and others, augmenting the risk for psychopathology (Cook et al. 2005; Feldman and Greenbaum 1997; Fonagy et al. 2017; Fonagy and Target 2002; Toth et al. 1997). Put differently, in terms of cognitive behavioral learning theory (Bandura 1986), such limited or maladaptive regulation strategies may also affect the individual’s ability to regulate stress during adolescence. Influenced by past experiences of neglect, the individual may have learned to blame him/herself for the lack of social support, leading to the adoption of more general self-blame strategies that might prevent him/her from integrating or appraising new information. This can in turn lead to withdrawal, anxiety and/or depression (Debbané et al. 2016; Fonagy et al. 2017; Rudrauf and Debbané 2018). Further, such strategies may also generate a vicious cycle in which individuals with internalizing symptoms are likely to continue to process information in a dysfunctional manner; this biased acquisition and processing style can contribute to the maintenance of psychopathology (Beck 2008) and limit the capacity to use adaptive strategies.

From a theoretical developmental perspective, using Erikson’s psychosocial stages (Erikson 1956) it might be that experiences of neglect may reduce feelings of trust and create higher levels of guilt and shame. These negative resolutions of the psychosocial crises are thought to reawaken with particular acuteness during adolescence, compromising the work of adaptively resolving their “Ego identity” and leading to lower levels of autonomy and more isolation, which might express itself in internalizing symptoms.

Certain limitations in the present investigation should be acknowledged. First, the modest size of our sample should be noted. We hope that future studies will replicate these findings using a larger group. In relation to this limitation, it should also be observed that our sample is a convenience sample and we cannot rule out selection bias, which might limit the generalizability of the findings. Second, as this study uses a cross-sectional design, and although the direction of the mediation is based on the assumption that childhood experience refers to the past, while CERS and internalizing behaviors are current experiences – with the first being more state-related and the later more symptom-related – we cannot rule out different directions. For example, it might also be that a higher level of internalizing would lead to extensive use of self-blame strategies, or recurrent memories of neglect. Future longitudinal studies should test these directions and investigate the long-term effects of neglect or CERSs on internalizing behavior. Third, as neglect was assessed using a retrospective self-report tool, reporting bias cannot be ruled out. Fourth we did not assess other forms of emotion dysregulation (e.g., general emotion regulation; Gross 1998) or attachment styles that could have accounted for the present effects. Likewise, our findings focused only on the adolescents’ self-report. Using stress-induced experimental designs or other related biophysiological variables, such as implicit emotion regulation (Etkin et al. 2011), might shed light on possible moderation effects that could have further implications in terms of resilience or psychotherapeutic interventions. Finally, it should be noted that in an attempt to explore neglectful experiences on a continuum, stressing the importance of examining below the radar neglectful experiences such as feelings of being unloved by a caregiver, may create a risk of overpathologizing, and researchers as well as clinicians should take this into consideration.

To conclude, to the best of our knowledge, this investigation is the first to demonstrate the association between experiences of neglect that are below the clinical cut-off, coping mechanisms in relation to stress in adolescence, and the emergence of psychopathology, in particular internalizing symptoms. Moreover, our finding on the relationship between the CERS of self-blame and internalizing behavior in nonclinical adolescents may point to a potential risk factor that could inform psychological interventions.

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by a grant from “Prix Marina Picasso, Foundation AEMD” to MD and from a grant from the Israeli Science Foundation (ISF, 51/16) to MT.

Compliance with Ethical Standards

Conflict of Interest

On behalf of all authors, the corresponding author states that there is no conflict of interest.

Footnotes

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

References

  1. Achenbach, T. M. (1991). Manual for the child behavior checklist/4–18 and 1991 profile. University of Vermont Department of Psychiatry.
  2. Achenbach TM, Rescorla LA. Manual for the ASEBA adult forms & profiles. Burlington: University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, & Families; 2003. [Google Scholar]
  3. Aldwin, C. M. (2007). Stress, coping, and development: An integrative perspective (2nd ed.). Guilford Press.
  4. Alink LRA, Cicchetti D, Kim J, Rogosch FA. Mediating and moderating processes in the relation between maltreatment and psychopathology: Mother-child relationship quality and emotion regulation. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2009;37:831–843. doi: 10.1007/s10802-009-9314-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Anda RF, Felitti VJ, Bremner JD, Walker JD, Whitfield C, Perry BD, Dube SR, Giles WH. The enduring effects of abuse and related adverse experiences in childhood. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience. 2006;256:174–186. doi: 10.1007/s00406-005-0624-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bandura A. The explanatory and predictive scope of self-efficacy theory. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology. 1986;4:359–373. doi: 10.1521/jscp.1986.4.3.359. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  7. Beck AT. The evolution of the cognitive model of depression and its neurobiological correlates. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2008;165:969–977. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.08050721. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y. Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society. Series B (Methodological) 1995;57:289–300. doi: 10.2307/2346101. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bernstein DP, Fink L, Handelsman L, Foote J, Lovejoy M, Wenzel K, Sapareto E, Ruggiero J. Initial reliability and validity of a new retrospective measure of child abuse and neglect. The American Journal of Psychiatry. 1994;151:1132–1136. doi: 10.1176/ajp.151.8.1132. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Blakemore SJ. The social brain in adolescence. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2008;9(267–277):267–277. doi: 10.1038/nrn2353. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Blakemore SJ. Development of the social brain in adolescence. Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine. 2012;105(3):111–116. doi: 10.1258/jrsm.2011.110221. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Blakemore SJ, Mills KL. Is adolescence a sensitive period for sociocultural processing? Annual Review of Psychology. 2014;65:187–207. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115202. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Bolger KE, Patterson CJ. Pathways from child maltreatment to internalizing problems: Perceptions of control as mediators and moderators. Development and Psychopathology. 2001;13:913–940. doi: 10.1017/S0954579401004096. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Cecil CA, Viding E, Fearon P, Glaser D, McCrory EJ. Disentangling the mental health impact of childhood abuse and neglect. Child Abuse & Neglect. 2017;63:106–119. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2016.11.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Cook A, Spinazzola J, Ford J, Lanktree C, Blaustein M, Cloitre M, DeRosa R, Hubbard R, Kagan R, Liautaud J, Mallah K, Olafson E, van der Kolk B. Complex trauma in children and adolescents. Psychiatric Annals. 2005;35:390–398. doi: 10.3928/00485713-20050501-05. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  16. d’Acremont M, Van der Linden M. How is impulsivity related to depression in adolescence? Evidence from a French validation of the cognitive emotion regulation questionnaire. Journal of Adolescence. 2007;30:271–282. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2006.02.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Debbané, M., Salaminios, G., Luyten, P., Badoud, D., Armando, M., Solida Tozzi, A., Fonagy, P., & Brent, B. K. (2016). Attachment, neurobiology, and mentalizing along the psychosis continuum. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 10. 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00406. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  18. Dubowitz H. Neglect in children. Pediatric Annals. 2013;42:73–77. doi: 10.3928/00904481-20130326-11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Dvir Y, Ford JD, Hill M, Frazier JA. Childhood maltreatment, emotional dysregulation, and psychiatric comorbidities. Harvard Review of Psychiatry. 2014;22:149–161. doi: 10.1097/HRP.0000000000000014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Egeland B, Sroufe A, Erickson M. The developmental consequence of different patterns of maltreatment. Child Abuse & Neglect. 1983;7:459–469. doi: 10.1016/0145-2134(83)90053-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Eisenberg, N., Spinrad, T. L., & Eggum, N. D. (2010). Emotion-related self-regulation and its relation to childrens maladjustment. Annual review of clinical psychology, 6, 495–525. 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.121208.131208 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  22. Erikson EH. The problem of ego identity. Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association. 1956;4:56–121. doi: 10.1177/000306515600400104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Etkin A, Egner T, Kalisch R. Emotional processing in anterior cingulate and medial prefrontal cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2011;15:85–93. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.11.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Feldman R, Greenbaum CW. Affect regulation and synchrony in mother–infant play as precursors to the development of symbolic competence. Infant Mental Health Journal. 1997;18:4–23. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0355(199721)18:1&#x0003c;4:AID-IMHJ2&#x0003e;3.0.CO;2-R. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  25. Fonagy P, Target M. Early intervention and the development of self-regulation. Psychoanalytic Inquiry. 2002;22:307–335. doi: 10.1080/07351692209348990. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  26. Fonagy, P., Cottrell, D., Phillips, J., Bevington, D., Glaser, D., & Allison, E. (2015). What works for whom? A critical review of treatments for children and adolescents (2nd edn). Guilford Publications.
  27. Fonagy P, Luyten P, Allison E, Campbell C. What we have changed our minds about: Part 1. Borderline personality disorder as a limitation of resilience. Borderline Personality Disorder and Emotion Dysregulation. 2017;4:11. doi: 10.1186/s40479-017-0061-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Fuhrmann D, Knoll LJ, Blakemore S-J. Adolescence as a sensitive period of brain development. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2015;19:558–566. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2015.07.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Garnefski N, Kraaij V. Bully victimization and emotional problems in adolescents: Moderation by specific cognitive coping strategies? Journal of Adolescence. 2014;37:1153–1160. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2014.07.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Garnefski N, Kraaij V, Spinhoven P. Negative life events, cognitive emotion regulation and emotional problems. Personality and Individual Differences. 2001;30:1311–1327. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(00)00113-6. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  31. Garnefski N, Legerstee J, Kraaij V, Van Den Kommer T, Teerds J. Cognitive coping strategies and symptoms of depression and anxiety: A comparison between adolescents and adults. Journal of Adolescence. 2002;25:603–611. doi: 10.1006/jado.2002.0507. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Garnefski N, Kraaij V, van Etten M. Specificity of relations between adolescents’ cognitive emotion regulation strategies and internalizing and externalizing psychopathology. Journal of Adolescence. 2005;28:619–631. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2004.12.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Gilbert R, Widom CS, Browne K, Fergusson D, Webb E, Janson S. Burden and consequences of child maltreatment in high-income countries. The Lancet. 2009;373(9657):68–81. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(08)61706-. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Gould F, Clarke J, Heim C, Harvey PD, Majer M, Nemeroff CB. The effects of child abuse and neglect on cognitive functioning in adulthood. Journal of Psychiatric Research. 2012;46:500–506. doi: 10.1016/J.JPSYCHIRES.2012.01.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Gross JJ. The emerging field of emotion regulation: An integrative review. Review of General Psychology. 1998;2:271–299. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2012.01.005. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  36. Hamilton JL, Shapero BG, Stange JP, Hamlat EJ, Abramson LY, Alloy LB. Emotional maltreatment, peer victimization, and depressive versus anxiety symptoms during adolescence: Hopelessness as a mediator. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology. 2013;42(3):332–347. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2013.777916. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Hayes, A. F. (2009). Beyond baron and Kenny: Statistical mediation analysis in the new millennium. Communication Monographs, 76, 408–420. 10.1080/03637750903310360.
  38. Hayes AF. Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis. 2. New York: Guilford Press; 2018. [Google Scholar]
  39. Hecker, T., Boettcher, V. S., Landolt, M. A., & Hermenau, K. (2018). Child neglect and its relation to emotional and behavioral problems: A cross-sectional study of primary school-aged children in Tanzania. Development and Psychopathology, 1–15. 10.1017/S0954579417001882. [DOI] [PubMed]
  40. Herringa RJ, Birn RM, Ruttle PL, Burghy CA, Stodola DE, Davidson RJ, Essex MJ. Childhood maltreatment is associated with altered fear circuitry and increased internalizing symptoms by late adolescence. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2013;110:19119–19124. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1310766110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Hildyard KL, Wolfe DA. Child neglect: Developmental issues and outcomes. Child Abuse & Neglect. 2002;26:679–695. doi: 10.1016/S0145-2134(02)00341-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Hildyard K, Wolfe D. Cognitive processes associated with child neglect. Child Abuse and Neglect. 2007;31(8):895–907. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2007.02.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Huh HJ, Kim KH, Lee HK, Chae JH. The relationship between childhood trauma and the severity of adulthood depression and anxiety symptoms in a clinical sample: The mediating role of cognitive emotion regulation strategies. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2017;213:44–50. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2017.02.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Jennissen S, Holl J, Mai H, Wolff S, Barnow S. Emotion dysregulation mediates the relationship between child maltreatment and psychopathology: A structural equation model. Child Abuse & Neglect. 2016;62:51–62. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2016.10.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Jermann F, Van der Linden M, d’Acremont M, Zermatten A. Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (CERQ) European Journal of Psychological Assessment. 2006;22:126–131. doi: 10.1027/1015-5759.22.2.126. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  46. Kendall PC, Chu B, Gifford A, Hayes C, Nauta M. Breathing life into a manual: Flexibility and creativity with manual-based treatments. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice. 1998;5:177–198. doi: 10.1016/S1077-7229(98)80004-7. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  47. Kessler RC, Berglund P, Demler O, Jin R, Merikangas KR, Walters EE. Lifetime prevalence and age-of-onset distributions of DSM-IV disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2005;62:593–602. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.6.593. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Kim J, Cicchetti D. Longitudinal pathways linking child maltreatment, emotion regulation, peer relations, and psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines. 2010;51:706–716. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02202.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Kim K, Mennen FE, Trickett PK. Patterns and correlates of co-occurrence among multiple types of child maltreatment. Child & Family Social Work. 2017;22:492–502. doi: 10.1111/cfs.12268. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Krueger RF, Kotov R, Watson D, Forbes MK, Eaton NR, Ruggero CJ, Simms LJ, Widiger TA, Achenbach TM, Bach B, Bagby RM, Bornovalova MA, Carpenter WT, Chmielewski M, Cicero DC, Clark LA, Conway C, DeClercq B, DeYoung CG, Docherty AR, Drislane LE, First MB, Forbush KT, Hallquist M, Haltigan JD, Hopwood CJ, Ivanova MY, Jonas KG, Latzman RD, Markon KE, Miller JD, Morey LC, Mullins-Sweatt SN, Ormel J, Patalay P, Patrick CJ, Pincus AL, Regier DA, Reininghaus U, Rescorla LA, Samuel DB, Sellbom M, Shackman AJ, Skodol A, Slade T, South SC, Sunderland M, Tackett JL, Venables NC, Waldman ID, Waszczuk MA, Waugh MH, Wright AGC, Zald DH, Zimmermann J. Progress in achieving quantitative classification of psychopathology. World Psychiatry. 2018;17:282–293. doi: 10.1002/wps.20566. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Krug EG, Mercy JA, Dahlberg LL, Zwi AB. The world report on violence and health. Lancet. 2002;360:1083–1088. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(02)11133-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Langevin R, Hébert M, Cossette L. Emotion regulation as a mediator of the relation between sexual abuse and behavior problems in preschoolers. Child Abuse & Neglect. 2015;46:16–26. doi: 10.1016/J.CHIABU.2015.02.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Lassri D, Luyten P, Cohen G, Shahar G. The effect of childhood emotional maltreatment on romantic relationships in young adulthood: A double mediation model involving self-criticism and attachment. Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research, Practice, and Policy. 2016;8(4):504–511. doi: 10.1037/tra0000134. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Leeb RT, Lewis T, Zolotor AJ. A review of physical and mental health consequences of child abuse and neglect and implications for practice. American Journal of Lifestyle Medicine. 2011;5:454–468. doi: 10.1177/1559827611410266. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  55. Loevaas MES, Sund AM, Lydersen S, Neumer S-P, Martinsen K, Holen S, Patras J, Adolfsen F, Reinfjell T. Does the transdiagnostic EMOTION intervention improve emotion regulation skills in children? Journal of Child and Family Studies. 2019;28:805–813. doi: 10.1007/s10826-018-01324-1. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  56. Maguire SA, Williams B, Naughton AM, Cowley LE, Tempest V, Mann MK, Teague M, Kemp AM. A systematic review of the emotional, behavioural and cognitive features exhibited by school-aged children experiencing neglect or emotional abuse. Child: Care, Health and Development. 2015;41:641–653. doi: 10.1111/cch.12227. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Manly JT, Kim JE, Rogosch FA, Cicchetti D. Dimensions of child maltreatment and children’s adjustment: Contributions of developmental timing and subtype. Development and Psychopathology. 2001;13:759–782. doi: 10.1017/S0954579401004023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Mennen FE, Kim K, Sang J, Trickett PK. Child neglect: Definition and identification of youth’s experiences in official reports of maltreatment. Child Abuse & Neglect. 2010;34:647–658. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2010.02.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Mills R, Alati R, O’Callaghan M, Najman JM, Williams GM, Bor W, Strathearn L. Child abuse and neglect and cognitive function at 14 years of age: Findings from a birth cohort. Pediatrics. 2011;127:4–10. doi: 10.1542/peds.2009-3479. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Moody G, Cannings-John R, Hood K, Kemp A, Robling M. Establishing the international prevalence of self-reported child maltreatment: A systematic review by maltreatment type and gender. BMC Public Health. 2018;18:1–15. doi: 10.1186/s12889-018-6044-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Norman RE, Byambaa M, De R, Butchart A, Scott J, Vos T. The long-term health consequences of child physical abuse, emotional abuse, and neglect: A systematic review and meta-analysis. PLoS Medicine. 2012;9:e1001349. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1001349. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. O’Hara M, Legano L, Homel P, Walker-Descartes I, Rojas M, Laraque D. Children neglected: Where cumulative risk theory fails. Child Abuse & Neglect. 2015;45:1–8. doi: 10.1016/J.CHIABU.2015.03.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Paquette D, Laporte L, Bigras M, Zoccolillo M. Validation de la version française du CTQ et prévalence de l’histoire de maltraitance. Santé Mentale au Québec. 2004;29:201–220. doi: 10.7202/008831ar. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Paus T, Keshavan M, Giedd JN. Why do many psychiatric disorders emerge during adolescence? Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2008;9:947–957. doi: 10.1038/nrn2513. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Peh CX, Shahwan S, Fauziana R, Mahesh MV, Sambasivam R, Zhang Y, Ong SH, Chong SA, Subramaniam M. Emotion dysregulation as a mechanism linking child maltreatment exposure and self-harm behaviors in adolescents. Child Abuse & Neglect. 2017;67:383–390. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2017.03.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Pollak SD, Cicchetti D, Hornung K, Reed A. Recognizing emotion in faces: Developmental effects of child abuse and neglect. Developmental Psychology. 2000;36:679–688. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.36.5.679. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Poole JC, Dobson KS, Pusch D. Do adverse childhood experiences predict adult interpersonal difficulties? The role of emotion dysregulation. Child Abuse & Neglect. 2018;80:123–133. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2018.03.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Puetz, V. B., Viding, E., Gerin, M. I., Pingault, J. B., Sethi, A., Knodt, A. R., Radtke, S. R., Brigidi, B. D., Hariri, A. R., & McCrory, E. (2019). Investigating patterns of neural response associated with childhood abuse v. childhood neglect. Psychological medicine, 13, 1–10. 10.1017/S003329171900134X [DOI] [PubMed]
  69. Rossouw TI, Fonagy P. Mentalization-based treatment for self-harm in adolescents: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 2012;51:1304–1313.e3. doi: 10.1016/J.JAAC.2012.09.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Rudrauf D, Debbané M. Building a cybernetic model of psychopathology: Beyond the metaphor. Psychological Inquiry. 2018;29:156–164. doi: 10.1080/1047840X.2018.1513685. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  71. Shaffer A, Yates T, Egeland B. The relation of emotional maltreatment to early adolescent competence: Developmental processes in a prospective study. Child Abuse & Neglect. 2009;33:36–44. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2008.12.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Shahar, G. (2015). Erosion: The psychopathology of self-criticism. Oxford University Press.
  73. Sharp C, Williams LL, Ha C, Baumgardner J, Michonski J, Seals R, Patel AB, Bielberg E, Fonagy P. The development of a mentalization-based outcomes and research protocol for an adolescent inpatient unit. Bulletin of the Menninger Clinic. 2009;73:311–338. doi: 10.1521/bumc.2009.73.4.311. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Shipman K, Edwards A, Brown A, Swisher L, Jennings E. Managing emotion in a maltreating context: A pilot study examining child neglect. Child Abuse and Neglect. 2005;29:1015–1029. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2005.01.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Spratt EG, Friedenberg SL, Swenson CC, Larosa A, De Bellis MD, Macias MM, Summer AP, Hulsey TC, Runyan DK, Brady KT. The effects of early neglect on cognitive, language, and behavioral functioning in childhood. Psychology. 2012;3:175–182. doi: 10.4236/psych.2012.32026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Steinberg L, Morris AS. Adolescent development. Annual Review of Psychology. 2001;52:83–110. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.83. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Sturge-Apple ML, Davies PT, Cummings EM. Impact of hostility and withdrawal in interparental conflict on parental emotional unavailability and children's adjustment difficulties. Child Development. 2006;77:1623–1641. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00963.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Sukhodolsky DG, Kassinove H, Gorman BS. Cognitive-behavioral therapy for anger in children and adolescents: A meta-analysis. Aggression and Violent Behavior. 2004;9:247–269. doi: 10.1016/J.AVB.2003.08.005. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  79. Swannell S, Martin G, Page A, Hasking P, Hazell P, Taylor A, Protani M. Child maltreatment, subsequent non-suicidal self-injury and the mediating roles of dissociation, alexithymia and self-blame. Child Abuse and Neglect. 2012;36:572–584. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2012.05.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Toth SL, Cicchetti D, Macfie J, Emde RN. Representations of self and other in the narratives of neglected, physically abused, and sexually abused preschoolers. Development and Psychopathology. 1997;9:781–796. doi: 10.1017/S0954579497001430. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Wechsler, D. (1997). Manual of the Wechsler adult intelligence scale–third edition (WAIS-III). Psychological Corporation.
  82. Wechsler, D. (2003). WISC-IV technical and interpretive manual. Psychological Corporation.
  83. Wen Z, Fan X. Monotonicity of effect sizes: Questioning kappa-squared as mediation effect size measure. Psychological Methods. 2015;20:193–203. doi: 10.1037/met0000029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Widom CS, DuMont K, Czaja SJ. A prospective investigation of major depressive disorder and comorbidity in abused and neglected children grown up. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2007;64:49–56. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.1.49. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Young KS, Sandman CF, Craske MG. Positive and negative emotion regulation in adolescence: Links to anxiety and depression. Brain Sciences. 2019;9(4):76. doi: 10.3390/brainsci9040076. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Journal of Child & Adolescent Trauma are provided here courtesy of Springer

RESOURCES