Abstract
Violence against children and adolescents, a highly prevalent problem, is a clear violation of child rights and has detrimental effects on later life outcomes. Programs that alleviate poverty address a structural determinant of child vulnerability and can thereby reduce child abuse. This paper investigates whether the Government of Zimbabwe’s Harmonized Social Cash Transfer (HSCT) Program, which combines cash transfers with complementary services, affects youth exposure to physical violence. The analysis uses data from a non-experimental impact evaluation and a difference-in-differences approach. Results show a 19-percentage point decline in the incidence of physical violence among youth four years into the program. HSCT-induced enhancements in beneficiary households’ purchasing capacity and food security, improvements in caregiver subjective well-being, and reductions in youth participation in economic work for pay could be mediating the program’s effects on youth abuse. This paper adds to the relatively scarce evidence on the impacts of anti-poverty policies on young people’s susceptibility to physical violence in developing countries.
Keywords: cash transfers, violence against children, child protection, social protection, children and youth, Zimbabwe
I. Introduction
Violence against children can take the form of “physical or mental violence, injury or abuse, neglect or negligent treatment, maltreatment or exploitation, including sexual abuse” (United Nations (UN) General Assembly, 1989; UN Committee on the Rights of the Child, 2011). More than one billion children (individuals aged two to 17 years), or approximately half of all children around the world, are likely being subjected to violence every year (Hillis et al., 2016). Often, authority figures such as caregivers and teachers are the perpetrators of physical violence against children, although peers are also common offenders. When it comes to the emotional abuse of children, parents and caretakers are again most likely to be responsible. Young children are particularly vulnerable to sexual abuse by individuals they know such as caregivers, while adolescents tend to be victimized outside their home. Intimate partners are the likely perpetrators of sexual violence against adolescent girls (United Nations International Children’s FUND (UNICEF), 2014).
Child abuse is a clear violation of child rights and has detrimental effects on later life outcomes. Experiences with violence in childhood aggravate mental health issues such as depression (Paolucci et al., 2001; Gershoff, 2002); hinder cognitive, social and emotional development (World Health Organization (WHO) & International Society for Prevention of Child Abuse and Neglect (ISPCAN), 2006); affect academic achievement (Paolucci et al., 2001; Ogando Portela & Pells, 2015); and increase the likelihood of being a victim to and/or perpetrator of violence in the future (Paolucci et al., 2001; Gershoff, 2002; Abramsky et al., 2011).
Poverty and other economic factors such as income inequality increase the risk for child neglect and abuse (WHO & ISPCAN, 2006; Hussey et al., 2006; Gilbert et al., 2009; Akmatov, 2011; Cancian et al., 2013; Meinck et al., 2015). Financial deprivation-child maltreatment linkages might emerge, for example, if caregiver stress from economic hardships leads to the neglect or maltreatment of children. Poor families might also use child or early marriage to relieve strains on household resources, which might expose children to abusive situations. Alternatively, youth in impoverished households might resort to risky practices, such as transactional sex, thus increasing their vulnerability to violence (Peterman et al., 2017).
Given that economic deprivation increases children’s susceptibility to abuse and maltreatment, child protection programs have often incorporated financial strengthening strategies such as cash and in-kind transfers, skills training, microfinance and support for job searches (Marcus & Page, 2014). In this analysis, we explore whether one such program is able to protect young people from abuse. Specifically, we identify the impacts of a government program providing unconditional cash transfers (CT) to households on youth exposure to physical violence.1 This paper adds to the relatively scarce evidence on the impacts of anti-poverty policies on young people’s susceptibility to physical violence in developing countries. Focusing on physical violence is important since acts such as physical discipline or corporal punishment might constitute one of “the most common form of violence against children” (UNICEF, 2014).2 By youth, we refer to individuals aged 13 to 24 years. We are thus able to look at the crucial period of adolescence—a time of major neuro-behavioral changes (Dahl et al., 2018) during which negative events such as exposure to violence could substantially shape an individual’s developmental trajectory. At the extreme, physical violence could lead to serious injury and in fact, unintentional and intentional injuries are a major cause of mortality among adolescents—accounting for 28 and 44 percent of deaths globally among those aged 10 to 14 years and 15 to 19 years respectively.
Our investigation adds to the literature on the relationship between CTs and violence, the bulk of which has looked specifically at domestic violence against women (Angelucci, 2008; Bobonis et al., 2013; Hidrobo and Fernald, 2013; Hidrobo et al., 2016, Buller et al., 2018; Heath et al., 2018). There are several studies that have examined the effects of social safety nets (SSNs) on youth or child exposure to violence, but while the collated evidence suggest that such programs could protect against sexual violence, it is not clear whether similar benefits exist for physically violent acts such as violent discipline by parents (Peterman et al., 2017).3 The Government of Zimbabwe’s Harmonized Social Cash Transfer Program (HSCT) delivers unconditional CTs to food-poor households with high dependency ratios in order to reduce food insecurity and protect orphans and vulnerable children (OVC). Apart from providing transfers, the HSCT addresses child protection by disseminating supplemental information on child wellbeing at payment distribution sites—this information aims to increase sensitivity to violence against children and awareness of social welfare services available for children—as well as by providing support in accessing required services. Moving forward, we refer to the non-cash component of this cash plus program as services or as complementary services.
Soon after the HSCT was launched in 2012, a non-experimental impact evaluation was set up to study the program’s effects on household and child well-being. Treatment households started receiving transfer payments after a baseline survey in May 2013 and a 12-month follow-up survey was conducted in July 2014. The first complementary services for children in beneficiary households began just before the 12-month follow-up survey so that the treatment at this follow-up survey essentially only consisted of cash. The 48-month follow-up survey took place in July and August 2017, four years after the cash transfers began and three years after the full suite of complementary services were initiated in treatment sites.
In this analysis, we use data from the three surveys of the HSCT impact evaluation to investigate whether the provision of cash and services changes the exposure of young individuals to physical abuse. We find no significant effects of the HSCT on youth exposure to violence 12-months into the program, but the impact estimates identified are positively signed suggesting increases in violence among treatment youth at the time. Perhaps the recently introduced HSCT complementary services had started sensitizing youth in CT households to issues related to violence and prompted greater reporting of abuse, but the program had not been operating long enough to impact actual levels of violence. In contrast, we detect a 19-percentage point decline in the incidence of violence among HSCT youth at the 48-month follow-up. An investigation into violence perpetrators shows that the overall reductions in abuse are not being driven by specific types of perpetrators.
To confirm that our results do represent the effect of the HSCT on violence reduction, we examine whether the program affects potential mechanisms. We find that beneficiary households increase the purchase of all consumption items as well as food, and experience higher levels of food security than households in the comparison group. The main survey respondents from the treatment group also report higher levels of subjective well-being and appear to be more optimistic about their future. This suggests that the alleviation of financial insecurity and the associated reduction in stress enables caregivers in HSCT households to provide a more protective environment for their youth. The influx of cash also appears to allow households to withdraw their youth from potentially dangerous casual and part-time work settings.
The rest of the paper is structured as follows. Section 2 discusses the linkages between poverty and child abuse. We describe the HSCT program, data, and the study sample and methods in Sections 3, 4 and 5 respectively. Section 6 contains the results and Section 7 covers robustness checks. We conclude with a discussion in Section 8.
II. Background
CTs are currently being used around the world as a major anti-poverty tool, and while their main objective is to mitigate the negative effects of extreme poverty, by specifically targeting a key structural risk factor for child abuse and maltreatment, these programs could also reduce youth exposure to violence. However, while much research has been conducted on the effects of these programs on child nutrition and health (Lagarde et al., 2007; Fiszbein et al., 2009; Leroy et al., 2009; Manley et al., 2013; Owusu-Addo & Cross, 2014; Bastagli et al., 2016; de Groot et al., 2017), and schooling (Fiszbein et al., 2009; Baird et al., 2013; Bastagli et al., 2016), impacts on childhood violence remain relatively unexplored (Peterman et al., 2017). Peterman et al. (2017) develop a framework to understand the pathways through which SSNs might impact emotional, physical and sexual violence against children.4 Given that SSNs are usually targeted to households, the authors postulate that they are likely to first influence household-level mechanisms and subsequently trigger caregiver/interpersonal-level and/or child-level pathways.
The household-level pathways are hypothesized to include economic security, labor force participation, time use, intra-household power dynamics and acute/chronic stress levels. To illustrate one way in which these factors could shape the risk of abuse—the alleviation of budget constraints has the potential to reduce stress levels of adults in the household and thereby deter them from physically disciplining young individuals. Alternatively, poverty alleviation within households could prevent youth from resorting to risky practices, such as transactional sex, that might expose them to violence.
At the caregiver and interpersonal level, SSNs could impact substance use, psychosocial wellbeing, caregiving practices, and conflict/violence within the household. For instance, improved caregiver wellbeing due to the income support could facilitate the adoption of better parenting practices and thereby reduce children’s problem behaviors. Child-level pathways encompass time in school, psychosocial wellbeing, time spent in risky settings, child marriage, and problem and risk behaviors. As an example, SSN-induced increases in school participation might cause children to withdraw from hazardous work environments, but might also put them at risk of violence perpetrated by authority figures and peers in school, as well as individuals encountered during commutes to school.
Peterman and colleagues emphasize that the violence effects of the different potential mechanisms are likely to be moderated by contextual factors (such as the established policy, institutional and legal framework) and by household, caregiver and child vulnerability characteristics (examples of which are social isolation and discrimination based on HIV status). SSN program features (like targeting and the specified gender of the recipient) are also bound to determine the extent to which programs are able to affect childhood violence.
The authors go on to review the evidence from 14 studies that explore child experiences of violence in households benefitting from SSNs. They conclude that while such programs have been able to protect adolescent girls in Africa from sexual violence, they have not had consistent impacts in other contexts or on younger children. Where program-driven changes in physical violence have been detected, the populations and specific outcomes examined have differed from the population and measure we are interested in. Two separate investigations into the impacts of an unconditional CT in Ecuador on child development find effects on the use of harsh parenting practices, but these hold only for certain sub-groups. Paxson and Schady (2010) see a reduction in the violent disciplining of children aged three to seven years in the poorest households, and Fernald and Hidrobo (2011) show a marginally significant increase in the violent disciplining of children aged one to three years in urban households. Note that the populations of interest in these two studies are much younger than the youth population we focus on. The one study that does point to CT-induced reductions in physical violence among adolescents (within the context of a conditional CT in rural South Africa) focuses just on girls and on abuse by intimate partners (Pettifor et al., 2016). Since caregivers, teachers and peers are common perpetrators of physical violence against youth (UNICEF, 2014), our broader categorization of physical abuse can speak to the potential of CTs to reduce risks from various quarters.
It is worth pointing out here that even though the relationship between CTs and child violence remains fairly understudied (Peterman et al, 2017), multiple studies have examined whether transfer programs have consequences for domestic violence against women (or intimate partner violence (IPV)) and shown that CTs can be a promising tool in reducing the exposure of women to abuse, while highlighting that effects are likely to depend on program features, household characteristics or contextual factors (Angelucci, 2008; Bobonis et al., 2013; Hidrobo & Fernald, 2013; Hidrobo et al., 2016, Buller et al., 2018; Heath et al., 2018).5 Given existing evidence on the protective effects of CTs for women, it is important to understand whether such beneficial effects hold for the different types of violence faced by other vulnerable demographics such as children.
III. Program description & study design
Initiated in 2012, the HSCT in Zimbabwe is oriented towards alleviating chronic food insecurity and poverty, and protecting OVCs. The program, which is part of the National Action Plan (NAP) for OVC, intends to harmonize the delivery of child protection interventions and cash transfers to poor households (Carolina Population Center, 2018; de Arruda, 2018).
Jointly funded by the Government of Zimbabwe, donors and UNICEF, the HSCT program targets labor constrained and food poor households. Based on data collected through a census in program wards, the Ministry of Public Service, Labor and Social Welfare (MPSLSW), which implements the program, identifies beneficiary households that meet the eligibility criteria. Households are classified as labor constrained if they do not have an able-bodied member between the ages of 18 and 59 years who has been able to work for more than three months, have a dependency ratio of three or more, or have a severely disabled or chronically ill member needing intensive care. Food poor households are those that fall below the food poverty line and are incapable of meeting essential needs—they consume one or no meals a day; are unable to purchase basic non-food items such as clothing; depend on begging or piece work; do not own valuable assets; and receive no regular support from relatives, pension systems or existing welfare programs.6 Labor constrained and food poor households are expected to contain the most vulnerable individuals—the poorest, orphans, elderly, disabled, chronically ill and those living with or affected by HIV (Seidenfeld et al., 2016).
Under the HSCT program, transfers are made to beneficiary households once in two months at designated payment points. The monthly value of the transfer depends on household size—$10, $15, $20 and $25 for households with one, two, three, and four or more members respectively. Households that are targeted by the program tend to be large (the median household size in the study sample at baseline is five) and so the majority of beneficiary households receive $25 per month. This is about 20 percent of pre-program monthly household expenditure (American Institutes for Research, 2013).
In order to enhance HSCT’s protective effects for children, several non-cash components have been incorporated into the program.7 Government agencies and non-government organization (NGO) partners have staff available at HSCT payment points to discuss welfare and protection issues for all children (child abuse is one of the topics covered), but also those that are especially relevant for children with disabilities and/or HIV/AIDS.8 Evidence from different settings indicates that conveying information on gender-based or IPV (through entertaining videos screened to groups of individuals) can help change attitudes towards violence (Banerjee et al., 2019), the willingness to report incidents, or even actual levels of violence (Green et al., 2019). Thus, information provided by the HSCT on child wellbeing could play an important role in shaping youth experiences with violence. HSCT payment points also have help desks available to take reports on a variety of cases of children in need of services—for example, those related to disability, to neglect or violence, and to birth registration—and these cases are referred to the appropriate agencies. For example, children with disabilities are directed to the National Case Management system. Efforts are made to link households to the health, education, protection and legal services they require. Finally, community-based volunteers are trained to identify, assist with and monitor children and families that need support.
An impact evaluation study was set up soon after HSCT’s launch to identify program impacts on food security, human development, child protection and other outcomes. The study design took advantage of the phased rollout of the HSCT across the country. Treatment households were from wards in early scale-up areas that entered the program in 2013 or Phase two (in the districts of Binga, Mwenzi and Mudzi) and comparison households were from wards in later scale-up areas that would enter the program in phase four, which would begin after the end of the study, and that were matched to the treatment wards (in the districts of UMP, Chiredzi and Hwange).9 The primary sampling unit in the study was the ward, the lowest administrative unit of the program. Sixty wards were sampled from intervention areas. These sixty wards were then matched to thirty wards in non-treatment areas on a range of agro-ecological characteristics (geography) and economic conditions (access to services, level of development), for a total of 90 study wards.
Regular program targeting was conducted by the MPSLSW according to HSCT operational guidelines in all the intervention wards as well as in the non-intervention wards. This resulted in a list of eligible beneficiary households in each of the 90 wards selected for the study. From these eligibility lists, the study team randomly selected 34 households per ward to obtain the desired sample size. A total of 3,063 household were ultimately surveyed at the baseline in 2013.
There are several key features of the study design worth highlighting. First, all study households were eligible for the HSCT, regardless of study arm. Second, households in Phase 2 regions entered the program in 2013 shortly after the baseline survey, while households in Phase 4 regions were scheduled to enter the program after the completion of the HSCT evaluation study. Third, delayed entry wards were matched to early entry wards on factors that might affect the evolution of outcomes, such as agro-ecological and economic conditions. Fourth, the targeting of households was supply-driven, and done through a census that listed all households in a ward and captured their information to estimate poverty scores and demographic composition. Take-up of the program by eligible households was universal (no household refused the transfer) and there were no behavioral conditions.
The implementation of the precise program targeting procedure in all wards (both early- and late-entry) led to an observationally equivalent comparison group—this is demonstrated by the household-level baseline balance tests we perform for the study sample in Table A1 in the Appendix (columns 1 and 2). Given our focus on youth in the current analysis, we also compare treatment and control households with youth members at baseline (columns 3 and 4), and the treatment and control households whose youth members actually participated in the HSCT impact evaluation’s youth module at any survey round, the module that provides the main data for our analysis as described below (columns 5 and 6). These statistics indicate that the HSCT evaluation study design produced a very strong non-experimental counterfactual for the treatment households in the study sample. In the absence of multiple rounds of pre-HSCT data, we cannot, however, formally test whether there were parallel trends across treatment status for our outcome of interest in the years before the program began. The assumption is that since treatment wards were matched to control wards on a wide range of agro-ecological and economic characteristics, they would have progressed similarly in the absence of the HSCT. The baseline survey was conducted in May 2013, treatment households started receiving transfers shortly after, and a 12-month follow-up survey was conducted in July 2014. Some of the complementary services for the children in beneficiary households began just before the first follow-up survey, but the full suite of complementary services began in earnest only after the 12-month survey. The delays were due to the time it took to finalize memorandums of understanding with partner organizations and to develop the protocols for the services being provided. The 48-month follow-up survey took place in July and August 2017. Each of the surveys were conducted after the annual harvest in Zimbabwe and are thus likely to capture household conditions at a time when food stores and resources are at a peak (Carolina Population Center, 2018). Institutional Review Board (IRB) approvals were secured from the American Institutes for Research’s Institutional Review Board and the Medical Research Council of Zimbabwe.
The military coup that ousted longtime president Robert Mugabe in November 2017 and the subsequent general election in Zimbabwe in July 2018 took place after the HSCT study ended. While the years that preceded these major events witnessed substantial political violence, it is important to point out that the study was purposefully designed to include areas dominated by the two major parties in the country, the Zimbabwe African National Union – Patriotic Front (ZANU-PF), the governing party, and the Movement for Democratic Change (MDC), the main opposition party. Thus, any results using the HSCT impact evaluation data are unlikely to be systematically biased by political activities, events or violence occurring in certain regions of Zimbabwe during the study period.
Several studies have documented the impacts of the HSCT on different outcomes, some using the data we employ in the current analysis. Bhalla et al. (2018) document that the HSCT improves food security and diet diversity in beneficiary households. In addition, the authors find that the HSCT enables households to purchase more food from markets and rely less on gifts of food, thereby increasing households’ ability to exercise choices over their food basket. Handa et al. (2018) pull information from several large government unconditional CTs in sub-Saharan Africa (SSA), including the Zimbabwe HSCT, to show that common negative perceptions regarding CTs do not hold for these programs—for example, they do not lead to higher consumption of temptation goods (alcohol and tobacco), they do not foster dependency, and neither do they create community-level price distortions and inflation. Finally, two multi-country qualitative analyses show that the HSCT, like other CTs in SSA, improves psychosocial wellbeing (Attah et al., 2016), and enhances livelihood choices and productive investments, though these effects are mediated by household vulnerabilities, labor constraints and the availability of economic opportunities (Fisher et al., 2017). Note that no other study systematically investigates how the HSCT has impacted youth wellbeing over time, which is what we do in the current analysis with regard to exposure to physical violence.
IV. Data and measures
At each wave of the HSCT evaluation study, a household survey collected data on a wide range of topics such as education, health, time use, household enterprises, credit, food security, social networks, expenditures and livestock. In addition, a separate youth survey was administered to understand how the program shaped outcomes experienced during the critical period when young individuals are transitioning to adulthood.10 The youth were asked about their sexual experiences, mental health, alcohol consumption, HIV and experiences with physical violence. These surveys were conducted in private after seeking consent from both parents and respondents (only the respondents) for those 17 years or younger (18 years or older).
The HSCT youth module asked respondents about their victimization to three categories of physical violence during the 12 months prior to the survey:
Has anyone ever slapped or pushed you?
Has anyone ever hit you with a fist, kicked you, or beat you with an object?
Has anyone ever used or threatened to use a knife or other weapon against you?11
Youth who responded to a physical violence question in the affirmative were also asked to specify whether the perpetrator of the last violent act was a parent/adult relative, boyfriend/girlfriend/intimate partner, authority figure (teacher/religious/community leader), peer/classmate, or other actor (for example, stranger). This information was collected separately for each type of violence that was measured.
The primary violence outcome that we examine in this analysis is an indicator variable that captures exposure to any kind of physical violence. The second aggregated measure of violence that we use denotes severe physical violence and it accounts for whether youth were hit with a fist, kicked or beaten with an object, or whether they were attacked or threatened with a knife or other weapon. We also look at the violence categories separately. In order to understand whether there are any changes in the violence committed by different categories of actors, we use categorical variables for the perpetrator of each type of violence. For brevity, we refer to the five categories as relative, partner, authority figure, peer and other.
As discussed in the background section above, an intervention like the HSCT could shape youth exposure to violence through different channels. The data collected through the household and youth surveys of the HSCT evaluation study allows us to explore whether there were effects on several of these potential channels. Specifically, we investigate household purchases and food security; caregiver subjective well-being and optimism; and youth schooling, work and partnership status. Table A2 in the Appendix describes all the outcomes that we examine in our analysis.
V. Sample and methods
The HSCT study sample at baseline consisted of 3,063 households—2,029 in the treatment group and 1,034 in the comparison group. Of these households, 86 percent and 84 percent were resurveyed at the 12-month and 48-month follow-up surveys respectively. There is no evidence of differential attrition by treatment status during either of the follow-up surveys (see Table A3).
At each survey wave, up to three youth were selected from every household to be interviewed for the youth module; if more than three eligible youth were present, the youngest three were prioritized. At baseline, youth between the ages of 13 and 20 years were surveyed, and at the 12-month follow-up survey, the age range for eligibility for the youth module was adjusted to 14–21 years. At the last follow-up, the age range for the youth module was further adjusted to 13–24 years to capture youth who were originally targeted at baseline, and also to allow for younger children to age into the youth module in anticipation of future data collection. The response rates to the HSCT impact evaluation youth survey was particularly low during the first two surveys—917 of 2,825 eligible youth (or 32 per cent) and 807 of 2,319 eligible youth (or 35 per cent) were interviewed during the baseline and 12-month follow-up surveys respectively. Of the 3,452 youth who were to be interviewed at final follow-up, 2,319 (or 67 percent) were surveyed. Given the low response rates to the first two rounds of the youth survey and the time gap between the first and last surveys (four years), there was a low likelihood of following the same youth across time—only 73 individuals were surveyed for the youth module at all three survey waves.
Table 1 shows that there were differences in characteristics between youth who were surveyed and eligible youth who were not surveyed at every survey wave—for example, those not interviewed were typically older (the age differences between non-respondents and respondents are statistically significant at baseline and 48-months), and from households with older heads (significant at all waves).12 However, as demonstrated in Table 2, the treatment and comparison group respondents at each wave are well balanced on a range of pre-treatment measures.13 Given the comparability of surveyed youth in treatment and control, contrasting young person’s outcomes across study arms allows us to approximate how the program shaped different outcomes, though the identified effects likely hold only for the surveyed youth who tend to be younger on average than all youth residing in the study households.
Table 1:
Variable | Baseline | 12-month follow-up | 48-month follow-up | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Youth Non-Respondents | Youth Respondents | Youth Non-Respondents | Youth Respondents | Youth Non-Respondents | Youth Respondents | |
Youth characteristics | ||||||
Female | 0.50 | 0.49 | 0.47 | 0.51* | 0.45 | 0.47 |
Age | 15.43 | 15.28* | 16.31 | 16.30 | 16.98 | 16.52*** |
Married/cohabiting at baselineǂ | 0.06 | 0.01*** | 0.04 | 0.03* | 0.02 | 0.04 |
Attending school at baselineǂ | 0.63 | 0.66 | 0.68 | 0.70 | 0.80 | 0.87*** |
Household head’s characteristics (baseline values) | ||||||
Female | 0.69 | 0.69 | 0.67 | 0.71 | 0.67 | 0.68 |
Age | 56.54 | 50.94*** | 58.28 | 51.95** | 54.76 | 53.43* |
Widow | 0.31 | 0.32 | 0.32 | 0.29 | 0.31 | 0.29 |
Divorced/separated | 0.09 | 0.08 | 0.09 | 0.09 | 0.08 | 0.10 |
Has some schooling | 0.63 | 0.61 | 0.59 | 0.65** | 0.63 | 0.63 |
Highest grade | 3.71 | 3.92 | 3.47 | 3.85** | 3.75 | 3.74 |
Household characteristics (baseline values) | ||||||
Household size | 6.53 | 6.65 | 6.46 | 6.42 | 6.01 | 6.22* |
Number of individuals 0–5 years | 0.86 | 0.86 | 0.84 | 0.88 | 0.82 | 0.91** |
Number of individuals 6–17 years | 3.25 | 3.42* | 3.21 | 3.23 | 2.92 | 3.06* |
Number of individuals 18–59 years | 1.64 | 1.64 | 1.60 | 1.55 | 1.48 | 1.53 |
Number of individuals 60 years and above | 0.78 | 0.72 | 0.81 | 0.75 | 0.79 | 0.72** |
Cares for at least one orphan | 0.16 | 0.13* | 0.17 | 0.15 | 0.10 | 0.11 |
Has a chronically ill member | 0.04 | 0.04 | 0.03 | 0.03 | 0.00 | 0.07*** |
Has a member with a disability | 0.03 | 0.02 | 0.04 | 0.02** | 0.18 | 0.04 |
Per capita expenditure | 22.70 | 23.61 | 23.07 | 24.08 | 24.28 | 23.89 |
Share of budget spent on food | 0.61 | 0.60 | 0.60 | 0.60 | 0.61 | 0.61 |
Owns or cultivated land in last year | 0.95 | 0.96 | 0.97 | 0.96 | 0.95 | 0.95 |
Total land operated (in hectares) | 6.05 | 8.40 | 7.13 | 4.99** | 6.10 | 6.60 |
Planted crops during last rainy season | 0.93 | 0.93 | 0.95 | 0.94 | 0.93 | 0.92 |
Uses crop production inputs | 0.27 | 0.28 | 0.26 | 0.29 | 0.30 | 0.25*** |
Owns livestock | 0.84 | 0.85 | 0.86 | 0.83 | 0.84 | 0.84 |
Operates non-farm business | 0.14 | 0.15 | 0.13 | 0.14 | 0.12 | 0.13 |
Someone was in wage employment at any time in last year | 0.13 | 0.15* | 0.12 | 0.14 | 0.11 | 0.11 |
Receives cash/labor/in-kind aid | 0.65 | 0.64 | 0.65 | 0.67 | 0.64 | 0.66 |
Provides cash/labor/in-kind aid | 0.24 | 0.25 | 0.24 | 0.23 | 0.23 | 0.25 |
Observations | 1,908 | 917 | 1,512 | 807 | 1,133 | 2,319 |
Statistically significant mean differences across groups based on Wald tests are represented by:
p<0.01
p<0.05
p<0.1.
The standard errors used for these tests are clustered at the ward-level.
These variables were measured at baseline only for those who were recorded as household members at the time. Baseline marital/cohabiting status is available for 75 percent of eligible youth across survey rounds (this information was collected only for those aged 12 years of older), and baseline school enrolment is available for 88 percent of eligible youth.
Table 2:
Baseline | 12-month follow-up | 48-month follow-up | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Control | Treatment | Control | Treatment | Control | Treatment | |
Youth characteristics | ||||||
Female | 0.48 | 0.52 | 0.60 | 0.62 | 0.49 | 0.47 |
Age | 15.33 | 15.32 | 16.49 | 16.15* | 17.14 | 16.74* |
Married/cohabiting at baselineǂ | 0.06 | 0.01 | 0.06 | 0.03 | 0.07 | 0.04 |
Attending school at baselineǂ | 0.55 | 0.63* | 0.66 | 0.69 | 0.79 | 0.85* |
Household head’s characteristics (baseline values) | ||||||
Female | 0.62 | 0.67 | 0.66 | 0.68 | 0.66 | 0.69 |
Age | 54.16 | 50.16* | 49.98 | 51.52 | 53.33 | 52.55 |
Widow | 0.33 | 0.30 | 0.29 | 0.28 | 0.29 | 0.28 |
Divorced/separated | 0.05 | 0.10 | 0.07 | 0.09 | 0.08 | 0.11 |
Has some schooling | 0.63 | 0.60 | 0.71 | 0.63 | 0.64 | 0.64 |
Highest grade | 3.70 | 3.68 | 4.19 | 3.91 | 3.78 | 3.82 |
Household characteristics (baseline values) | ||||||
Household size | 7.61 | 7.09 | 6.84 | 6.55 | 6.66 | 6.47 |
Number of individuals 0–5 years | 1.14 | 0.86* | 1.09 | 0.82* | 1.05 | 0.98 |
Number of individuals 6–17 years | 3.83 | 3.67 | 3.44 | 3.44 | 3.20 | 3.24 |
Number of individuals 18–59 years | 1.87 | 1.81 | 1.59 | 1.56 | 1.66 | 1.55 |
Number of individuals 60 and above years | 0.77 | 0.74 | 0.71 | 0.72 | 0.74 | 0.69 |
Cares for at least one orphan | 0.15 | 0.16 | 0.18 | 0.13 | 0.09 | 0.14** |
Has a chronically ill member | 0.03 | 0.03 | 0.04 | 0.02 | 0.04 | 0.06 |
Has a member with a disability | 0.03 | 0.04 | 0.01 | 0.02 | 0.04 | 0.02 |
Per capita expenditure | 23.73 | 22.11 | 27.21 | 25.94 | 25.11 | 24.23 |
Share of budget spent on food | 0.60 | 0.60 | 0.62 | 0.61 | 0.61 | 0.62 |
Owns or cultivated land in last year | 0.98 | 0.98 | 0.93 | 0.97 | 0.93 | 0.96* |
Total land operated (in hectares) | 10.61 | 5.87 | 5.83 | 4.80 | 6.72 | 5.60 |
Planted crops during last rainy season | 0.96 | 0.94 | 0.90 | 0.95 | 0.91 | 0.92 |
Uses crop production inputs | 0.31 | 0.27 | 0.28 | 0.29 | 0.24 | 0.23 |
Owns livestock | 0.88 | 0.87 | 0.78 | 0.85 | 0.87 | 0.84 |
Operates non-farm business | 0.22 | 0.13* | 0.19 | 0.09** | 0.15 | 0.10 |
Someone was in wage employment at any time in last year | 0.16 | 0.15 | 0.16 | 0.14 | 0.12 | 0.11 |
Receives cash/labor/in-kind aid | 0.72 | 0.64 | 0.75 | 0.66 | 0.73 | 0.66 |
Provides cash/labor/in-kind aid | 0.23 | 0.30 | 0.23 | 0.27 | 0.25 | 0.25 |
Observations | 273 | 644 | 253 | 554 | 722 | 1,597 |
Statistically significant weighted mean differences across groups based on Wald tests are represented by:
p<0.01
p<0.05
p<0.1.
The standard errors used for these tests are clustered at the ward-level.
These variables were measured at baseline only for those who were recorded as household members at the time. Baseline marital/cohabiting status is available for 64 percent of youth respondents across waves (this information was collected only for those aged 12 years of older), and baseline school enrolment is available for 84 percent of youth respondents.
After stacking the youth data from the three survey waves, we estimate the following difference-in-differences (DiD) model:
1 |
where Yiht represents an outcome for youth i living in household h at survey wave t (t=1 for the baseline survey, =2 for the 12-month follow-up and =3 for the 48-month follow-up), Xiht are youth characteristics (sex and age) at time t, and Zh are baseline values of several household characteristics—log household size, the household heads’ sex, age, marital status and education, household demographic composition (the number of individuals in different age groups) and indicators for the province of residence. The vector δt contains survey round fixed effects and Th=1 if household h resides in a treatment site and =0 if it resides in a comparison site. The vector of terms representing the interaction between the treatment variable and each of the time fixed effects is δt*Th—its coefficients represent the DiD estimators for program impacts at the two follow-up survey rounds. εiht is the error term for youth i in household h at time t.
We use linear probability models and estimate (1) for all indicator variable outcomes, though later on in the analysis we check whether results are sensitive to estimation with a non-linear model. As discussed in the Data section, in order to examine whether there were changes in the abusive actions of specific types of actors, we use an outcome variable for each type of violence to designate who among the mutually exclusive perpetrator categories was responsible for the last such abusive act against the respondent. Since the perpetrator variables represent unordered categories, we probe these outcomes by estimating (1) with multinomial logit models and present marginal effects or program impacts on the probability of violence by a certain type of actor (Maddala, 1986; Cameron & Trivedi, 2010). When exploring the perpetrators of violence, we restrict the sample to only those victimized by that kind of abuse.
In order to enhance the external validity of our estimates, we account for the incomplete response rates to the youth survey and weight all models with the inverse of the estimated probability of youth response. We adjust the youth weights with household weights. At baseline, household weights were constructed to make the study sample representative of all eligible households in the study regions. These baseline weights were updated at follow-up surveys to account for household attrition using inverse probability weighting (Carolina Population Center, 2018). Finally, we cluster the standard errors in (1) at the level of the primary sampling unit (the ward).
We discuss above that the response rate to the youth survey was fairly low at baseline and at 12-months, but that 67 percent of targeted youth were interviewed during the final follow-up. To understand whether the estimates identified by the main DiD specification are driven by low response in the first two surveys, we also compare treatment and control youth only at 48-months with a cross-sectional model. Essentially, this is a single-difference model with the treatment indicator and all the control variables.
VI. Results
Prevalence of violence, and community culture and norms at baseline
We first present summary statistics for youth reports of violence at baseline (Table 3, Panel A). Almost half of all youth report being subjected to some form of physical violence and 24 percent of youth say they met with severe physical violence. The most frequent type of abuse is being slapped or pushed—40 percent of youth face this kind of violence. The most serious abuse—being attacked or threatened with a knife or other weapon—affects four percent of respondents. Relatives and peers are the most common perpetrators of violence against youth (perpetrators for specific types of violence are examined only for those who report facing that kind of abuse). Virtually none of the baseline means of the violence measures are statistically different for treatment and comparison youth.
Table 3:
Panel A: Youth violence | |||
Variable | All youth | Control group | Treatment group |
Physical violence | 0.48 | 0.44 | 0.49 |
Severe physical violence | 0.24 | 0.23 | 0.25 |
Slapped/pushed | 0.40 | 0.37 | 0.41 |
Hit with fist/kicked/beaten with object | 0.22 | 0.21 | 0.23 |
Attacked or threatened with knife/other weapon | 0.04 | 0.03 | 0.05 |
Perpetrator - Slapped/pushed | |||
Relative | 0.29 | 0.31 | 0.29 |
Partner | 0.05 | 0.09 | 0.04 |
Authority figure | 0.24 | 0.25 | 0.24 |
Peer | 0.36 | 0.31 | 0.38 |
Other | 0.05 | 0.04 | 0.05 |
Perpetrator - Hit with fist/kicked/beaten with object | |||
Relative | 0.32 | 0.25 | 0.35 |
Partner | 0.06 | 0.13 | 0.04 |
Authority figure | 0.23 | 0.32 | 0.19* |
Peer | 0.33 | 0.26 | 0.36 |
Other | 0.06 | 0.05 | 0.06 |
Perpetrator - Attacked or threatened with knife/other weapon | |||
Relative | 0.44 | 0.45 | 0.43 |
Partner | 0.04 | 0.00 | 0.06 |
Authority figure | 0.05 | 0.00 | 0.06 |
Peer | 0.33 | 0.25 | 0.36 |
Other | 0.14 | 0.30 | 0.10 |
Observations | 917 | 273 | 644 |
Panel B: Community culture and norms | |||
Variable | All wards | Control wards | Treatment wards |
Percentage of households practicing different religions | |||
Traditional | 26.37 | 23.65 | 27.68 |
Christianity | 65.03 | 66.46 | 64.34 |
Islam | 0.62 | 0.00 | 0.93 |
Combination of traditional and other | 7.66 | 6.04 | 8.44 |
Percentage of households that are polygamous‡ | 26.13 | 19.30 | 29.25 |
Possible for wives to inherit property from husbands | 0.99 | 0.96 | 1.00 |
Possible for widows to be inherited by brother/other male relative of the deceased | 0.26 | 0.15 | 0.31 |
More than half of secondary school aged children are enrolled in school | |||
Boys | 0.63 | 0.60 | 0.64 |
Girls | 0.60 | 0.60 | 0.59 |
Orphans | 0.47 | 0.44 | 0.49 |
Alcohol is available within the communityƚ | 0.81 | 0.79 | 0.82 |
More than half the men drink alcohol regularlyƚ | 0.28 | 0.34 | 0.25 |
More than half the women drink alcohol regularlyƚ | 0.01 | 0.00 | 0.02 |
Drinking is a problem in the communityƚ | 2.12 | 1.97 | 2.20 |
Observations | 80 | 26 | 54 |
Statistically significant mean differences across groups based on Wald tests are represented by:
p<0.01
p<0.05
p<0.1.
Youth-level statistics are calculated using weights. Standard errors used for these tests are clustered at the ward-level.
Information is available only for 21 control wards and 46 treatment wards.
Collected during the 12-month follow-up survey only.
In 2011, a nationally representative survey in Zimbabwe (the National Baseline Survey on the Life Experiences of Adolescents or NBSLEA) collected data on childhood experiences of violence. Even though the HSCT baseline survey was conducted a few years after the NBSLEA (in 2013), it is informative to compare the prevalence of violence in the current study sample with the NBSLEA sample. When we restrict our focus to HSCT youth aged 13–17 years (since the physical abuse questions in the NBSLEA used the same reference period as the HSCT surveys only for this age group), we find that 44 percent of girls and 55 percent of boys experience some form of physical violence. Prevalence levels are similar among youth aged 13–17 years in the NBSLEA sample—47 percent of girls and 48 percent of boys report experiencing physical abuse (ZIMSTAT et al., 2011) but note that the NBSLEA survey only asked about the violence perpetrated by partners, parents, adult relatives, and authority figures (the HSCT surveys ask about peers and other actors as well).14
As part of the community-level questionnaire that was administered during the HSCT surveys, information was collected on community culture and norms. We present baseline statistics by treatment status on several of these measures in Panel B of Table 3.15 Note that none of the community-level differences are statistically significant.
Table 4:
(1) | (2) | (3) | (4) | (5) | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Dependent variable: | Physical violence | Severe physical violence | Slapped/pushed | Hit with a fist/kicked/beaten with object | Attacked/threatened with knife/other weapon | |
Panel A: Main specification - difference-in-differences using data from three survey rounds | ||||||
Treatment | 0.057 (0.051) | 0.039 (0.050) | 0.031 (0.044) | 0.033 (0.048) | 0.018 (0.016) | |
12-months | −0.131*** (0.043) | −0.019 (0.045) | −0.194*** (0.051) | −0.015 (0.042) | −0.010 (0.016) | |
48-months | 0.052 (0.049) | 0.050 (0.056) | 0.011 (0.039) | 0.048 (0.058) | 0.023 (0.015) | |
12-month treatment impact | 0.041 (0.061) | 0.006 (0.056) | 0.075 (0.062) | 0.007 (0.052) | −0.005 (0.021) | |
48-month treatment impact | −0.189*** (0.062) | −0.109 (0.067) | −0.141*** (0.049) | −0.102 (0.068) | −0.042* (0.022) | |
Observations | 4,038 | 4,038 | 4,037 | 4,037 | 4,032 | |
R-squared | 0.053 | 0.035 | 0.046 | 0.042 | 0.009 | |
Baseline mean of dependent variable | 0.477 | 0.243 | 0.399 | 0.223 | 0.041 | |
Panel B: Cross-sectional specification using only 48-month data | ||||||
48-month treatment impact | −0.131*** (0.037) | −0.069* (0.037) | −0.108*** (0.028) | −0.067 (0.041) | −0.023* (0.012) | |
Observations | 2,317 | 2,317 | 2,316 | 2,316 | 2,312 | |
R-squared | 0.052 | 0.037 | 0.045 | 0.044 | 0.010 | |
Control group mean of dependent variable | 0.441 | 0.252 | 0.344 | 0.232 | 0.056 |
Robust standard errors presented in parentheses are adjusted for clustering at the level of the ward of residence.
p<0.01
p<0.05
p<0.1.
Controls include youth age and gender, and baseline values of the following household characteristics - log household size, main respondent age, gender, education and marital status, household demographic composition and indicators for the province of residence. Weights are applied to approximate effects for all eligible youth in the study regions. All physical violence measures capture abuse in the previous 12-months.
Finally, at the end of Panel B, Table 3, we examine summary statistics on community-level alcohol consumption measures. Unfortunately, these data were not collected at baseline, but at the first follow-up survey. Arguably these practices would not have changed substantially in one year, particularly since the HSCT did not provide services to deter alcohol consumption, and so we assume that the first follow-up responses on these variables reflect pre-HSCT conditions. Treatment and control wards are balanced on all alcohol consumption-related measures.
HSCT operations
Before estimating the impacts of the HSCT on youth exposure to physical abuse, it is important to understand whether the program was actually implemented as intended. The HSCT impact evaluation included an operational performance module in the household surveys administered at follow-up survey rounds and its data can help gauge household experiences with the cash and service components of the program over time.
About 92 percent of treatment households at each follow-up survey report receiving at least one payment from the HSCT.16 There is evidence of little to no treatment contamination—only 0.2 (zero) percent of control households at 48-months (12-months) say that they ever received transfers under the HSCT. Recall that the HSCT is supposed to make payments every two months. When asked about payment schedules, 96 (99) percent of currently benefitting households during the final follow-up (12-month follow-up) say they received their last HSCT payment in the previous two months and 78 (91) percent of these households say they expect their next installment within the following two months. These numbers suggest that the CT operations of the program were functioning relatively regularly.
The plus component of the HSCT is oriented towards increasing beneficiary households’ understanding of child protection issues, and their awareness and adoption of available child services. If the HSCT did indeed implement its complementary services, we would expect treatment households to fare better than control households on the knowledge and use of child services. We probe whether this is the case in Table A4. The specifications used for these results are single-difference models and are estimated separately for each wave.17
At first follow-up, we do not detect any program impacts on awareness of services (columns 1 and 3). Given that only some of the HSCT services had started before this survey round, these null results are to be expected. In contrast, beneficiary households at final follow-up are significantly more likely to name at least one resource for children within their community (columns 2 and 4). Treatment households are also significantly more likely to say that they know the point person for child protection issues in their ward—the community childcare worker (columns 5 and 6).18 We do not, however, find any evidence that the HSCT impacted the use of child services—these estimates are presented in columns 7–10. Despite the lack of effects on this last measure, it is reasonable to assume that the higher awareness of child services among treatment households versus control households stems from information received at the HSCT payment points. This is supported by information collected only from current program beneficiaries at 48-months—71 percent of household respondents confirm that beneficiaries receive services or information apart from CTs at the HSCT payment points. The evidence presented in this sub-section indicates that HSCT recipient households did receive cash and were likely exposed to at least some complementary services. Any program effects we identify in our analysis should thus be interpreted to be the result of both cash and child support services. Since all treatment households receive cash plus services, it is not possible for us to disentangle the impacts of the different components of the HSCT.
Main results
In Table 4, Panel A, we present the main results of our analysis. All but one of the 12-month impact estimates are positively signed, suggesting that the HSCT might have increased youth exposure to physical abuse at this time, but none of these estimates are statistically significant. Recall that some of the HSCT complementary services aimed at enhancing child wellbeing had only just begun to operate before this survey was conducted. These activities could have started sensitizing treatment youth to issues related to violence, thereby prompting greater reporting of any abuse that was taking place. However, given that the program—at least the CTs—had been in operation for only a year (and some program components hadn’t even been implemented yet), actual impacts on violence may not have been able to materialize by this time.
Turning to the 48-month coefficients, we find that treatment youth are 19 percentage points less likely to face any physical violence than comparison youth at this time, a difference that is statistically significant at the one percent level (column 1). The impact estimate for severe physical violence (column 2) indicates a decline, but the coefficient does not attain statistical significance. Upon examining the different types of violence separately, we find that the results for any physical violence are driven by the first category—being slapped or pushed (column 3), which is the most common and perhaps the least severe kind of violence that was measured. The coefficient on the variable for being attacked or threatened with a weapon (column 5) is marginally significant, but very few youth report facing this type of violence—only four percent at baseline.
As discussed earlier, the response rate to the HSCT youth survey was low during the first two survey rounds. Since a much higher proportion of targeted youth were surveyed at final follow-up (67 percent), contrasting treatment and control respondents at this stage alone would likely be best able to approximate how youth fared under the HSCT. We thus now focus on data from only this wave—in Table 4, Panel B, the 48-month cross-sectional program estimates continue to show that treatment youths experience lower levels of physical violence than control youth and that these benefits are driven largely by reductions in being slapped or pushed.
Next, we probe whether there is any heterogeneity in the physical violence results by gender. At baseline, a higher proportion of boys—52 percent—report experiencing any form of physical violence than girls—44 percent. Table A5 in the Appendix shows that while the HSCT impact estimates for the different violence outcomes at final follow-up are negatively signed for both groups, the violence deterring effects of the HSCT are larger in magnitude for boys. At 48-months, boys in the treatment group are about 28 percentage points less likely to be victimized by any form of physical abuse; the same figure for girls is one-third the size. It should be noted though that the impact estimates observed for boys in this table are not statistically different from the corresponding estimates for girls despite the large differences in the point estimates.
A growing body of evidence from developing countries suggests that authority figures in school commonly use corporal punishment against students (for example, see Ogando Portela & Pells, 2015; Bashir et al., 2018). In the current study sample, it does seem like schooling is a risk factor for violence—at baseline, a greater share of youth in school (55 percent) report facing physical violence than youth not in school (37 percent). Given that schooling shapes exposure to violence, HSCT’s impacts on the incidence of physical violence could very well vary for those in and out of school. We investigate whether there is any such heterogeneity in Table A6 in the Appendix.
In columns 1 to 5 in Table A6, we look for differential effects by contemporaneous schooling (whether or not an individual is currently attending an education institution). At final follow-up, we do not find differences in program impacts for youth in school and out of school with one exception—the HSCT’s effect on youth vulnerability to severe physical violence is higher for the former, but this difference is only marginally significant. As we show later, the HSCT had no discernable impacts on educational outcomes, and so the lack of heterogeneity in final program effects on violence by contemporaneous schooling is not surprising. In the remaining columns of Table A6 in the Appendix, we probe whether there is any heterogeneity in the effects on violence by baseline schooling but fail to find evidence of such trends.19
During the 48-month follow-up, the youth module included several questions about the environment that the respondents are in and their relationship with different individuals. We examine these outcomes here to get a sense of youths’ perceptions about their living conditions. Since these questions were not asked in the earlier survey rounds, we cannot use empirical specification (1) to examine these outcomes, but estimate a single-difference model with the treatment indicator and all the usual control variables. Table 5 contains these results. Young individuals in treatment households appear to be significantly less worried about relationships at home and outside, be less likely to witness a parent being subject to IPV and experience lower levels of humiliation by adults. Collectively, these estimates demonstrate that treatment youth are in more protective settings than control youth, and are consistent with our main finding that the HSCT program reduces youth exposure to violence.
Table 5:
(1) | (2) | (3) | (4) | (5) | (6) | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Dependent variable: | Youth is worried about relationship with people at home | Youth is worried about relationship with people outside home | Youth has seen parent being subjected to intimate partner violence at any point in time | Adult humiliated youth in front of others in last 12 months | Adult made youth feel unwanted in last 12 months | Adult threatened to abandon/told youth to leave home in last 12 months |
48-month treatment impact | −0.067** (0.030) | −0.070** (0.034) | −0.038*** (0.014) | −0.067*** (0.024) | −0.030 (0.026) | −0.013 (0.014) |
Observations | 2,315 | 2,315 | 2,297 | 2,297 | 2,304 | 2,304 |
R-squared | 0.060 | 0.052 | 0.028 | 0.028 | 0.028 | 0.034 |
Control group mean of dependent variable | 0.476 | 0.525 | 0.095 | 0.209 | 0.166 | 0.098 |
Robust standard errors presented in parentheses are adjusted for clustering at the level of the ward of residence.
p<0.01
p<0.05
p<0.1.
Controls include youth age and gender, and baseline values of the following household characteristics - log household size, main respondent age, gender, education and marital status, household demographic composition and indicators for the province of residence. Weights are applied to approximate effects for all eligible youth in the study regions.
Next, we examine whether there are any changes in the types of perpetrators committing violence against youth. We look only at perpetrators who slapped/pushed youth and punched/kicked/beat youth with an object. As highlighted above, only a small proportion of individuals are threatened or attacked with a weapon and we do not have enough variation on the variable capturing perpetrators for this type of violence to achieve model convergence. We present the marginal effects of the HSCT on the probability of violence by different categories of actors in Table 6. There are almost no statistically significant effects.20 This suggests that the declines in physical violence that we observe in Table 4 are not driven by specific types of actors.
Table 6:
(1) | (2) | |
---|---|---|
Slapped/pushed | Hit with a fist/kicked/beaten with object | |
12-month treatment impact | ||
Relative | −0.149 (0.115) | −0.198* (0.111) |
Partner | 0.148** (0.072) | 0.150 (0.093) |
Authority figure | 0.060 (0.121) | 0.050 (0.175) |
Peer | −0.095 (0.084) | −0.131 (0.130) |
Other actor | 0.036 (0.052) | 0.130** (0.060) |
48-month treatment impact | ||
Relative | −0.010 (0.091) | −0.155 (0.107) |
Partner | 0.102* (0.061) | 0.144 (0.091) |
Authority figure | 0.039 (0.087) | 0.135 (0.105) |
Peer | −0.081 (0.075) | −0.056 (0.095) |
Other actor | −0.050 (0.045) | −0.068 (0.043) |
Observations | 1,201 | 711 |
Baseline mean - violence by relative | 0.294 | 0.319 |
Baseline mean - violence by partner | 0.053 | 0.066 |
Baseline mean - violence by authority figure | 0.244 | 0.229 |
Baseline mean - violence by peer | 0.363 | 0.331 |
Baseline mean - violence by other actor | 0.046 | 0.055 |
Robust standard errors presented in parentheses are adjusted for clustering at the level of the ward of residence.
p<0.01
p<0.05
p<0.1.
Controls include youth age and gender, and baseline values of the following household characteristics - log household size, main respondent age, gender, education and marital status, household demographic composition and indicators for the province of residence. Weights are applied to approximate effects for all eligible youth in the study regions. Data was collected on the perpetrator who committed the last act of a specific type of violence in the previous 12-months.
Impacts on potential pathways
In the Background section above, we discuss Peterman and colleagues’ 2017 framework which highlights the various pathways through which programs like HSCT could influence childhood experiences of violence. In this section, we explore the impacts of the Zimbabwe CT on several of these potential pathways to understand whether they could have played a role in bringing about the observed declines in youth abuse. We separately examine outcomes at the level of the household, the caregiver and the youth.
In order to capture the HSCT’s effects on household economic security, we probe the value of per capita monthly household purchases of all items and of food items. For this estimation, we use model (1), but exclude the youth-level control variables of age and gender. Additionally, we restrict our analysis to only those households that had a young individual between the ages of 13 and 24 years at baseline. Table 7, which contains the results of this investigation, shows positive HSCT effects on both purchase measures.21 The enhanced purchasing power of transfer-receiving households is likely to improve their control over consumption choices and their sense of self-reliance, conditions that might reduce stress related to income and food insecurity.
Table 7:
(1) | (2) | |
---|---|---|
Dependent variable: | Monthly household purchases per capita - all items | Monthly household purchases per capita - food items |
Treatment | −0.159 (0.918) | −0.134 (0.557) |
12-months | 1.711** (0.720) | 0.610 (0.463) |
48-months | 5.863*** (0.547) | 2.433*** (0.355) |
12-month treatment impact | 2.298*** (0.854) | 1.401** (0.553) |
48-month treatment impact | 2.828* (1.526) | 2.125 (1.298) |
Observations | 5,040 | 5,040 |
R-squared | 0.028 | 0.011 |
Baseline mean of dependent variable | 11.022 | 3.703 |
Robust standard errors presented in parentheses are adjusted for clustering at the level of the ward of residence.
p<0.01
p<0.05
p<0.1.
Controls include baseline values of the following household characteristics - log household size, main respondent age, gender, education and marital status, household demographic composition and indicators for the province of residence. Weights are applied to approximate effects for all eligible households in the study regions. The sample is restricted to households that had youth members at baseline.
During the baseline, the household survey was administered to an individual within each household who was designated as the main respondent. The person most knowledgeable about the surveyed topics (such as health, education and consumption) was selected to be interviewed, usually the household head. Another household member was surveyed only if the head was not available. At follow-up waves, attempts were made to re-interview the same individual as the main respondent. The HSCT evaluation surveys collected data on the main respondent’s perceptions regarding household food security, individual wellbeing and future expectations. We probe whether there are program effects on these reports under the assumption that the main respondent is likely to be the caregiver of children and youth in the household, or to be an appropriate proxy for the caregiver. In examining caregiver-level outcomes, we use a model akin to equation (1), except that the outcome and individual-level controls for this part of the analysis are at the level of the main respondent. As in our analysis of household-level pathways, we focus only on households that had at least one member aged 13 to 24 years at baseline.
In Table 8 we report impacts on the following outcomes—whether main respondents consume three or more meals on a regular basis, the Household Food Insecurity Access Scale (Coates et al., 2007), a Subjective Well-being Scale (adapted from the Satisfaction with Life Scale developed in Diener et al., 1985), whether respondents consider their households to be currently better off than the previous year, and optimism about the future. Most of these measures are indicator variables with the exception of the two scales on which higher values indicate more of the construct being measured—greater food insecurity (higher is worse) and enhanced well-being (higher is better). We describe these variables in more detail in Table A2 in the Appendix.
Table 8:
(1) | (2) | (3) | (4) | (5) | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Dependent variable: | Consumed three or more meals in a day | Household Food Insecurity Access Scale | Subjective Well-being Scale | Household is better off compared to last year | Belief that household will be better off in one year |
Treatment | −0.003 (0.019) | −0.007 (0.512) | −0.062 (0.323) | −0.016 (0.024) | −0.010 (0.028) |
12-months | 0.193*** (0.023) | −3.488*** (0.605) | 1.244*** (0.313) | 0.088*** (0.031) | 0.174*** (0.039) |
48-months | 0.287*** (0.021) | −2.802*** (0.542) | 2.676*** (0.405) | 0.266*** (0.040) | 0.146*** (0.025) |
12-month treatment impact | 0.083** (0.034) | 0.042 (0.750) | 0.802** (0.394) | 0.394*** (0.038) | 0.170*** (0.044) |
48-month treatment impact | 0.072** (0.034) | −1.839** (0.728) | 0.865* (0.464) | 0.224*** (0.046) | 0.188*** (0.035) |
Observations | 5,035 | 4,691 | 5,031 | 5,035 | 4,977 |
R-squared | 0.115 | 0.084 | 0.091 | 0.144 | 0.070 |
Baseline mean of dependent variable | 0.291 | 14.013 | 9.725 | 0.103 | 0.242 |
Robust standard errors presented in parentheses are adjusted for clustering at the level of the ward of residence.
p<0.01
p<0.05
p<0.1.
Controls include caregiver age and gender, and baseline values of the following household characteristics - log household size, main respondent age, gender, education and marital status, household demographic composition and indicators for the province of residence. Weights are applied to approximate effects for all eligible households in the study regions. The sample is restricted to households that had youth members at baseline.
The estimates presented in Table 8 demonstrate that the HSCT program brings about improvements on all potential caregiver-level mechanisms by final follow-up—caregivers in treatment households consume more meals in a day (column 1), face lower levels of food insecurity (column 2), experience enhanced levels of well-being (column 3), consider their households to be better off than before (column 4) and exhibit higher levels of optimism (column 5).
Do the CTs alter other family dynamics? Any need felt by unmarried caregivers to marry in order to alleviate financial constraints might be precluded once households start receiving CTs. New adult members might perpetrate violence against youth or protect them from other sources of violence, and so the entry of adults into the household could change young individual’s susceptibility to abuse. Given the typical profile of HSCT beneficiary household caregivers—elderly women, we do not expect such family compositional changes, or the lack thereof, to play a major role within the HSCT context.22 To confirm this, we present program effects on the likelihood of a caregiver being married or co-habiting with a partner. We focus on the caregiver’s partnership status rather than the mother’s partnership status since 16 percent of the youth sample are orphans (with the mother or both parents being dead), 24 percent don’t have mothers living in the household (marital status was collected only for individuals currently in the household) and 12 percent have aged out of the category for which mother’s identity was collected (this group included children aged 18 years and below).23 Table A8, column 1 looks at program impacts on an indicator variable for whether or not the caregiver has a partner—there are no HSCT effects on this measure.24
Simply looking at marital or partnership status might not be very informative if households are polygamous—a caregiver who has a spouse at baseline and a second spouse at first follow-up would not experience a change in marital status despite there being a change to household composition. This could theoretically be a concern for the context we are studying since Table 3, Panel B indicates that the average community (or ward) has 26 percent of households practicing polygamy at baseline, though note that this information is available only for 67 of the 90 communities included in the study sample (as indicated in the footnotes to Table 3).
Unfortunately, the household surveys administered by the HSCT impact evaluation did not collect information on polygamy within the household. However, it is possible to impute whether the main respondents at each survey round are in polygamous relationships. Information was collected on all current household members’ relationship to the main respondent. By counting the number of spouses of the caregiver, we can back out the polygamous status of the main respondent. At every wave, 99 percent of main respondents had zero or one spouse. It is, thus, reasonable to assume that polygamous practices are not shaping HSCT’s impacts on youth violence in the study sample. We further bolster this conclusion, by presenting program impacts for several other outcomes capturing household composition in columns 2–4 of Table A8. When probing household size in column 2, we do find at marginally significant decrease at final follow-up, but the estimates in the subsequent columns show no change in the adult composition of households (individuals older than youths) over time.
The last category of potential HSCT-youth violence mediators we look at are youth-level factors. We examine whether the program influences youths’ schooling outcomes (current enrolment and recent attendance), work activities (engagement in casual or part-time labor) and partnership status (currently married or co-habiting). See Table A2 in the Appendix for more details on these outcome variables.
Table 9 presents the impacts we identify for potential youth-level pathways using equation (1). The HSCT program does not appear to have impacted youth schooling (columns 1 and 2) and the formation of relationships/marriages (column 4). However, at the 48-month follow-up, young individuals in the treatment group are 12.5 percentage points less likely to engage in casual work than their counterparts in the control group (column 3).25 In other words, the HSCT seems to have allowed households to withdraw their youth from economic work activity.
Table 9:
(1) | (2) | (3) | (4) | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Dependent variable: | Currently enrolled in an educational institute | Number of days attended school in the week before the interviewƚ | Did any work as a casual/part-time/maricho laborer in the last 12 months | Currently married/co-habiting/has a partner |
Treatment | 0.061 (0.044) | 0.130 (0.252) | 0.045 (0.060) | −0.020 (0.041) |
12-months | −0.013 (0.071) | 0.480 (0.291) | −0.069 (0.051) | 0.052 (0.062) |
48-months | 0.075** (0.034) | 0.156 (0.259) | −0.011 (0.052) | −0.004 (0.046) |
12-month treatment impact | 0.018 (0.077) | −0.167 (0.329) | −0.029 (0.061) | −0.007 (0.066) |
48-month treatment impact | −0.008 (0.044) | −0.227 (0.315) | −0.125** (0.063) | −0.012 (0.049) |
Observations | 4,034 | 1,930 | 4,025 | 4,041 |
R-squared | 0.355 | 0.030 | 0.110 | 0.264 |
Baseline mean of dependent variable | 0.606 | 4.250 | 0.245 | 0.147 |
Robust standard errors presented in parentheses are adjusted for clustering at the level of the ward of residence.
p<0.01
p<0.05
p<0.1.
Controls include youth age and gender, and baseline values of the following household characteristics - log household size, main respondent age, gender, education and marital status, household demographic composition and indicators for the province of residence. Weights are applied to approximate effects for all eligible youth in the study regions.
In examining this outcome, the sample is restricted to those attending an educational institute that was not closed for holiday during the reference period.
A concern that arises with viewing the outcomes in Tables 7–9 as plausible pathways through which the HSCT reduced physical violence among youth is that these outcomes might have been determined simultaneously with youth violence or they might even be shaped by violence—for example, parents whose children or youth experience a reduction in their exposure to abuse might have higher subjective well-being. Note though that the HSCT had detectable positive effects on at least the household- and caregiver-level mediators by the 12-month follow-up (save one—column 2 in Table 8 which looks at the Household Food Insecurity Access Scale), whereas effects on youth violence did not materialize until the final follow-up. It is thus reasonable to posit that HSCT shaped youth exposure to violent actions through the household- and caregiver-level measures examined in Tables 7 and 8.
The results we provide on potential mechanisms corroborate that dynamics within the households are likely to lead to the HSCT-induced declines in youth violence observed in our main analysis. However, these findings cannot speak to whether factors outside the household—for example, improved community conditions as a result of the benefits experienced by multiple households—are also playing a role. In fact, as our investigation into violence by different types of perpetrators shows (Table 6), the overall results do not seem to be driven solely by relatives.
VII. Robustness checks
In this section, we examine whether the main results are sensitive to several changes to the study sample or estimation procedure.
At each survey wave of the HSCT impact evaluation study, data was collected on all members of the study households. In case there was an individual living in the household who had not been there during a previous survey, information was specifically collected about when and why the individual had joined the household. Within the sample that we use for our analysis, we have 442 youth who enter the study sample only at final follow-up (the ‘new’ youth). The inclusion of these individuals in our sample could be problematic because of two reasons. Firstly, youth who have recently joined treatment households might not have had time to individually benefit from the HSCT program.26 We do know when 76 percent of the ‘new’ youth at final follow-up join their households—of these, 66 percent enter their households only the year before or the year of the final survey. Note though that the new individuals in the treatment group would experience the protective effects of the program channeled through household and caregiver pathways since their households would have been receiving transfers since the start of the program. Secondly, the entry of the new youth into the study households could undermine the validity of our results if there are systematically different factors responsible for changing household composition across the treatment and control groups. If the new youth in the control group are more likely to have experienced physical violence prior to joining the households, this could explain the positive program impacts we observe at the second follow-up.
To understand whether the ‘new’ youth in the sample at final follow-up are driving the main results that we identify, we drop these individuals and re-run the violence models for the remaining sample. The impact estimates presented in Table A9 in the Appendix show that this redefinition of the sample does little to change our results.
Recall that we use weights in all our models—essentially to enhance the external validity of our estimates. Are these weights affecting the results? Given that the youth who were surveyed at each wave were balanced across treatment assignment on baseline measures (as shown in Table 2), estimating equation (1) without the weights would be sufficient to identify results that are internally valid. We thus re-examine the violence categories with an unweighted model (see Table A10 in the Appendix). As in the main results, we detect a decrease in any youth abuse at 48-months and find that this change is driven largely by a reduction in being slapped or pushed.
While all the primary violence measures that we probe are indicator variables, we use linear probability models (LPM) in our main analysis for ease of interpretation. In Table A11 in the Appendix, we re-examine these outcomes with probit models and find that the results remain unchanged.27
The results also hold if we include youth age fixed effects instead of controlling for age as a continuous variable as in the main specification—in other words, the results are not being driven by patterns of violence specific to certain age groups. Finally, all impact estimates are unchanged if we add province-specific linear time trends to the empirical model to account for potential province-wide linear trends in the patterns of violence. These results are available upon request.
VIII. Discussion
In this paper, we examine whether the Government of Zimbabwe’s HSCT, a cash plus poverty alleviation and child protection program, influences the victimization of youth to physical violence. Physical abuse is highly prevalent among the youth population that we focus on—at baseline (2013), 48 per cent of the youth report having been slapped, pushed, punched, kicked, beaten with an object or threatened/attacked with a gun/other weapon in the previous year. The HSTC is not able to significantly shape violence by the 12-month follow-up in 2014, though impact estimates are positively signed suggesting greater youth exposure to physical abuse in CT-receiving households at the time. Perhaps the introduction of the initial HSCT complementary child protection services just before this survey round started sensitizing youth to issues related to abuse and prompted greater reporting of violence. However, given that the program—at least the CTs—had been in operation for only a year, there simply may not have been enough time for actual impacts on violence to emerge. In contrast, the HSCT is able to reduce youth exposure to physical abuse after four years of implementation, decreasing violence by a sizeable 19 percentage points in 2017. Further analyses indicate that enhancements in beneficiary household purchases of food and other items, food security, improvements in caregiver subjective well-being and reductions in youth participation in economic work for pay could be the channels through which HSCT is able to reduce self-reported physical violence among adolescents and young adults.
Cash plus programs, such as the one we study, combine income transfers with complementary interventions. While the interactive effects of the separate components of these programs have the potential to enhance overall impacts in domains of interest, it is worth keeping in mind that the monetary and non-monetary costs of these programs are also higher than those of cash-only programs. Implementing transfers along with different interventions requires higher government capacity, supply-side improvements, training for service providers, and constant coordination among and monitoring of multiple actors. The ultimate efficacy of cash plus programming is likely to reflect how successfully these aspects are maneuvered.
In early 2017, the High Court in Zimbabwe ruled that the corporal punishment of children in homes and schools is a violation of constitutional rights. While the ruling is yet to be approved by the Constitutional Court, the government has said that it is considering passing legislation to ban corporal punishment (Global Initiative to End all Corporal Punishment of Children, 2020). Given that the use of physical force as a means of disciplining children is widely considered to be acceptable in the country (Ndoma, 2017), a legal ban, should it go into effect in the near future, might not be able to reduce physical abuse and punishment of children and youth by itself. However, as the results of the current analysis of a national CT program in Zimbabwe suggest, using cash to alleviate poverty, a structural determinant of child abuse and maltreatment, and complementary services can preclude or limit child exposure to physical violence and the subsequent harms experienced by the victims of abuse.
Highlights.
A program providing cash transfers and child support services in Zimbabwe reduces youth exposure to physical abuse.
Physical violence among youth (aged 13 to 24 years) declines by 19 percentage points four years into the program.
Improvements in household food security and caregiver subjective well-being could be channeling the observed reductions in violence.
Acknowledgements:
The Zimbabwe Harmonized Social Cash Transfer (HSCT) program evaluation was funded by UNICEF-Zimbabwe and 3IE (head office: New Delhi, India). The first two rounds of the HSCT evaluation were conducted by AIR and UNC-CH for the Government of Zimbabwe, under contract to UNICEF-Zimbabwe. The third wave was funded by 3IE, under contract to UNC-CH. The evaluation benefitted from the technical and logistical input of a large group of professionals from multiple agencies, and are listed here by affiliation and then alphabetically within affiliation: AIR (Andi Coombes, Thomas de Hoop, Cassandra Jessee, Leah Prencipe, Hannah Reeves, David Seidenfeld (PI), Rosa Castro Zarzur); Centre of Applied Social Sciences (CASS), Zimbabwe (Billy Mukamuri); FAO (Silvio Diadone, Benjamin Davis); Ministry of Public Service, Labour, and Social Welfare, Government of Zimbabwe (Sydney G. Mhishi, Lovemore Dumba); Ruzivo Trust (Prosper Mutondi); UNICEF Office of Research-Innocenti (Tia Palermo, Amber Peterman, Leah Prencipe); UNICEF-Zimbabwe (Noriko Izumi, Leon Muwoni, Lauren Rumble, Elayn Sammon); UNC-CH (Sarah Abdoulayi, Gustavo Angeles, Garima Bhalla, Averi Chakrabarti, Sudhanshu Handa, Mary Jane Hill, Adria Molotsky, Frank Otchere), and Zimbabwe National Statistics Agency (Evelyn Humngwe, Lovemore Ziswa). We acknowledge the patience exercised by the Zimbabwean households during interviews and the input and dedication of the team of supervisors, enumerators, and drivers from CASS and Ruzivo Trust during data collection. Institutional Review Board approvals were secured from the American Institutes for Research’s Institutional Review Board and the Medical Research Council of Zimbabwe. We are grateful to Jeremy Moulton, Benjamin Mason Meier and Pamela Jagger for the feedback they provided on earlier drafts of this paper.
Appendix
Table A1:
All households | Households with youth at baseline | Households contributing youth respondents at various survey rounds | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Control | Treatment | Control | Treatment | Control | Treatment | |
Household head’s characteristics | ||||||
Female | 0.66 | 0.69 | 0.64 | 0.69* | 0.65 | 0.69 |
Age | 59.25 | 56.43 | 56.24 | 52.57 | 53.04 | 52.81 |
Widow | 0.37 | 0.37 | 0.30 | 0.32 | 0.30 | 0.31 |
Divorced/separated | 0.07 | 0.10 | 0.07 | 0.09 | 0.08 | 0.09 |
Has some schooling | 0.61 | 0.56* | 0.67 | 0.62* | 0.64 | 0.62 |
Highest grade | 3.48 | 3.27 | 3.94 | 3.75 | 3.79 | 3.73 |
Household characteristics | ||||||
Household size | 5.14 | 5.03 | 6.23 | 6.21 | 6.21 | 5.98 |
Number of individuals 0–5 years | 0.81 | 0.78 | 0.98 | 0.90 | 0.98 | 0.91 |
Number of individuals 6–17 years | 2.21 | 2.22 | 2.86 | 3.01 | 2.92 | 2.88 |
Number of individuals 18–59 years | 1.30 | 1.20 | 1.66 | 1.57 | 1.57 | 1.46 |
Number of individuals 60 and above years | 0.82 | 0.82 | 0.72 | 0.73 | 0.74 | 0.73 |
Cares for at least one orphan | 0.16 | 0.16 | 0.21 | 0.22 | 0.20 | 0.20 |
Has a chronically ill member | 0.37 | 0.37 | 0.38 | 0.37 | 0.39 | 0.37 |
Has a member with a disability | 0.28 | 0.25 | 0.27 | 0.26 | 0.29 | 0.25* |
Per capita expenditure | 31.82 | 30.85 | 24.71 | 24.21 | 25.85 | 25.57 |
Share of budget spent on food | 0.63 | 0.63 | 0.62 | 0.62 | 0.62 | 0.62 |
Owns or cultivated land in last year | 0.92 | 0.94 | 0.94 | 0.96 | 0.93 | 0.96 |
Total land operated (in hectares) | 6.12 | 5.90 | 6.47 | 6.92 | 6.52 | 5.80 |
Planted crops during last rainy season | 0.87 | 0.90 | 0.92 | 0.94 | 0.91 | 0.93 |
Uses crop production inputs | 0.23 | 0.23 | 0.25 | 0.25 | 0.26 | 0.25 |
Owns livestock | 0.79 | 0.77 | 0.85 | 0.83 | 0.85 | 0.83 |
Operates non-farm business | 0.13 | 0.11 | 0.16 | 0.12 | 0.16 | 0.12* |
Someone was in wage employment at any time in last year | 0.10 | 0.11 | 0.12 | 0.13 | 0.10 | 0.12 |
Receives cash/labor/in-kind aid | 0.76 | 0.67** | 0.72 | 0.67 | 0.72 | 0.66 |
Provides cash/labor/in-kind aid | 0.23 | 0.24 | 0.23 | 0.26 | 0.24 | 0.26 |
Observations | 1,034 | 2,029 | 615 | 1,214 | 582 | 1,265 |
Statistically significant weighted mean differences across groups based on Wald tests are represented by:
p<0.01
p<0.05
p<0.1.
The standard errors used for these tests are clustered at the ward-level.
Table A2:
Variable | Definition |
---|---|
Main outcomes | |
Physical violence | =1 if youth experienced any type of physical violence (being slapped/pushed, hit with a fist/kicked/beaten with an object or attacked/threatened with a knife/other weapon) in the previous 12 months, =0 otherwise |
Severe physical violence | =1 if youth experienced any severe physical violence (hit with a fist/kicked/beaten with an object or attacked/threatened with a knife/other weapon) in the previous 12 months, =0 otherwise |
Slapped/pushed | =1 if youth was slapped or pushed in the previous 12 months, =0 otherwise |
Hit with a fist/kicked/beaten with object | =1 if youth was hit with a fist, kicked or beaten with an object in the previous 12 months, =0 otherwise |
Attacked/threatened with knife/other weapon | =1 if youth was attacked or threatened with a knife or other weapon in the previous 12 months, =0 otherwise |
Perpetrator the last time youth was slapped/pushed | =1 if parent or adult relative, =2 if a boyfriend or girlfriend or intimate partner, =3 if an authority figure (teacher, religious or community leader), =4 if a peer or classmate, =5 if another individual (for example, a stranger) |
Perpetrator the last time youth was hit with a fist/kicked/beaten with object | =1 if parent or adult relative, =2 if a boyfriend or girlfriend or intimate partner, =3 if an authority figure (teacher, religious or community leader), =4 if a peer or classmate, =5 if another individual (for example, a stranger) |
Perpetrator the last time youth was attacked/threatened with knife/other weapon | =1 if parent or adult relative, =2 if a boyfriend or girlfriend or intimate partner, =3 if an authority figure (teacher, religious or community leader), =4 if a peer or classmate, =5 if another individual (for example, a stranger) |
Supplementary outcomes | |
Youth is worried about relationship with people at home | =1 if youth reports being worried about her relationship with people in her home, =0 if she is not worried at all |
Youth is worried about relationship with people outside home | =1 if youth reports being worried about her relationship with people she does not live with (such as friends and neighbors), =0 if she is not worried at all |
Youth has seen parent being subjected to intimate partner violence at any point in time | =1 if youth has ever seen or heard a parent being punched, kicked or beaten up by the other parent, or their boyfriend or girlfriend, =0 otherwise |
Adult humiliated youth in front of others in last 12 months | =1 if an adult said or did something on purpose to humiliate youth in front of others in the last 12 months, =0 otherwise |
Adult made youth feel unwanted in last 12 months | =1 if an adult made youth feel unwanted in the last 12 months, =0 otherwise |
Adult threatened to abandon/told youth to leave home in last 12 months | =1 if an adult threatened to abandon youth or told her to leave home in the last 12 months, =0 otherwise |
Household-level mediators | |
Monthly household expenditure per capita - all items | Value of household total expenditure per capita (from purchases) in the four weeks before the survey (in 2017 dollars) |
Monthly household expenditure per capita - food items | Value of household food expenditure per capita (from purchases) in the four weeks before the survey (in 2017 dollars) |
Caregiver-level mediators | |
Consumed three or more meals in a day | =1 if main respondent reports regularly consuming three or more meals in a day, =0 otherwise |
Household Food Insecurity Access Scale | This scale was developed by the Food and Nutritional Technical Assistance project (FANTA) of the U.S. Agency for International Development (USAID) (Coates et al., 2007). Main respondents are asked whether their households had to do any of the following in the four weeks before the survey due to a lack of resources: worry that their household would not have enough food, not eat preferred foods, consume a limited variety of foods, eat foods they did not want to eat, consume smaller meals than needed, eat fewer meals, have no food to eat, go to sleep at night hungry, and go a whole day and night without eating. Respondents answer to each question with ‘No’ (a score of zero), ‘Rarely’ (one), ‘Sometimes’ (two), or ‘Often’ (three). The responses to the nine items are summed and the final score ranges from zero (complete food security) to 27 (extreme food insecurity). |
Subjective Well-being Scale | This scale is based on responses to seven questions and captures overall life satisfaction (the measure we use is similar to the Satisfaction with Life Scale developed by Diener et al., 1985). Main respondents were presented with the following statements during the interview - ‘In most ways my life is close to ideal’, ‘The conditions of my life are excellent’, ‘I am satisfied with my life’, ‘So far I have gotten the important things I want in life’, ‘If I could live my life over, I would change almost nothing’. ‘I feel positive about my future’, and ‘I generally feel happy’. Answer options are on a five-point Likert scale, ranging from strongly disagree (a score of one) to strongly agree (five). The final score lies between seven and 35, with higher values representing more subjective well-being. |
Household is better off compared to last year | =1 if main respondent considers her household to be doing better off than 12 months ago, =0 otherwise |
Belief that household will be better off in one year | =1 if main respondent thinks that life will be better one year from now, =0 otherwise |
Other caregiver/household-level mediators | |
Caregiver is married or co-habiting | =1 if main respondent at baseline is currently married or co-habiting =0 otherwise |
Household size | Number of household members |
Number of female adults (>=25) | Number of female household members aged 25 years or older |
Number of male adults (>=25) | Number of male household members aged 25 years or older |
Youth-level mediators | |
Currently enrolled in an educational institute | =1 if youth is currently attending an educational institution, =0 otherwise |
Number of days attended school in the week before the interview | Number of days the youth attended school in the week before the survey |
Did any work as a casual/part-time/maricho laborer in the last 12 months | =1 if youth did this kind of work for anyone who is not a member of the household in the past 12 months, =0 otherwise |
Currently married/co-habiting/has a partner | =1 if youth is currently married, co-habiting or has a boyfriend/girlfriend, =0 otherwise |
Table A3:
12-month follow-up | 48-month follow-up | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
Control | Treatment | Control | Treatment | |
Household head’s characteristics | ||||
Female | 0.66 | 0.69 | 0.66 | 0.69 |
Age | 59.69 | 56.23 | 61.58 | 56.14 |
Widow | 0.37 | 0.37 | 0.37 | 0.37 |
Divorced/separated | 0.08 | 0.10 | 0.07 | 0.10 |
Has some schooling | 0.61 | 0.56 | 0.61 | 0.56* |
Highest grade | 3.46 | 3.27 | 3.52 | 3.20* |
Household characteristics | ||||
Household size | 5.14 | 5.02 | 5.15 | 5.04 |
Number of individuals 0–5 years | 0.80 | 0.77 | 0.81 | 0.78 |
Number of individuals 6–17 years | 2.21 | 2.22 | 2.21 | 2.23 |
Number of individuals 18–59 years | 1.29 | 1.20 | 1.29 | 1.21 |
Number of individuals 60 and above years | 0.83 | 0.82 | 0.83 | 0.82 |
Cares for at least one orphan | 0.16 | 0.16 | 0.15 | 0.16 |
Has a chronically ill member | 0.37 | 0.37 | 0.38 | 0.36 |
Has a member with a disability | 0.28 | 0.25 | 0.27 | 0.26 |
Per capita expenditure | 33.37 | 30.95 | 32.36 | 30.56 |
Share of budget spent on food | 0.62 | 0.63 | 0.63 | 0.62 |
Owns or cultivated land in last year | 0.91 | 0.94 | 0.91 | 0.94 |
Total land operated (in hectares) | 5.77 | 5.84 | 6.04 | 5.62 |
Planted crops during last rainy season | 0.87 | 0.90 | 0.86 | 0.90 |
Uses crop production inputs | 0.24 | 0.24 | 0.25 | 0.23 |
Owns livestock | 0.78 | 0.78 | 0.78 | 0.77 |
Operates non-farm business | 0.14 | 0.11* | 0.13 | 0.11 |
Someone was in wage employment at any time in last year | 0.11 | 0.10 | 0.10 | 0.10 |
Receives cash/labor/in-kind aid | 0.77 | 0.66*** | 0.75 | 0.67** |
Provides cash/labor/in-kind aid | 0.23 | 0.24 | 0.23 | 0.24 |
Observations | 882 | 1,748 | 842 | 1,725 |
Statistically significant weighted mean differences across groups based on Wald tests are represented by:
p<0.01
p<0.05
p<0.1.
The standard errors used for these tests are clustered at the ward-level. At baseline, 3,063 households were surveyed; 85.9 percent and 83.8 percent of these households were re-surveyed respectively during the 12-month and 48-month follow-up rounds.
Table A4:
(1) | (2) | (3) | (4) | (5) | (6) | (7) | (8) | (9) | (10) | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Dependent variable: | Awareness of services | Knows a community childcare worker in the ward of residence‡ | Use of services | |||||||
Sample: | All households | Households with youth at baseline | All households | Households with youth at baseline | All households | Households with youth at baseline | ||||
Follow-up at: | 12-months | 48-months | 12-months | 48-months | 48-months | 48-months | 12-months | 48-months | 12-months | 48-months |
Impact | 0.005 (0.028) | 0.085** (0.042) | 0.028 (0.037) | 0.121*** (0.045) | 0.264*** (0.032) | 0.292*** (0.042) | 0.007 (0.029) | 0.024 (0.015) | 0.029 (0.038) | 0.027 (0.022) |
Observations | 2,630 | 2,566 | 1,602 | 1,609 | 2,566 | 1,609 | 2,630 | 2,566 | 1,602 | 1,609 |
R-squared | 0.142 | 0.371 | 0.154 | 0.333 | 0.163 | 0.152 | 0.137 | 0.035 | 0.155 | 0.034 |
Control group mean of dependent variable | 0.119 | 0.388 | 0.141 | 0.379 | 0.336 | 0.364 | 0.124 | 0.084 | 0.147 | 0.105 |
Robust standard errors presented in parentheses are adjusted for clustering at the level of the ward of residence.
p<0.01
p<0.05
p<0.1.
Controls include baseline values of the following household characteristics - log household size, main respondent age, gender, education and marital status, household demographic composition and indicators for the province of residence. Weights are applied to approximate effects for all eligible households in the study regions.
This information was collected only at final follow-up.
Table A5:
GIRLS | BOYS | |||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
(1) | (2) | (3) | (4) | (5) | (6) | (7) | (8) | (9) | (10) | |
Dependent variable: | Physical violence | Severe physical violence | Slapped/pushed | Hit with fist/kicked/beaten with object | Attacked/threatened with knife/other weapon | Physical violence | Severe physical violence | Slapped/pushed | Hit with fist/kicked/beaten with object | Attacked/threatened with knife/other weapon |
Treatment | 0.065 (0.068) | 0.062 (0.062) | 0.067 (0.065) | 0.057 (0.058) | 0.006 (0.023) | 0.047 (0.076) | 0.009 (0.069) | −0.005 (0.069) | 0.010 (0.071) | 0.021 (0.020) |
12-months | −0.156*** (0.055) | 0.017 (0.069) | −0.231*** (0.067) | 0.030 (0.068) | −0.028 (0.017) | −0.068 (0.070) | −0.051 (0.066) | −0.115* (0.066) | −0.050 (0.065) | 0.004 (0.030) |
48-months | 0.002 (0.050) | 0.012 (0.059) | 0.019 (0.054) | 0.012 (0.056) | −0.008 (0.017) | 0.108 (0.078) | 0.089 (0.086) | 0.006 (0.063) | 0.087 (0.092) | 0.049** (0.025) |
12-month treatment impact | 0.107 (0.085) | −0.013 (0.087) | 0.141* (0.081) | −0.014 (0.083) | 0.014 (0.026) | −0.079 (0.083) | 0.011 (0.080) | −0.049 (0.081) | 0.008 (0.079) | −0.017 (0.036) |
48-month treatment impact | −0.089 (0.072) | −0.028 (0.073) | −0.136** (0.064) | −0.010 (0.067) | −0.011 (0.028) | −0.279*** (0.092) | −0.179* (0.094) | −0.141* (0.077) | −0.186* (0.099) | −0.066** (0.030) |
Observations | 1,962 | 1,962 | 1,962 | 1,962 | 1,958 | 2,076 | 2,076 | 2,075 | 2,075 | 2,074 |
R-squared | 0.046 | 0.044 | 0.044 | 0.055 | 0.017 | 0.082 | 0.057 | 0.057 | 0.072 | 0.027 |
Baseline mean of dependent variable | 0.439 | 0.220 | 0.362 | 0.194 | 0.042 | 0.517 | 0.267 | 0.437 | 0.254 | 0.040 |
Robust standard errors presented in parentheses are adjusted for clustering at the level of the ward of residence.
p<0.01
p<0.05
p<0.1.
Controls include youth age and gender, and baseline values of the following household characteristics - log household size, main respondent age, gender, education and marital status, household demographic composition and indicators for the province of residence. Weights are applied to approximate effects for all eligible youth in the study regions. All physical violence measures capture abuse in the previous 12 months.
Table A6:
Panel A: Heterogeneous effects by contemporaneous schooling | Panel B: Heterogeneous effects by baseline schooling | ||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
(1) | (2) | (3) | (4) | (5) | (1) | (2) | (3) | (4) | (5) | ||
Dependent variable: | Physical violence | Severe physical violence | Slapped/pushed | Hit with a fist/kicked/beaten with object | Attacked/threatened with knife/other weapon | Dependent variable: | Physical violence | Severe physical violence | Slapped/pushed | Hit with a fist/kicked/beaten with object | Attacked/threatened with knife/other weapon |
12-month treatment impact | −0.100 (0.096) | −0.138 (0.083) | −0.052 (0.077) | −0.117 (0.080) | −0.032 (0.038) | 12-month treatment impact | −0.112 (0.106) | −0.117 (0.094) | −0.033 (0.090) | −0.096 (0.092) | −0.051 (0.031) |
12-month treatment impact*Currently enrolled in an educational institute | 0.246* (0.148) | 0.235* (0.133) | 0.252** (0.115) | 0.204 (0.133) | 0.046 (0.043) | 12-month treatment impact*Enrolled in an educational institute at baseline | 0.211 (0.147) | 0.177 (0.119) | 0.166 (0.137) | 0.152 (0.121) | 0.056 (0.035) |
48-month treatment impact | −0.244*** (0.076) | −0.219*** (0.072) | −0.165** (0.065) | −0.200*** (0.072) | −0.048 (0.033) | 48-month treatment impact | −0.218** (0.091) | −0.169* (0.088) | −0.206** (0.084) | −0.147* (0.085) | −0.018 (0.042) |
48-month treatment impact*Currently enrolled in an educational institute | 0.097 (0.093) | 0.184* (0.094) | 0.039 (0.093) | 0.158 (0.098) | 0.009 (0.033) | 48-month treatment impact*Enrolled in an educational institute at baseline | 0.100 (0.103) | 0.145 (0.101) | 0.124 (0.107) | 0.113 (0.099) | −0.022 (0.052) |
Observations | 4,029 | 4,029 | 4,028 | 4,028 | 4,023 | Observations | 3,405 | 3,405 | 3,405 | 3,405 | 3,401 |
R-squared | 0.063 | 0.051 | 0.049 | 0.056 | 0.011 | R-squared | 0.052 | 0.039 | 0.042 | 0.044 | 0.013 |
Baseline mean of dependent variable | 0.479 | 0.244 | 0.400 | 0.224 | 0.041 | Baseline mean of dependent variable | 0.479 | 0.244 | 0.400 | 0.224 | 0.041 |
Robust standard errors presented in parentheses are adjusted for clustering at the level of the ward of residence.
p<0.01
p<0.05
p<0.1.
Controls include youth age and gender, and baseline values of the following household characteristics - log household size, main respondent age, gender, education and marital status, household demographic composition and indicators for the province of residence. Specifications also contain fixed effects for treatment status and survey rounds, and interaction terms between the contemporaneous/baseline school enrolment indicator and these variables. Weights are applied to approximate effects for all eligible youth in the study regions. All physical violence measures capture abuse in the previous 12 months.
Table A7:
Panel A: Heterogeneous effects by current partnership status | Panel B: Heterogeneous effects by baseline partnership status | ||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
(1) | (2) | (3) | (4) | (5) | (1) | (2) | (3) | (4) | (5) | ||
Dependent variable: | Physical violence | Severe physical violence | Slapped/pushed | Hit with a fist/kicked/beaten with object | Attacked/threatened with knife/other weapon | Physical violence | Severe physical violence | Slapped/pushed | Hit with a fist/kicked/beaten with object | Attacked/threatened with knife/other weapon | |
12-month treatment impact | 0.028 (0.062) | −0.011 (0.059) | 0.083 (0.064) | −0.008 (0.054) | −0.006 (0.023) | 12-month treatment impact | 0.007 (0.066) | −0.023 (0.063) | 0.073 (0.070) | −0.017 (0.057) | −0.011 (0.021) |
12-month treatment impact*Current marital status | −0.175 (0.224) | −0.122 (0.225) | −0.376 (0.261) | −0.155 (0.214) | 0.029 (0.034) | 12-month treatment impact*Baseline marital status | −0.069 (0.325) | −0.049 (0.316) | −0.164 (0.357) | −0.116 (0.309) | 0.018 (0.093) |
48-month treatment impact | −0.192*** (0.058) | −0.110* (0.061) | −0.146*** (0.051) | −0.101 (0.062) | −0.046* (0.023) | 48-month treatment impact | −0.176** (0.079) | −0.111 (0.081) | −0.154** (0.071) | −0.100 (0.080) | −0.063* (0.036) |
48-month treatment impact*Current marital status | −0.294 (0.218) | −0.306 (0.214) | −0.283 (0.235) | −0.321 (0.214) | 0.053* (0.031) | 48-month treatment impact*Baseline marital status | −0.415 (0.277) | −0.245 (0.237) | −0.484* (0.273) | −0.278 (0.238) | 0.021 (0.062) |
Observations | 4,021 | 4,021 | 4,020 | 4,020 | 4,016 | Observations | 2,598 | 2,598 | 2,598 | 2,598 | 2,595 |
R-squared | 0.057 | 0.038 | 0.051 | 0.044 | 0.011 | R-squared | 0.060 | 0.048 | 0.061 | 0.056 | 0.022 |
Baseline mean of dependent variable | 0.477 | 0.243 | 0.399 | 0.223 | 0.041 | Baseline mean of dependent variable | 0.477 | 0.243 | 0.399 | 0.223 | 0.041 |
Robust standard errors presented in parentheses are adjusted for clustering at the level of the ward of residence.
p<0.01
p<0.05
p<0.1.
Controls include youth age and gender, and baseline values of the following household characteristics - log household size, main respondent age, gender, education and marital status, household demographic composition and indicators for the province of residence. Specifications also contain fixed effects for treatment status and survey rounds, and interaction terms between the current/baseline married indicator and these variables. Weights are applied to approximate effects for all eligible youth in the study regions. All physical violence measures capture abuse in the previous 12 months.
Table A8:
(1) | (2) | (3) | (4) | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Dependent variable: | Caregiver is married or co-habiting | Household size | Number of female adults (>=25 years) | Number of male adults (>=25 years) |
Treatment | −0.022 (0.027) | 0.224 (0.176) | 0.000 (0.040) | −0.047 (0.036) |
12-months | 0.014 (0.027) | −0.117 (0.074) | 0.017 (0.015) | 0.002 (0.018) |
48-months | 0.001 (0.026) | 2.100*** (0.239) | 0.048 (0.031) | −0.024 (0.034) |
12-month treatment impact | 0.002 (0.032) | −0.043 (0.096) | 0.009 (0.022) | −0.033 (0.025) |
48-month treatment impact | −0.005 (0.032) | −0.431* (0.255) | −0.038 (0.037) | 0.041 (0.040) |
Observations | 4,747 | 5,048 | 5,048 | 5,048 |
R-squared | 0.268 | 0.157 | 0.018 | 0.292 |
Baseline mean of dependent variable | 0.560 | 6.215 | 1.144 | 0.647 |
Robust standard errors presented in parentheses are adjusted for clustering at the level of the ward of residence.
p<0.01
p<0.05
p<0.1.
Controls in column 1 include caregivers’ gender and age, and baseline values of the following household characteristics - log household size, main respondent education, household demographic composition and indicators for the province of residence. Columns 2–4 control for baseline values of the following household characteristics – main respondent age, gender, education and marital status, and indicators for the province of residence (baseline demographic characteristics are omitted since the outcome variables are related to household composition). Weights are applied to approximate effects for all eligible households in the study regions. The sample is restricted to household that had youth members at baseline.
Table A9:
(1) | (2) | (3) | (4) | (5) | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Dependent variable: | Physical violence | Severe physical violence | Slapped/pushed | Hit with a fist/kicked/beaten with object | Attacked/threatened with knife/other weapon |
Treatment | 0.049 (0.050) | 0.035 (0.050) | 0.026 (0.044) | 0.030 (0.048) | 0.017 (0.016) |
12-months | −0.132*** (0.044) | −0.014 (0.045) | −0.199*** (0.052) | −0.011 (0.041) | −0.010 (0.016) |
48-months | 0.037 (0.040) | 0.056 (0.050) | −0.010 (0.037) | 0.054 (0.053) | 0.025 (0.015) |
12-month treatment impact | 0.044 (0.062) | 0.003 (0.056) | 0.078 (0.063) | 0.005 (0.051) | −0.005 (0.021) |
48-month treatment impact | −0.175*** (0.056) | −0.106* (0.062) | −0.121** (0.049) | −0.100 (0.063) | −0.042* (0.023) |
Observations | 3,596 | 3,596 | 3,596 | 3,596 | 3,592 |
R-squared | 0.048 | 0.038 | 0.041 | 0.043 | 0.009 |
Baseline mean of dependent variable | 0.477 | 0.243 | 0.399 | 0.223 | 0.041 |
Robust standard errors presented in parentheses are adjusted for clustering at the level of the ward of residence.
p<0.01
p<0.05
p<0.1.
Controls include youth age and gender, and baseline values of the following household characteristics - log household size, main respondent age, gender, education and marital status, household demographic composition and indicators for the province of residence. Weights are applied to approximate effects for all eligible youth in the study regions. The new youth are those who joined the study households between the 12-month follow-up and the 48-month follow-up surveys.
Table A10:
(1) | (2) | (3) | (4) | (5) | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Dependent variable: | Physical violence | Severe physical violence | Slapped/pushed | Hit with a fist/kicked/beaten with object | Attacked/threatened with knife/other weapon |
Treatment | −0.004 (0.041) | −0.010 (0.038) | −0.003 (0.038) | −0.012 (0.037) | 0.010 (0.014) |
12-months | −0.198*** (0.042) | −0.072* (0.041) | −0.202*** (0.044) | −0.062 (0.040) | −0.019 (0.014) |
48-months | −0.002 (0.035) | 0.009 (0.036) | −0.008 (0.033) | −0.000 (0.037) | 0.014 (0.013) |
12-month treatment impact | 0.100* (0.059) | 0.050 (0.049) | 0.089 (0.057) | 0.041 (0.047) | 0.008 (0.019) |
48-month treatment impact | −0.120** (0.047) | −0.043 (0.044) | −0.111** (0.043) | −0.034 (0.043) | −0.027 (0.017) |
Observations | 4,038 | 4,038 | 4,037 | 4,037 | 4,032 |
R-squared | 0.053 | 0.024 | 0.048 | 0.027 | 0.008 |
Baseline mean of dependent variable | 0.477 | 0.243 | 0.399 | 0.223 | 0.041 |
Robust standard errors presented in parentheses are adjusted for clustering at the level of the ward of residence.
p<0.01
p<0.05
p<0.1.
Controls include youth age and gender, and baseline values of the following household characteristics - log household size, main respondent age, gender, education and marital status, household demographic composition and indicators for the province of residence. All physical violence measures capture abuse in the previous 12 months.
Table A11:
(1) | (2) | (3) | (4) | (5) | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Dependent variable: | Physical violence | Severe physical violence | Slapped/pushed | Hit with a fist/kicked/beaten with object | Attacked/threatened with knife/other weapon |
12-month treatment impact | 0.045 (0.061) | 0.009 (0.054) | 0.082 (0.059) | 0.009 (0.049) | −0.003 (0.022) |
48-month treatment impact | −0.186*** (0.060) | −0.106* (0.061) | −0.135*** (0.048) | −0.097 (0.061) | −0.041* (0.022) |
Observations | 4,038 | 4,038 | 4,037 | 4,037 | 4,032 |
Baseline mean of dependent variable | 0.477 | 0.243 | 0.399 | 0.223 | 0.041 |
Robust standard errors presented in parentheses are adjusted for clustering at the level of the ward of residence.
p<0.01
p<0.05
p<0.1.
Controls include youth age and gender, and baseline values of the following household characteristics - log household size, main respondent age, gender, education and marital status, household demographic composition and indicators for the province of residence. Weights are applied to approximate effects for all eligible youth in the study regions. All physical violence measures capture abuse in the previous 12 months. We present marginal effects of the treatment at the different follow-up waves.
Footnotes
Declaration of Interest: None.
Children’s experiences with physical violence could include “corporal punishment and all other forms of torture, cruel, inhuman or degrading treatment or punishment as well as physical bullying and hazing by adults or by other children”. Corporal punishment is punishment with physical force and might involve smacking, slapping, spanking, kicking, shaking, pinching, biting, burning and other similar activities (UNICEF, 2014).
The UNICEF (2014) report also summarizes data from the Demographic and Health Surveys (DHS) of multiple countries that highlight that adolescent girls have a higher likelihood of experiencing physical abuse than sexual abuse.
Promising results on the linkages between SSNs and child or youth exposure to sexual violence tend to come from studies that focus on adolescent girls in Africa (Peterman et al., 2017).
The authors classify the following types of schemes as social safety net programs—conditional CTs, unconditional CTs, unconditional in-kind transfers, public work or cash for work programs, and vouchers or fee waivers.
For example, the effects of CTs could vary depending on transfer size (Angelucci, 2008) and women’s education (Hidrobo and Fernald, 2013). CT programs tend to provide transfers to women and since resource inequality within households is associated with higher levels of IPV (Cools and Kotsadam, 2017), the CTs could increase domestic abuse.
A household is considered to be food poor if it cannot meet the minimum food energy needs of household members—at least 2,100 kilocalorie per adult equivalent.
Cash plus programs combine CTs with complementary services or interventions that target specific domains of interest. The services that are part of the ‘plus’ components often differ across programs. For example, the Adolescent Girls Initiative in Kenya provides several interventions such as support for community-driven efforts oriented towards improving the status of girls, and health and financial education for adolescents (Austrian et al., 2016). Another scheme focused on adolescents—the Tanzania Productive Social Safety Net cash plus model for safe transitions to adulthood—implements an intervention that builds livelihood skills and employment opportunities for youth, delivers health and gender equity messaging, and strengthens links to existing government services (Tanzania Cash Plus Evaluation Team, 2018). Some cash plus programs focused on improving child wellbeing incorporate nutrition- and health-focused behavior change communication for mothers and/or other household members (Barry et al., 2017; Ahmed et al., 2019).
The NGO partners are J.F. Kapnek Trust, AfricAid and Childline Zimbabwe.
In fact, due to the subsequent financial crisis in Zimbabwe, the HSCT did not end up being expanded.
All HSCT evaluation surveys can be found here: https://transfer.cpc.unc.edu/countries-2/zimbabwe/.
The DHS domestic violence module uses similar questions (UNICEF, 2014), as does the World Health Organization’s Violence Against Women Instrument (Ellsberg and Heise, 2005).
The field work protocol explicitly stated that enumerators were to interview the three youngest members in the eligible age range if more than three were available; this explains why interviewed youth are younger than eligible non-interviewed youth. The individual who we refer to as the household head is the main respondent—the person interviewed for the household (or main) module of the HSCT surveys. The objective was to interview the head of the household.
It is also worth emphasizing that data was collected in a standardized way from both treatment and control regions.
Consistent with evidence from multiple settings on the links between poverty and child abuse/maltreatment (WHO & ISPCAN, 2006; Hussey et al., 2006; Gilbert et al., 2009; Akmatov, 2011; Cancian et al., 2013; Meinck et al., 2015), analysis of the NBSLEA data points to low socio-economic status being a risk factor for physical violence among children and youth in Zimbabwe (Izumi & Baago Rasmussen, 2018).
The community surveys were administered to a group of community leaders including teachers, village headmen and business owners.
We speculate that the small section of treatment households that do not report benefitting from the HSCT could be misreporting this information. No treatment households were excluded from the program during the study period, nor is there evidence of eligible households explicitly refusing to enroll in the program.
We look at effects separately for each follow-up round since data on awareness was collected differently across survey rounds—during the 12-month follow-up survey, only households that had a child referred to child protection and support services were asked whether they could name any service for children in their community, such as health services, food/nutrition related services or psychological support; at the 48-month survey, the question on awareness of child services was asked to all households.
This question was only asked at final follow-up.
Since individuals in marriages or relationships might be susceptible to systematically different patterns of violence (such as IPV), we also examine whether there is any heterogeneity in results by current or baseline partnership status (whether an individual is married or co-habiting). There is no evidence of such differential program effects—see estimates in Table A7 in the Appendix.
The signs on the coefficients suggest that the program might have brought about declines in violence by relatives and peers, but increases in abuse by partners and authority figures.
As mentioned previously, an earlier study documented HSCT’s effects on total household consumption from different sources—from purchases, own production and gifts (Bhalla et al., 2018). The analysis demonstrated that recipient households increase consumption from own purchases and reduce their reliance on gifts. This allows households to exercise greater choice in selecting their food basket and leads to improved diet diversity.
Table A1 shows that the average household head is a woman above the age of 50.
The HSCT specifically sought to protect orphans among other vulnerable children. Hence, it is not surprising that a non-trivial proportion of youth in our sample are orphans. Zimbabwe, like other countries in SSA, has a high number of orphans, largely due to the HIV/AIDS epidemic (Seidenfeld et al., 2016).
Here we look at the person who is the main respondent during the baseline survey, presumably the household head. While all efforts were made to interview the same individual as the main respondent during the follow-up rounds, this was not always possible.
Respondents could also potentially have been employed in work for a wage, salary, commission or payment in kind. In practice, however, 97 percent or more of the youth did not engage in such work during any survey wave (the reference period being the week before the survey). There are no statistically significant program effects on this type of work by young individuals.
A subsection of the youth module respondents interviewed at follow-up waves had not been recorded as household members during the HSCT baseline household survey, but were included in the household rosters collected at first follow-up—93 and 92 of those interviewed for the youth module at the 12-month and 48-month surveys respectively. These youth would have been exposed to the HSCT program for at least the three years between the last two surveys.
Impact estimates are the same when we estimate logit models (results available upon request).
Contributor Information
Averi Chakrabarti, Department of Global Health and Population, T.H. Chan School of Public Health, Harvard University, 90 Smith Street, Boston, MA 02120.
Sudhanshu Handa, Department of Public Policy, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill (UNC-CH).
Gustavo Angeles, Department of Maternal & Child Health, Gillings School of Global Public Health, UNC-CH.
David Seidenfeld, American Institutes for Research (AIR).
References
- Abramsky T, Watts CH, Garcia-Moreno C, Devries K, Kiss L, Ellsberg M, … & Heise L (2011). What factors are associated with recent intimate partner violence? Findings from the WHO multi-country study on women’s health and domestic violence. BMC public health, 11(1), 109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ahmed A, Hoddinott JF, Roy S, & Sraboni E (2019). Transfers, nutrition programming, and economic well-being: Experimental evidence from Bangladesh (International Food Policy Research Institute Discussion Paper No. 01879; ). [Google Scholar]
- Akmatov MK (2011). Child abuse in 28 developing and transitional countries—results from the Multiple Indicator Cluster Surveys. International Journal of epidemiology, 40(1), 219–227. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- American Institutes for Research. (2013). Baseline Report for Zimbabwe’s Harmonised Social Cash Transfer Programmes. Washington DC: American Institutes for Research. [Google Scholar]
- Angelucci M (2008). Love on the rocks: Domestic violence and alcohol abuse in rural Mexico. The BE Journal of Economic Analysis & Policy, 8(1). [Google Scholar]
- Attah R, Barca V, Kardan A, MacAuslan I, Merttens F, & Pellerano L (2016). Can social protection affect psychosocial wellbeing and why does this matter? Lessons from cash transfers in sub-Saharan Africa. The Journal of Development Studies, 52(8), 1115–1131. [Google Scholar]
- Austrian K, Muthengi E, Mumah J, Soler-Hampejsek E, Kabiru CW, Abuya B, & Maluccio JA (2016). The adolescent girls initiative-Kenya (AGI-K): study protocol. BMC public health, 16(210). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baird S, Ferreira FH, Özler B, & Woolcock M (2013). Relative effectiveness of conditional and unconditional cash transfers for schooling outcomes in developing countries: a systematic review. Campbell systematic reviews, 9(8). [Google Scholar]
- Banerjee A, La Ferrara E, & Orozco V (2019). Entertainment, Education and Attitudes Towards Domestic Violence. American Economic Review Papers and Proceedings, 109, 133–137. [Google Scholar]
- Barry O, Maïdoka AM, & Premand P (2017). Promoting Positive Parenting Practices in Niger through a Cash Transfer Programme. Retrieved from: https://bernardvanleer.org/app/uploads/2017/06/ECM17_12_Niger_Barry.pdf.
- Bashir S, Lockheed M, Ninan E, & Tan JP (2018). Facing forward: Schooling for learning in Africa. Retrieved from World Bank website: http://documents.worldbank.org/curated/en/113581538650102794/pdf/Facing-Forward-Schooling-for-Learning-in-Africa.pdf.
- Bastagli F, Hagen-Zanker J, Harman L, Sturge G, Barca V, Schmidt T, & Pellerano L (2016). Cash transfers: what does the evidence say? A rigorous review of impacts and the role of design and implementation features. Retrieved from Overseas Development Institute website: https://www.odi.org/sites/odi.org.uk/files/resource-documents/11316.pdf.
- Bhalla G, Handa S, Angeles G, & Seidenfeld D (2018). The effect of cash transfers and household vulnerability on food security in Zimbabwe. Food Policy, 74, 82–99. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bobonis GJ, González-Brenes M, & Castro R (2013). Public transfers and domestic violence: The roles of private information and spousal control. American Economic Journal: Economic Policy, 5(1), 179–205. [Google Scholar]
- Buller AM, Peterman A, Ranganathan M, Bleile A, Hidrobo M, & Heise L (2018). A mixed-method review of cash transfers and intimate partner violence in low-and middle-income countries. The World Bank Research Observer, 33(2), 218–258. [Google Scholar]
- Cameron AC, & Trivedi PK (2010). Microeconometrics using stata (Vol. 2). College Station, TX: Stata press. [Google Scholar]
- Cancian M, Yang MY, & Slack KS (2013). The effect of additional child support income on the risk of child maltreatment. Social Service Review, 87(3), 417–437. [Google Scholar]
- Carolina Population Center. (2018). Zimbabwe’s Harmonised Social Cash Transfer Programme—Endline Impact Evaluation Report. Chapel Hill: Carolina Population Center. [Google Scholar]
- Coates J, Swindale A, & Bilinsky P (2007). Household Food Insecurity Access Scale (HFIAS) for Measurement of Household Food Access: Indicator Guide (Vol. 3). Washington, D.C.: FHI 360/FANTA. [Google Scholar]
- Cools S, & Kotsadam A (2017). Resources and intimate partner violence in Sub-Saharan Africa. World Development, 95, 211–230. [Google Scholar]
- Dahl RE, Allen NB, Wilbrecht L, & Suleiman AB (2018). Importance of investing in adolescence from a developmental science perspective. Nature, 554(7693), 441. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Arruda PL (2018). Zimbabwe’s social protection system and its harmonized social cash transfer programme (International Policy Centre for Inclusive Growth (IPC-IG) Working Paper No. 175). Brasilia: IPC-IG. [Google Scholar]
- de Groot R, Palermo T, Handa S, Ragno LP, & Peterman A (2017). Cash transfers and child nutrition: pathways and impacts. Development Policy Review, 35(5), 621–643. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diener ED, Emmons RA, Larsen RJ, & Griffin S (1985). The satisfaction with life scale. Journal of personality assessment, 49(1), 71–75. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ellsberg M, & Heise L. (2005) Researching Violence Against Women: A Practical Guide for Researchers and Activists. Washington DC, United States: World Health Organization, PATH. [Google Scholar]
- Fernald LC, & Hidrobo M (2011). Effect of Ecuador’s cash transfer program (Bono de Desarrollo Humano) on child development in infants and toddlers: a randomized effectiveness trial. Social science & medicine, 72(9), 1437–1446. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fisher E, Attah R, Barca V, O’Brien C, Brook S, Holland J, … & Pozarny P (2017). The livelihood impacts of cash transfers in sub-Saharan Africa: beneficiary perspectives from six countries. World Development, 99, 299–319. [Google Scholar]
- Fiszbein A, Schady NR, Ferreira FHG, Grosh M, Kelleher N, Olinto P, & Skoufias E (2009). Conditional cash transfers: reducing present and future poverty. World Bank Publications. [Google Scholar]
- Gershoff ET (2002). Corporal punishment by parents and associated child behaviors and experiences: a meta-analytic and theoretical review. Psychological bulletin, 128(4), 539. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gilbert R, Widom CS, Browne K, Fergusson D, Webb E, & Janson S (2009). Burden and consequences of child maltreatment in high-income countries. The lancet, 373(9657), 68–81. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Global Initiative to End all Corporal Punishment of Children. (2020). Corporal Punishment of Children in Zimbabwe. Retrieved from: http://www.endcorporalpunishment.org/wp-content/uploads/country-reports/Zimbabwe.pdf.
- Green DP, Wilke A, & Cooper J (2019). Countering violence against women at scale: A mass media experiment in rural Uganda. Retrieved from: https://www.poverty-action.org/sites/default/files/publications/GreenWilkeCooper2019.pdf.
- Handa S, Daidone S, Peterman A, Davis B, Pereira A, Palermo T, & Yablonski J (2018). Myth-busting? Confronting six common perceptions about unconditional cash transfers as a poverty reduction strategy in Africa. The World Bank Research Observer, 33(2), 259–298. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heath R, Hidrobo M, & Roy S (2018). Cash transfers, polygamy, and intimate partner violence: Experimental evidence from Mali. [Google Scholar]
- Hidrobo M, & Fernald L (2013). Cash transfers and domestic violence. Journal of health economics, 32(1), 304–319. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hidrobo M, Peterman A, & Heise L (2016). The effect of cash, vouchers, and food transfers on intimate partner violence: evidence from a randomized experiment in Northern Ecuador. American Economic Journal: Applied Economics, 8(3), 284–303. [Google Scholar]
- Hillis S, Mercy J, Amobi A, & Kress H (2016). Global prevalence of past-year violence against children: a systematic review and minimum estimates. Pediatrics, 137(3), e20154079. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hussey JM, Chang JJ, & Kotch JB (2006). Child maltreatment in the United States: prevalence, risk factors, and adolescent health consequences. Pediatrics, 118(3), 933–942. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Izumi N, & Baago Rasmussen L (2018). The Multi-Country Study on the Drivers of Violence Affecting Children in Zimbabwe: Using a mixed methods, multi-stakeholder approach to discover what drives violence. Vulnerable children and youth studies, 13(1), 65–74. [Google Scholar]
- Lagarde M, Haines A, & Palmer N (2007). Conditional cash transfers for improving uptake of health interventions in low-and middle-income countries: a systematic review. Jama, 298(16), 1900–1910. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leroy JL, Ruel M, & Verhofstadt E (2009). The impact of conditional cash transfer programmes on child nutrition: a review of evidence using a programme theory framework. Journal of development effectiveness, 1(2), 103–129. [Google Scholar]
- Maddala GS (1986). Limited-dependent and qualitative variables in econometrics (Vol. 3). Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Manley J, Gitter S, & Slavchevska V (2013). How effective are cash transfers at improving nutritional status?. World development, 48, 133–155. [Google Scholar]
- Marcus R, & Page E (2014). Economic strengthening activities in child protection interventions: an adapted systematic review. London: ODI. [Google Scholar]
- Meinck F, Cluver LD, Boyes ME, & Mhlongo EL (2015). Risk and protective factors for physical and sexual abuse of children and adolescents in Africa: A review and implications for practice. Trauma, Violence, & Abuse, 16(1), 81–107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ndoma S (2017). Contrary to court ruling, Zimbabweans endorse parental right to physically discipline children. Retrieved from: https://afrobarometer.org/sites/default/files/publications/Dispatches/ab_r7_dispatchno156_corporal_punishment_in_zimbabwe.pdf. [Google Scholar]
- Ogando Portela MJ, & Pells K (2015). Corporal punishment in schools: longitudinal evidence from Ethiopia, India Peru and Viet Nam (Innocenti Discussion Paper No. 2015–02). Florence: United Nations Children’s Fund (UNICEF) Office of Research—Innocenti; Retrieved from: https://www.unicef-irc.org/publications/pdf/CORPORAL%20PUNISHMENT%20IDP2finalrev.pdf. [Google Scholar]
- Owusu-Addo E, & Cross R (2014). The impact of conditional cash transfers on child health in low-and middle-income countries: a systematic review. International journal of public health, 59(4), 609–618. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paolucci EO, Genuis ML, & Violato C (2001). A meta-analysis of the published research on the effects of child sexual abuse. The Journal of psychology, 135(1), 17–36. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paxson C, & Schady N (2010). Does Money Matter? The Effects of Cash Transfers on Child Development in Rural Ecuador. Economic Development and Cultural Change, 59(1), 187–229. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peterman A, Neijhoft A, Cook S, & Palermo TM (2017). Understanding the linkages between social safety nets and childhood violence: a review of the evidence from low-and middle-income countries. Health policy and planning, czx033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pettifor A, MacPhail C, Hughes JP, Selin A, Wang J, Gómez-Olivé FX, … & Suchindran C (2016). The effect of a conditional cash transfer on HIV incidence in young women in rural South Africa (HPTN 068): a phase 3, randomised controlled trial. The Lancet Global Health, 4(12), e978–e988. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seidenfeld D, Dumba L, Handa S, Muwoni L, Reeves H, & Sammon E (2016). Zimbabwe—Using evidence to overcome political and economic challenges to starting a national unconditional cash transfer programme In Davis B, Handa S, Hypher N, Winder Rossi N, Winters P, & Yablonski J (Eds.), From evidence to action: the story of cash transfers and impact evaluation in Sub-Saharan Africa (pp.226–246). New York, NY: Food and Agriculture Organization of the United Nations, United Nations Children’s Fund and Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Tanzania Cash Plus Evaluation Team. (2018). A Cash Plus Model for Safe Transitions to Healthy and Productive Adulthood: Baseline Report. Florence: UNICEF Office of Research—Innocenti. [Google Scholar]
- UN Committee on the Rights of the Child. (2011). General Comment No. 13. The Right of the Child to Freedom from All Forms of Violence. Retrieved from: https://www2.ohchr.org/english/bodies/crc/docs/CRC.C.GC.13_en.pdf.
- UN General Assembly. (1989). Convention on the Rights of the Child. Retrieved from: https://www.ohchr.org/en/professionalinterest/pages/crc.aspx.
- UNICEF. (2014). Hidden in plain sight: A statistical analysis of violence against children. New York: UNICEF. [Google Scholar]
- World Health Organization & International Society for Prevention of Child Abuse and Neglect. (2006). Preventing child maltreatment: a guide to taking action and generating evidence. Retrieved from: https://apps.who.int/iris/bitstream/handle/10665/43499/9241594365_eng.pdf;jsessionid=BF05F3E39E03B65DAE73978CB627C2DE?sequence=1.
- Zimbabwe National Statistics Agency (ZIMSTAT), United Nations Children’s Fund (UNICEF) & Collaborating Centre for Operational Research and Evaluation (CCORE). (2013). National Baseline Survey on Life Experiences of Adolescents, 2011. [Google Scholar]