Abstract
Accumulating evidence has suggested that the molecular transcriptional mechanism contributes to Alzheimer’s disease (AD) and its endophenotypes of cognitive decline and neuropathological traits, β-amyloid (Aβ) and phosphorylated tangles (TAU). However, it is unknown what is the impact of the AD risk factors, personality characteristics assessed by the NEO Five-Factor Inventory, on the human brain’s transcriptome. Using postmortem human brain samples from 466 subjects, we found that neuroticism has a significant overall impact on the brain transcriptome (omnibus P = 0.005) but not the other four personality characteristics. Focused on those cognitive decline related gene co-expressed modules, neuroticism has nominally significant associations (P < 0.05) with four neuronal modules, which are more related to PHFtau than Aβ across all eight brain regions. Furthermore, the effect of neuroticism on cognitive decline and AD might be mediated through the expression of module 7 and TAU pathology (P = 0.008). To conclude, neuroticism has a broad impact on the transcriptome of human brains, and its effect on cognitive decline and AD may be mediated through gene transcription programs related to TAU pathology.
Subject terms: Molecular neuroscience, Psychiatric disorders
Introduction
Alzheimer’s disease (AD) is the most common form of aging-related dementia, affecting over 5.8 million people in the United States alone (www.alz.org). Many risk factors contributing to AD risk have been identified; some are genetic while others are environmental exposures or life experiences. For some risk factors, e.g., APOE ε4, the series of molecular events that lead to the accumulation of β-amyloid (Aβ) and phosphorylated tangles (TAU), subsequent loss of cognitive function, and, eventually, a diagnosis of dementia has been well documented1. However, other risk factors remain poorly characterized at the molecular level. Here, we explored the molecular mechanism linking neuroticism to AD risk2. Understanding this mechanism may yield important clues to the changes in RNA expression that contribute to the onset of AD and new avenues for developing therapeutics. Neuroticism, or proneness to distress, is associated with faster cognitive decline and greater AD risk3. By contrast, conscientiousness—being organized, completing purposeful action, and having a drive to achieve—is associated with slower cognitive decline and lower AD risk3. While both have previously been reported to contribute to AD4,5, the role of neuroticism has been more consistently replicated3. The molecular composition of these personality traits and the series of molecular events that lead from personality traits to AD in the brain is unknown.
Recently, large-scale molecular data—such as RNA sequence (RNAseq) data from the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC)6—have been generated from the brains of individuals who were deeply characterized while they were alive. Specifically, we repurposed data6 generated from 542 participants in two cohort studies of cognitive aging, the Religious Orders Study (ROS) and the Rush Memory and Aging Project (MAP), which include prospective brain autopsy of each participant7. We previously examined the molecular features of the aging cortex that relate to AD pathology and cognitive decline6. Here, we investigate the molecular consequences of personality traits leveraging data from the NEO Five Factor personality inventory. The DLPFC is one brain region that has been implicated in neuroticism based on magnetic resonance imaging8.
Materials and methods
Participants
The ROS and the Rush Memory and Aging Project (MAP) are longitudinal studies which have enrolled >3600 subjects without known dementia at baseline7. Both studies were approved by an Institutional Review Board of Rush University Medical Center. All participants signed an informed consent, an Anatomical Gift Act, and a repository consent allowing their data to be repurposed. The participants undergo detailed cognitive testing on an annual basis and other ante-mortem phenotyping including the five personal traits: neuroticism, conscientiousness, extraversion, openness, and agreeableness. These personal variables are measured using 12 items from the NEO Five-Factor Inventory, and each item has scores which range from 0 to 4 and are summed to yield a maximum composite score of 48, where a higher score indicates greater magnitude of these personal traits5,9–11. The two studies are run by the same group of investigators and are designed to be analyzed jointly as the same procedures are used to capture these traits in both studies.
After death, a structured neuropathologic examination is performed to obtain quantitative measures of Aβ, TAU, and other pathologies as well as a pathologic diagnosis of AD. Aβ protein is identified by molecularly-specific immunohistochemistry and quantified by image analysis12,13. TAU pathology is measured by immunohistochemistry using the AT8 antibody which recognizes a phosphorylated Tau peptide. The values are the percentage area occupied by Aβ using image analysis. TAU proteins represent the density using stereology. Both Aβ and TAU are measured in eight human brain regions: hippocampus, entorhinal cortex, midfrontal cortex, inferior temporal, angular gyrus, calcarine cortex, anterior cingulate cortex, and superior frontal cortex12,13, and the mean across the eight brain regions are used as the overall level of Aβ or TAU. Results are transformed by taking the square root to fit a normal distribution. The informed consent was obtained from all subjects and the current analyses was approved by the Institutional Review Board of Columbia University Irving Medical Center.
One hemisphere is frozen at the time of autopsy, and this material was accessed to generate RNAseq data from the DLPFC.
Gene expression modules
For the current study, there are 466 subjects with both measurements of the brain transcriptome and at least one of the five personality traits. The detailed methods by which the RNAseq were generated and analyzed were described in a prior report6. In brief, gray matter from the DLPFC were processed, and RNA was extracted from each sample (Qiagen’s miRNeasy mini kit and the RNase free DNAse Set). A cDNA library was prepared from each sample and then sequenced using the Illumina HiSeq platform with 101 bp paired-end reads and an average coverage of 50 million reads. The obtained sequences were aligned to the reference genome of GRCh37 using Bowtie14, and the level of expression of individual genes were estimated by the RSEM v1.2.3115 package. This was followed by quantile normalization and batch effect removal using the Combat algorithm16. At the quality control stage, those genes with less than 4 reads in 100 individuals are removed, leading to 13,484 genes with expression measures. Groups of co-expressed genes (“modules”) across the 542 subjects were defined using the SpeakEasy17 consensus clustering algorithm; the 13,484 genes were collapsed into 47 modules of co-expressed genes with an average of 330 genes per module.
Genotype data
A meta-analysis of the genome-wide association study (GWAS) has reported that 70 single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) from 47 genes were significantly associated with neuroticism18. There were 69 SNPs were available in ROSMAP genotype dataset. ROSMAP genotype array and imputations were described in details previously19. Briefly, we used the genotype data which were imputed against the Haplotype Reference Consortium reference v1.1. There were 398 subjects with data of genotype, neuroticism trait, and gene expression modules.
Statistical analysis
As an explorative study design, we used the maximum sample size which was available for each analysis. We applied a generalized linear regression model using the R “glm” function to conduct the association tests in the study. These models adjust for the covariates of age at death, education, sex, race, study, and technical variables of gene expression experiments such as the experimental batches and RNA integrity score (RIN score). For the mediation analysis, we used the “mediation” R package (https://cran.rproject.org/web/packages/mediation/ vignettes/mediation.pdf). The outcome variables followed normal distributions and the variances between groups were assumed to be similar. We compared the mediation P value to the direct effect P value. A P value of <0.05 is considered as statistical significance.
Pathway analysis
Based on the four neuroticism-associated co-expression gene modules (m7, m6, m127, and m131), we identified 20 genes associated with neuroticism (FDR P < 0.05). We conducted the pathway enrichment analysis with these 20 using “STRINGdb” (version 10) against the functional Kyoto Encyclopedia of Genes and Genomes (KEGG) Pathway database (https://www.genome.jp/kegg/pathway.html)20 with FDR correction21. In order to provide an overall view of the general function of the whole module, we also conducted pathway analysis on all the genes included in each of the four neuroticism-associated gene modules.
Results
Population characteristics
We first reproduced the role of neuroticism and conscientiousness in the subset of 466 deceased ROSMAP participants (mean of age at death = 88 years and 62% female) which have (1) personality inventory data; (2) at least two measures of cognitive performance so that a person-specific slope of cognitive decline can be calculated and (3) transcriptomic data (Table 1). We found that neuroticism contributes to accelerated cognitive decline (beta = −2.11 × 10−3, P = 3.48 × 10−3) while conscientiousness is protective against cognitive decline (beta = 2.38 × 10−3, P = 2.58 × 10−2) controlling for the age at death, sex, race, education and study center. The association with neuroticism remained significant after further controlling for neuropathologic indices such as Aβ, Lewy body, and vascular risk factors. However, after controlling for TAU, the association of neuroticism and cognitive decline became borderline (beta = −1.24 × 10−3, P = 5.3 × 10−2), and it became non-significant when the model is adjusted for all of the neuropathologic indices together (beta = −9.71 × 10−4, P = 0.11) (Supplementary Table S1). These results are very consistent with prior analyses4,5 of other subsets of ROSMAP participants. Having established the role of these two personality traits in aging-related cognitive decline in our sample, we moved on to identify the molecular features which are associated with them.
Table 1.
Male | Female | All | P | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Numbera | 179 | 287 | 466 | |
Age at death (years)b | 86 (6.6) | 90 (6.6) | 88 (6.8) | 2.56E−07 |
Education (years)b | 17.5 (3.7) | 16.1 (3.2) | 16.7 (3.5) | 7.76E−05 |
White (N, %)c | 176, 98% | 285, 99% | 461 (99%) | 0.59 |
Neuroticismb | 16 (6.2) | 17 (6.7) | 17 (6.5) | 0.04 |
Conscientiousnessb | 33 (5.5) | 34 (4.6) | 34 (5.0) | 0.02 |
Opennessb | 25 (6.1) | 26 (4.8) | 25 (5.3) | 0.23 |
Agreeablenessb | 33 (4.4) | 34 (3.5) | 34 (3.9) | 0.08 |
Extraversionb | 30 (5.9) | 30 (6.0) | 30 (6.0) | 0.27 |
Aβ pathology (square root) | 1.3 (1.1) | 1.6 (1.1) | 1.5 (1.1) | 0.05 |
TAU pathology (square root) | 1.8 (1.3) | 2.2 (1.2) | 2.0 (1.3) | 0.009 |
Aβ β-amyloid, TAU abnormally phosphorylated Tau protein, AT8.
aN represent the total number of subjects with measurements of any of the five personality traits: neuroticism, conscientiousness, openness, agreeableness, and extraversion.
bRepresent the mean and standard deviation of each trait in all subjects, males and females and the P values of their differences between males and females using t test.
cRepresent the count and percentage of each trait in all subjects, males and females and the P values of their differences between males and females using chi-square test.
Neuroticism has an overall impact on modules of co-expressed genes
We first assessed the impact of neuroticism on the aging DLPFC’s transcriptome by conducting an omnibus test, which evaluates the distribution of the associations with all of the 47 groups of co-expressed cortical gene (which we refer to as “modules”). We find that neuroticism has a broad effect on cortical RNA profiles: there is an excess of suggestive associations that are unlikely to have occurred by chance (P = 0.005) (Fig. 1A). However, none of the other four personality traits: conscientiousness, agreeableness, openness, extraversion, has an overall impact on the transcriptome (P > 0.05) (Supplementary Fig. S1). Another way to illustrate this finding is to illustrate that, at a threshold of P < 0.05, neuroticism is associated with 18 (38%) of the 47 cortical modules (Fig. 1B). For comparison, the other four personality traits, together, are only associated with four modules at this threshold (Supplementary Fig. S1). Conscientiousness influences one of these four modules, but this module is unrelated to cognitive decline. Thus, neuroticism has an outsized association with the transcriptome of the aging brain compared to other personality traits.
Neuroticism has an impact on modules of co-expressed genes that are associated with cognitive decline
We previously reported that 11 modules of co-expressed genes have significant associations with cognitive decline in these data6, and four of these modules reached nominal significance for association with neuroticism: modules m7 (P = 0.02), m6 (P = 0.02), m127 (P = 0.04), and m131 (P = 0.04) (Fig. 1B). The association between neuroticism and the expression levels of each of the four modules are positive for m7 and m6 but negative for m127 and m131 (Fig. 1C, D and Supplementary Fig. S2). This means that subjects with more severe neuroticism have higher expression levels of the genes included in the m7 and m6 modules but lower expression levels of the genes included in m127 and m131. We previously reported that subjects with accelerated cognitive decline have higher expression levels of genes in m7 and m6 but lower levels in m127 and m1316. In this case, the direction of the associations among neuroticism, modules and cognitive decline is consistent, showing that subjects with more severe neuroticism have more rapid cognitive decline, higher expression levels of m7 and m6 but lower levels of m127 and m131. Thus, we prioritized a set of modules that may be responding to greater neurotic behavior and cause downstream cognitive decline. However, none of the 11 modules display a relationship to conscientiousness, which is protective for AD; that process may work through other mechanisms or may work through another brain region.
We further conducted the association tests with the individual gene included in the modules m7, m6, m127, and m131 to identify those individual genes which are related to neuroticism. Out of the 499 genes in these 4 modules, 20 have FDR P < 0.05, 18 of which are in m6 and the remaining 2 are in m7 (Fig. 1E). The pathway enrichment analysis for these 20 genes highlights the involvement of mRNA surveillance pathway (FDR_P = 0.008, hits = 2 (2%)) and protein processing in endoplasmic reticulum (FDR_P = 0.01, hits = 2 (1%)) (Table 2). In order to have a general understanding of the neuroticism-associated modules, we also added the pathway analysis (Supplementary Table S2) for all the genes included in each module and the top pathway for m7 was MAPK signaling pathway (FDR_P = 6.27 × 10−4, hits = 5 (2%)) and the top pathway for m6, m127 and 131 was the same, which was the metabolic pathways (FDR_P = 0.03, 9.36 × 10−4, and 7.92 × 10−4, hits = 13 (1%), 8 (0.7%) and 5 (0.4%) for m6, m127 and 131, respectively).
Table 2.
Term_id | Proteins | Hits | P value | FDR | Term_description |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
3015 | 86 | 2 | 0.0004 | 0.008 | mRNA surveillance pathway |
4141 | 162 | 2 | 0.001 | 0.013 | Protein processing in endoplasmic reticulum |
601 | 26 | 1 | 0.009 | 0.047 | Glycosphingolipid biosynthesis—lacto and neolacto series |
514 | 31 | 1 | 0.011 | 0.047 | Other types of O-glycan biosynthesis |
3410 | 33 | 1 | 0.011 | 0.047 | Base excision repair |
5030 | 49 | 1 | 0.017 | 0.051 | Cocaine addiction |
310 | 50 | 1 | 0.017 | 0.051 | Lysine degradation |
We also annotated the cell types which may express the four modules associated with both neuroticism and cognitive decline (Fig. 1F). All of the four modules are annotated to be expressed in neurons. Both modules m7 and m6 are enriched in the genes expressed by GABAergic neurons, while m127 is enriched in genes expressed by cholinergic neurons and m131 is enriched in genes expressed by other excitatory neurons.
Neuroticism and cognitive decline associated co-expressed gene modules are related to AD pathology
We further analyzed which AD pathology, Aβ or TAU, are primarily involved in neuroticism pathology. Neuroticism has a positive association with TAU (P = 0.03) but not Aβ (P = 0.16) in this subsample of participants (Fig. 2A), both of these measures are calculated as the mean densities across eight different brain regions. We then repeated our analysis by replacing the neuroticism trait with the 69 SNPs which were reported to be associated with neuroticism in GWAS and were available in ROSMAP participants18. Only one SNP was associated with Aβ, which was rs2073498 in the RASSF1 locus (BETA = 0.238, SE = 0.106, P = 0.026). There were three SNPs with significant associations with TAU, which were rs7199949 at PRSS53 (BETA = −0.172, SE = 0.086, P = 0.046), rs240780 at ASCC3 (BETA = 0.223, SE = 0.083, P = 0.007), and rs3173615 at TMEM106B (BETA = 0.22, SE = 0.083, P = 0.008). Thus, overall, we have observed that neuroticism was associated with TAU pathology more than with Aβ pathology.
For the four modules associated with both neuroticism and cognitive decline, three of them have smaller P values for TAU than the P values for Aβ based on our previously reported associations6 and only module m7 was reported to have significant association with the average level of TAU (Fig. 2B). More detailed analysis with the eight region-specific measures of TAU and Aβ also suggest that the effect of module m7 on TAU outperformed all of the other tested associations (Fig. 2C).
Relationship between neuroticism and module 7 gene expression
As noted above, the neuroticism GWAS18 reported that there were 70 SNPs associated with neuroticism, and these 70 SNPs are annotated as involving 47 discrete genes in susceptibility to neuroticism. Although none of the 47 genes were found in m7 (data were not shown), rs739134 in the C22orf46 locus was significantly associated with both neuroticism (BETA = 1.545, SE = 0.56, P = 0.006) and module 7 gene expression (BETA = 0.005, SE = 0.002, P = 0.016) (Fig. 3A), indicating a possible causal effect of neuroticism on m7. However, rs739134 cannot be treated as the instrumental variable to infer the causal effect of neuroticism on m7 because the effect of rs739134 on m7 remains significant after adjusting for neuroticism (BETA = 0.0045, SE = 0.002, P = 0.037), indicating that this variant may have pleiotropic effects; it may affect both m7 and neuroticism through different pathways (Fig. 3B). Thus, we cannot conduct the mendelian randomization to infer the suggested causal effect of neuroticism on m7.
Mediation modeling to propose a sequence of events
Using a rigorous statistical methodology and our cross-sectional data obtained from autopsy material, we assessed the most likely location of each module in the sequence of events leading to AD to help to generate hypotheses that can be tested in preclinical and in vitro models. Mediation modeling suggests that the four modules of co-expressed genes which are altered in expression in the frontal cortex by neuroticism may contribute to accelerating aging-related cognitive decline. For example, using m7 which is the module most strongly associated with cognitive decline, the model in which neuroticism is associated with faster cognitive decline (P = 0.008) through an association with higher m7 expression is more significant than the alternative model where neuroticism has a direct causal effect on cognitive decline which bypasses m7 expression (P = 0.042) (Fig. 3 left). We find similar results for modules m6 and m127 but not m131 (Supplementary Table S3).
Based on our previous and current report that m7 is the module most directly contributing to the accumulation of TAU pathology6, we added this variable to our model to more precisely evaluate the mechanism (Fig. 3 right). Thus, we propose that the most likely scenario in terms of the role of m7 in neuroticism’s effect on AD is the following sequence of events: more neuroticism → more m7 in the frontal cortex → more TAU pathology accumulation → more cognitive decline → greater likelihood of developing AD dementia. m127 and m131 are not associated with TAU pathology and may therefore work through another mechanism to mediate the effects of neuroticism on cognitive decline (mediation p < 0.01).
Discussion
Neuroticism appears to have a broad impact on the biology of the older brain, as measured by RNA expression: ~1/5 of our modules of co-expressed genes are associated with an individual’s level of neuroticism at a nominal p < 0.05 threshold. Thus, we have started to map out the molecular changes in the target organ (brain) that relate to neuroticism, an important personality trait that is a risk factor for AD and aging-related cognitive decline. The molecular substrate of neuroticism remains poorly understood as few studies have systematically assessed this question in large numbers of individuals, so this report is an important step forward in demonstrating a widespread biological effect of neuroticism in the frontal cortex. These large changes in older individuals beg the question of whether the changes are clinically meaningful: are they associated with symptoms, with changes in cognition?
We explored this question in detail and found that three of the modules associated with neuroticism are also associated with the slope of cognitive decline and a diagnosis of AD, providing a potential molecular link between AD and its risk factor, neuroticism. Since association studies of cross-sectional data, such as the one that we performed, cannot formally resolve the direction of causality, we used rigorous statistical methodology to test for mediation which enables us to propose a sequence of events that best fits our cross-sectional data. We considered deploying mendelian randomization methods to address this question which can leverage the 70 SNPs reported to be associated with neuroticism; however, the lack of SNP associations with module expression prevented this. The one SNP in the C22orf46 locus that shows some level of effect on both traits appears to have a pleiotropic effect; thus a genetic approach to this question was not informative. Nonetheless, our mediation analyses are very useful in generating hypotheses that can then be tested in cell culture or mouse model systems. In our analyses, we find that module m7, the module with the strongest association with neuroticism is most likely to increase in expression in response to increased neurotic behavior and to then contribute to cognitive decline (Fig. 3). In addition, we have found that the neuroticism was related to TAU pathology more than Aβ, which was in line with the previous GWAS report18 that one of the neuroticism susceptibility loci includes MAPT, the gene encoding tangles. These insights are important because, if validated in future studies, it could guide the development of future drugs to prevent AD. For example, a drug reducing m7 expression may be ineffective in preventing neuroticism, but it may be helpful to prevent the spread of negative molecular changes in the cortex that lead to cognitive decline and AD. Since personality traits like neuroticism cannot be readily changed, blocking the dysfunctional consequences of such personality traits may be the best option with which to manage individuals at risk of AD because of their personality traits.
We focused on neuroticism given that it is the personality trait most strongly associated with risk of AD and that its association has been well validated. Nonetheless, we also evaluated four other major personality traits in our analyses to be thorough, and, while we found some associations, they were much fewer than the neuroticism associations and did not relate to cognitive decline. It is possible that the effects of these other personality traits may be more subtle and/or that we need to sample other brain regions to identify their effects.
Our study has many strengths, including its large sample size and largely community-based design, which facilitates repurposing our results and may offer insights translatable to the older community. We also have very deep RNAseq profiles, which enhanced the modules’ information content. Nonetheless, there are important limitations as well: we have already discussed the cross-sectional nature of the data that is a practical limitation of autopsy studies and prevents us from formally resolving the issue of causality. In addition, the average age at death was 88 years, which may make it challenging to extrapolate our results to the population of younger older individuals where many treatments for AD will need to be targeted.
Overall, we have prioritized a set of molecular changes in the frontal cortex that are involved in both neuroticism and aging-related cognitive decline and AD. We have also proposed a specific sequence of events that can now be tested in model systems. This hypothesis can be refined by performing many of the analyses reported here at the gene level instead of at the level of the module of co-expressed genes: this will facilitate validation studies and will be necessary to enable drug development efforts by presenting a specific target. We have therefore laid a strong foundation for future biological investigations of the effects of an important AD risk factor that affects many individuals: neuroticism.
Supplementary information
Acknowledgements
We are grateful to the participants in the Religious Order Study, the Rush Memory and Aging Project. This work is supported by the US National Institutes of Health U01AG61356, RP30AG10161, R01AG17917, R01AG15819. Y.M. is supported by the 2019-AARF-644521.
Author contributions
C.H.D. and Y.M.: study concept and design, data analysis and manuscript draft. C.C.W.: guidance of omnibus test. D.A.B.: provide data and critical review of manuscript.
Data availability
All the data and analysis output are available via the AD Knowledge Portal (https://adknowledgeportal.synapse.org). The AD Knowledge Portal is a platform for accessing data, analyses, and tools generated by the Accelerating Medicines Partnership (AMP-AD) Target Discovery Program and other National Institute on Aging (NIA)-supported programs to enable open-science practices and accelerate translational learning. The data, analyses and tools are shared early in the research cycle without a publication embargo on secondary use. Data is available for general research use according to the following requirements for data access and data attribution (https://adknowledgeportal.synapse.org/DataAccess/Instructions). The link to the data for this manuscript is https://adknowledgeportal.synapse.org/Explore/Studies/DetailsPage?Study=syn3219045.
Conflict of interest
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary information
The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1038/s41398-021-01253-6.
References
- 1.Theendakara V, Peters-Libeu CA, Bredesen DE, Rao RV. Transcriptional effects of ApoE4: relevance to Alzheimer’s disease. Mol. Neurobiol. 2018;55:5243–5254. doi: 10.1007/s12035-017-0757-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Wilson RS, Begeny CT, Boyle PA, Schneider JA, Bennett DA. Vulnerability to stress, anxiety, and development of dementia in old age. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry. 2011;19:327–334. doi: 10.1097/JGP.0b013e31820119da. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.D’Iorio A, et al. Meta-analysis of personality traits in Alzheimer’s disease: a comparison with healthy subjects. J. Alzheimers Dis. 2018;62:773–787. doi: 10.3233/JAD-170901. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Wilson RS, et al. Premorbid proneness to distress and episodic memory impairment in Alzheimer’s disease. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry. 2004;75:191–195. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2003.026260. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Wilson RS, Schneider JA, Arnold SE, Bienias JL, Bennett DA. Conscientiousness and the incidence of Alzheimer disease and mild cognitive impairment. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 2007;64:1204–1212. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.10.1204. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Mostafavi S, et al. A molecular network of the aging human brain provides insights into the pathology and cognitive decline of Alzheimer’s disease. Nat. Neurosci. 2018;21:811–819. doi: 10.1038/s41593-018-0154-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Bennett DA, et al. Religious orders study and rush memory and aging project. J. Alzheimers Dis. 2018;64:S161–S189. doi: 10.3233/JAD-179939. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Lucas I, Balada F, Blanco E, Aluja A. Prefrontal cortex activity triggered by affective faces exposure and its relationship with neuroticism. Neuropsychologia. 2019;132:107146. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2019.107146. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Krueger KR, Wilson RS, Shah RC, Tang Y, Bennett DA. Personality and incident disability in older persons. Age Ageing. 2006;35:428–433. doi: 10.1093/ageing/afl028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Wilson RS, et al. Neuroticism, extraversion, and mortality in a defined population of older persons. Psychosom. Med. 2005;67:841–845. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000190615.20656.83. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Buchman AS, et al. Neuroticism, extraversion, and motor function in community-dwelling older persons. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry. 2013;21:145–154. doi: 10.1016/j.jagp.2012.10.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Bennett DA, Schneider JA, Arvanitakis Z, Wilson RS. Overview and findings from the religious orders study. Curr. Alzheimer Res. 2012;9:628–645. doi: 10.2174/156720512801322573. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Bennett DA, et al. Overview and findings from the rush Memory and Aging Project. Curr. Alzheimer Res. 2012;9:646–663. doi: 10.2174/156720512801322663. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Langmead B, Trapnell C, Pop M, Salzberg SL. Ultrafast and memory-efficient alignment of short DNA sequences to the human genome. Genome Biol. 2009;10:R25. doi: 10.1186/gb-2009-10-3-r25. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Li B, Dewey CN. RSEM: accurate transcript quantification from RNA-Seq data with or without a reference genome. BMC Bioinforma. 2011;12:323. doi: 10.1186/1471-2105-12-323. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Johnson WE, Li C, Rabinovic A. Adjusting batch effects in microarray expression data using empirical Bayes methods. Biostatistics. 2007;8:118–127. doi: 10.1093/biostatistics/kxj037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Gaiteri C, et al. Identifying robust communities and multi-community nodes by combining top-down and bottom-up approaches to clustering. Sci. Rep. 2015;5:16361. doi: 10.1038/srep16361. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Nagel M, et al. Meta-analysis of genome-wide association studies for neuroticism in 449,484 individuals identifies novel genetic loci and pathways. Nat. Genet. 2018;50:920–927. doi: 10.1038/s41588-018-0151-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Felsky D, et al. Neuropathological correlates and genetic architecture of microglial activation in elderly human brain. Nat. Commun. 2019;10:409. doi: 10.1038/s41467-018-08279-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Szklarczyk D, et al. STRING v10: protein-protein interaction networks, integrated over the tree of life. Nucleic Acids Res. 2015;43:D447–D452. doi: 10.1093/nar/gku1003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Benjamini YHY. Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J. R. Stat. Soc. 1995;57:289–300. [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
All the data and analysis output are available via the AD Knowledge Portal (https://adknowledgeportal.synapse.org). The AD Knowledge Portal is a platform for accessing data, analyses, and tools generated by the Accelerating Medicines Partnership (AMP-AD) Target Discovery Program and other National Institute on Aging (NIA)-supported programs to enable open-science practices and accelerate translational learning. The data, analyses and tools are shared early in the research cycle without a publication embargo on secondary use. Data is available for general research use according to the following requirements for data access and data attribution (https://adknowledgeportal.synapse.org/DataAccess/Instructions). The link to the data for this manuscript is https://adknowledgeportal.synapse.org/Explore/Studies/DetailsPage?Study=syn3219045.