Abstract
Acute caffeine intake can delay sleep initiation and reduce sleep intensity, particularly when consumed in the evening. However, it is not clear whether these sleep disturbances disappear when caffeine is continuously consumed during daytime, which is common for most coffee drinkers. To address this question, we investigated the sleep of twenty male young habitual caffeine consumers during a double-blind, randomized, crossover study including three 10-day conditions: caffeine (3 × 150 mg caffeine daily), withdrawal (3 × 150 mg caffeine for 8 days, then switch to placebo), and placebo (3 × placebo daily). After 9 days of continuous treatment, electroencephalographically (EEG)-derived sleep structure and intensity were recorded during a scheduled 8-h nighttime sleep episode starting 8 (caffeine condition) and 15 h (withdrawal condition) after the last caffeine intake. Upon scheduled wake-up time, subjective sleep quality and caffeine withdrawal symptoms were assessed. Unexpectedly, neither polysomnography-derived total sleep time, sleep latency, sleep architecture nor subjective sleep quality differed among placebo, caffeine, and withdrawal conditions. Nevertheless, EEG power density in the sigma frequencies (12–16 Hz) during non-rapid eye movement sleep was reduced in both caffeine and withdrawal conditions when compared to placebo. These results indicate that daily caffeine intake in the morning and afternoon hours does not strongly impair nighttime sleep structure nor subjective sleep quality in healthy good sleepers who regularly consume caffeine. The reduced EEG power density in the sigma range might represent early signs of overnight withdrawal from the continuous presence of the stimulant during the day.
Subject terms: Sleep, Slow-wave sleep
Introduction
Caffeine is the most popular psychoactive substance in the world1, consumed daily by around 80% of the population2. While caffeine is frequently used to counteract sleepiness and boost performance3, its consumption is commonly avoided in the evening4,5 to prevent adverse consequences on nocturnal sleep3. The sleep disrupting effects of caffeine are mainly attributed to its influence on the homeostatic component of sleep-wake regulation. Sleep homeostasis describes the increase in sleep pressure during time awake and its dissipation during the following sleep episode6, which has been suggested to be related to rising and decreasing concentrations of adenosine7. Caffeine is an adenosine receptor antagonist, which blocks the A1 and A2A adenosine receptors in the central nervous system1. It may, thus, attenuate the increase in sleep pressure during wakefulness8 and lead to delayed sleep initiation and more superficial sleep9.
The effects of caffeine intake on the quality and quantity of sleep depend on the timing of its consumption. More specifically, caffeine consumed in the evening hours prolongs sleep latency10–14, reduces total sleep time (TST)10–12,14,15, shortens deep sleep10,12–15, and decreases electroencephalographically (EEG)-derived slow-wave activity (SWA)10, while activity in the sigma range is increased10. However, evening caffeine intake only accounts for approximately 10–20% of the total daily caffeine intake in regular consumers4,5. It needs to be elucidated whether habitual caffeine intake restricted to the morning and afternoon hours similarly affects nighttime sleep.
Furthermore, not only the timing but also the frequency of preceding caffeine intake prior to sleep may be an important factor for the repercussions on sleep. The majority of the worldwide population consumes caffeine on a daily basis2, which can lead to tolerance development due to the recurrent supply of the psychostimulant1. In line with these results, the sleep-disrupting effects of continuous high-dose caffeine in the morning, afternoon, and evening (3 × 400 mg) intake vanished and only stage 4 sleep remained reduced after 1 week of caffeine intake12. However, whether more sensitive markers for sleep intensity such as spectral sleep EEG measures, adapt to the long-term exposure to the stimulant has to our best knowledge not yet been investigated.
Importantly, not only caffeine per se, but also the state of acute abstinence to which regular consumers expose themselves every night, might affect sleep. This so-called overnight abstinence represents the start of a caffeine withdrawal phase16. Withdrawal symptoms such as increased tiredness17, longer sleep duration, and better sleep quality18 can be observed at a subjective level starting roughly 12 h after last caffeine intake17. However, the influence of caffeine withdrawal on objective EEG-derived sleep variables were not systematically reported up to date and remain to be compared against a placebo-baseline.
Here we aimed at determining whether daily caffeine intake during morning and afternoon hours impairs nighttime sleep structure and sleep intensity after continuous daytime caffeine intake over 9 days. We hypothesized a reduced depth of sleep after caffeine intake, indexed in shortened slow-wave sleep (SWS) duration and a decrease in SWA compared to placebo. Moreover, we hypothesized that the abrupt cessation from the daily intake generates acute subjective withdrawal symptoms, and changes sleep structure and intensity compared to both the daily caffeine intake and the placebo-baseline.
Results
Salivary caffeine levels
Caffeine levels significantly differed between each of the three conditions (main effect of condition: F2,90.7 = 46.12, p < 0.001) with the highest levels in the caffeine condition and the lowest in the placebo condition (post-hoc comparisons: pall < 0.01). In addition, a significant interaction of the factors condition and time (F2,89.6 = 10.65, p < 0.001) confirmed that caffeine levels were modulated by time with levels decreasing during nighttime sleep in the caffeine condition only (post-hoc comparison: p < 0.001), see Fig. 1.
Sleep
Table 1 summarizes the statistical analyses of subjective sleep quality and objective sleep structure assessed during nighttime sleep. Analyses of subjective sleep quality assessed with the Leeds Sleep Evaluation Questionnaire (LSEQ) did not reveal significant differences among the three conditions in any of the four domains of sleep quality (pall > 0.05).
Table 1.
Parameter | Placebo | Caffeine | Withdrawal | Factor condition |
---|---|---|---|---|
Subjective (LSEQ) | ||||
Getting to sleep | 47.00 ± 3.20 | 51.39 ± 3.95 | 57.02 ± 4.82 | F2,37.6 = 2.30, p = 0.11 |
Quality of sleep | 42.25 ± 2.72 | 42.71 ± 3.55 | 45.05 ± 4.42 | F2,37.5 = 0.22, p = 0.81 |
Awake following sleep | 50.18 ± 3.10 | 44.53 ± 3.43 | 45.03 ± 4.10 | F2,36.7 = 0.99, p = 0.38 |
Behavior following wakening | 47.98 ± 3.57 | 42.37 ± 4.25 | 39.63 ± 4.44 | F2,37.7 = 1.32, p = 0.28 |
Objective (PSG) | ||||
TST (min) | 440.88 ± 8.16 | 450.97 ± 5.13 | 447.59 ± 10.13 | F2,31.3 = 0.83, p = 0.45 |
SE (%) | 91.85 ± 1.70 | 93.94 ± 1.08 | 93.31 ± 2.11 | F2,31.2 = 0.86, p = 0.43 |
SL1 (min) | 13.18 ± 3.22 | 10.33 ± 1.39 | 8.67 ± 1.88 | F2,31.8 = 1.47, p = 0.25 |
SL2 (min) | 16.56 ± 3.47 | 16.67 ± 2.08 | 16.82 ± 4.53 | F2,31.9 = 0.02, p = 0.98 |
RL (min) | 76.65 ± 10.85 | 85.21 ± 18.86 | 86.56 ± 13.86 | F2,28.5 = 0.33, p = 0.72 |
Stage 1 (%) | 9.62 ± 1.36 | 7.79 ± 0.89 | 8.75 ± 1.12 | F2,31.6 = 1.19, p = 0.32 |
Stage 2 (%) | 48.90 ± 1.24 | 49.70 ± 2.09 | 48.08 ± 1.57 | F2,29.0 = 0.42, p = 0.66 |
Stage 3 (%) | 9.38 ± 0.70 | 10.15 ± 1.20 | 10.98 ± 1.00 | F2,29.8 = 1.03, p = 0.37 |
Stage 4 (%) | 7.11 ± 1.58 | 5.24 ± 1.64 | 6.85 ± 1.62 | F2,28.1 = 0.53, p = 0.59 |
SWS (%) | 16.49 ± 1.73 | 15.39 ± 2.13 | 17.83 ± 1.75 | F2,28.3 = 0.62, p = 0.55 |
NREM (%) | 65.39 ± 1.19 | 65.09 ± 1.06 | 65.91 ± 1.34 | F2,28.9 = 0.14, p = 0.87 |
REM (%) | 24.99 ± 1.60 | 27.11 ± 1.12 | 25.34 ± 1.81 | F2,30.1 = 0.73, p = 0.49 |
WASO (%) | 6.49 ± 2.18 | 4.35 ± 1.26 | 6.49 ± 3.50 | F2,31.4 = 0.49, p = 0.62 |
LSEQ Leeds Sleep Evaluation Questionnaire; PSG polysomnography. Values represent means ± standard errors for each variable and condition. Lower values in the four scales of the subjective ratings indicate worse sleep quality. TST total sleep time (stages 1–4 + REM sleep); SE sleep efficiency (TST/time in bed × 100); SL1: sleep latency to stage 1, i.e. time from lights off to first occurrence of stage 1; SL2: sleep latency to stage 2, i.e. time from lights off to first occurrence of stage 2; RL REM sleep latency, i.e. time from sleep onset to first occurrence of REM sleep; SWS slow-wave sleep (stages 3–4); NREM non-rapid eye movement sleep (stages 2–4); REM rapid eye movement sleep; WASO wakefulness after sleep onset.
In line with these results, the analyses of the polysomnography (PSG) did not reveal significant differences in total sleep time (TST), sleep efficiency (SE), sleep latencies, or the relative amount of sleep stages among the three conditions (pall > 0.05).
In a next step, we analyzed all-night EEG power density in the range of 0.75–32 Hz over the central derivations recorded during non-rapid eye movement (NREM) sleep. In contrast to our assumptions, we did not find any significant differences among the three conditions in the lower frequency bins (0.75–13.25 Hz; pall > 0.05). However, power density was significantly reduced compared to placebo in the sigma range during both withdrawal (frequency bins 13.5–17.25 Hz and 18–18.5 Hz; pall < 0.05) and caffeine (frequency bins 13.5–16 Hz; pall < 0.05).
In a second step, we were interested in the temporal dynamics of both SWA and sigma activity across the night assessed during NREM sleep. As depicted in Fig. 2 (top panel), SWA showed a typical temporal pattern with increased activity during the first NREM cycle followed by a steady decline across the night (main effect of time: F39,613 = 26.28, p < 0.001). However, differences among the three conditions did not reach significance (main effect of condition: F2,178 = 1.33, p = 0.27). Also, the interaction of condition and time was not significant (F78,1060 = 0.89, p = 0.74).
As illustrated in Fig. 2 (bottom panel), sigma activity was significantly reduced in both the caffeine and withdrawal conditions compared to placebo intake (main effect of condition: F2,209 = 19.96, p < 0.001; post-hoc comparisons: p < 0.001) and the interaction of condition and time tended to be significant (F78,1049 = 1.25, p = 0.08).
Taken together, we could not confirm our assumption of a caffeine-induced reduction of sleep depth, neither in terms of shorter SWS nor in terms of reduced SWA in the caffeine compared to the placebo condition. Based on the discrepancies between the present results and a previous study about the effects of chronic caffeine intake on sleep12, we thus explored whether differences in the individual levels of caffeine before sleep could explain the variance within SWS and SWA. However, no significant effects were observed when controlling for dependent observations within subjects (p > 0.05).
Subjective caffeine withdrawal symptoms
Analyses of the relative withdrawal symptoms yielded a significant main effect of condition (F2,20.2 = 11.30, p < 0.01) indicating more withdrawal symptoms during the withdrawal compared to the caffeine condition (post-hoc comparison: p < 0.01), depicted in Fig. 3. This effect was modulated by time (interaction of condition × time: F2,37.2 = 3.43, p = 0.04), such that the increase in symptoms during the withdrawal compared to caffeine condition was particularly present during the last measurement (p < 0.01), i.e. 31 h after the last caffeine intake in the withdrawal condition.
Discussion
The aim of the present study was to investigate the influence of daily daytime caffeine intake and its cessation on nighttime sleep in habitual caffeine consumers under strictly controlled laboratory conditions. Strikingly, caffeine consumption did not lead to clear-cut changes in nighttime sleep structure nor in subjective sleep quality when assessed 8 and 15 h after the last intake in the caffeine and withdrawal condition, respectively. The evolution of subjective withdrawal symptoms indicates that withdrawal becomes perceivable at earliest between 27–31 h after intake. However, compared to placebo, EEG power density was reduced in the sigma range during both caffeine and withdrawal conditions. We conclude that daily daytime intake of caffeine does not strongly influence nighttime sleep structure nor subjective sleep quality in healthy men when consumed in the morning, midday, and in the afternoon. In contrast to the reported increases in sigma activity after acute caffeine intake10, the observed changes in the sigma frequencies might point to early signs of caffeine withdrawal which occur due to overnight abstinence and presumably derive from preceding caffeine-induced changes in adenosine signaling.
To quantify the influence of caffeine on sleep, the stimulant is commonly administered close to the onset of a sleep episode10–14, for instance within 1 h prior to bedtime10,11,13,14. Taking into account that caffeine plasma levels peak within 30–75 min following caffeine ingestion19, consumption within 1 h prior to sleep allows the stimulant to exert its maximum effects at sleep commencement. Indeed, the sleep disrupting effects of caffeine are frequently reported to affect sleep initiation or the first half of the sleep episode10–14. Moreover, sleep intensity, which is usually strongest at the beginning of the night20, was particularly disrupted during the first sleep cycle, as indexed in reduced SWS and SWA10. However, caffeine intake in the evening, particularly after 9 pm is rare5, presumably to avoid impairment of subsequent sleep3. Up to date it remained fairly unclear whether caffeine intake in the morning and afternoon still bears the potential to disrupt nighttime sleep. While we observed a delay of 25 min in sleep episodes during caffeine intake prior to the laboratory part, PSG-derived data after 9 days of regular caffeine intake did not yield a significant change in sleep architecture. Thus, our data provide first evidence that daily daytime caffeine intake does not necessarily alter subsequent sleep structure and SWA when consumed > 8 h prior to sleep. Importantly, our findings do not preclude potential impairments of nighttime sleep after morning caffeine intake, if preceded by several days of abstinence from the stimulant21. It rather appears likely that the duration of preceding caffeine consumption drives the discrepancies between acute and chronic effects of caffeine on sleep.
Chronic caffeine intake induces some tolerance development in both physiological measures such as cortisol22, blood pressure23, heart rate24, and also subjective measures such as alertness18. Over time, the stimulatory effects of the substance vanish potentially due to changes in adenosine levels25 and/or adenosine receptors26–28. Accordingly, a 1-week treatment of caffeine reduced the sleep disrupting effects, even under conditions of high evening dosages12. Thus, the available evidence and the absence of clear-cut changes in the present study point to adaptive processes in sleep initiation, sleep structure, and subjective sleep quality due to the long-term exposure to the stimulant.
However, chronic caffeine consumption bears the risk of withdrawal symptoms when abruptly ceased. These symptoms have been reported to occur as early as 6 h but with peak intensity being reached within 20–51 h after last caffeine intake17. While 25 h of caffeine abstinence might not affect nighttime sleep structure12, 32 h of abstinence improved subjective sleep quality18. Thus, scheduling the start of the sleep episode to 15 h after the last caffeine intake, as in our withdrawal condition, was probably too early to detect changes in sleep structure or subjective sleep quality. In line with this assumption, volunteers subjectively indicated withdrawal symptoms 31 h after caffeine abstinence in the withdrawal condition compared to caffeine. Thus, our findings support the notion that the alterations in sleep structure and subjective sleep quality induced by caffeine abstinence potentially develop at a later stage (> 27 h) of caffeine withdrawal.
Most strikingly and unexpectedly, a reduction in NREM sigma activity during both the withdrawal and caffeine conditions was observed, a phenomenon which is commonly reported under conditions of enhanced sleep pressure29–32. Thus, it seems at first glance in contrast to the reported increases in this frequency range10,21 and the well-known alerting effects after acute caffeine intake18. However, during conditions of chronic caffeine intake, mice showed a deeper sleep compared to placebo33. Moreover, repeated caffeine intake enhances the sensitivity of adenosine binding34 presumably due to upregulated adenosine receptors26–28 or changes in the functions of adenosine receptor heteromers35. These neuronal alterations in the adenosinergic system might drive the commonly observed changes in the homeostatic sleep-wake regulation such as increased sleepiness when caffeine intake is suddenly ceased17. As reported previously, we also observed in the present study higher subjective sleepiness following caffeine withdrawal when compared to the placebo and caffeine conditions36. Thus, the reduction in sigma activity might reflect adenosinergic changes which already emerge 8 and 15 h after the last caffeine intake in the caffeine and withdrawal condition, respectively. This reduction might reflect withdrawal symptoms which chronic consumers reverse daily by the first caffeine dose. Given the high prevalence of daily caffeine consumers in the society, these findings stress the importance to carefully control for prior caffeine intake when assessing sleep in order to exclude potential confounding by induced withdrawal symptoms which are only detectable in the microstructure of sleep.
Our study has some limitations which must be taken into careful consideration when interpreting the present findings. First, age moderates the effects of caffeine on sleep11,14. Thus, the present results cannot be generalized to other age groups such as to middle-aged consumers which are more vulnerable to the caffeine-induced effects on sleep11,14. Second, only a limited number of participants were studied. However, a well-controlled study design was employed and power calculation on the basis of an earlier study12 indicated a sufficient sample size. Third, we do not have any information about the participants’ genetic polymorphisms which have been shown to modulate the metabolism of caffeine37. In addition, a genetic variation of the ADORA2A genotype has been linked with caffeine sensitivity to the effects on sleep38. Thus, carriers of this genetic variance are more likely to curtail caffeine consumption and are consequently excluded from the present study leading to a selection bias. However, the focus of the present study was to investigate habitual caffeine consumers as they represent the majority of the worldwide population2. Fourth, to reduce variance in the data incurred by the influence of the menstrual cycle on sleep39 and the interaction between caffeine metabolism and the use of oral contraceptives40,41, only male volunteers were included which clearly reduces the generalizability of the findings.
In conclusion, we report evidence that daily daytime intake of caffeine and its cessation has no strong effect on sleep structure or subjective sleep quality. However, the quantitative EEG analyses revealed reduced activity in the sigma range during both caffeine and withdrawal. These subtle alterations point to early signs of caffeine withdrawal in the homeostatic aspect of sleep-wake regulation which are already present as early as 8 h after the last caffeine intake. Thus, habitual caffeine consumers constantly expose themselves to a continuous change between presence and absence of the stimulant. Around the clock, their organisms dynamically adapt and react to daily presence and nightly abstinence.
Methods
Participants
Twenty male volunteers were recruited into the present study through online advertisements and flyers distributed in public areas. Interested individuals aged between 18 and 35 years old (mean age ± SD: 26.4 ± 4 years) and reporting a daily caffeine consumption between 300 and 600 mg (mean intake ± SD: 478.1 ± 102.8 mg) were included. The self-rating assessment for the daily amount of caffeine intake was structured based on Bühler et al.42, and the amount of caffeine content was defined according to Snel and Lorist3. To ensure good health, volunteers were screened by self-report questionnaires and a medical examination conducted by a physician. Additionally, all volunteers reported good sleep quality assessed with the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI; score ≤ 5)43 and showed no signs of sleep disturbances (SE > 70%, periodic leg movements < 15/h, apnea index < 10) in a PSG recorded during an adaptation night in the laboratory scheduled prior to the start of the study. To control for circadian misalignment, volunteers who reported shiftwork within 3 months and transmeridian travels (crossing > 2 time zones) within 1 month prior to study admission were excluded. Further exclusion criteria comprised body mass index (BMI) < 18 or > 26, smoking, drug use, and extreme chronotype assessed by the Morningness-Eveningness Questionnaire (MEQ; score ≤ 30 and ≥ 70)44. To reduce variance in the data incurred by the effect of menstrual cycle on sleep39 and the interaction between caffeine metabolism and the use of oral contraceptives40,41, only male volunteers were studied. A detailed description of the study sample can be found in Weibel et al.36.
All volunteers signed a written informed consent and received financial compensation for study participation. The study was approved by the local Ethics Committee (EKNZ) and conducted according to the Declaration of Helsinki.
Design and protocol
We employed a double-blind, randomized, crossover study including a caffeine, a withdrawal, and a placebo condition. Volunteers were allocated to the order of the three conditions based on pseudo-randomization, for more details see Weibel et al.36. As illustrated in Fig. 4, each condition started with an ambulatory part of 9 days, followed by a laboratory part of 43 h. In each condition, participants took either caffeine (150 mg) or placebo (mannitol) in identical appearing gelatin capsules (Hänseler AG, Herisau, Switzerland) three times daily, scheduled at 45 min, 255 min, and 475 min after awakening, for a duration of 10 days. This regimen was applied based on a previous study investigating tolerance to the effects of caffeine and caffeine cessation18. To enhance caffeine withdrawal in the withdrawal condition, treatment was abruptly switched from caffeine to placebo on day nine of the protocol (255 min after wake-up, 15 h before sleep recording).
During the 9 days of the ambulatory part, volunteers were asked to maintain a regular sleep-wake rhythm (± 30 min of self-selected bedtime/wake-up time, 8 h in bed, no daytime napping), verified by wrist actimetry (Actiwatch, Cambridge Neurotechnology Ltd., Cambridge, United Kingdom), and to keep subjective sleep logs. While the participants were compliant, they scheduled sleep episodes differently within the accepted range of ± 30 min. During intake of caffeine (i.e. caffeine and withdrawal condition), the ambulatory sleep episodes were on average around 25 min later as compared to placebo (results see supplements). The duration of the ambulatory part was set for 9 days based on the maximum duration of withdrawal symptoms17 and thus, to avoid carry-over effects from the previous condition. Furthermore, volunteers were requested to refrain from caffeinated beverages and food (e.g. coffee, tea, soda drinks, and chocolate), alcohol, nicotine, and medications. Caffeine abstinence and compliance to the treatment requirements were checked by caffeine levels from the daily collection of fingertip sweat of which results are reported in the supplemental material of Weibel et al.36 and which indicate very good adherence to the treatments.
On day nine, volunteers admitted to the laboratory at 5.5 h prior to habitual sleep time. Upon arrival, a urinary drug screen (AccuBioTech Co., Ltd., Beijing, China) was performed to ensure drug abstinence. Electrodes for the PSG were fitted and salivary caffeine levels collected. An 8-h nighttime sleep episode was scheduled at volunteers’ habitual bedtime starting 8 and 15 h after the last caffeine intake in the caffeine and withdrawal condition, respectively. The next day, volunteers rated their subjective sleep quality by the LSEQ45 and potential withdrawal symptoms by the Caffeine Withdrawal Symptom Questionnaire (CWSQ)46.
To reduce potential masking effects on our outcome variables, we standardized food intake, light exposure, and posture changes throughout the laboratory part. Accordingly, volunteers were housed in single apartments under dim-light (< 8 lx) during scheduled wakefulness and 0 lx during sleep. Volunteers were asked to maintain a semi-recumbent position during wakefulness, except for restroom breaks. In addition, volunteers received standardized meals in regular intervals. Social interactions were restricted to team members and no time-of-day cues were provided throughout the in-lab protocol.
Salivary caffeine
To characterize individual caffeine levels during nighttime sleep, we report salivary caffeine levels assessed 3 h prior to the scheduled sleep episode and 5 min after wake-up. Samples were stored at 5 °C following collection, later centrifuged (3000 rpm for 10 min) and subsequently kept at − 28 °C until analyses. Liquid chromatography coupled to tandem mass spectrometry was used to analyze the levels of caffeine. One dataset in the withdrawal condition was lost.
Subjective sleep quality
Subjective sleep quality was assessed 10 min upon scheduled wake-up time with a paper and pencil version of the LSEQ45. Volunteers were asked to rate 10 items on visual analogue scales which are grouped into four domains (getting to sleep (GTS), quality of sleep (QOS), awake following sleep (AFS), and behavior following wakening (BFW)).
Polysomnographic recordings
PSG was continuously recorded during 8 h of nighttime sleep using the portable V-Amp device (Brain Products GmbH, Gilching, Germany). Grass gold cup electrodes were applied according to the international 10–20 system including two electrooculographic, two electromyographic, two electrocardiographic, and six electroencephalographic derivations (F3, F4, C3, C4, O1, O2). Channels were referenced online against the linked mastoids (A1, A2). Signals were recorded with a sampling rate of 500 Hz and a notch filter was online applied at 50 Hz.
Each epoch of 30 s of the recorded PSG data was visually scored according to standard criteria47 by three trained team members blind to the condition. SWS was additionally classified into stage 3 and 4 based on Rechtschaffen and Kales48. The scoring agreement between the three scorers was regularly confirmed to reach > 85%.
TST was defined as the sum of the time spent in sleep stages 1–4 and rapid eye movement (REM) sleep. Sleep latency to stage 1 and 2 was calculated as minutes to the first occurrence of the corresponding sleep stage following lights off. REM sleep latency was calculated as minutes to the first occurrence of REM sleep following sleep onset. NREM sleep was calculated as sum of sleep stages 2, 3 and 4. All sleep stages are expressed as relative values (%) of TST.
Spectral analysis was performed by applying fast Fourier transformation (FFT; hamming, 0% overlapped, 0.25 Hz bins) on 4-s time windows. Artifacts were manually removed based on visual inspection, and data were log-transformed prior to spectral analyses. All-night EEG power density during NREM sleep was analyzed for each 0.25 Hz frequency bin in the range of 0.75–32 Hz recorded over the central derivations (C3, C4). SWA was defined as EEG power density between 0.75–4.5 Hz and sigma activity between 12–16 Hz. Sleep cycles were defined based on adapted rules developed by Feinberg and Floyd49 and divided into 10 NREM and four REM sleep intervals within each cycle. Ten nights were excluded from sleep analyses due to technical problems (placebo: n = 3; caffeine: n = 4; withdrawal: n = 3).
Caffeine withdrawal symptoms
Withdrawal symptoms were first assessed 35 min after wake-up and subsequently prior to each treatment administration with the self-rating CWSQ46. Twenty-three items are grouped into seven factors (fatigue/drowsiness, low alertness/difficulty concentrating, mood disturbances, low sociability/motivation to work, nausea/upset stomach, flu-like feelings, headache) and were rated on a 5 point scale by choosing between 1 (not at all) and 5 (extremely). Prior to analyses, eight items have been reversed scored as they were positively worded (e.g. alert or talkative) in the questionnaire. To assess caffeine withdrawal, we first calculated a sum score comprising all 23 items of the caffeine withdrawal questionnaire. Missing responses to single items were replaced by the median response of each condition over all volunteers in the respective time of assessment. In a next step, we calculated relative withdrawal symptoms in the caffeine and withdrawal condition (i.e. the difference of the withdrawal score in the caffeine and withdrawal condition respectively minus the score of the placebo condition). The data of one volunteer was lost due to technical difficulties.
Statistical analyses
Analyses were performed with the statistical package SAS (version 9.4, SAS Institute, Cary, NC, USA) by applying mixed model analyses of variance for repeated measures (PROC MIXED) with the repeated factors ‘condition’ (placebo, caffeine, withdrawal) and/or ‘time’ (levels differ per variable) and the random factor ‘subject’. The LSMEANS statement was used to calculate contrasts and degrees of freedom were based on the approximation by Kenward and Roger50. Post-hoc comparisons were adjusted for multiple comparisons by applying the Tukey-Kramer method. A statistical significance was defined as p < 0.05. One dataset has been excluded from all the analyses due to non-compliance with the treatment requirements (caffeine: n = 1).
Supplementary Information
Acknowledgements
The present work was performed within the framework of a project granted by the Swiss National Science Foundation (320030_163058) and was additionally funded by the Nikolaus und Bertha Burckhardt-Bürgin-Stiftung and the Janggen-Pöhn-Stiftung. Further, we thank our interns Andrea Schumacher, Laura Tincknell, Sven Leach, and all our study helpers for their help in data acquisition and all our volunteers for participating in the study. Moreover, we gratefully acknowledge the help in study organization provided by Dr. Ruta Lasauskaite and the medical screenings conducted by Dr. med. Martin Meyer and Dr. med. Corrado Garbazza.
Author contributions
C.R., C.C. and S.B. designed the study; J.W., Y.S.L. and HS collected the data; J.W., C.R. and C.C. analyzed and interpreted the data; J.W. and C.R. drafted the manuscript; C.C., Y.S.L. and H.P.L. critically revised the manuscript regarding its intellectual content; J.K., S.R. and K.R. provided the resources for the caffeine measurements and performed its analyses; all authors reviewed the present article.
Data availability
The present data are available upon request from the corresponding author.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher's note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
These authors contributed equally: Christian Cajochen and Carolin F. Reichert.
Supplementary Information
The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1038/s41598-021-84088-x.
References
- 1.Fredholm BB, Bättig K, Holmén J, Nehlig A, Zvartau EE. Actions of caffeine in the brain with special reference to factors that contribute to its widespread use. Pharmacol. Rev. 1999;51:83–133. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Heckman MA, Weil J, GonzalezdeMejia E. Caffeine (1,3,7-trimethylxanthine) in foods: a comprehensive review on consumption, functionality, safety, and regulatory matters. J. Food Sci. 2010;75:R77–R87. doi: 10.1111/j.1750-3841.2010.01561.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Snel J, Lorist MM. Effects of caffeine on sleep and cognition. Prog. Brain Res. 2011;190:105–117. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-444-53817-8.00006-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Martyn D, Lau A, Richardson P, Roberts A. Temporal patterns of caffeine intake in the United States. Food Chem. Toxicol. 2018;111:71–83. doi: 10.1016/j.fct.2017.10.059. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Lieberman HR, Agarwal S, Fulgoni VL., 3rd Daily patterns of caffeine intake and the association of intake with multiple sociodemographic and lifestyle factors in US adults based on the NHANES 2007–2012 surveys. J. Acad. Nutr. Diet. 2019;119:106–114. doi: 10.1016/j.jand.2018.08.152. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Borbély AA. A two process model of sleep regulation. Hum. Neurobiol. 1982;1:195–204. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Porkka-Heiskanen T. Sleep homeostasis. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2013;23:799–805. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2013.02.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Landolt HP. Sleep homeostasis: a role for adenosine in humans? Biochem. Pharmacol. 2008;75:2070–2079. doi: 10.1016/j.bcp.2008.02.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Clark I, Landolt HP. Coffee, caffeine, and sleep: A systematic review of epidemiological studies and randomized controlled trials. Sleep Med. Rev. 2017;31:70–78. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2016.01.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Landolt HP, Dijk DJ, Gaus SE, Borbély AA. Caffeine reduces low-frequency delta-activity in the human sleep EEG. Neuropsychopharmacology. 1995;12:229–238. doi: 10.1016/0893-133x(94)00079-F. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Drapeau C, et al. Challenging sleep in aging: the effects of 200 mg of caffeine during the evening in young and middle-aged moderate caffeine consumers. J. Sleep Res. 2006;15:133–141. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2006.00518.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Bonnet MH, Arand DL. Caffeine use as a model of acute and chronic insomnia. Sleep. 1992;15:526–536. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Carrier J, et al. Effects of caffeine are more marked on daytime recovery sleep than on nocturnal sleep. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2007;32:964–972. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301198. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Robillard R, Bouchard M, Cartier A, Nicolau L, Carrier J. Sleep is more sensitive to high doses of caffeine in the middle years of life. J. Psychopharmacol. 2015;29:688–697. doi: 10.1177/0269881115575535. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Drake C, Roehrs T, Shambroom J, Roth T. Caffeine effects on sleep taken 0, 3, or 6 hours before going to bed. J. Clin. Sleep Med. 2013;9:1195–1200. doi: 10.5664/jcsm.3170. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.James JE, Rogers PJ. Effects of caffeine on performance and mood: withdrawal reversal is the most plausible explanation. Psychopharmacology. 2005;182:1–8. doi: 10.1007/s00213-005-0084-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Juliano LM, Griffiths RR. A critical review of caffeine withdrawal: empirical validation of symptoms and signs, incidence, severity, and associated features. Psychopharmacology. 2004;176:1–29. doi: 10.1007/s00213-004-2000-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.James JE. Acute and chronic effects of caffeine on performance, mood, headache, and sleep. Neuropsychobiology. 1998;38:32–41. doi: 10.1159/000026514. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Mandel HG. Update on caffeine consumption, disposition and action. Food Chem. Toxicol. 2002;40:1231–1234. doi: 10.1016/s0278-6915(02)00093-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Dijk DJ, Czeisler CA. Contribution of the circadian pacemaker and the sleep homeostat to sleep propensity, sleep structure, electroencephalographic slow waves, and sleep spindle activity in humans. J. Neurosci. 1995;15:3526–3538. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.15-05-03526.1995. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Landolt HP, Werth E, Borbély AA, Dijk DJ. Caffeine intake (200 mg) in the morning affects human sleep and EEG power spectra at night. Brain Res. 1995;675:67–74. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(95)00040-W. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Lovallo WR, et al. Caffeine stimulation of cortisol secretion across the waking hours in relation to caffeine intake levels. Psychosom. Med. 2005;67:734–739. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000181270.20036.06. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Lovallo WR, et al. Blood pressure response to caffeine shows incomplete tolerance after short-term regular consumption. Hypertension. 2004;43:760–765. doi: 10.1161/01.Hyp.0000120965.63962.93. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Denaro CP, Brown CR, Jacob P, 3rd, Benowitz NL. Effects of caffeine with repeated dosing. Eur. J. Clin. Pharmacol. 1991;40:273–278. doi: 10.1007/bf00315208. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Conlay LA, Conant JA, deBros F, Wurtman R. Caffeine alters plasma adenosine levels. Nature. 1997;389:136. doi: 10.1038/38160. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Johansson B, Georgiev V, Lindström K, Fredholm BB. A1 and A2A adenosine receptors and A1 mRNA in mouse brain: effect of long-term caffeine treatment. Brain Res. 1997;762:153–164. doi: 10.1016/S0006-8993(97)00378-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Fredholm BB. Adenosine actions and adenosine receptors after 1 week treatment with caffeine. Acta Physiol. Scand. 1982;115:283–286. doi: 10.1111/j.1748-1716.1982.tb07078.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Shi D, Nikodijevic O, Jacobson KA, Daly JW. Chronic caffeine alters the density of adenosine, adrenergic, cholinergic, GABA, and serotonin receptors and calcium channels in mouse brain. Cell Mol. Neurobiol. 1993;13:247–261. doi: 10.1007/BF00733753. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Borbély AA, Baumann F, Brandeis D, Strauch I, Lehmann D. Sleep deprivation: effect on sleep stages and EEG power density in man. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 1981;51:483–495. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(81)90225-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Dijk DJ, Hayes B, Czeisler CA. Dynamics of electroencephalographic sleep spindles and slow-wave activity in men—effect of sleep-deprivation. Brain Res. 1993;626:190–199. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(93)90579-C. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Finelli LA, Borbély AA, Achermann P. Functional topography of the human nonREM sleep electroencephalogram. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2001;13:2282–2290. doi: 10.1046/j.0953-816x.2001.01597.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Knoblauch V, Martens WL, Wirz-Justice A, Cajochen C. Human sleep spindle characteristics after sleep deprivation. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2003;114:2258–2267. doi: 10.1016/s1388-2457(03)00238-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Panagiotou M, Meijer M, Meijer JH, Deboer T. Effects of chronic caffeine consumption on sleep and the sleep electroencephalogram in mice. J. Psychopharmacol. 2018 doi: 10.1177/0269881118806300. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Ferré S. An update on the mechanisms of the psychostimulant effects of caffeine. J. Neurochem. 2008;105:1067–1079. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-4159.2007.05196.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Ciruela F, et al. Presynaptic control of striatal glutamatergic neurotransmission by adenosine A1–A2A receptor heteromers. J. Neurosci. 2006;26:2080–2087. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3574-05.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Weibel J, et al. Caffeine-dependent changes of sleep-wake regulation: Evidence for adaptation after repeated intake. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry. 2019;99:109851. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2019.109851. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Nehlig A. Interindividual differences in caffeine metabolism and factors driving caffeine consumption. Pharmacol. Rev. 2018;70:384–411. doi: 10.1124/pr.117.014407. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Rétey JV, et al. A genetic variation in the adenosine A2A receptor gene (ADORA2A) contributes to individual sensitivity to caffeine effects on sleep. Clin. Pharmacol. Ther. 2007;81:692–698. doi: 10.1038/sj.clpt.6100102. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Shechter A, Boivin DB. Sleep, hormones, and circadian rhythms throughout the menstrual cycle in healthy women and women with premenstrual dysphoric disorder. Int. J. Endocrinol. 2010;2010:259345. doi: 10.1155/2010/259345. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Balogh A, et al. Influence of ethinylestradiol-containing combination oral contraceptives with gestodene or levonorgestrel on caffeine elimination. Eur. J. Clin. Pharmacol. 1995;48:161–166. doi: 10.1007/bf00192743. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Abernethy DR, Todd EL. Impairment of caffeine clearance by chronic use of low-dose oestrogen-containing oral contraceptives. Eur. J. Clin. Pharmacol. 1985;28:425–428. doi: 10.1007/bf00544361. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Bühler E, Lachenmeier DW, Schlegel K, Winkler G. Development of a tool to assess the caffeine intake among teenagers and young adults. Ernahrungs Umschau. 2013;61:58–63. [Google Scholar]
- 43.Buysse DJ, Reynolds CF, 3rd, Monk TH, Berman SR, Kupfer DJ. The Pittsburgh sleep quality index: a new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry Res. 1989;28:193–213. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(89)90047-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Horne JA, Ostberg O. A self-assessment questionnaire to determine morningness-eveningness in human circadian rhythms. Int. J. Chronobiol. 1976;4:97–110. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Parrott AC, Hindmarch I. Factor analysis of a sleep evaluation questionnaire. Psychol. Med. 1978;8:325–329. doi: 10.1017/S0033291700014379. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Juliano LM, Huntley ED, Harrell PT, Westerman AT. Development of the caffeine withdrawal symptom questionnaire: caffeine withdrawal symptoms cluster into 7 factors. Drug Alcohol. Depend. 2012;124:229–234. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2012.01.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Berry, R. B. et al. The AASM manual for the scoring of sleep and associated events: rules, terminology and technical specifications, version 2.0. American Academy of Sleep Medicine: Darien, IL (2012).
- 48.Rechtschaffen A, Kales A. A Manual of Standardized Terminology, Techniques and Scoring System for Sleep Stages of Human Subjects. Bethesda, MD: US Dept of Health, Education and Welfare, Public Health Service; 1968. [Google Scholar]
- 49.Feinberg I, Floyd TC. Systematic trends across the night in human sleep cycles. Psychophysiology. 1979;16:283–291. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1979.tb02991.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Kenward MG, Roger JH. Small sample inference for fixed effects from restricted maximum likelihood. Biometrics. 1997;53:983–997. doi: 10.2307/2533558. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The present data are available upon request from the corresponding author.