Skip to main content
PLOS One logoLink to PLOS One
. 2021 Mar 1;16(3):e0244276. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0244276

Smartphone use patterns and problematic smartphone use among preschool children

Jeong Hye Park 1,*,#, Minjung Park 2,#
Editor: Tzipi Horowitz-Kraus3
PMCID: PMC7920339  PMID: 33647038

Abstract

Background

The purpose of the present study was to identify smartphone use patterns associated with problematic smartphone use (PSU) among preschool children. Little is known about PSU patterns in younger children, although the age for first smartphone use is decreasing.

Methods

We applied a cross-sectional study design to analyze data obtained from a nationwide survey on smartphone overdependence conducted in 2017 by the South Korean Ministry of Science and ICT and the National Information Society Agency. Data from 1,378 preschool children were analyzed using binomial logistic regression analysis. This study was conducted in compliance with STROBE (Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology).

Results

Seventeen percent of the sample met the criteria for PSU. The odds of PSU significantly increased with frequent smartphone use and in children who used a smartphone for more than two hours per day. Using smartphones to watch TV shows or videos for entertainment or fun significantly increased the odds of PSU, whereas using smartphones for education, games, and social networking did not.

Conclusions

The findings indicate that one of five preschool children using smartphones could experience PSU. Compared to other age groups, PSU in young children may be more associated with their caregivers. To prevent PSU in preschool children, caregivers need information about the total screen time recommended for children, smartphone use patterns associated with PSU, suggestions for other activities as possible alternatives to smartphone use, and strategies to strengthen children’s self-regulation with regards to smartphone use.

Background

By 2021, the population of smartphone users worldwide is expected to reach 3.8 billion, and to continue its rapid growth [1]. A smartphone is a mobile phone that can be used as a handheld computer and connects to the internet [2]. Smartphone applications (apps) are computer programs designed for a specific purpose [3] and are widely developed and used. Convenient internet access using apps has resulted in smartphones rapidly becoming a necessity in everyday life; smartphones help people work, study, acquire or share information, create or maintain social relationships, and enjoy leisure activities.

Despite these benefits, however, a growing body of literature reports negative consequences and possible dangers associated with smartphones [47]. Of the negative effects of smartphone use, problematic smartphone use (PSU) involves excessive or uncontrollable use, preoccupation, neglect of other activities, and continued use despite the evidence of potential harm [46]. Moreover, PSU may have various negative effects on children’s physical and psychological health, social interaction, and cognitive development; excessive exposure to smartphones has been associated with poor vision, poor sleep quality, and psychological dependence [46]. Children may be disconnected from actual social interactions, including interactions with other children and the people around them. They may also have difficulties in deep thinking and concentration since many smartphone apps are designed to be accessed and used intuitively, and to easily allow moves from one option to another to get interesting information. This could harm brain development and educational attainment [46].

Factors associated with PSU in middle and late childhood have been found to be the excessive use of entertainment apps (e.g., games), instant messengers, and social networking services (SNS) [5, 6, 811], as well as high frequency and long duration smartphone use patterns [6]. Furthermore, children’s PSU has been seen to contribute to a low quality of life [12], low quality of friendship [13], and poor mental health outcomes [5].

Recently, studies show that, on average, smartphones are first used at age four to five [1416], and the age of first exposure is gradually decreasing [1719]. Preschool children, who are between three to six years old, are making significant progress in cognitive, linguistic, and psychosocial development [20]. Inappropriate smartphone use patterns formed in early childhood can have a serious impact on a child’s future.

Previous studies report that the average cumulative screen time of preschool children was 4.1 hours per day, which is beyond the recommended amount of time [21], and preschoolers’ excessive screen time could increase their risk of inattention problems [22]. However, the effect of excessive smartphone use in preschool children has not yet been well studied.

South Korea has a high penetration rate of smartphone and internet use. In 2017, 89.5% of people over age three had smartphones [23] and 90.3% used the internet [24]. In addition, 99.4% of internet users have accessed the internet wirelessly, most using a smartphone (94.1%) [24]. Among smartphone users in South Korea, the prevalence of PSU among children under age 10 rose from 17.9% in 2016 to 20.7% in 2018, the largest increase among all age groups [23]. Even for young children, smartphones are more accessible and universal than other devices [1419]. Therefore, it is important to identify smartphone use patterns and sociodemographic characteristics contributing to PSU in preschool children. Understanding PSU and associated factors in preschool children is critical for developing appropriate prevention strategies.

Methods

Design

We applied a cross-sectional study design that complied with the Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (STROBE) statement [25].

Participants and data collection

Participants

Data were collected by asking children’s main caregivers in 1,279 households to observe and report on their child’s behaviors or attitudes. The data from 1,378 preschool children aged three to six who attended daycare centers or kindergartens were analyzed. The main caregiver was defined as the person who plays the primary role in caring for the child; this person was asked to report on the child’s activities at home.

Data collection

All data in the present study were obtained from the 2017 South Korean nationwide Survey on Smartphone Overdependence conducted by the Ministry of Science and ICT and the National Information Society Agency [26]. This survey is conducted annually to secure data for policymaking on appropriate smartphone use. The 2017 survey targeted smartphone users age 3–69 in all households nationwide as of September 1, 2017. After stratifying the population into 17 cities/provinces, the enumeration district was derived as the first sampling unit using proportional probability to size the systematic sampling in proportion to the number of households. The number of households was derived as the second sampling unit using systematic sampling. The complete enumeration of smartphone users age 3–69 was conducted in 10,000 households nationwide. The number of participants was 29,712. Smartphone users were defined as individuals who used the internet access feature of smartphones at least once a month, which aligned with recent changes in defining internet use since internet features became mobile and more accessible [23, 24].

Data were collected via interview surveys by trained investigators who visited participants’ households between September and November 2017. The collected data were verified first by the supervisor of a given enumeration district; a second verification was conducted via telephone with a random sample of over 30% of the collected questionnaires. As a third verification, statistics experts checked the range of responses, the divergence between questions, and the consistency of responses. If there were any errors in the data, the participant was contacted to collect the ancillary data, and the data were supplemented or re-examined to verify their validity.

Measurements

Smartphone use patterns

Smartphone use patterns included frequency and duration per day, the type of the accessed app, and the degree of app use. The average frequency and duration of smartphone use were collected by examining global estimates of media use [27] to find out how much preschool children tend to use a smartphone on average. The question used to gauge the frequency of use was, “How often did your child use a smartphone on a typical day in the last month?” Question steps were used to derive a more accurate answer. Each participant was requested to search their memory for all episodes of her/his child’s smartphone use ranging from one day to several days prior to the questionnaire, to separate unusual activities from smartphone-related activities, and to add up all the smartphone-use-episode lengths across days in the last week and last month sequentially. The duration of use was estimated by multiplying the average time per use by the frequency of use per day. Trained investigators helped participants to relay the smartphone use of their children in realistic terms, rather than according to any distorted self-image. App type was classified based on factors found to be related to PSU in previous studies: web surfing, games, television (TV)/video, music, webtoon/fiction, instant messengers, SNS, and education [5, 6, 811]. Games, TV/video, music, and webtoon/fiction were for entertainment or fun in the present study, whereas educational apps were designed around curricula with specific goals to form desirable habits, to communicate academic or social skills, or to teach intended lessons [28] such as handwashing habits, instructive fairy tales, math fundamentals, English as a second language, science, crossword puzzles, and so on. The degree of use for each app type was measured on a seven-point Likert scale (0 = do not use at all, 1 = use rarely, 7 = use very frequently). Smartphones included tablet devices.

Problematic smartphone use

Problematic smartphone use for preschool children was measured with the Korean-language Smartphone Overdependence Scale (S-scale) for children [26, 29]. The S-scale was developed to screen for PSU in each age group, including children, adolescents, adults, and elderly people, based on the Smartphone Addiction Proneness Scale (SAPS), developed in 2011 [30] and the SAPS, restructured in 2014 [31]. This scale focuses on the propensity of problematic use rather than addiction criteria [29]. The scale for children is an observer scale and consists of three subscales, including the main concept of PSU: self-control failure, salience, and serious consequences. “Self-control failure” refers to a condition in which a person is not able to control her/himself on smartphone use according to self-set goals. A sample item (reverse-scored) was “my child stops using the smartphone on his/her own without parental involvement at a given time.” “Salience” is the degree to which smartphone use becomes the most salient and important activity in one’s daily life. A sample item was “my child loves to play with a smartphone more than anything else.” “Serious consequences” refers to negative physical, psychological, and social consequences resulting from PSU. A sample item was “my child rarely plays or learns except for on his/her smartphone.” The scale contained nine items evaluated on a four-point scale (1 = strongly disagree, 4 = strongly agree), with higher scores indicating more serious PSU. In the scale development study [29], the threshold for PSU was 24 points or higher out of 36 points, which was derived by the proposed criteria of Internet Gaming Disorder in the DSM-5 [32]. The threshold point indicates an early stage of smartphone dependence in which parents and their children are experiencing conflicts in playing and learning with regard to smartphone use [29]. The study sample for the prior research on scale development comprised 201 parents of children under 10 years old [29]. In the receiver operating characteristic curve, the sensitivity of the reference point was 0.70, the specificity was 0.63, and the area under the receiver operator curve was 0.67 (standard error 0.05, p<.001). Cronbach’s alpha for the scale at validation was 0.75; Cronbach’s alpha in the present study was 0.80.

Adjusting variables

The following sociodemographic characteristics of children and family were included: child’s sex, child’s age, family size, household income, parents’ employment status, the number of family members, main caregiver in the family, and the age of the main caregiver. In addition, the main caregiver’s perception of his/her child’s smartphone dependence was assessed with the question, “How much do you think your child is dependent on a smartphone compared to the other children around him/her?” with responses rated on a scale from 1 (’not dependent at all’) to 5 (’very dependent’).

Ethical considerations

The present study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of the Korea National Institute for Bioethics Policy (IRB number: P01-201905-21-011) for using the data obtained from the Ministry of Science and ICT and the National Information Society Agency [26]. Informed consent was exempted since the data was de-identified for the participants’ personal information and provided to researchers.

Data analysis

All data were analyzed using IBM SPSS statistics, version 23.0 for Windows (IBM Corporation, U.S.). First, data cleaning was performed to bring the quality of data to a reliable level according to practice guidelines [33]. Descriptive statistics (frequencies, percentages, means, and standard deviations) were used to examine the sex and age of each child, family size, parents’ employment status, household income, the number of family members, the main caregiver in the family, and the age of the main caregiver, the perception of the main caregiver on his/her child’s smartphone dependence and use patterns. Crude differences between the non-PSU group and the PSU group were analyzed by χ2 test and independent samples t-test. Binary logistic regression was also employed to investigate the potential risk factors of PSU in preschool children. We entered all variables listed in Tables 1 and 2 in the logistic regression model because selection bias may affect the significance of the results in univariate regression [34]. Household income was a nominal variable, with the dummy variable categories used in the regression analysis derived from the univariate analysis results presented in Table 1.

Table 1. Differences between non-PSU and PSU according to the characteristics of the children and families (N = 1,378).

Variables Categories Smartphone use t/χ2 p
Non-PSU (N = 1,142) PSU (N = 236)
N (%) or mean (SD)
Children’ characteristics
Sex Male 526 (46.1) 100 (42.4) 1.072 .315
Female 616 (53.9) 136 (57.6)
Age (years) 4.6 (1.11) 4.8 (1.06) -2.404 .017
Family characteristics
Family size (number of members) 3.6 (0.92) 3.5 (0.81) 1.333 .183
Parents’ employment status Both 452 (39.6) 99 (41.9) 0.458 .512
One 690 (60.4) 137 (58.1)
Household income*
Per month
(1,000 KRW)
Less than 2,000 34 (3.0) 6 (2.5) 60.317 <.001
2,000–4,000 419 (36.7) 127 (53.8)
4,000–6,000 271 (23.7) 76 (32.2)
6,000–8,000 149 (13.0) 12 (5.1)
8,000–10,000 92 (8.1) 9 (3.8)
More than 10,000 177 (15.5) 6 (2.5)
Number of preschool children in the household 1 977 (85.6) 204 (86.4) 0.692 .707
2 162 (14.2) 32 (13.6)
3 3 (0.3) 0 (0)
Main caregiver Parent 939 (82.2) 215 (91.1) 11.323 <.001
Grandparent 203 (17.8) 21 (8.9)
Main caregiver’s age (years) 40.8 (9.83) 38.9 (7.61) 3.351 .001
Main caregiver’s perception of his/her child’s smartphone dependence 2.5 (0.83) 3.0 (0.83) -7.024 <.001

Abbreviation: PSU = problematic smartphone use, SD = standard deviation

*One million KRW was 915 USD and mean household income was 4,300 thousand KRW in Korea in 2017.

Table 2. Differences between non-PSU and PSU in smartphone use patterns (N = 1,378).

Variables Categories Smartphone use t/χ2 p
Non-PSU (N = 1,142) PSU (N = 236)
N (%) or mean (SD)
Frequency of use
(times per day)
4.4 (3.04) 28.0 (29.28) -12.362 <.001
Duration of use
(hours per day)
(minutes per use)
0.8 (0.47) 2.5 (1.86) -14.082 <.001
Less than 0.5 223 (19.5) 15 (6.4) 564.722 <.001
0.5–1 737 (65.5) 37 (15.7)
1–1.5 115 (10.1) 46 (19.5)
1.5–2 49 (4.3) 28 (11.9)
More than 2 18 (1.6) 110 (46.6)
13.1 (7.79) 8.2 (10.72) 8.176 <.001
Degree of use Education 3.1 (2.01) 3.0 (2.62) 0.552 .582
Web surfing 1.4 (1.98) 0.9 (1.87) 3.242 .001
Games 3.1 (2.38) 4.2 (2.29) -7.083 <.001
TV/video 2.6 (2.54) 4.0 (2.30) -8.004 <.001
Music 2.6 (2.46) 2.7 (2.50) -0.498 .619
Webtoons/fiction 0.6 (1.56) 1.1 (2.19) -3.128 .002
Messenger 1.4 (2.09) 1.4 (2.27) 0.164 .869
SNS 0.7 (1.65) 0.6 (1.44) 1.269 .205

Abbreviation: PSU = problematic smartphone use, SD = standard deviation, SNS = social networking services.

Differences between non-PSU and PSU in smartphone use patterns

Table 2 shows the differences in smartphone use patterns between the two groups. Smartphone use duration was significantly longer in the PSU group (χ2 = 564.722, p < 0.001); the PSU group spent 2.5 hours per day (SD 1.86), whereas the non-PSU group spent 0.8 hours (SD 0.47) (t = -14.082, p < 0.001). However, the PSU group spent 8.2 minutes (SD 10.72) per use and the non-PSU group spent 13.1 minutes (SD 7.79) (t = 8.176, p< 0.001). Smartphone use frequency was also significantly higher in the PSU group (t = -12.362, p<.001); 28.0 (SD 29.28) per day in the PSU group and 4.4 (SD 3.04) per day in the non-PSU group. The highest degree of use among app types was education and games in the non-PSU group; 3.1 (SD 2.01) and 3.1 (SD 2.38) out of 7 points, respectively. The highest degree of use in the PSU group was games and TV/video watching: 4.2 (SD 2.29) and 4.0 (SD 2.30) respectively. The TV/video (t = -8.004, p<.001) and games (t = -7.083, p<.001) differed the most in the degree of use between the two groups. Education app use did not differ significantly between the two groups (t = 0.552, p = .582).

Results

Differences between non-PSU and PSU by children’s and family characteristics

Of the 1,378 children, 1,142 (82.9%) were placed in the non-PSU group and 236 (17.1%) in the PSU group. Table 1 shows the differences in the characteristics of the children and families between the two groups. The PSU group was slightly older than the non-PSU group and the difference was significant (t = -2.404, p = .017). Household income distribution was also significantly different between the two groups (t = 60.317, p<.001). In the PSU group, the main caregiver was more often a parent (t = 11.323, p<.001), and their average age was younger (t = 3.351, p<.001). Caregivers in the PSU group had a higher awareness of their children’s smartphone dependence (t = -7.024, p<.001), but the average score was 3.0 out of 5 (SD 0.83), which implied that they perceived their child’s smartphone use to be similar to that of other children.

Potential risk factors for PSU

A multivariate binominal logistic regression analysis was performed to investigate the potential risk factors for PSU in preschool children (Table 3). Using a smartphone for more than two hours per day contributed most to the increased odds of being in the PSU group (OR = 7.85, 95% CI = 3.03–20.31). Moreover, higher smartphone use frequency also increased the odds of PSU (OR = 1.39, 95% CI = 1.32–1.48). TV/video watching significantly increased the odds of PSU (OR = 1.17, 95% CI = 1.02–1.35). However, education was negatively associated with PSU (OR = 0.84, 95% CI = 0.71–0.98). Both parents being employed significantly increased the PSU odds ratio (OR = 2.03, 95% CI = 1.05–3.91). Lower household income also significantly increased the PSU odds ratio (OR = 11.24, 95% CI = 3.56–35.48; OR = 9.32, 95% CI = 3.07–28.26, respectively). The main caregiver’s age was also positively associated with PSU (OR = 1.06, 95% CI = 1.00–1.12).

Table 3. Potential PSU risk factors.

Factors B p OR 95% CI
Child’s sex (ref. = male) Female 0.416 .175 1.52 0.83–2.77
Child’s age 0.165 .253 1.18 0.89–1.57
Family size 0.093 .659 1.10 0.73–1.66
Parents’ employment status
(ref. = one of both)
Both 0.708 .035 2.03 1.05–3.91
Household income
(ref. = more than 6,000)
4,000–6,000 2.232 <.001 9.32 3.07–28.26
Less than 4,000 2.420 <.001 11.24 3.56–35.48
Number of preschool children in the household 0.145 .737 1.16 0.50–2.70
Main caregiver (ref. = grandparent) Parent 0.329 .667 1.39 0.31–6.21
Main caregiver’s age 0.058 .046 1.06 1.00–1.90
Main caregiver’s perception 0.226 .287 1.25 0.83–1.90
Frequency of smartphone use 0.332 <.001 1.39 1.32–1.48
Duration of smartphone use
(ref. = less than 1 hour)
1–2 hours 0.632 .072 1.88 0.95–3.75
More than 2 hours 2.061 <.001 7.85 3.03–20.31
Degree of use Education -0.180 .028 0.84 0.71–0.98
Web surfing -0.076 .428 0.93 0.77–1.12
Games 0.115 .103 1.12 0.98–1.29
TV/video 0.160 .023 1.17 1.02–1.35
Music -0.022 .775 0.98 0.84–1.14
Webtoons/fiction -0.054 .658 0.95 0.75–1.20
Messenger 0.145 .050 1.16 1.02–1.34
SNS 0.016 .865 1.02 0.84–1.23

Model effect χ2 = 919.196, p<0.001; Nagelkerke’s R2 = 81.2%; Hosmer & Lemeshow χ2 = 2.531, p = .960; Classification accuracy = 94.6%.

Reference: non-PSU.

Abbreviations: PSU = problematic smartphone use; OR = odds ratio; CI = confidential interval; SNS = social networking services; ref. = reference.

Discussion

This study examined the prevalence and potential risk factors of PSU in preschool children, which have not yet been explored in detail. We found PSU prevalence among preschool children to be 17.1%, while previous studies have reported a PSU prevalence of 23.3% on average in middle and late childhood, adolescence, and young adulthood, ranging between 10 and 30% [5]. Although lower than that seen in older children, PSU prevalence in preschool children is not low. Moreover, many children as young as three to four years old can use mobile devices and participate in media multitasking without assistance [17]. The PSU rates among children are increasing every year [23, 24], making it a prominent issue that raises concerns about the potential impact on society and the need for countermeasures.

Among the potential risk factors for PSU, smartphone use frequency increased the odds of being in the PSU group by 1.39 times. The PSU group’s mean smartphone use frequency (28.0 times per day) was about six times higher than that of the non-PSU group (4.4 times per day). Conversely, children in the PSU group used smartphones for an average of 8.2 minutes in a single use, while children in the non-PSU group used them for 13 minutes. Consequently, the children in the PSU group used smartphones more often but for shorter periods than those in non-PSU group, which implies that the desire to use smartphones arises repeatedly and is very easily satisfied. This finding suggests that frequent smartphone use could be an indicator of PSU [6, 11].

Among the app types, TV/video apps significantly increased the odds of PSU, whereas game, messenger, and SNS apps were not related to PSU. Education was significantly negatively associated with PSU, which is consistent with previous study results [10, 35]. The results show that the main concern for preschool children with PSU is TV/video app use. Watching TV/videos is associated with increased screen time and sedentary behavior [3639]. Screen time includes the use of any electronic screens, such as TV/video/DVD, computers, smartphones, tablet devices, etc. [36]. Longer screen time could be harmful to various aspects of children’s health and development [3840], and may reduce exposure to family, social, and physical activities [41]. Until recently, the primary screen time medium was TV [38, 39]. However, recent trends suggest that the primary screen medium may be switching from TV to smartphones because smartphones are portable and easily accessible from anywhere. Smartphones can lead to longer screen time and hence have a more serious influence on children than TV. Preschool children spend much of their time with their caregivers. It is very common to find caregivers holding smartphones and they may easily let their children use the smartphones [1417], mainly to keep them quiet or to prevent them from distracting caregivers during work, to put them to sleep, or to persuade them to eat during mealtimes [1618, 42]. Caregivers may not recognize the extent or possible effects of the children’s smartphone use [16, 36, 42]. A study in Turkey showed that the majority of parents caring for preschool children had installed smartphone apps for their children that were mainly for entertainment and not for education, raising concerns among parents [16]. In one study of parents in six European countries with children between four and six years old, most of parents mentioned that their children like watching TV and most did not worry about their children’s TV watching time; for example, there were no planned rules for including an educational aspect [42]. Thus, easy-to-use apps available whenever caregivers need them could be their child’s favorite TV shows or videos. Children of preschool age watched TV for an average of half-an-hour to two hours per day for up to a maximum of five hours [16, 42, 43]. In the present study, the average duration of smartphone use in the PSU group was two-and-a-half hours per day, which is about three times more or 100 minutes longer than that of the non-PSU group (0.8 hours per day). TV/video app type was one of the most frequently used apps by children in the PSU group. They may get used to smartphones gradually, similar to TV, and both children and caregivers may be unable to control children’s smartphone use [42, 43].

The present study found that the absolute amount of smartphone use time is closely related to PSU and using a smartphone for more than two hours per day substantially increases the risk of PSU (odds ratio 7.85). Previous studies suggested limiting the duration of smartphone use for preschool children to less than half an hour per day to prevent PSU [14, 15]. The American Academy of Pediatrics has recommended limiting total screen time for preschool children to less than one hour per day with high-quality programs [44]. It is therefore necessary to include smartphone use in controlling total screen time for preschool children. First, the caregivers of preschool children should not show smartphones to their children for their convenience. If they decide to give smartphones to their children, the limits must be clear and well-chosen and consistently enforced so that children will learn to accept the limits and comply with them [20]. The present study additionally found that PSU was associated with parent’s employment status, caregiver’s age, and household income.

This study has some limitations. First, the data were collected by asking the main caregivers to observe and report on their children’s smartphone use at home. Therefore, it is possible that survey results reflect only their viewpoints. Second, this study used a cross-sectional design, which limits it to examining factors associated with PSU among preschool children rather than deriving causal relationships. Third, important factors associated with main caregivers or parents that have not been included in the present study may significantly influence smartphone use. Fourth, children could be exposed to other various screens, but there were not any available data regarding such exposure.

Conclusions

We found that PSU prevalence among preschool children was similar to that of other age groups, implying a need to pay attention to smartphone use by young children. The most significant use pattern associated with the PSU was the overall duration of smartphone use, indicating that children should keep their smartphone use within one hour per day and balance its function between education and fun for healthy use. Children’s smartphone use frequency should be monitored because frequent short-duration smartphone use may be a symptom of PSU. In addition, TV/video app use may be related to the caregivers’ tendency to allow TV shows or videos that children like to watch on smartphones without formal rules on limiting screen time. Therefore, to prevent PSU in young children, caregivers should set rules for the total screen time and provide children with more opportunities for physical activities. Furthermore, information should be provided to caregivers on how to set rules for total screen time and to suggest activities to replace children’s smartphone use. In addition, to strengthen children’s self-regulation of smartphone use, consistent discipline is required.

Smartphone use is continuously increasing, and the age of first smartphone use is gradually decreasing. Countries that have not yet investigated or managed PSU in young children should consider assessing PSU to develop appropriate countermeasures.

Acknowledgments

Thanks to the institutes and the anonymous participants.

Data Availability

The datasets analyzed during the current study are available in the Ministry of Science and ICT and the National Information Society Agency [https://www.data.go.kr/en/index.do]. Data access requests can be submitted through the ‘Request Data’ function in the website menu.

Funding Statement

This study was supported by the Korean Medicine R&D program funded by Ministry of Health & Welfare through the Korea Health Industry Development Institute (KHIDI) (HI20C0867). The funder had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

  • 1.Statista. Number of smartphone users worldwide from 2016 to 2021 (in billions). Nov 11, 2019. https://www.statista.com/statistics/330695/number-of-smartphone-users-worldwide/
  • 2.Cambridge Dictionary. Smartphone. 2019. https://dictionary.cambridge.org/dictionary/english/smartphone
  • 3.Cambridge Dictionary. Application. 2019. https://dictionary.cambridge.org/dictionary/english/application
  • 4.Panova T, Carbonell X. Is smartphone addiction really an addiction? J Behav Addict. 2018;7:252–9. 10.1556/2006.7.2018.49 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Sohn S, Rees P, Wildridge B, Kalk NJ, Carter B. Prevalence of problematic smartphone usage and associated mental health outcomes amongst children and young people: a systematic review, meta-analysis and GRADE of the evidence. BMC Psychiatry. 2019;19:356. 10.1186/s12888-019-2350-x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Fischer-Grote L, Kothgassner OD, Felnhofer A. Risk factors for problematic smartphone use in children and adolescents: a review of existing literature. Neuropsychiatrie. 2019;33:179–90. 10.1007/s40211-019-00319-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Elhai JD, Dvorak RD, Levine JC, Hall BJ. Problematic smartphone use: a conceptual overview and systematic review of relations with anxiety and depression psychopathology. J Affect Disord. 2017;207:251–259. 10.1016/j.jad.2016.08.030 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Choi J, Rho MJ, Kim Y, Yook IH, Yu H, Kim D-J, et al. Smartphone dependence classification using tensor factorization. PLoS One. 2017;12:e0177629. 10.1371/journal.pone.0177629 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Kuss DJ, Griffiths MD. Online social networking and addiction—a review of the psychological literature. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2011;8:3528–3552. 10.3390/ijerph8093528 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Jeong S-H, Kim H, Yum J-Y, Hwang Y. What type of content are smartphone users addicted to? SNS vs. games. Comput Hum Behav. 2016;54:10–17. [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Liu CH, Lin SH, Pan YC, Lin YH. Smartphone gaming and frequent use pattern associated with smartphone addiction. Medicine. 2016;95:e4068. 10.1097/MD.0000000000004068 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Lu L, Xu D-D, Liu H-Z, Zhang L, Ng CH, Ungvari GS, et al. Mobile phone addiction in Tibetan and Han Chinese adolescents. Perspec Psychiatr Care. 2018;1–7. 10.1111/ppc.12336 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Kim H-J, Min J-Y, Min K-B, Lee T-J, Yoo S. Relationship among family environment, self-control, friendship quality, and adolescents’ smartphone addiction in South Korea: findings from nationwide data. PLoS One. 2018;13:e0190896. 10.1371/journal.pone.0190896 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Jang JJ, Jeong IS. Related factors of smartphone addiction among preschool children. J Korean Soc Sch Community Health Educ. 2015;16:65–81. [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Lee M, Park S. Factors associated with smartphone overdependency in preschool children. Child Health Nurs Res. 2018;24:383–92. [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Genc Z. Parents’ perceptions about the mobile technology use of preschool aged children. Procedia Soc Behav Sci. 2014;146:55–60. [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Kabali HK, Irigoyen MM, Nunez-Davis R, Budacki JG, Mohanty SH, Leister KP, et al. Exposure and use of mobile media devices by young children. Pediatrics. 2015;136:1044. 10.1542/peds.2015-2151 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Ryu MH. A study if infants’/toddlers’ use of smartphones and their mothers’ perception on smartphone using. Korea J Child Care Educ. 2014;86:307–29. [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Domoff SE, Radesky JS, Harrison K, Riley H, Lumeng JC, Miller AL. A naturalistic study of child and family screen media and mobile device use. J Child Fam Stud. 2019;28:401–10. 10.1007/s10826-018-1275-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Ricci SS, Kyle T, Carman S. Maternity and pediatric nursing. 3rd ed. In: Growth and development of the preschooler. Philadelphia: Wolters Kluwer; 2017. pp. 1022–1049. [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Tandon PS, Zhou C, Lozano P, Christakis DA. Preschoolers’ total daily screen time at home and by type of child care. J Pediatr. 2011;158:297–300. 10.1016/j.jpeds.2010.08.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Tamana SK, Ezeugwu V, Chikuma J, Lefebvre DL, Azad MB, Moraes TJ, et al. Screen-time is associated with inattention problems in preschoolers: results from the CHILD birth cohort study. PloS One. 2019;14:e0213995. 10.1371/journal.pone.0213995 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Choi DJ, Kim YS, Eum NR, Kim HS. 2018 The survey on smartphone phone overdependence. Ministry of Science and ICT and National Information Society Agency. 2018. https://www.nia.or.kr/site/nia_kor/ex/bbs/View.do?cbIdx=65914&bcIdx=20876&parentSeq=20876
  • 24.No MS, Heo HN, Choi YJ, Lee HS. Survey on the internet usage. Ministry of Science and ICT and Korea Internet & Security Agency. 2017. https://www.nia.or.kr/site/nia_kor/ex/bbs/View.do?cbIdx=99870&bcIdx=20794&parentSeq=20794
  • 25.von Elm E, Altman DG, Egger M, Pocock SJ, Gotzsche PC, Vandenbroucke JP. The strengthening the reporting of observational studies in epidemiology (STROBE) statement: guidelines for reporting observational studies. Int J Surg. 2014;12:1495–1499. 10.1016/j.ijsu.2014.07.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Choi DJ, Kim YS, Um NR, Kim HS. The survey on smart phone overdependence. Ministry of Science and ICT, National Information Society Agency. 2017. http://eng.nia.or.kr/site/nia_eng/main.do
  • 27.Vandewater EA, Lee SJ. Measuring children’s media use in the digital Age: issues and challenges. Am Behav Sci. 2009;52:1152–1176. 10.1177/0002764209331539 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Kirkorian HL, Wartella EA, Anderson DR. Media and young children’s learning. Future Child. 2008;18:39–61. 10.1353/foc.0.0002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.National Information Society Agency. 2016 Digital culture forum policy research report. December 2016. NIA V-RER-B-16007. In: Kwon SJ, Eum NR. Final report on smartphone overdependence scale. pp. 253–281. http://dl.nanet.go.kr/SearchDetailView.do?cn=MONO1201708132
  • 30.Kim D, Lee Y, Lee J, Nam JK, Chung Y. Development of Korean smartphone addiction proneness scale for youth. PLoS ONE. 2014;9:e97920. 10.1371/journal.pone.0097920 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Kim D, Kwon MS, Jung YJ, Lee YH, Jeon HJ, Kim BG, et al. Restructing the smart media addiction proneness scale. November 2014. NIA V-PER-14029. National Information Society Agency.
  • 32.American Psychiatric Association. Internet gaming in DSM-5. 2013. https://www.psychiatry.org/patients-families/internet-gaming
  • 33.Osborne JW. Best practices in data cleaning: a complete guide to everything you need to do before and after collecting your data. United States of America: SAGE Publications, Inc; 2013. [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Wang H, Peng J, Wnag B, Lu X, Zheng J, Wang K, et al. Inconsistency between univariate and multiple logistic regressions, Shanghai Arch Psychiatry. 2017;29:124–128. 10.11919/j.issn.1002-0829.217031 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Lee H, Kim JW, Choi TY. Risk factors for smartphone addiction in Korean adolescents: smartphone use patterns. J Korean Med Sci. 2017;32:1674–1679. 10.3346/jkms.2017.32.10.1674 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Hinkley T, McCann JR. Mothers’ and father’s perceptions of the risks and benefits of screen time and physical activity during early childhood: a qualitative study. BMC Public Health. 2018;18:1271. 10.1186/s12889-018-6199-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Vanderloo LM. Screen-viewing among preschoolers in childcare: a systematic review. BMC Pediatrics. 2014;14:205. 10.1186/1471-2431-14-205 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Carson V, Kuzik N, Hunter S, Wiebe SA, Spence JC, Friedman A, et al. Systematic review of sedentary behavior and cognitive development in early childhood. Prev Med. 2015;78:115–122. 10.1016/j.ypmed.2015.07.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Lee EY, Carson V. Physical activity, sedentary behaviour, and psychosocial well-being among young South Korean children. Child Care Health Dev. 2018;44:108–116. 10.1111/cch.12491 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Sarwar M, Soomro T. Impact of smartphone’s on society. Eur J Sci Res. 2013;93:216–226. [Google Scholar]
  • 41.CDC. Child development. 2019. https://www.cdc.gov/ncbddd/childdevelopment/facts.html
  • 42.de Decker E, de Craemer M, de Bourdeaudhuij I, Wijndaele K, Duvinage K, Koletzko B, et al. Influencing factors of screen time in preschool children: an exploration of parents’ perceptions through focus groups in six European countries. Obes Rev. 2012;13:75–84. 10.1111/j.1467-789X.2011.00961.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Jago R, Wood L, Zahra J, Thompson JL, Sebire SJ. Parental control, nurturance, self-efficacy, and screen viewing among 5- to 6-year-old children: a cross-sectional mediation analysis to inform potential behavior change strategies. Child Obes. 2015;11:139–147. 10.1089/chi.2014.0110 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.American Academy of Pediatrics. Children and media tips from the American academy of pediatrics. 2016. https://www.aap.org/en-us/about-the-aap/aap-press-room/Pages/American-Academy-of-Pediatrics-Announces-New-Recommendations-for-Childrens-Media-Use.aspx

Decision Letter 0

Tzipi Horowitz-Kraus

23 Jul 2020

PONE-D-20-05258

Smartphone use patterns and problematic smartphone use among preschool-age children

PLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Park,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

Please submit your revised manuscript by Sep 06 2020 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Tzipi Horowitz-Kraus, PhD

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

2. You indicated that you had ethical approval for your study. In your Methods section, please ensure you have also stated whether you obtained consent from parents or guardians of the minors included in the study or whether the research ethics committee or IRB specifically waived the need for their consent.

3. We note you have included a table to which you do not refer in the text of your manuscript. Please ensure that you refer to Table 2 in your text; if accepted, production will need this reference to link the reader to the Table.

4. Please include captions for your Supporting Information files at the end of your manuscript, and update any in-text citations to match accordingly. Please see our Supporting Information guidelines for more information: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/supporting-information.

Additional Editor Comments (if provided):

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Partly

Reviewer #2: Partly

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: *Did all children in this age group go to daycare centers or kindergartens?; was there anyone at homecare? It must be stated in the text.

*Smartphone usage time; How was it calculated? what about weekdays and weekend usage times?

*Did children always use the smartphone with their parents?

*Exposure to other screens should be stated as a confounding factor, if no data was avaliable, it should be stated as a limitation of the study.

*Parental ages and education levels, and number of sibling should be given.

*English editing is necessary.

Reviewer #2: This manuscript describes phone surveys of caregivers with children ages 3-6 years (N= 1,378) to examine the relation between smartphone use patterns and problematic smartphone use (PSU) in preschoolers in South Korea. Based on a nationally representative sample, the authors found that 17% of children were classified as a PSU group. Also, the likelihood of PSU was positively predicted by frequent smartphone use, more than 2 hours of daily media use, and higher exposure to TV shows and videos. Also, the author found that family characteristics (caregiver age, parent employment status, household income) were associated with PSU. The authors concluded that problematic smartphone use is prevalent among preschoolers and emphasized the role of caregivers in preventing PSU among preschoolers.

The problematic smartphone use among young children is a relatively understudied topic in the field. This manuscript fills in this gap by describing preschoolers’ problematic smartphone usage and exploring demographic and media use factors. Also, the author presented a representative sample with a good reflection of the variations and diversity, enhancing the generalizability of the results.

Having said that, I think further information is required in the introduction, methods, and discussion sections to understand and interpret the findings clearly. First, in the introduction section, I think the author should provide more details on theoretical or conceptual justifications for studying problematic smartphone use in preschoolers by connecting their research question with the existing literature on predictors and outcomes of screen time in preschoolers. Next, the authors should clarify their research procedure in the method section. The design and analysis seem appropriate, but further details are necessary. In particular, it was not clear to me how the authors measured the frequency and amount of smartphone use. Also, I would like to see how the ‘education’ category was defined and how it would differ from the rest of the categories. Lastly, the discussion section requires a revision. In the discussion, some of the interpretations are broad statements that seem to go beyond data. Also, the discussion section includes is a very long paragraph that contains several different ideas, which should be restructured and revised to improve clarity and flow. Here are my specific questions, comments, and suggestions.

Introduction

Page 4. The authors stated that “PSU in preschool-age children has not yet been well studied” in the introduction. I think it is a valid point, but this could be connected to the existing literature on predictors and outcomes of screen time in preschoolers (see Tandon et al.., 2011; Tamana et al., 2019). That way, the authors will have a foundation to tie their research to the field of young children and media effects throughout the manuscript (explaining their rationales, interpreting the results, and highlighting their contributions).

Tandon, P. S., Zhou, C., Lozano, P., & Christakis, D. A. (2011). Preschoolers’ total daily screen time at home and by type of child care. The Journal of pediatrics, 158(2), 297-300.

Tamana, S. K., Ezeugwu, V., Chikuma, J., Lefebvre, D. L., Azad, M. B., Moraes, T. J., ... & Dick, B. D. (2019). Screen-time is associated with inattention problems in preschoolers: Results from the CHILD birth cohort study. PloS one, 14(4), e0213995.

Methods

Page 6. The authors included “education” as one of the app types. However, it was not clear to me how the "education" category was defined. I was wondering how education would differ from the rest of the categories. Children may be playing games or watching educational television programs. There is a large body of research showing the importance of media content and not just the amount of screen time within this age group. For example, a longitudinal study showed a positive impact of educational media such as Sesame Street on language and cognitive development in young children. On the other hand, entertainment content has shown to have no or negative effect, and violent media content has negative consequences (see Kirkorian et al., 2008).

Kirkorian, H. L., Wartella, E. A., & Anderson, D. R. (2008). Media and young children's learning. The Future of children, 39-61.

Page 6. Please provide sufficient details on how smartphone use patterns (frequency, duration) were measured. In its current form, it is hard for another researcher to reproduce the survey items described. I am not sure if the recall was based on a “typical” day method (i.e., estimate the amount of screen time in a “typical” day) or a diary method (e.g., thinking of yesterday, how much time did you spend..; see Vandewater & Lee, 2009).

Vandewater, E. A., & Lee, S. J. (2009). Measuring children's media use in the digital age: issues and challenges. American Behavioral Scientist, 52(8), 1152-1176.

Results

Page 7. “The study sample comprised 201 parents of children under 10 years old.” I had to pause and go back to double-check whether this refers to the sample in the current study or whether it represents the sample for the prior research on scale development. I think it is the former, but it would be helpful to state the relevant study clearly.

Pages 9-10: In the results section, the authors used “participants” to describe children. Here are some examples: 1) the first subheading “Differences between non-PSU and PSU by participant and family characteristics,” 2) Table 1 title “Differences between non-PSU and PSU according to participant and family characteristics” and 3) Table 1 under Family characteristics “Number of participants in the household.” Given that this study is based on parent reports, labeling children as participants may lead to some confusion. The authors may consider replacing “participants” with “children” in the results section for clarity.

Pages 10. As I mentioned earlier in the introduction section, the “education” category is not clear. How does it differ from games and TV/video?

Discussion

Page 14. The authors said, “Watching TV/videos is a well-established cause of increased screen time and sedentary behavior [12, 34-36].” I am not sure this statement is supported by the sources cited. If I understood correctly, the cited sources mainly focus on associations rather than providing any “causal” evidence.

Pages 14-15. The paragraph starting with “Among the potential risk factors” is very long and contains several different points, which I found hard to follow. This should be split into two or more paragraphs and revised to increase clarity.

Page 15. In the discussion, the author listed the findings from different prior studies. I think these previous studies need to be described with a bit more details about their samples so that readers can understand the contexts of their findings: “Caregivers mentioned that their children like watching TV and most did not worry about their children’s TV watching time; for example, there were no planned rules for including an educational aspect [22]. Children of this age watched TV for an average of 0.5 - 2 hours per day for up to a maximum of five hours [6, 22, 38].”

Page 16. I am not sure if the following statement is accurate based on the data presented in the manuscript: “Our findings that PSU decreased when one parent worked, but not when both worked, and that PSU increased with older main caregivers suggests that while one parent is at work, the other parent, not a grandparent, is caring for the child all day.” That is, how does this finding suggest that the other parent is caring for the child all day? I think this should be replaced with the authors’ interpretation of the results (i.e.., PSU was associated with parent’s employment status and caregiver’s age).

Page 16. “We also found that PSU was less prevalent in higher-income households, suggesting that more attention and care provided by the main caregiver allows children to avoid excessive smartphone use.” The authors made a direct connection between household income and the level of attention and care children received from their caregivers. I think this is a very broad claim that seemed to go beyond data. I suggest the authors reconsider this.

Minor points

Page 22. 33. There is a typo: preschoolers inn childcare -> preschoolers in childcare

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: No

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2021 Mar 1;16(3):e0244276. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0244276.r002

Author response to Decision Letter 0


21 Sep 2020

We thank you and the reviewers for your thoughtful suggestions and insights, which have enriched the manuscript and helped us to produce a more concise and higher quality account of our research.

We attached the file 'Response to Reviewers'.

Attachment

Submitted filename: Response to reviewers.docx

Decision Letter 1

Tzipi Horowitz-Kraus

8 Dec 2020

Smartphone use patterns and problematic smartphone use among preschool children

PONE-D-20-05258R1

Dear Dr. Park,

We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication.

An invoice for payment will follow shortly after the formal acceptance. To ensure an efficient process, please log into Editorial Manager at http://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the 'Update My Information' link at the top of the page, and double check that your user information is up-to-date. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

Kind regards,

Tzipi Horowitz-Kraus, PhD

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments (optional):

Reviewers' comments:

Acceptance letter

Tzipi Horowitz-Kraus

2 Feb 2021

PONE-D-20-05258R1

Smartphone use patterns and problematic smartphone use among preschool children

Dear Dr. Park:

I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact onepress@plos.org.

If we can help with anything else, please email us at plosone@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access.

Kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Dr. Tzipi Horowitz-Kraus

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Response to reviewers.docx

    Data Availability Statement

    The datasets analyzed during the current study are available in the Ministry of Science and ICT and the National Information Society Agency [https://www.data.go.kr/en/index.do]. Data access requests can be submitted through the ‘Request Data’ function in the website menu.


    Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES