Abstract
Background:
Internet addiction and poor mental health are two pervasive problems during adolescence. This study aimed to determine whether Internet addiction and poor mental health status exhibited a bidirectional relationship in which either variable could become a risk factor for the onset of the other.
Methods:
Longitudinal school-based survey with a baseline sample of 1547 students among 8 schools (10th graders) in Japan surveyed in 2015 and followed up 1 year later. The schools are located in the middle city. We assessed internet addiction using the Japanese version of the Diagnostic Questionnaire developed by Young and mental health status using the 12-item General Health Questionnaire. Regression analyses including the covariates tested whether Internet addiction was related to the onset of poor mental health among youth who had never internet addiction, and poor mental health was related to the onset of internet addiction.
Results:
The incidence for Internet addiction and poor mental health during one year were 22.0% and 8.8%, respectively. Multiple regression analyses showed that poor mental health (adjusted odds ratio: 2.17 [95%CI: 1.45–3.25]) promoted new onset of Internet addiction and Internet addiction (adjusted odds ratio: 2.39 [95%CI: 1.36–4.20]) also promoted new onset of poor mental health.
Conclusion:
Internet addiction and poor mental health status each increased the risk of onset of the other. Adolescents, their parents and schools need to take policies to use properly Internet.
Keywords: Internet addiction, Adolescents, Japan, General health questionnaire, Cohort study
Introduction
In modern society, the Internet is an indispensable tool in daily life; however, several studies have reported negative effects of Internet addiction (IA) on schoolwork, the home environment, and physical and mental health (1–5). IA has become a serious public health issue observed in all age groups; however, its effects are particularly detrimental to young people, who are in the process of growing both mentally and socially (6). Epidemiological surveys examining IA have been conducted in various regions, and a meta-analysis showed that the worldwide IA prevalence was 6.0%, 95% CI [5.1–6.9]. Specifically, the highest (10.9%; 95% CI [5.4–16.3]) and lowest (2.6%; 95% CI [1.0–4.1]) prevalence were observed in the Middle East and Northern and Western Europe, respectively (7). In addition, the prevalence of IA observed in East Asian adolescents were 10.4% in China (8), 17.4% in Taiwan (9), from 17% to 26.8% in Hong Kong (10), and 10.7% in South Korea (4). Japanese adolescents, which used Young’s diagnostic questionnaire, reported that 6.4% of boys and 9.9% of girls exhibited suspected Internet dependence (11). Differences in prevalence are thought to result from differences in sample sizes, screening tests, and research designs. However, the overall results indicate that IA is common in adolescents.
Moreover, in a systematic review, IA risk factors were categorized according to individual psychological characteristics, parental and family issues, Internet usage status/individual awareness, and other elements such as schoolwork (12). Psychological characteristics included mental health conditions such as depression, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, social anxiety, and suicide attempts (4, 13). As most research regarding IA have involved cross-sectional studies, it has been difficult to ascertain causal relationships. Accordingly, longitudinal studies conducted to calculate incidence rates are of greater academic value relative to that of cross-sectional studies. Although longitudinal research examining IA in high school students is scarce, several studies have explored the mutual relationship between IA and poor mental health (14–17). For example, Cho et al. compared individuals with and without anxiety or depression at baseline and reported that the risk of IA in those with anxiety or depression was 1.07 times higher than that observed for those without either condition (16). In addition, the relative risk of depression in people who used the Internet pathologically was approximately 2.5 times higher than that observed for people not demonstrated pathological Internet use (17). These longitudinal studies demonstrated a bidirectional relationship between IA and mental health, in which either could be the cause or result of the other, in adolescents. This bidirectional relationship indicates that IA increases the incidence of poor mental health, and poor mental health increases the possibility of the onset of IA, creating a vicious cycle. Therefore, the establishment of an understanding of this relationship is extremely important. However, no studies have been conducted to examine the bidirectional relationship between IA and poor mental health to date.
Therefore, we used epidemiological tools that had demonstrated validity and reliability, to conduct a 1-year longitudinal survey examining IA in adolescents. The main purpose of the study was to determine whether IA and poor mental health status exhibited a bidirectional relationship in which either variable could become a risk factor for the onset of the other.
Methods
Participants and data collection
We invited students from 8 high schools in a middle city to participate in the study. The study sample included 1,547 students who entered high schools in 2015. Two surveys were conducted with the same population, using self-administered questionnaires: 1) a baseline survey and 2) a follow-up survey. The baseline survey was conducted from Apr to Jul of 2015, and the follow-up survey was conducted from Apr to Jul of 2016. We sent a letter to the principal of each school, with sufficient questionnaires and envelopes for all the students enrolled at the school. Class teachers at the schools that agreed to participate the survey distributed the questionnaires to the students. To protect the privacy of respondents and obtain candid responses, the questionnaire contained a statement confirming that completed questionnaires would not be seen by the teachers. Upon completion of the questionnaires, the students were asked to seal them in envelopes with adhesive flaps. Collection and delivery of the questionnaires were entrusted to the teachers, instructed to follow the guidelines for conducting the survey. The teachers collected the sealed envelopes and sent them back to the Department of Public Health and Epidemiology, Faculty of Medicine, Oita University without opening them. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants. The study was approved by the local ethics committee and registered at ClinicalTrials.gov (identifier: UMIN000027587).
Questionnaires
The main questions in the study pertained to the following: (i) sex, (ii) Internet usage, (iii) mental health status, (iv) lifestyle behaviors, and (v) sleep status. We used the Japanese version of the Diagnostic Questionnaire developed by Young to assess problematic Internet use (18). We followed Young’s proposal of dichotomized distinction of IA via a cutoff point of 5 (18). The reliability and validity of the scale have been examined (19). In this study, Cronbach’s alpha is from 0.710 to 0.726.
The Japanese version of the 12-item General Health Questionnaire was used to evaluate mental health status (20, 21). Participants with total scores of ≥4 were considered to have poor mental health; this cutoff point was deemed valid in previous studies (22, 23). The reliability and validity of the scale have ranged from 0.78 to 0.95 and from 0.83 to 0.95 (20, 24). In this study, Cronbach’s alpha is from 0.808 to 0.820. In addition, we asked about interpersonal relationships (i.e., problems with friends and whether the participants had people who understood them).
The lifestyle factors assessed included daily breakfast consumption, snacking, skipping meals, exercise habits, grooming and appearance, and study hours. Concerning skipping breakfast, those who did not eat breakfast every day were defined as having a “skipping breakfast habit.” Concerning snacking habits, those who ate more than 1 snack per day were defined as having a “snacking habit.” Regarding the habit of skipping meals, those who missed more than 1 meal per day were defined as having “a habit of skipping meals.” Concerning exercise habits, those who exercised on more than 10 d per month were defined as having an “exercise habit.” Concerning grooming and appearance, those who were often or always careful about their appearance were considered to care about their “grooming and appearance.” With respect to study hours, those who studied for more than 2 h per day were considered to “study for long hours.”
Sleep status was assessed in terms of sleep duration, subjective sleep quality, insomnia symptoms, and excessive daytime sleepiness. Subjective sleep quality was assessed using the question “How would you assess the quality of your sleep?” (Responses included “very good,” “good,” “bad,” and “very bad”); participants whose responses were “bad” or “very bad” were considered to have poor subjective sleep quality. Insomnia symptoms experienced during the preceding month were assessed using 3 questions: 1) “Do you have difficulty initiating sleep?” 2) “Do you have difficulty remaining asleep?” 3) “Do you experience early morning awakening?” The responses for each included the following: “never,” “seldom,” “sometimes,” “often,” and “always.” “Often” and “always” were considered affirmative responses to the item, and an affirmative response to any of the items indicated the presence of insomnia symptoms. Excessive daytime sleepiness was assessed using the Japanese version of the Epworth Sleepiness Scale (25). Responses are provided using a scale ranging from 1 to 4. The presence of excessive daytime sleepiness was determined using a cutoff point of 11.
Statistical analyses
First, the incidence of IA at the time of the follow-up survey was calculated using a chi-squared test. In this analysis, only participants who did not exhibit IA at baseline assessment were selected. Second, logistic regression analysis was used to identify the factors that predicted IA. We performed multiple logistic regression modeling with backward stepwise variable selection to identify factors associated with IA. Independent variables introduced gender, poor mental health, grooming appearance, skipping breakfast, snacking, skipping meals, insomnia, sleep quality, excessive daytime sleepiness, studying hour, and exercise habit n the baseline survey. Variables with P-values of >0.10 were eliminated from the model. Only participants who did not exhibit IA at baseline assessment were included in this analysis selected. Third, the incidence of poor mental health in the follow-up survey was calculated using a chi-squared test. Only participants who did not exhibit mental health in the baseline assessment were included in this analysis. Forth, logistic regression analysis was performed to identify the factors that predicted poor mental health. Only participants who did not display poor mental health in the baseline assessment were included in this analysis. We used multiple logistic regression modeling with backward stepwise variable selection to identify factors associated with poor mental health. Variables with Pvalues of >0.10 were eliminated from the model. Independent variables introduced gender, internet addiction, grooming appearance, skipping breakfast, snacking, skipping meals, insomnia, sleep quality, excessive daytime sleepiness, studying hour, and exercise habit in the baseline survey. The levels of statistical significance were set at P<0.05. The SPSS 17.0 software package (Chicago, IL, USA) was used for all analyses.
Results
Overall, 1,522 of the 1,547 students invited to participate in the baseline survey agreed to do so. The number of students whose baseline and follow-up survey data could be linked to the names provided in the questionnaires was 1,440. Of these students, 1,434 agreed to participate in the follow-up survey. Data for 92.7% of the participants were ultimately analyzed.
Table 1 shows the participants’ baseline characteristics. The proportion of girls who participated in the study (54%) was higher relative to that of boys (46%). The prevalence of IA was 12.3%. The prevalence of poor mental health was 41.7%.
Table 1:
Variable | N | % | |
---|---|---|---|
Gender | Boys | 711 | 46.0 |
Internet addiction | 190 | 12.3 | |
Poor Mental health status | 645 | 41.7 | |
Insomnia | 147 | 9.5 | |
Poor Sleep quality | 204 | 13.2 | |
Excessive daytime sleepiness | 692 | 44.7 | |
Grooming appearance | 1121 | 72.5 | |
Skipping breakfast | 142 | 9.2 | |
Snacking habit | 587 | 37.9 | |
Habit of skipping meals | 132 | 8.5 | |
Study 2 hours over/everyday | 453 | 29.3 | |
Exercise habit | 885 | 57.2 |
Table 2 shows the incidence of IA in the follow-up survey according to participants’ characteristics. The new IA incidence rate for all participants was 8.8%. The IA incidence rate increased significantly according to mental health status, gender, and snacking habits. In addition, the incidence rate observed in participants with poor mental health was significantly higher relative to that of participants with good mental health.
Table 2:
Variable | N | Total | Incidence rate (%) | P-value | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Total | 108 | 1229 | 8.8 | ||
Mental health | Good | 49 | 775 | 6.3 | <0.001 |
Poor | 59 | 454 | 13.0 | ||
Gender | Boy | 38 | 580 | 6.6 | 0.009 |
Girl | 70 | 649 | 10.8 | ||
Insomnia | No | 99 | 1126 | 8.8 | 0.985 |
Yes | 9 | 103 | 8.7 | ||
Sleep quality | Good | 89 | 1084 | 8.2 | 0.071 |
Poor | 18 | 141 | 12.8 | ||
Grooming appearance | No | 35 | 314 | 11.1 | 0.089 |
Yes | 73 | 913 | 8.0 | ||
Snacking habits | Yes | 52 | 480 | 10.8 | 0.043 |
No | 56 | 748 | 7.5 | ||
Habit of skipping meals | Yes | 13 | 96 | 13.5 | 0.088 |
No | 95 | 1130 | 8.4 | ||
Exercise habit | No | 54 | 507 | 10.7 | 0.053 |
Yes | 54 | 722 | 7.5 |
P-values were calculated with the chi-squared test.
In each section, missing data have been excluded from the statistical analyses.
Table 3 shows the predictors of new-onset IA. Poor mental health (adjusted odds ratio: 2.17 [95%CI: 1.45–3.25]) was identified as factor that promoted new incidence of IA. Table 4 shows the incidence of poor mental health at baseline and follow-up assessment. The incidence rate for poor mental health for all participants was 22.0%, and that observed for participants who exhibited IA and snacking habits were significantly higher relative to that of the reference group.
Table 3:
Variable | N | AOR | 95%CI | P-value | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Gender | Boy | 567 | 1.00 | 0.011 | |||
Girl | 641 | 1.73 | 1.13 | − | 2.65 | ||
Poor mental health | No | 756 | 1.00 | < 0.001 | |||
Yes | 452 | 2.17 | 1.45 | − | 3.25 |
AOR: adjusted odds ratio, CI: confidence interval. Participants with missing data were excluded from the analysis.
Logistic regression analyses were conducted on participants who did not have internet addiction at the baseline survey. Adjusted for gender, mental health status, snacking habit, and grooming appearance by multiple logistic regression using stepwise method.
Table 4:
Variable | N | Total | Incidence (%) | P-value | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Total | 198 | 900 | 22.0 | ||
Internet addiction | No | 164 | 787 | 20.8 | <0.001 |
Yes | 23 | 56 | 41.1 | ||
Gender | Boy | 91 | 449 | 20.3 | 0.211 |
Girl | 107 | 451 | 23.7 | ||
Insomnia | No | 182 | 850 | 21.4 | 0.079 |
Yes | 16 | 50 | 32.0 | ||
Sleep quality | Good | 179 | 834 | 21.5 | 0.165 |
Poor | 18 | 62 | 29.0 | ||
Grooming appearance | No | 46 | 236 | 19.5 | 0.270 |
Yes | 152 | 662 | 23.0 | ||
Snacking habits | No | 58 | 356 | 16.3 | 0.001 |
Yes | 140 | 543 | 25.8 | ||
Habit of skipping meals | No | 16 | 62 | 25.8 | 0.463 |
Yes | 182 | 835 | 21.8 | ||
Exercise habit | No | 90 | 370 | 24.3 | 0.160 |
Yes | 108 | 530 | 20.4 |
P-values were calculated with the chi-squared test.
In each section, missing data have been excluded from the statistical analyses
Table 5 shows the predictors of new-onset of poor mental health. IA (adjusted odds ratio: 2.39 [95%CI: 1.36–4.20]) was identified as factor that promoted new onset of poor mental health.
Table 5:
Variable | N | AOR | 95%CI | P-value | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Internet addiction | No | 768 | 1.00 | 0.003 | |||
Yes | 56 | 2.39 | 1.36 | − | 4.20 | ||
Insomnia | No | 779 | 1.00 | 0.039 | |||
Yes | 45 | 1.97 | 1.03 | − | 3.75 |
Participants with missing data were excluded from the analysis. Logistic regression analyses were conducted on participants who did not have poor mental health at the baseline survey. Adjusted for internet addiction, insomnia, and snacking habit by multiple logistic regression.
Discussion
This was the first longitudinal study to examine IA in high school students in Japan. The response rate for the survey was unusually high. In addition, the results demonstrated a relationship between the IA and poor mental health. Therefore, the findings could serve as an important resource in the development of future IA prevention strategies.
Moreover, participants with poor mental health at baseline assessment developed IA easily. Regarding psychological factors, a high correlation between Internet dependence and mental health problems, such as depression, has been reported in numerous cross-sectional (3, 5, 26–28) and longitudinal studies (14–16, 29, 30). Therefore, anxiety disorders and depression are risk factors for the future onset of IA. Conversely, IA is known to increase the risk of depression. For example, relative to those without IA, participants with IA were approximately 2.5 times more likely to develop depression (17). Many studies have reported IA in East Asia; in addition, in the European Union, previous IA and emotional problems were important predictors of IA 2 years later, and both factors were associated with the development of IA over the long term (31). IA and mental health problems could occur in any country. Compensatory Internet use to escape from psychosocial problems explained Internet dependence (32). In addition, a survey involving Japanese high school students showed that the process of escaping psychological pain by browsing the Internet increased the tendency toward latent Internet dependence and exerted a negative effect on daily life (33). In recent years, high school students have begun to use the Internet almost entirely via their smartphones. This provides various types of diversion at a low cost regardless of time or location. The Internet is an optimum tool via which to escape from reality, and its use as a coping strategy for stress is understandable, particularly in high school students whose time tends to be limited because they attend lessons at school, participate in after-school club activities, and cram school learning. Moreover, a meta-analysis reported a relationship between IA and mental health conditions such as depression, anxiety disorder, and alcohol dependence (34). These results suggested a bidirectional relationship between IA and mental health, in which both variables could be either a risk factor or an outcome. However, none of the previous studies that reported this bidirectional relationship included the same target population. Therefore, the current study provide novel information regarding the relationship between IA and poor mental health.
Aside from poor mental health, the factors that predicted IA onset in the current study included gender, grooming appearance, and snacking habits. With regard to gender, previous research showed that male was a risk factor for online game addiction (28, 35, 36). In contrast, there was no difference in IA between genders (37). In a survey conducted by the Japanese Ministry of Internal Affairs and Communications, the proportion of high school students with a strong tendency toward Internet dependence was highest in girls, at 5.2%, with proportions of 3.9% and 4.6% for boys and the overall sample. With respect to sex, the effects of sex could have been influenced by factors such as race, region of residence, or the device used to access the Internet. Future research should examine these factors. This study had several limitations. For example, the study period was limited to 1 year, which might have been an insufficient period within which to record actual IA rates; future studies should measure IA rates for longer periods. In addition, the sample included only first- and second-year students from high schools in a single provincial city in Japan, which could have led to sampling bias. Future research should examine whether similar results are obtained with students in different grades and districts. Furthermore, both nonresponse bias and reporting bias could have occurred. Moreover, the study did not examine some factors associated with IA, such as self-efficacy and home environment, identified in previous research (38, 39).
Conclusion
A 1-year longitudinal study of Japanese high school students demonstrated a relationship between IA and poor mental health, in which IA and poor mental health could be the cause or result of the other. We hope that future endeavors to improve mental health and increase self-management skills in adolescents will lead to the prevention of IA. Adolescents, their parents and educational institutions need to take policies to use properly the Internet.
Acknowledgements
This study was supported by a grant from the Japan Society for the Promotion of Science (KAKENHI Grant Number 26507009).
Footnotes
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that there is no conflict of interests.
Ethical considerations
Ethical issues (Including plagiarism, informed consent, misconduct, data fabrication and/or falsification, double publication and/or submission, redundancy, etc.) have been completely observed by the authors.
References
- 1.Van den Bulck J. (2004). Television viewing, computer game playing, and Internet use and self-reported time to bed and time out of bed in secondary-school children. Sleep, 27(1), 101–104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Chen YL, Gau SS. (2016). Sleep problems and internet addiction among children and adolescents: a longitudinal study. J Sleep Res, 25(4), 458–465. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Kim K, Ryu E, Chon MY, et al. (2006). Internet addiction in Korean adolescents and its relation to depression and suicidal ideation: a questionnaire survey. Int J Nurs Stud, 43(2), 185–192. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Ko CH, Yen JY, Yen CF, Chen CS, Chen CC. (2005). Gender differences and related factors affecting online gaming addiction among Taiwanese adolescents. J Nerv Ment Dis, 193(4), 273–277. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Kawabe K, Horiuchi F, Ochi M, Oka Y, Ueno S. (2016). Internet addiction: Prevalence and relation with mental states in adolescents. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci, 70(9), 405–412. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Chakraborty K, Basu D, Vijaya Kumar KG. (2010). Internet addiction: consensus, controversies, and the way ahead. East Asian Arch Psychiatry, 20(3), 123–132. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Cheng C, Li AY. (2014). Internet addiction prevalence and quality of (real) life: a meta-analysis of 31 nations across seven world regions. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw, 17(12), 755–760. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Wu X-S, Zhang Z-H, Zhao F, et al. (2016). Prevalence of Internet addiction and its association with social support and other related factors among adolescents in China. J Adolesc, 52, 103–111. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Lin M-P, Wu JY-W, You J, Hu W-H, Yen C-F. (2018). Prevalence of internet addiction and its risk and protective factors in a representative sample of senior high school students in Taiwan. J Adolesc, 62, 38–46. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Shek DTL, Yu L. (2016). Adolescent Internet Addiction in Hong Kong: Prevalence, Change, and Correlates. J Pediatr Adolesc Gynecol, 29(1 Suppl), S22–30. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Mihara S, Osaki Y, Nakayama H, et al. (2016). Internet use and problematic Internet use among adolescents in Japan: A nationwide representative survey. Addict Behav Rep, 4, 58–64. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Lam LT. (2014). Risk factors of Internet addiction and the health effect of internet addiction on adolescents: a systematic review of longitudinal and prospective studies. Curr Psychiatry Rep, 16(11), 508. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Chang FC, Chiu CH, Lee CM, Chen PH, Miao NF. (2014). Predictors of the initiation and persistence of internet addiction among adolescents in Taiwan. Addict Behav, 39(10), 1434–1440. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.van den Eijnden RJ, Meerkerk GJ, Vermulst AA, Spijkerman R, Engels RC. (2008). Online communication, compulsive Internet use, and psychosocial well-being among adolescents: a longitudinal study. Dev Psychol, 44(3), 655–665. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Ko CH, Yen JY, Chen CS, Yeh YC, Yen CF. (2009). Predictive values of psychiatric symptoms for internet addiction in adolescents: a 2-year prospective study. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med, 163(10), 937–943. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Cho SM, Sung MJ, Shin KM, Lim KY, Shin YM. (2013). Does psychopathology in childhood predict internet addiction in male adolescents? Child Psychiatry Hum Dev, 44(4), 549–555. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Lam LT, Peng ZW. (2010). Effect of pathological use of the internet on adolescent mental health: a prospective study. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med, 164(10), 901–906. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Young KS. (1998). Internet addiction: The emergence of a new clinical disorder. CyberPsychology & Behavior, 1(3), 237–244. [Google Scholar]
- 19.Yong Kim-Fong R. (2013). The reliability and validity of three internet addiction instruments in the Japanese population (Doctoral dissertation, Tokyo University).20. Goldberg DP, Rickels K, Downing R, Hesbacher P (1976). A comparison of two psychiatric screening tests. Br J Psychiatry, 12961–67. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Goldberg DP, Rickels K, Downing R, Hesbacher P. (1976). A comparison of two psychiatric screening tests. Br J Psychiatry, 129:61–67. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Doi Y, Minowa M. (2003). Factor structure of the 12-item General Health Questionnaire in the Japanese general adult population. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci, 57(4), 379–383. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Kaneita Y, Ohida T, Osaki Y, et al. (2006). Insomnia among Japanese adolescents: a nationwide representative survey. Sleep, 29(12), 1543–1550. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Biddle L, Gunnell D, Sharp D, Donovan JL. (2004). Factors influencing help seeking in mentally distressed young adults: a cross-sectional survey. Br J Gen Pract, 54(501):248–253. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Jackson C. (2007). The General Health Questionnaire. Occupational Medicine-Oxford, 57(1), 79–79. [Google Scholar]
- 25.Takegami M, Suzukamo Y, Wakita T, et al. (2009). Development of a Japanese version of the Epworth Sleepiness Scale (JESS) based on item response theory. Sleep Med, 10(5), 556–565. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Petry NM, Rehbein F, Gentile DA, et al. (2014). An international consensus for assessing internet gaming disorder using the new DSM-5 approach. Addiction, 109(9), 1399–1406. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Niemz K, Griffiths M, Banyard P. (2005). Prevalence of pathological Internet use among university students and correlations with self-esteem, the General Health Questionnaire (GHQ), and disinhibition. Cyberpsychol Behav, 8(6), 562–570. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Yen JY, Ko CH, Yen CF, Wu HY, Yang MJ. (2007). The comorbid psychiatric symptoms of Internet addiction: attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), depression, social phobia, and hostility. J Adolesc Health, 41(1), 93–98. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.van den Eijnden RJ, Spijkerman R, Vermulst AA, van Rooij TJ, Engels RC. (2010). Compulsive internet use among adolescents: bidirectional parent-child relationships. J Abnorm Child Psychol, 38(1), 77–89. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Wastlund E, Norlander T, Archer T. (2001). Internet blues revisited: replication and extension of an Internet paradox study. Cyberpsychol Behav, 4(3), 385–391. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Strittmatter E, Parzer P, Brunner R, et al. (2016). A 2-year longitudinal study of prospective predictors of pathological Internet use in adolescents. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry, 25(7), 725–734. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Kardefelt-Winther D. (2014). A conceptual and methodological critique of internet addiction research: Towards a model of compensatory internet use. Computers in Human Behavior, 31, 351–354. [Google Scholar]
- 33.Ohno S. (2016). Internet escapism and addiction among Japanese senior high school students. International Journal of Culture and Mental Health, 9(4), 399–406. [Google Scholar]
- 34.Ho RC, Zhang MW, Tsang TY, et al. (2014). The association between internet addiction and psychiatric co-morbidity: a meta-analysis. BMC Psychiatry, 14:183. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Ko CH, Yen JY, Chen CC, Chen SH, Yen CF. (2012). The association between Internet addiction and psychiatric disorder: a review of the literature. Eur Psychiatry, 27(1):1–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Tsai HF, Cheng SH, Yeh TL, et al. (2009). The risk factors of Internet addiction--a survey of university freshmen. Psychiatry Res, 167(3):294–299. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Chou C, Condron L, Belland JC. (2005). A Review of the Research on Internet Addiction. Educ Psychol Rev, 17(4), 363–388. [Google Scholar]
- 38.Jeong EJ, Kim DH. (2011). Social activities, self-efficacy, game attitudes, and game addiction. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw, 14(4): 213–221. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Mythily S, Qiu S, Winslow M. (2008). Prevalence and correlates of excessive Internet use among youth in Singapore. Ann Acad Med Singap, 37(1): 9–14. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]