Abstract
How do cells make efficient collective decisions during tissue morphogenesis? Humans and other organisms use feedback between movement and sensing known as ‘sensorimotor coordination’ or ‘active perception’ to inform behaviour, but active perception has not before been investigated at a cellular level within organs. Here we provide the first proof of concept in silico/in vivo study demonstrating that filopodia (actin-rich, dynamic, finger-like cell membrane protrusions) play an unexpected role in speeding up collective endothelial decisions during the time-constrained process of ‘tip cell’ selection during blood vessel formation (angiogenesis). We first validate simulation predictions in vivo with live imaging of zebrafish intersegmental vessel growth. Further simulation studies then indicate the effect is due to the coupled positive feedback between movement and sensing on filopodia conferring a bistable switch-like property to Notch lateral inhibition, ensuring tip selection is a rapid and robust process. We then employ measures from computational neuroscience to assess whether filopodia function as a primitive (basal) form of active perception and find evidence in support. By viewing cell behaviour through the ‘basal cognitive lens' we acquire a fresh perspective on the tip cell selection process, revealing a hidden, yet vital time-keeping role for filopodia. Finally, we discuss a myriad of new and exciting research directions stemming from our conceptual approach to interpreting cell behaviour.
This article is part of the theme issue ‘Basal cognition: multicellularity, neurons and the cognitive lens’.
Keywords: angiogenesis, active perception, basal cognition, morphogenesis, Notch lateral inhibition, filopodia
1. Introduction
Morphogenesis of multicellular tissues and organs is a remarkably precise, robust and adaptive process [1–3]. How do cells manage to coordinate their behaviour so precisely and rapidly, given the complexities and time constraints of the developing embryo environment they reside within? We will argue, and provide the first proof of concept evidence, that cells in tissue development have evolved a basal cognitive process of active perception in order to deliver on the adaptive problem-solving required of them during morphogenesis.
We focus here on angiogenesis, the process of new blood vessel sprouting from pre-existing ones.
Angiogenesis is critical for tissue development, homeostasis and repair across organs in the body and is frequently dysregulated in disease [4–6]. Angiogenesis is driven first and foremost by sprouting endothelial cells (ECs) as they form the new blood vessel walls. EC behaviour is highly dynamic and needs to be tightly coordinated in space and time to effectively perfuse the surrounding tissue. Despite extensive progress to better understand angiogenesis since the discovery of its role in cancer progression in the 1970s [7,8], many aspects of the timing and cell dynamics involved are not yet understood, largely due to an early lack of live-imaging methods [9]. Vascular networks are highly specialized morphologically and functionally in different organs of the body [10,11]. Although a remarkably consistent core set of molecular pathways and processes have been identified that appear to lie at the heart of angiogenesis across tissues and species [12] organ-specific fine-tuned adaptations to this core are beginning to be uncovered [13,14]. It is clear that much remains to be understood about how ECs gain information about the local tissue in order to adapt and construct organ-specific vascular network topologies and functions [15–17].
In the first core step of angiogenesis, ECs in a vessel collectively decide (select) who will be the most migratory (the ‘tip’ cell) and lead a new vessel ‘sprout’ that branches out from the pre-existing vessel (figure 1a) the other cells then follow behind (stalk cells). Tip cells are well-studied and characterized by their long thin, highly dynamic protrusions—filopodia (figure 1b). Two sprouts eventually fuse to form vessel loops that can then support blood flow (figure 1a). This process repeats iteratively, building up a dense looped network (the ‘primary plexus’) that is later remodelled and pruned through cell rearrangement and blood flow to a more familiar hierarchical tree-like structure [18,19].
Crucially, we recently found through an in silico/in vivo study that if the cells are slower to collectively select the new tip cells, then this has real consequences for the resulting vascular network topology: the slower the selection, the less dense the network (figure 1a) [17]. We have begun to uncover a growing number of ‘temporal regulators’ of EC collective decisions (e.g. tissue-derived factors like semaphorins [17] and cell-specific factors such as the tetraspanin-like protein Tm4sf18 [20]) that can fine tune branch density in a potentially organ-specific manner.
Here we demonstrate that filopodia, rather than just being features of the selected migratory tip cells, provide a vital, overlooked ‘temporal regulator’ role, keeping the selection ‘decision-making’ process timely. This highlights a novel role for cell shape changes in enhancing cell decision-making. Namely, filopodia extensions and retractions (movements) alter receptor locations and activations (sensations), amplifying differences between the cells in a positive feedback cycle. This is a form of sensorimotor coordination which, we argue, could constitute a basal form of active perception (AP). To our knowledge, active perception has until now only been studied and identified in higher organisms and prokaryotic cells [21,22].
First, we will give a brief introduction and context to the biological and basal cognitive concepts proposed in this study. We detail the tip selection process and present our time-ordering argument for the overlooked role of filopodia during selection. Second, we present our original research findings providing: (a) the first in vivo evidence during zebrafish embryo development that filopodia loss slows selection as predicted in silico in our cell-based simulations with the previously well-validated memAgent-spring model (MSM) [23–27], (b) simulations identifying that filopodia enable rapid and robust selection by conferring bistable decision-making properties to individual ECs due precisely to the feedback coupling of sensation and movement of filopodia and (c) a simple information theory metric to extract quantitative ‘signatures’ of active perception, which has potential to be extended and applied to future in vitro/in vivo and in silico experiments aiming to detect and further delineate the role of cell-level AP in tissues.
Filopodia are well documented during angiogenesis in most organs in the body. We propose that their time-keeping role during selection could be a ubiquitous part of the angiogenic process. Moreover, AP properties could be endowed to other types of cells by different processes and shape-changing mechanisms. The scope of this study, however, is to provide the first proof of concept and simple, small step. We therefore conclude with a discussion into the potentially wide-reaching new research avenues this study opens up, both drilling down into the several different detailed molecular mechanisms potentially underlying AP in cells of different tissue-beds and ways that AP could present a novel ‘higher-level’ target to therapeutically modify cell behaviour to tune branching to a desired density in diseased or engineered tissues.
2. Background
(a). Notch tip cell selection is too slow
The process to select new tip cells is currently considered to be well-understood and already well-characterized to occur via Notch-Dll4 lateral inhibition [28–30] triggered primarily by Vascular Endothelial Growth Factor (Vegf) released when the surrounding tissue becomes hypoxic (low in oxygen; figure 1c) [31,32]. However, this widely accepted selection mechanism does not consider timing or the specific dynamics of the process. We recently argued that this has caused us to miss a critical, speed-controlling feature, required for normal selection [9,33]. The multiple cycles of gene expression required by Notch-Dll4 lateral inhibition to amplify initially small differences in input Vegf signal of neighbouring ECs on a vessel to select the heterogenous pattern of tip cells is seemingly incompatible with the rapid, dynamic changes in EC state, identity and behaviour observed in angiogenesis [34,35]. We recently showed that selection of tip cells is a specifically time-constrained step of the process during zebrafish intersegmental vessel formation [20]. In this phase of zebrafish development, tip cells emerge within a ‘selection window’ of approximately 8 h ending at 24 h post fertilization (hpf), after which no new tip cells are selected. One cycle of Notch-Dll4 lateral inhibition takes approximately 4–6 h in vitro in mouse ECs [36] suggesting the currently accepted selection process would have to take closer to 16–24 h to select each tip cell. This is true even accounting for the more rapid gene expression cycles in zebrafish (approx. 2 h for one cycle, [37]). Thus, we hypothesize that Notch lateral inhibition alone is too slow to account for in vivo selection.
(b). A role for filopodia as time-keepers during collective cell decisions
So how do endothelial cells make rapid tip selection decisions? Early simulations with the MSM of Vegf–Notch selection first pointed to filopodia as an unexpected temporal regulator of the process [24], however the significance of those early simulations was not clear until now. Given that filopodia are normally used as a marker of the selected tip cell phenotype, this was a surprising prediction—how could the consequence of selection be part of the cause? We recently presented a carefully revised time-ordering of the precise events during selection, which highlighted a missed role for filopodia before tip cells are selected [9].
Time-ordered account of selection: When Vegf binds and activates Vegf receptors (Vegfr) on existing endothelial cells, the cells respond on the order of seconds by locally polymerizing actin and pushing out the cell membrane into thin protrusive shapes—filopodia—which are then rapidly extended and retracted [38,39]. These in turn move the Vegf sensors through the environment, as they reside on the deforming cell membrane. This creates a rapid positive feedback to Vegf signalling, which in itself can amplify and structure the differential Vegf input between neighbouring cells, requiring fewer cycles through the slow (on the order of hours) Notch gene-regulatory lateral inhibition mechanism to ‘stabilize’ the decision. Thus, filopodia are chronologically primary in the decision-making process, and Notch is secondary—cells ‘move while deciding’ (figure 1c), contrary to the prevailing view that filopodia movement are associated with the selected tip cells ‘decide then move’ [40–42].
The role of filopodia in angiogenesis has been surprisingly elusive, despite their clear abundance during the process. They were first identified as a guidance mechanism in angiogenesis, helping the selected tip cells to follow Vegf gradients [43]. However, they were later deemed to be ‘dispensable for guidance’ in zebrafish ISV sprouting, albeit still slowing down sprouting when reduced [44]. Filopodia together with lamellipodia are thought to anchor tip cells to extracellular substrates by focal contact points, together pulling the tip cell forward during migration and sprouting [45] though filopodia are known to be inhibitory to migration in other cell migration contexts [46]. They play a key role in facilitating the formation of anastomoses with other ECs [25,47,48]. However, these filopodia likely play a number of roles on ECs, both before and after tip cell phenotypes are selected. Filopodia have previously been defined to expand the spatial limits of cell–cell communication during Notch lateral inhibition patterning in drosophila bristle selection [49], by allowing them to reach out and contact more distant cells, but have not to our knowledge before been investigated as a temporal adaptor of Notch patterning, altering the speed and dynamic properties of Notch lateral inhibition.
(c). Filopodia and active perception
The chronological ordering of the selection process (figure 1d) brings movement to the forefront and highlights a potential role for ‘sensorimotor’ feedback in informing Notch selection decisions. ‘Moving while deciding’ and sensorimotor feedback are central to early definitions of AP in the field of psychology [50–53] where AP is well known to enhance human decision-making and execution of other cognitive tasks such as language acquisition [54]. Thus, we consider whether filopodia may constitute a simple cell-level form of AP enhancing collective tip selection decisions.
A particularly relevant example of the necessity of active perception at the human scale is found in the study of saccadic eye movements. In this context, sensory input obtained by a stationary/passive individual is not sufficient to facilitate its behaviour without local movements of sensors on the otherwise immobilized body. The necessity of movement is related to the anatomy of a human eye—which has a highly concentrated cluster of sensory cells at a region known as the fovea. The area of the fovea is small, and sensory acuity falls off rapidly in regions further away from it. Vision is largely facilitated by the eye undergoing abrupt movements, purposefully darting the fovea around visual scenes and gathering relevant information (at a rate comparable to an optimal Bayesian model). An observer would not be able to accurately characterize a visual scene without the ability to move their fovea [55].
Indeed, biology across scales is rife with examples where AP is required for behaviour including organisms ranging from Escherichia coli to Caenorrhabditis elegans, Drosophila and humans [21,55–57]. At the closest scale to ECs—single prokaryotic cells—the run and tumble behaviour of E. coli during chemotaxis is a well-established example of such a case, where the noise induced by the random arrival of chemoattractant molecules makes it physically impossible for a stationary bacterium to know which direction is ‘up’ the chemoattractant gradient [56,58–60]. Though their particular modes of movement may differ, the commonality between these examples is that the agents in question all must use coordinated sensorimotor activity to gather enough information to facilitate their behaviours.
We aim here, however, not just to make a simple analogy that filopodia on ECs are akin to active perception in higher or single-celled organisms, but to take the first steps in creating the analysis tools needed to rigorously prove that the effect of filopodia on EC selection speed is specifically due to the coupling of movement and sensation that they generate. As AP has become more widely recognized and studied in a diversity of subject areas, we can draw on a rich repertoire of tools and approaches from psychology [50–53], robotics [61–63] and neuroscience [64,65] for investigation of AP in cell biology.
There is an initiative among leading biologists to reframe the notion of what it means to explain the causes of a biological event [66]. It has been suggested that the classic feed-forward view based on molecular and genetic pathways, particularly in the context of developmental biology, falls short of explaining the adaptability and plasticity of large-scale anatomical pattern formation even when they accurately describe the interactions of the individual molecular components [9,67,68]. Recent proposals prescribe the basal cognitive lens [69]—a strategy based on exploiting the deep parallels between anatomical pattern regulation and aspects of cognition such as perception, memory and learning to better model and ultimately control these processes. Thus, this study fits neatly into the paradigm of basal cognition, framing the anatomical pattern formation in angiogenesis in terms of perception, action and behaviour.
3. Results
(a). Filopodia speed up tip cell selection in silico and in vivo
Early simulation predictions with the memAgent model [24] predicted that filopodia and Vegf gradients alter selection speed. Significant additions and extensions were rapidly made to this primitive agent-based model to incorporate a spring mesh to better model cell shape changes of the cell membrane by the actin cortex beneath [25]. Thereafter, the model was referred to as the memAgent-spring model (MSM) and has been widely used and extended to explore many other roles of filopodia and cell–cell interaction mechanisms during angiogenesis, with a wealth of predictions validated in vivo [26,27,35,70]. Here, we present the first confirmation and extended study showing the MSM, with its more realistic and verified cell shape-changing spring mesh, recapitulates the precursor models first predictions. In order to answer the question ‘do filopodia impact Notch selection timing’, we constrain the MSM to simulate only the very first, small rapid protrusive filopodia movements of ECs that are otherwise fixed in space, mimicking a first pre-existing vessel stimulated by Vegf in a tissue (see Methods—MSM).
We found that even small changes to the parameter determining the probability of extending a filopodium (F) in the model (initiating a new one or continuing to extend an existing one) resulted in a delayed selection of the tip/stalk alternating ‘salt and pepper’ pattern (figure 2a). F was normally set to 2 in all published works with this model. To assess loss-of-function (LOF) and gain-of-function (GOF) effects, we varied this value in increments of 0.5 in the range [0 : 3] with the most distinct changes to selection timing found with the values 0.5 and 3 (figure 2b,c). To be clear, by LOF and GOF we mean partial rather than total reductions and increases in function. We define the ‘time to pattern’ as the time until certain criteria capturing the salt and pepper pattern are stably met (see Methods—MSM). In agreement with the precursor model, a reduction in VEGF gradient steepness or concentration also slow patterning (figure 2c and electronic supplementary material, figure S1). New simulations predict that GOF filopodia extension can rescue patterning speed in LOF VEGF conditions (figure 2c,d). This points to an intrinsic interrelation of filopodia effects on tip selection speed with the quality of local activating signal in their environment.
To provide the first experimental validation of these predictions in vivo we analysed tip selection in the dorsal aorta (DA) of zebrafish embryos. We chose developing zebrafish in particular because they are (i) easily amenable and well-validated for live-imaging angiogenesis being transparent during development [71] and (ii) sprouting of ISVs from the DA is a highly stereotypical process with simple trackable branch initiations arising from a single pre-existing DA vessel [47], very similar to the simulation set-up used above.
First, using high spatiotemporal resolution imaging of emerging tip cells in live zebrafish embryos, we confirmed our time-ordered argument to be correct, as filopodia were observed on ECs residing in the DA at timepoints prior to tip cell selection and ISV branching (figure 2d). We then used low-dose latrunculin B (LatB) from these early timepoints to specifically disrupt filopodia extension by perturbing F-actin nucleation at filopodia (but not other actin nucleation sites) [27,44]. A dose-response study previously found that low doses of LatB, within the range of 0.08 and 0.1 µg ml−1, specifically disrupt filopodia in zebrafish ISVs, without impacting cortical actin, cell shape changes or causing the myocardial embryonic lethal issues that arise with higher LatB doses [44].
This severely perturbed the speed of tip–stalk decision-making. Unusually, it also extended the otherwise stereotypical time window for tip cell selection (albeit by only 3 h; figure 2e). The extended time window was akin to observations in Dll4-deficient embryos, suggesting that the selection window is determined largely by filopodia and Notch, but not by Vegfr signalling [20]. Moreover, quantification of the number of ECs residing in the DA that are selected to branch revealed that a loss of filopodia severely restricts the number of tip cells selected to enter branching ISVs (figure 2e). Hence, these observations were consistent with a key role for filopodia-mediated temporal control of the collective decision-making process.
Next, we wanted to assess whether the reduced rates of tip cell selection observed in the absence of filopodia (figure 2e) were associated with defects in lateral inhibition and establishment of the salt and pepper patterning of Vegfr-active tip cells. To observe tip cell patterning, zebrafish embryos were immunostained for pErk, a highly-specific readout for active Vegfr in EC tip cells [20,27,72], at the indicated time points during tip cell selection (figure 2f). In control embryos, Vegfr-active ECs were abundant and heterogeneously spread throughout the dorsal aorta during early phases of tip cell selection (up to 21 hpf). This broad and random distribution then rapidly resolved to a refined, highly regular salt and pepper pattern of Vegfr-active ECs by 22 hpf, indicating robust tip cell patterning coincident with initiation of ISV branching, as previously described [73]. Consequently, tip cells that initiated ISV branching were consistently pErk-positive (figure 2f).
In contrast, the tip cell patterning process was delayed in LatB-treated embryos lacking filopodia (figure 2f). In particular, the earliest patterning step whereby ECs exhibited heterogeneous Vegfr activity throughout the DA was prolonged by up to 3 h in LatB-treated embryos, with non-patterned clusters of pErk-positive cells evident until 24 hpf and longer. Importantly, this perturbation of tip cell patterning was not due to an overall embryonic delay (data not shown).
To characterize the generality of this effect, we quantified the pErk pixel values within a rectangular region along the DA in ImageJ. The left side of the rectangle was aligned to the anterior side of the first patch of bright pErk staining seen in the DA for all of our data at 22 hpf (our highest data timepoint, N = 5, control n = 46, LatB n = 43). We then created a plot profile of the rectangle in ImageJ to generate a list of pErk pixel brightness values along the anterior-posterior axis. When we averaged the values of the pErk brightness profiles over all embryos, we found that the control had a more spatially periodic appearance of pErk brightness along the DA (figure 2f, left subpanel). This is descriptive of the fact that ECs in the DA at 22 hpf already have regular differential activation patterns corresponding to ISV locations across the fish in the control group. In the LatB condition, there were still some elements of periodicity, but much of it is lost when averaging across fish and experiments. This suggests that LatB impinges on the ability of the cells to reliably create a differential pattern in Vegfr activations.
To quantify this difference in spatial periodicity, we employed the Lomb-Scargle Periodogram—which is a commonly used signal processing algorithm used to assess the presence of periodic modes [74]. This algorithm works essentially by checking how well sine waves of a given frequency are capable of describing the data, creating a ‘spectrum’ of values to evaluate the fit through a range of frequencies (figure 2f, right subpanel). In the control case, we find a sharp peak in the frequency spectrum at approximately 0.07 Hz, and a significantly smaller peak in the area around approximately 0.02 Hz. The 0.07 Hz mode indicates the presence of periodic peaks of period length units in pErk activations: a regular spacing that corresponds to the spacing of the ISVs. On the other hand, the 0.02 Hz peak corresponds roughly to a spatial period that spans our entire length axis and thus is merely the negligible equivalent of a ‘constant’ offset term in the sinusoidal fit. In the LatB condition, the 0.07 Hz peak loses more than half of its amplitude—indicating that a large part of the periodicity is not present, and the pattern is markedly more diffuse.
Hence, in vivo observations were consistent with simulation predictions that filopodia speed up the collective decision-making process. Likewise, tip cells were slower to emerge from the DA and lead ISV branches (figure 2f), in keeping with the time ordering of events predicted in the simulations, whereby (i) Vegfr is initially activated in many cells in response to Vegf before (ii) slower commitment to tip identity and behaviour defined by filopodia-regulated Dll4-Notch lateral inhibition. Hence, filopodia may influence the speed of tip cell selection to ensure blood vessel branching occurs within the precise time constraints of the developing tissue.
(b). Filopodia generate bistable switches in individual EC decisions in silico
The ability to dynamically select between two distinct phenotypes while intermediate states are unstable is a hallmark of bistable regulatory systems. In regulatory circuits, bistability is a mechanism well documented to enable continuous systems to act like discrete switches, by destabilizing possible intermediate values of the relevant state parameters in all but the two stable configurations [75–78]. Bistable systems are robust because small perturbations around one of their steady states simply lead to the system returning to its original configuration due to the inherent dynamics involved.
In a vessel comprised ECs with bistable tip/stalk properties, the attractiveness of choosing one state or the other would enable them to come to a collective patterning decision faster by avoiding drawn out battles in intermediary states. We recently identified that positive feedback to the Vegf sensory system by TM4SF18 slowed selection with indications that this was due to bistable properties [20]. Hence, we seek here to evaluate the hypothesis that positive feedback specifically caused by filopodia movements up a gradient of Vegf speed up selection by conferring bistable decision dynamics to individual cells using a specifically adapted MSM in silico modelling set-up, building on preliminary studies [79].
To explain our approach and expected results when assessing this hypothesis, we will use a simple mechanical analogy of a bistable system. The analogy is of a metallic ball found on a double-well landscape (figure 3a). In this case, the physical shape of the landscape supporting the ball against gravity creates two stable (attractor) states, i.e. the bottom of each well. In the presence of some small noise able to overcome locally stable bumps in the surface, this mechanical system will not linger in an intermediate state at the top of the barrier or on its slope. Transitions between the two locally stable states take place abruptly, whenever forces on the ball exceed the potential barrier imposed by the presence of gravity. Moreover, slowly tuning the environment by moving a magnet positioned underneath the track left to right and back again (such that the ball is restricted to the left/right/left valleys) uncovers a signature behaviour of bistable systems, namely hysteresis (figure 3b). Charting the position of the ball as a function of the position of the magnet clearly reveals the initial-condition dependence of the ball's response. For any position of the magnet within a window around the barrier, the ball can settle into either of the two valleys, but which one depends on its recent history of positions (figure 3c). It cannot be stable in the transition state on top of the barrier; it will fall into one of the valleys. In a system without hysteresis, such as for example if the landscape was completely flat (figure 3b), the ball's resting position would be independent of its past values (depending only on the instantaneous value of the magnet's position) and two curves would be perfectly overlapping each other (figure 3d). Our hypothesis, framed this way, would implicate filopodia in the generation of the hill in the middle of the hypothetical ‘landscape’ in this analogy (figure 3e,f,g).
To assess whether filopodia confer the bistable property of hysteresis to individual EC decisions during selection, we established a modified version of the tip cell selection MSM designed to perform hysteresis analysis on a single EC (see Methods—MSM hysteresis). One crucial difference to the model here is that though we simulate three cells, we only test the dynamical properties of the single central cell. The two ‘cells’ either side of the central EC in the model do not act or respond as ECs, but merely provide fixed sources of external Dll4 that the central cell can bind (Dext) akin to the magnet in the mechanical analogy (figure 4a). When the side cells have a fixed high level of Dll4, the central cell becomes inhibited via this Notch-Dll4 signalling prohibiting it from becoming a tip cell (figure 4b). Conversely when the side cells are fixed to present low Dll4, the middle cell receives no Notch signal and extends filopodia, becoming a selected tip (figure 4c). To assess hysteresis in simulations, Dext starts at zero and is then increased in small increments up to Dmax then decremented back down to zero, akin to the magnet movement left and right.
To evaluate the hypothesis that filopodia moving through a Vegf gradient generates positive feedback in the Vegfr system and confers bistability, we disaggregated that idea into parts and performed simulations varying each part. First, we covaried the probability of filopodia extension (F) with the slope of the linear Vegf gradient (V) and analysed the Dll4 dynamics of the middle cell (Dcell) as Dext levels were incremented up and down. In the ‘normal Vegf gradient’ (V = 0.04) with standard filopodia responsiveness (F = 2) both calibrated previously and used throughout MSM studies [24]) a bistable window is indeed apparent (figure 4d, control Vegf).
When F is reduced under normal environmental conditions (F = 0.5, V = 0.04), the loss in filopodia movement eliminates the bistable properties of the MSM-Hyst cell (figure 4d). Similarly, when the steepness of the Vegf gradient is halved (V = 0.02, LOF), the bistability is lost (F = 0.5,2) —though the hysteresis is partially rescued by GOF in filopodia movement (F = 3) (figure 4d). In cases with high filopodia activity and positive feedback from a steep Vegf gradient, such as F = 3, V = 0.04, the bistable window remains intact.
As a reduction in Vegf gradient steepness incurs a knock-on reduction to general Vegf concentration in this model (see Methods—MSM), this could be causing the very low Dcell values and a loss of bistability simply due to insufficient Vegf to initially activate the cells in the Vegf gradient LOF condition (figure 3d). To control for this, we used a previously calibrated version of the model with no gradient (a uniform distribution of Vegf) but using a concentration high enough to stimulate cells to achieve a pattern. Using our precursor memAgent model, we previously reported a slower time to select when Vegf is distributed uniformly compared to in a gradient [24] and indeed we see bistability is lost in these conditions in the MSM regardless of the level of F and the concentration of Vegf in the environment (electronic supplementary material, figure S2).
Taken together these simulations predict that positive feedback to VEGF sensing by means of filopodia movements through a Vegf gradient do confer rapid and robust bistable switch-like properties to single ECs experiencing changing Dll4 conditions from their neighbour cells.
Next, to test the importance of guidance up the Vegf gradient, as opposed to merely Vegf levels and filopodia responsiveness, we uncoupled the intrinsic feedback in the MSM between Vegfr (sensing) and filopodia extension (movement). We did so by modulating the guidance parameter G, which denotes the probability that a moving filopodium will choose to extend in the direction up the gradient—else taking a random step (figure 4e). G ranges from 0 to 1, with its default value set to 0.9 based on experimental observations [43].
By varying the guidance parameter (G), we found bistability properties are visibly disrupted with lower values, i.e. when the movement is less coupled to sensing (figure 4f, G = 0.1) at which point the active state loses stability. Though this low threshold value appears to minimize the importance of guidance in the hysteresis properties, the simplicity of the linear vertical gradient in the square lattice-based model means that a filopodium would step in the correct direction of positive feedback approximately 1/3 of the time even if it is moving completely at random. When adjusted for this fact, we see that even an approximately 50% reduction in effective guidance (from approximately 90% adjusted to 40% adjusted) is sufficient to diminish bistability. Conversely, GOF had no significant effects, but given that G is normally close to its maximum value this is to be expected.
In summary, altering parameters involving the guidance (G), filopodial responsiveness (F) or the gradient itself (V) can cause an abrupt transition to non-bistable tip/stalk dynamics. The bistability in this system is contingent on all three components being present—filopodia movement, the presence of a gradient, and the ability of the filopodia to navigate up the gradient.
(c). Measures of ‘enriched’ sensation by coordinated motion suggest simulated filopodia are a basal form of AP
Our results so far indicate (i) a role for filopodia in speeding up collective tip cell selection in vivo (figure 2) and (ii) that positive feedback between sensing and movement via filopodia confer bistable switch-like properties to facilitate rapid decision-making (figures 3 and 4). Here we ask the question are filopodia, therefore, a form of active perception? Are they akin to a basal cognitive function exploiting sensorimotor coordination to enable endothelial cells to adapt their function to the changing local environmental conditions by making ‘better’ (faster, more efficient) decisions?
To answer this question, we adopt the formalism of Lungarella, Sporns and Pegors who define active perception as the enrichment of the statistical structure of an individual's sensory input resulting from coordinated sensory and motor activity [64,80,81]. To detect active perception, they suggest a series of measures to probe statistical structure in a compressed representation of sensory input (the ‘Sensory Map’) induced by coordinated sensorimotor activity. They found that the measures they used have distinctive signatures, such as reduced Shannon entropy when their model systems are allowed to coordinate sensory and motor activity [64].
In order to apply this approach to ECs, we first established a simple two-cell version of the in silico MSM, as this is the minimal number of cells required to achieve the patterning outcome, and thus the simplest first case to delineate AP in a collective decision (figure 5a). We here identify the cells reaching a ‘pattern’ by comparing their Dll4 levels in order to pinpoint the earliest timepoint that Notch selection has stably occurred (in contrast to the longer-term Vegfr and filopodia dependent ‘time to pattern’ score used previously). A pattern is recorded when one of the two cells has obtained and maintained a substantial lead in Dll4 for a long enough time (see Methods—MSM hysteresis). The cells were again fixed in space and only able to extend and retract filopodia into a VEGF gradient, as per all simulations in this study.
We define our sensory map to be the projection of all activations of the Vegfr-2 receptors of both cells onto the fixed cylindrical vessel surface (figure 5b). Receptor activations occurring on a filopodium are projected from their point in three-dimensional space down to its nearest point on the cylindrical shell; this mapping preserves directional information (as opposed to collapsing activations down to the filopodium's base position on the cell surface) and results in a fixed spatial representation of the sensory input that the cells experience, overcoming the challenge of measurements on a dynamically changing form. The sensory map is considered at the scale of the vessel rather than the individual cell, i.e. we are specifically asking the question here does the vessel employ AP to solve a collective cell decision problem? Though this representation necessarily loses some information, we hypothesized the sensory map would retain enough directional information to detect a difference in the measures. This approach considers the question of AP at the scale of the vessel rather than the individual cell.
To search for signatures of active perception, we calculate the following measures across the sensory map at each time step in the simulation:
(1) Intensity (I): the sum of Vegfr activations (Vegfr*) across the sensory map with dimension N. This value is in units of Vegfr activations.
(2) Shannon Information Entropy (H): of the spatial distribution of Vegfr activations, for which we infer a distribution by binning with Laplace smoothing [82]. This is in units of Bits (basic unit of information, here quantifying uncertainty).
Where pi is the inferred probability density of the amount of Vegfr activations at the ith site of the sensory map.
Rather than extensively testing for any possible structure in the sensory map, we chose these AP signature measures in particular because changes in these would be straightforward to interpret yet form a basis and approach easily extended to other measures in the future. Shannon Entropy (H) has a particularly useful property—it increases for broader distributions. For example, a sharply peaked Gaussian distribution will have a lower entropy than a uniform distribution over the same space. Although it does not strictly model coherence of values, we can use H as a proxy to describe the spatial spread of activating receptors on the cell surface (figure 5c). Crucially, this corresponds to the goal of the tissue as a whole to adopt an inhomogeneous pattern. As the entropy of activations decreases, the two-cell system gets pushed towards the state of one cell being a tip cell and receiving all of the sensory input and the other being a stalk cell and receiving none of it. Therefore, H may be interpreted as a quantitative readout of progress towards the goal of inhomogeneous cell state caused by unequal sensing.
We hypothesized that signatures of active perception would be apparent in the time evolution of the measures applied to the sensory map. For example, a reduction in the entropy of the sensory map over time would mean the driving Vegf signal that the vessel is receiving is being biased to be less spatially homogeneous across it. In the beginning, both cells have an equal chance of ‘winning’ and becoming the tip cell (this is due to stochasticity in the simulated filopodia extension process). Ultimately, the cell that wins is determined by the sensory input that the pair receive. If one cell starts to get more of the growth signal, this would both facilitate the production of an inhomogeneous pattern (e.g. more sensory input localized on one region of the vessel—the winning cell) and correspondingly reduce the measured entropy of the system's sensory map. If active perception is driving this entropy decrease, then the disruption of active perception would reduce or remove the reduction in entropy.
In terms of the intensity of Vegf activations across the vessel, we expected that the intensity as a function of time takes on a different form, for example by switching from a linear to a nonlinear regime due to increased feedback between movement and sensing. Overall, we expect a filopodia (LOF motor response) in a low Vegf gradient (LOF stimulus) would have a reduced AP ability to one with a stronger coupling between sensing the Vegf (V) and the motor response to extend filopodia (F) (i.e. F and V normal or GOF).
As before, we varied the probability of extending filopodia (via F) and the steepness of the Vegf gradient/concentration (V) to see how these measures change. There are marked differences between the sensing and motor response LOF and GOF cases, as shown by the behaviour of the measures as a function of time (figure 5d). In the normal filopodia conditions, entropy appears to decline slightly before a stable pattern is achieved—and then goes down by approximately 1 bit after the patterning is achieved. The fact that entropy decreases before the patterning is detectable in Dll4 levels, however, may be susceptible to the choice of pattern Dll4 criterion parameter (see Methods—AP signatures). Intensity takes a somewhat logistic-like shape at later times (figure 5d) after the initial phase of small fluctuations. In the cases where filopodia (actions) are inhibited (F LOF), entropy declines less sharply or not at all and the logistic-like shape of the intensity starts to flatten and ultimately disappears (figure 5d). In the GOF condition of F, we find that the cells undergo large fluctuations in Vegfr activations, which slow down patterning. This implicates filopodia in the generation of these patterns in the sensory map.
The large fluctuations in the GOF conditions of the F parameter are biologically accurate and reflect the behaviour of endothelial cells when they are exposed to too much Vegf. Fluctuations are explainable as a consequence of coupled negative and positive feedback mechanisms. This is a well-studied outcome in angiogenesis and has been linked to the malformation of vasculature in environments with high Vegf, such as those surrounding tumours [36]. The fact that alterations in AP-related features of the model can bring the system into and out of this regime highlights the promise of AP machinery as a potential therapeutic target [4].
A reduction in entropy indicates that the amplification of differences in the sensed Vegf signal have begun. A reduction in spatial homogeneity of the driving sensory signals is evident before the patterning decision at the level of Notch signalling is completed (figure 5d), thus sensory map structuring is predicted to bias one cell to win before lateral inhibition commits this choice. A sharper reduction in entropy corresponds to a faster patterning decision.
Logistic-like shapes as in the intensity plot (figure 5d), on the other hand, commonly result from variables undergoing positive feedback, where the rate of change of some value is positively proportional to its current magnitude but subject to a global resource constraint (here, the limited actin resources filopodia use to extend). Hence, the shape of the time evolution of the sensory map may indicate the positive feedback facilitated by filopodia. In the previous section, simulations predict that positive feedback from filopodia generates hysteresis and bistability in EC states. Here, by analysing through the cognitive lens we have found evidence of this positive feedback in the time evolution of the intensity measure of Vegfr activations.
When we alter the guidance parameter (G), we see that the AP signature changes in line with F LOF (figure 5e). In the normal and GOF cases (G = 0.9,1.0) the same pre-emptive, slight reduction in entropy before the Notch patterning decision is made, followed by a drop of approximately 1 bit after. Intensity also responds to GOF in the way one would expect, with a late time logistic-like shape in the case of G = 1.0 (GOF) and G = 0.9. In the LOF cases where G = 0.4, 0.0, we notice the partial and complete loss of these observed structures. This suggests that the signatures we measured correspond to the effects of active perception as a whole, rather than just one of its components.
Taken together, we find entropy and intensity of Vegfr activations on the sensory map can be used as a signature to connect the effects of the three key component parts of the system under the unified, basal cognitive conceptual umbrella of ‘basal active perception’. When active perception is present, the entropy of activations on the sensory map is reduced before a patterning decision is observable in Dll4 levels, and the intensity of activations over time has a logistic-like form at later times. These findings correspond to ways in which the cells direct their receptors to facilitate rapid and robust patterning.
4. Conclusion and discussion
Here we present the first proof of concept in silico and in vivo study supporting the theory that endothelial cells utilize filopodia as a form of sensorimotor feedback (active perception) to speed up Notch decisions during angiogenesis. This highlights the functional importance of the movement of Vegfr through the environment via a deforming surface, rather than merely its basic functionality as a receptor. We find in silico that the shape-changing movements of filopodia provide positive feedback to Vegfr–Vegf signalling, which endows the tip–stalk selection machinery with bistable, rapid and robust switch-like properties and hysteresis. Lastly, we begin to unpack the notion that filopodia on ECs may constitute a basal form of active perception. We apply information theory measures used in computational neuroscience, where AP is more well-studied, which indeed indicate that this system meets a necessary but not sufficient criterion for filopodia ‘enriching’ sensory input, providing the first rigorous step towards understanding and analysing the way sensor-motor feedback contributes to EC adaptive behaviour during angiogenesis.
Our simulations identify three critical components to the system that ensure ECs are endowed with bistable properties and complete selection rapidly—filopodia, a Vegf gradient and a direct coupling mechanism between them (e.g. guidance). However, our simulations predict that a gain in one of these components can rescue bistability and selection timing given a reduction of another. This strongly suggests the importance of the interlinked nature of these components rather than properties of the individual parts in isolation. In tissues with very shallow gradients or with minimal guidance coupling—e.g. more randomly directed filopodia as in zebrafish ISVs [44]—a GOF adaptation of filopodia extension could permit ECs to nevertheless keep selection occurring on time within tissue time constraints.
Our work raises an important new role for filopodia in ensuring the Notch selection of tip cells occurs in a timely, rapid and robust manner. Contrary to the prevailing literature that considers the role for filopodia being after tip cells are selected, here we show that they play a key role before: movement must be primary and part of the decision process rather than secondary, after the decision has been made. We thus propose a shift in perspective to considering even small cell shape-changing movements as potentially critical parts of the cellular repertoire of tools, helping them to solve complex problems during tissue morphogenesis.
The conventional way to explain biological events is at the scale of molecules, proteins and genetic pathways. But in tandem to this well-trodden explanatory framework we find viewing events and processes in terms of active perception, through the perspective of basal cognition, brings great benefits for revealing hidden mechanisms in cells for two main reasons:
-
(1)
Investigating AP naturally brings embodiment (the cells body shape and organizational material structure) to the forefront. Often, we can forget the spatial shape of cells when reducing mechanisms to molecular/genetic pathways [83]. We recently found that tip cell ECs manipulate their body shape and size during sprouting in order to bypass slow Notch selection altogether and rapidly assign one daughter (the larger cell) to be the tip [27]. This work shows that cell embodiment factors (such as shape, size and movement) can have the same importance as molecular and genetic factors in a developmental mechanism. Actively emphasizing how these dynamically couple to molecular pathways opens the door to a wealth of unexpected insights and hidden knowledge that ‘well-studied processes' considered only from a non-spatial, molecular/genetic perspective may have missed. The field of biomechanics, putting force changes on an equal footing with molecular signalling by viewing cells simultaneously from a mechanical and molecular perspective, revealed a multitude of hidden abilities of cells [84]. Yet, we are only just scratching the surface of how the dynamics of cell shape and geometry feed back to signalling [85–87].
-
(2)
Pathways are a type of model for understanding biological events at a specific molecular spatial and temporal scale. Despite the tremendous successes of pathways centric biology, it is necessary to build on them and ask broader questions involving feedback, control and information flow if the goal is to understand and control biological outcomes at the higher scales, such as organism/tissue-level patterning and complex behaviour [67,68]. For example, if one endeavours to teach a rat a circus trick, it is advantageous to train the rat using positive and negative reinforcement rather than trying to micromanage every individual neuron in the rat's body [67]. Controlling morphogenic outcomes that result from the collective patterning decisions of cells may similarly benefit from such a behaviour-level approach, which the emerging basal cognition field can provide.
An immediate benefit of framing the tip selection process in terms of AP is the number of new and exciting questions and research directions that open up. Below we briefly highlight a selection.
5. Open questions arising from the basal active perception perspective
(a). Do other cell shape-changing movements form part of basal AP mechanisms?
Here we focused on filopodia as their presence in angiogenesis in widely characterized, yet their role has remained unclear. However, ECs also exhibit lamella-type extensions and triangular veils [25,45]. These protrusions are known to play roles in cell migration but a role in enriching sensory input to alter timing of cell decision-making (basal AP) has not been considered. Both work at a marginally slower timescale to filopodia and are generated by cross connected actin rather than parallel bundles. Furthermore, the shape change is fundamentally different; these are more convex or triangular respectively than the long thin filopodia. Altogether, shape-signalling feedback could vary greatly on each kind of cell protrusion, potentially providing a variety of basal AP mechanisms for cells to combine when enriching sensory input for problem-solving.
(b). Do endothelial cells continue to use basal AP while migrating and sprouting?
Here we focused on the very first, small rapid protrusive movements of ECs that are otherwise positionally fixed in a pre-existing vessel detecting a Vegf signal, before the longer-timescale full-body movements of tip cell migration during sprouting or EC tip and stalk position changes within vessels (cell rearrangement) have begun [34,35]. This was to avoid interference of other roles filopodia or longer time-scale full-body movements in this first study. However, Notch signalling continues after the first tip cells are selected, and the maintenance of a differential salt and pepper Notch pattern is required to generate differential adhesion along the sprout, which drives the cell rearrangements [23]. Thus, the ability to continually and rapidly re-establish the Notch salt and pepper pattern when individual cells change positions must still be required [9,33]. It is reasonable to hypothesize that filopodia continue to play a decision-making role on migrating ECs within sprouts when the Dll4 signal from neighbours becomes less clear after a local position-switching event disrupts the cell-by-cell Notch pattern—akin to the mid-range Dext levels in the hysteresis simulations. Indeed, in an integrated in silico/in vivo study of PFKFB3-driven glycolysis in filopodia formation we found that a reduction in filopodia perturbed cell rearrangement dynamics indirectly by impacting Notch patterning timing.
(c). Do endothelial cells use basal AP to solve other tasks than selection during angiogenesis?
The study presented here focuses on the tip cell selection step in angiogenesis, which can be seen as a key collective coordination/decision ‘task’. However, there are many different problems cells solve in order to construct the full vascular network that could be posed as ‘tasks’ cells might efficiently solve using shape-signalling feedback, AP or other basal cognition mechanisms. For example, filopodia are known to improve tip cell fusion during anastomosis [25,47], but exactly how remains unclear. ECs in formed vessels often exhibit a cilia—a sole protrusion into the lumen—which aids the sensing of flow through its movement and thus could be a form of basal AP [88].
(d). Is basal AP ubiquitous across cell types and tissues?
The really tantalizing question is how widespread basal AP mechanisms like this one may be, among the myriad of cells in tissues of different organisms? Is it an evolutionarily conserved and common mechanism employed by cells to enhance their adaptive problem-solving or a rarity, emergent only if a set of tissue conditions are met? Life-critical processes such as morphogenesis, wound healing and repair are by their nature time constrained, which we can imagine would then favour evolution of basal cognitive processes that can help to solve them faster. Cell shape changes occur as a matter of course during morphogenesis and are currently largely considered from a biomechanical stance; however, they may also confer basal AP properties by enriching sensory input, justifying wider application of the basal cognitive lens in tissue morphogenesis research. A further cell type within organisms that stands out as a prime candidate for basal AP are macrophages as they have distinctively protrusive, dynamical filopodia and lamella, which appear to play multiple roles aiding the cell to rapidly traverse very different tissue environment ‘terrains’ while finding and catching foreign bodies to phagocytose [89].
(e). What are the molecular players comprising basal AP mechanisms?
Here we co-modulated the three core interrelated components of basal AP (sensing, movement and the coupling between them) and targeted filopodia as a first coarse-grained approach. The next step is to delineate the precise molecular pathways coupling sensing and movement within filopodia, such as receptor trafficking mechanisms within filopodia and assessing whether membrane shape-detecting proteins like F-BAR [90] may play a role in coupling signalling to the shape changes. Interestingly Tm4sf18, which we found conferred positive feedback to Vegfr sensing and facilitated switch-like rapid selection in vivo, has been previously shown to increase filopodia formation [91], making it a prime target as a basal AP component. In a prior study to delineate the role of Cdc42 (a small GTPase involved in the regulation of actin-based morphogenesis and cell polarity [92–95]) in angiogenesis, we found that ECs lacking Cdc42 in the mouse retina in vivo have a competitive disadvantage to assume tip positions compared to ctrl ECs in chimaeric angiogenic sprouts [96]. Cdc42-KO-EC at tip positions lacked filopodia and instead protruded only few very short and studded protrusions from their surfaces. This suggests a crucial and cell-autonomous requirement for Cdc42 in tip cell selection. If a wider array of basal AP mechanisms were found across tissue contexts, the natural next step is to check if there is a core set of evolutionarily conserved AP molecular pathways—or if cells make use of very different proteins with the same functionality to couple sensing and motion in different contexts.
6. Challenges of active perception investigation at the cell scale
There are many opportunities but also challenges and responsibilities to carefully consider, given the impact that basal cognition-focused studies such as this could have for the field of cell and tissue biology within higher organisms. One such problem is to balance and temper the psychological concepts and claims in AP studies made with higher organisms correctly, when working at the cell level. Similarly, there is a need to consider the empirical tools and viability of features of experimental design for study of AP at the different scales.
To establish conclusive evidence for active perception, Yang et al. [22] suggest that it is necessary to compare the actions of the system in question to those of a model of what an idealized active sensor would do given the same goal (for example, a model utilizing Bayesian decision theory [55,97]). Characterizing active perception in our system this way is challenging because doing so would require assuming that endothelial cells have a known goal in their actions. Even if cells are optimizing towards a goal it is often the case that they are simultaneously solving a number of problems in parallel. This makes it hard on teleological grounds to devise a simple idealized model for comparison (except for potentially viewing the negative feedback of lateral inhibition as a form of teleology [98]). Thus, we took a simpler, indirect approach when assessing the presence and effects of active perception in angiogenesis as used by Lungarella et al. [80].
We defined, akin to Lungarella et al., basal AP for cells in tissues as specifically meaning coupled, direct two-way feedback between motion and sensing (i.e. not requiring indirect processing through gene regulation) that enriches sensory input leading to improvement of a collective cell problem-solving process in morphogenesis. This minimal definition avoids unhelpful overfitting of human-level psychological connotations to cells such as perception, action, goals and intentionality while enabling empirical testing of basal AP in the future. This enables us to now use verifiable molecular approaches in vivo and in vitro to prove the coupling between motion and sensing. Together with signature changes to the sensation enriching measures, this is a way to measure the resulting effect on the cell coordination process in question. Here we focused on the first simplest sensory enriching measures—the time evolution of receptor activations and entropy. We plan to explore and extend to more complex metrics in the future, such as using transfer entropy to assess the strength of sensorimotor coupling rather than just searching for its signatures [64,99].
Overall, we hope that this study acts as a first step into a new research programme elucidating the extent and variety of basal AP mechanisms that cells from different tissues may have evolved to enable complex collective cell coordination processes. Importantly, as many seemingly unrelated pathways may constitute component parts of basal AP mechanism, we propose that basal AP is inherently accessible to therapeutic modulation by targeting any of these to control and tune individual to collective cell behaviour in tissues.
7. Methods
(a). In silico methods
(i). The memAgent-spring model
The MSM was first developed and described in full by Bentley et al. [24,25] and has since been used in a wide range of studies to explore VEGF/Notch and tip cell dynamics during angiogenesis with many predictions being validated in vivo from this model [26,27,35,70]. Being the only angiogenesis tip selection model with sophisticated filopodia dynamics, it was the natural choice for extension in studies here. Briefly, in this model cell morphology is represented as a surface comprised many small agents (membrane agents, ‘memAgents’) connected by springs following Hooke's Law (which confer tension to the changing cell shape, akin to the actin cortex beneath a cells membrane) (figure 2b). The memAgents move in continuous space but are snapped to a gridded lattice to implement discrete local rules of agent interactions (grid sites represent 0.5 × 0.5 × 0.5-μm cubes of space). At each time-step, an equal number of the cell's current, total Vegf receptors are distributed to each memAgent over the cell's current surface shape. Dll4 ligands are only distributed to any memAgent in Moore neighbourhoods of memAgents from other cells (on the interface between cells or ‘junctions’). The ambient Vegf is modelled as a fixed linear gradient or a uniformly distributed value, where in the linear case each grid site (g) starting from y = 0 has Ig level, calculated as where y is the y-axis coordinate position of g and V is the slope of the gradient.
Model initialization and parametrization: The model was initialized with 10 cells in a row, one per vessel cross section (e.g. Figure 2b), representing a collection of endothelial cells in a pre-existing vessel competing to sprout into the space above (represented very simply here as just a fixed VEGF gradient extending into the y-axis above the horizontal row of cells). All parameters were kept the same as previously published [17,20,24,25] except those being varied to match the experimental conditions here, described below. For example, a cell is 10 µm wide and spans one cross section of a pre-existing vessel with 6 µm diameter (matching in vivo capillary dimensions). Each cell comprises approximately 1200 memAgents initially.
Signalling: Simplifying the description of the competitive receptor biology between different Vegfrs in the original MSM, as it is not the direct focus of this study, we can state that the number of active Vegf receptors of a given memAgent m is calculated as —where the * denotes an activated receptor–ligand complex. Notch is activated in each memAgent (resulting in the local removal of Dll4 from neighbouring cell's memAgents), up to the value of Nm. If then ; if , then .
Filopodia: MemAgents extend filopodia by one grid site length at a time by creating non-branched chains of new memAgents adhered to the environment (fixed in position by adhesions to the environment every 2 µm, as per [46]). These events are stochastic, with approximate probability:
where F is a tunable constant varied in simulations here. Filopodia do not extend unless they have enough available actin and Vegf receptors activate. For full rules behind the actin dynamics and spring settings driving the filopodia extension and retraction, see Bentley et al. [25]. Filopodia retract if the memAgent at the top of the chain has not extended for Fmax time steps. Retraction occurs by iteratively snapping the chain of springs connecting adhered memAgents along the filopdium back to the next memAgent in the chain, while deleting memAgents as they go (based on in vitro observations that filopodia retract in short rapid bursts to the nearest adhesion along it, which then releases, triggering the next rapid retraction back and so on). This can be imagined as similar to the way a climber inserts periodic anchors to the rope as they climb so that they only fall back quickly to the last anchor point. With filopodia, every anchor eventually is pulled away by this force in short bursts, until the filopodium has fully retracted to its base. However, their total length is limited by the total amount of actin available to the cell, which gets used up on every filopodia extension of 1 μm. The original model in Bentley et al. [25] generates full cell migration by releasing filopodia adhesions and allowing the springs to pull the cell forward akin to veil/lamella advance [46]. Here, however, we keep adhesions fixed to control variables, prohibiting these other forms of protrusions, to ask specifically whether shape changes due to filopodia play a role in selection timing.
Genetic regulation: Briefly, Notch lateral inhibition is implemented each time step as follows:
and
where σ, δ are constants and denotes Vegfr* after a time delay (28 time steps) corresponding to the time it takes for translocation of activated receptor signalling to the nucleus, transcription to alter gene expression, and subsequently protein levels at the cell surface. The new and Dcell levels are then redistributed to the memAgents and the process repeats on the next time step.
Parametrization: All parameters in the model are set as in Bentley et al. [25], which were previously calibrated to experimental data. To investigate active perception, we focus here on simulations varying filopodia extension (F) and the input signal gradient (V). For normal EC, F = 2 and V = 0.04, guidance G = 0.9. We fine tuned three parameters for this study (filspacing, tokenStrength and filtipmax) by covarying them and exploring the phase space of the system, arriving at values very close to their previously published values, giving no change to normal system behaviour, but putting the model at a more precise critical point so that varying other parameters in our LOF/GOF studies accessed different behavioural phases of the model. Parameter values are detailed in table 1.
Table 1.
parameter (name/symbol) | description | setting value or range | justification |
---|---|---|---|
Pattern Dll4 criterion | amount of Dll4 advantage required for cell to be declared ‘winner’ | 600 | heuristically estimated: appears larger than the amplitude of possible oscillatory modes |
Pattern Stability criterion | amount of time required to be spent with advantage of Pattern Dll4 criterion until pattern declared stable | 100 | heuristically estimated: appears longer than the phase of possible oscillatory modes |
Guidance (G) | probability that an extending filopodium will choose to step into the direction of locally maximal sensed VEGF concentration | in range (0,1), control 0.9 | originally set to 0.9, Bentley et al. [25] |
Vconcst (V) | vegf concentration parameter; flat value distributed everywhere in uniform case, or defines the slope of linear gradient in gradient case | gradient in range (0.01,0.04), control 0.04; uniform in range (0.5,1.4), control 0.8 | Bentley et al. [25] |
FilVary (F) | filopodia propensity to extend parameter | in range (0,3), control 2 | Bentley et al. [25] |
Filtipmax (Fmax) | maximum number of time steps elapsed between extensions; if more time elapses, the filopodia begins retracting instead | 15 | fine tuned; previously filtipmax = 30 in [24] and 10 in [25] |
TokenStrength | simulated actin cost of extending filopodia | 1 | tine tuned; previously TokenStrength = 3 in [25] |
FilSpacing | spacing between filopodia on cell body, in units of memAgents | 2 | tine tuned; previously FilSpacing = 1 in [25] and 4 in [24] |
We switch off veil advancing, lamella mechanisms in simulations, create permanent adhesions at the base of filopodia prohibiting cell body movements and switch off cell rearrangement position changing junction-based mechanisms [23] fixing cells to their initial positions in the pre-existing vessel.
Time to pattern: To assess patterning times (when a clear and complete alternating salt and pepper pattern of tip and stalk cells have been selected) the model was run 100 times for a maximum number of time steps (2000 for Vegf gradients and 5000 for uniform Vegf). For the 10-cell version of the MSM the pattern must meet criteria as per Bentley et al. [24]. Tip cells are defined as cells that have sprouted filopodia and have not been inhibited (as per in vivo tip cell characterization). The patterning criterion is that four or five non-adjacent tip cells must be active at a given time. The stability criterion dictates that the pattern of tip cells must persist for 100 time steps (chosen as it allows for a cycle through the Notch pathway to occur which might alter the pattern if unstable). Once these criteria are met, the collective of cells is deemed ‘patterned’ and the time is recorded.
(ii). The MSM hysteresis model
To test our hypothesis that filopodia speed up patterning due to the positive feedback of sensing and acting generating bistable properties in single cells, we developed a new simulation testbed extension to the MSM that parallels the mechanical ball and magnet example of a bistable system (figure 3a–c). We focus on how an individual cell's state changes as it experiences different, controlled levels of inhibition from two immobilized and de-coupled neighbouring cells. Lateral inhibition from these two neighbours (‘external Dll4’ or Dext) affects the individual EC under investigation (its current Dll4 level, ‘Dcell’, is used as proxy for its active/inhibited state), but these neighbours are uncoupled from being inhibited themselves (figure 4a). Dcell is therefore analogous to the ‘ball position’ in the mechanical example of a bistable system described in the Results section and the fixed neighbours Dext to the magnet position; raising Dext can push the cell into a more inhibited (low Dcell) state and vice versa.
In a hysteresis analysis, the external driving signal is quasi-statically raised and lowered in such a way that the dynamical variable stays at or close to its equilibrium value as the driving signal is varied. We start with Dext set to zero and thus simulate a fully active EC with long filopodia (figure 3c). We then slowly raise the Dext in increments of 50 until full inhibition is achieved (figure 3b), then reverse the process until it is yet again relieved. If filopodial active perception indeed creates bistability, we expect the transition between active/inhibited states to take place abruptly, at a Dext threshold that is higher for the active → inhibited transition than the reverse.
Assessing quasi-static equilibrium: As the model is stochastic, we use a method to evaluate the quasi-stability of the system before incrementing the Dext level as in hysteresis studies the system is expected to reach steady state before incrementing. In order to approximate quasi-static equilibrium while changing Dext, we tested the stability of Dcell levels using the following method: (i) we allowed a transient time window (250 time steps) to pass, (ii) we calculated the average and standard deviation of Dcell in two adjacent, non-overlapping sliding windows of size 250, (iii) we tested whether and , where μc/μp denote the average Dcell and its standard deviation in the last/previous window (ε = 0.5). If the EC does not pass the test or a maximum time limit of 60 000 was not yet reached, we increment the window. Otherwise we move on to the next Dext.
Characterizing Dcell fluctuations at intermediate Dext levels in calibrating quasi-static equilibrium assessment: We arrived at the generous ε threshold value, used to assess the quasi-static equilibrium, by observing the fluctuations of Dcell close to the bistable region. Due to the stochastic nature of filopodia extensions, Dcell fluctuations close to the bistable region are large (electronic supplementary material, figure S3a,b). Because of this inherent stochasticity, the exact Dext thresholds at which abrupt transitions occur are not the same from simulation to simulation. Moreover, if the time window between increments is very large (well above the biological time scales involved in patterning), the EC may undergo stochastic transitions between metastable active/inhibited states. To understand the nature of these transitions, we set up a second set of simulations. In this case, we started the EC with Dext = 0 and ran the simulation for 104 time steps, after which we abruptly increased Dext to a non-zero target value, and performed 3 × 104 more steps. Exposure to low Dext (500) keeps the EC in an active state, albeit with sizable Dcell fluctuations (electronic supplementary material, figure S3c). At the intermediate Dext level of 850, however, the long simulation showcases abrupt, stochastic transitions between active and inhibited states (electronic supplementary material, figure S3d). Lastly, high Dext (950) causes a one-way transition from active to a strongly inhibited state (electronic supplementary material, figure S3e). Even here, the transition is not immediate (approx. 0.3 × 104 steps), but it is irreversible within the time window of the run.
(iii). Active perception signature methods
The model set-up: For work involving the AP signatures, we set up a minimal two-cell MSM vessel.
Time to pattern: Rather than the Vegfr and filopodia criteria as in the first MSM simulations, here we used the simpler and filopodia-independent Dcell approach as developed in the hysteresis simulations above to determine the earliest time indicator of when the dynamical system has gone past the point of no return into the attractor of tip versus stalk. We simply measured the total Dll4 levels of the two cells and declared the system ‘patterned’ once one of the cells has maintained an advantage of 600 ligands over the other for 100 time steps (the 100 time steps of waiting were subtracted from the final number once elapsed). Once a cell achieves an advantage of 600 Dll4 (a value 6% of Dmax, decided on heuristically), in the absence of fluctuations (which we test by waiting 100 time steps), the dynamics of the system seem to always ensure that cell to be selected as the tip. This is a heuristic method and makes many assumptions such as the absence of a long-timescale fluctuations and the presence of the bistable dynamics we witnessed in §2 of this paper, reasonable for the constrained parameter ranges of F, V and G investigated here. The second assumption does not render the claims made in this paper circular, because the measures/heuristics used to characterize the bistability (thus ruling out slow fluctuations) were different from those used that involved assuming its presence.
Sensory map: To calculate the sensory map, we first calculated the displacement vector from the Vegfr activation occurring on memAgents existing away from the main cell body (e.g. on filopodia) to the centre of the circular cross section of the cylindrical cell body. Then we normalized this displacement vector and multiplied it by the radius of the blood vessel to point to the closest region on the surface of the cell to the activation. We mapped all activations out in space onto the cell surface in this way. One caveat of this approach is the activations from one cell can be attributed to the neighbour cell surface area if the filopodia reaches over it. However, the sensory map here represents the vessel level activations and sensing of the environment rather than precise individual cell sensing levels.
Estimating entropy: To estimate Shannon entropy, we used the standard binning approach and Laplace smoothing to turn the data into a distribution. We did not have to quantize any data because of the discrete nature of Vegfr activations and the discrete spatial grid that the MSM exists on.
(b). Experimental methods
(i). Animals
Zebrafish embryos, larvae and adults were maintained according to UK Home Office regulations. All studies were approved by the University of Manchester Ethical Review Board. Zebrafish strains were maintained at pH 7.4, a temperature of 28°C and exposed to 14 h light and 10 h dark cycles. Zebrafish lines used in this study were the Tg(kdrl: nlsEGFP)zf109 strain [100] and Tg(kdrl:ras-mCherry)s896 strain [101].
(ii). Time-lapse imaging
Confocal microscopy of live Tg(kdrl:nlsEGFP)zf109 and Tg(kdrl:ras-mCherry)s896 embryos was performed as previously described [20,27]. Tg(kdrl:nlsEGFP)zf109 embryos were manually dechorionated and incubated with EtOH carrier or 0.08 µg ml−1 latrunculin B from 16 hpf prior to live imaging at 19 hpf. Tg(kdrl:ras-mCherry)s896 embryos were manually dechorionated prior to imaging at approximately 24 hpf. Embryos were mounted in 1% low-melt agarose in glass bottom dishes, which were subsequently filled with media supplemented with 0.0045% 1-phenyl-2-thiourea and 0.1% tricaine, as well as EtOH carrier or 0.08 µg ml−1 latrunculin B for Tg(kdrl:nlsEGFP)zf109 embryos. Tg(kdrl:nlsEGFP)zf109 embryos were imaged using a 20x dipping objective on a Zeiss LSM 700 confocal microscope. Tg(kdrl:ras-mCherry)s896 embryos were imaged using a 20x dipping objective on a Zeiss LSM 880 confocal microscope with Airyscan. Embryos were maintained at 28°C and stacks were recorded at every 0.3 h for Tg(kdrl:nlsEGFP)zf109 embryos or every 0.3 s for Tg(kdrl:ras-mCherry)s896 embryos. Tracking of cell motility was performed in ImageJ using the manual tracking plugin. All cell tracking recordings were normalized at each time point relative to the position of the dorsal aorta to account for any dorsal or ventral drift of embryos during imaging.
(iii). Perk immunostaining
Whole-mount immunostaining for pErk was performed as previously described [20,27]. Briefly, Tg(kdrl:nlsEGFP)zf109 embryos were manually dechorionated and incubated with EtOH carrier or 0.08 µg ml−1 latrunculin B from 16 to 18 hpf prior to fixation in PFA at the indicated time points. Embryos were washed in MeOH, bleached in 3% H2O2 in MeOH, washed again in MeOH, stored in MeOH at −20°C for 2 days in MeOH before equilibration with PBT (PBS, 0.1% Tween-20). Embryos were cryoprotected in 30% sucrose in PBT before further equilibration in PBT, incubation with 150 mM Tris–HCl (pH 9.0) and heating to 70°C for 15 min. Embryos were then washed in PBT, dH2O, acetone, PBT and TBST (TBS, 0.1% Tween-20, 0.1% Triton X-100), prior to incubation overnight at 4°C with block solution (TBST, 1% BSA, 10% goat serum). Embryos were subsequently incubated overnight with anti-phospho-ERK1/2 antibody (1 : 250, Cell Signaling; #4370) in blocking buffer, washed with TBST, washed with Maleic buffer (150 mM maleic acid, 100 mM NaCl, 0.001% Tween-20, pH 7.4), blocked in 2% blocking reagent (Sigma-Aldrich) in Maleic buffer for 3 h and incubated with goat anti-rabbit IgG-HRP (1 : 1000) in 2% blocking reagent in Maleic buffer overnight at 4°C. Embryos were then washed in Maleic buffer and then PBS at room temperature prior to incubation with 50 µl amplification diluent with 1 µl Tyramide-Cy3 (Perkin Elmer) for 3 h at room temperature in the dark. Embryos were finally washed over several days in TBST at room temperature. Embryos were then mounted in 1% low-melt agarose in glass bottom dishes and imaged using a 20x dipping objective on a Zeiss LSM 700 confocal microscope. Finally, to identify pErk-positive endothelial cells, kdrl:nlsEGFP signal was used to create a binary channel mask to isolate only nuclear signal in the pErk channel.
Acknowledgments
We would like to thank Dylan Feldner-Busztin for Python data science support.
Ethics
Zebrafish embryos, larvae and adults were maintained according to UK Home Office regulations. All studies were approved by the University of Manchester Ethical Review Board.
Data accessibility
The code has been fully open sourced and can be found here: https://github.com/Bentley-Cellular-Adaptive-Behaviour-Lab/mem-agent-spring-model-activeperception.
Authors' contributions
B.Z. first conceived of using the information theory AP measures, designed and carried out simulations and analysis and drafted the manuscript; G.C. carried out in vivo experiments and data analysis; I.M.A. created all schematics and figures and participated in manuscript drafting; K.V.-V. carried out simulations and analysis; T.M. carried out simulations and analysis and critically revised the manuscript; K.I.H carried out simulations and analysis; R.T. carried out in vivo experiments and data analysis; E.R.R first conceived of the single-cell bistability study, participated in design of the study, participated in data analysis and critically revised the manuscript; S.P.H designed the in vivo experiments, performed supervision and participated in manuscript drafting; K.B. first conceived of the filopodia as active perception theory, participated in design of the study, participated in data analysis, performed supervision and drafted the manuscript.
Competing interests
We declare we have no competing interests.
Funding
This research was funded in whole, or in part, by the Wellcome Trust (FC001751). For the purpose of Open Access, the author has applied a CC BY public copyright licence to any Author Accepted Manuscript version arising from this submission. K.B., B.Z., T.M., K.V.-V. and I.A. were supported by The Francis Crick Institute, which receives its core funding from Cancer Research UK (FC001751), the UK Medical Research Council (FC001751) and the Wellcome Trust (FC001751). S.P.H. was funded by the Wellcome Trust (ref. 219500/Z/19/Z). S.P.H. and R.T. were funded by the British Heart Foundation (PG/18/67/33891). G.C. is a BBSRC DTP PhD student. K.B. and E.R.R. were funded in part by BIDMC for early work on the project while located at BIDMC. E.R.R. was funded by BIDMC and NIH grant no. HL077348-03 while at BIDMC during early work towards this study, then by the College of Wooster (Henry Luce III Funds) and NIH grant no. HL119322-04. K.I.H was supported by institutional training grant T32 HL07893 from the NHLBI of the NIH during early work on this project while located at BIDMC.
References
- 1.Goodwin BC, Kauffman S, Murray J. 1993. Is morphogenesis an intrinsically robust process? J. Theor. Biol. 163, 135-144. ( 10.1006/jtbi.1993.1112) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Abouchar L, Petkova M, Steinhardt C, Gregor T. 2014. Fly wing vein patterns have spatial reproducibility of a single cell. J. R. Soc. Interface 11, 20140443. ( 10.1098/rsif.2014.0443) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Piatt J. 1955. Regeneration of the spinal cord in the salamander. J. Exp. Zool. 129, 177-207. ( 10.1002/jez.1401290109) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Carmeliet P, Jain RK. 2011. Principles and mechanisms of vessel normalization for cancer and other angiogenic diseases. Nat. Rev. Drug Discovery 10, 417-427. ( 10.1038/nrd3455) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Potente M, Gerhardt H, Carmeliet P. 2011. Basic and therapeutic aspects of angiogenesis. Cell 146, 873-887. ( 10.1016/j.cell.2011.08.039) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Herbert SP, Stainier DY. 2011. Molecular control of endothelial cell behaviour during blood vessel morphogenesis. Nat. Rev. Mol. Cell Biol. 12, 551-564. ( 10.1038/nrm3176) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Folkman J. 1974. Tumor angiogenesis. In Advances in cancer research (eds G Klein, S Weinhouse, A Haddow), pp. 331-358. Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Elsevier. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Folkman J. 1971. Tumor angiogenesis: therapeutic implications. New Engl. J. Med. 285, 1182-1186. ( 10.1056/NEJM197108122850711) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Bentley K, Chakravartula S. 2017. The temporal basis of angiogenesis. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 372, 20150522. ( 10.1098/rstb.2015.0522) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Risau W. 1997. Mechanisms of angiogenesis. Nature 386, 671-674. ( 10.1038/386671a0) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Fruttiger M. 2007. Development of the retinal vasculature. Angiogenesis 10, 77-88. ( 10.1007/s10456-007-9065-1) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Geudens I, Gerhardt H. 2011. Coordinating cell behaviour during blood vessel formation. Development 138, 4569-4583. ( 10.1242/dev.062323) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Ramasamy SK, Kusumbe AP, Wang L, Adams RH. 2014. Endothelial Notch activity promotes angiogenesis and osteogenesis in bone. Nature 507, 376-380. ( 10.1038/nature13146) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Simons M, Gordon E, Claesson-Welsh L. 2016. Mechanisms and regulation of endothelial VEGF receptor signalling. Nat. Rev. Mol. Cell Biol. 17, 611. ( 10.1038/nrm.2016.87) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.LeCouter J, Lin R, Ferrara N. 2002. Endocrine gland–derived VEGF and the emerging hypothesis of organ-specific regulation of angiogenesis. Nat. Med. 8, 913-917. ( 10.1038/nm0902-913) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Aird WC. 2007. Phenotypic heterogeneity of the endothelium: I. Structure, function, and mechanisms. Circ. Res. 100, 158-173. ( 10.1161/01.RES.0000255691.76142.4a) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Kur E, Kim J, Tata A, Comin CH, Harrington KI, da Costa FL, Bentley K, Gu C. 2016. Temporal modulation of collective cell behavior controls vascular network topology. Elife 5, e13212. ( 10.7554/eLife.13212) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Blanco R, Gerhardt H. 2013. VEGF and Notch in tip and stalk cell selection. Cold Spring Harbor Perspect. Med. 3, a006569. ( 10.1101/cshperspect.a006569) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Franco CA, et al. 2015. Dynamic endothelial cell rearrangements drive developmental vessel regression. PLoS Biol. 13, e1002163. ( 10.1371/journal.pbio.1002163) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Page DJ, Thuret R, Venkatraman L, Takahashi T, Bentley K, Herbert SP. 2019. Positive feedback defines the timing, magnitude, and robustness of angiogenesis. Cell Reports 27, 3139-3151. ( 10.1016/j.celrep.2019.05.052) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Gomez-Marin A, Louis M. 2012. Active sensation during orientation behavior in the Drosophila larva: more sense than luck. Curr. Opin Neurobiol. 22, 208-215. ( 10.1016/j.conb.2011.11.008) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Yang SC.-H., Wolpert DM, Lengyel M. 2016. Theoretical perspectives on active sensing. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 11, 100-108. ( 10.1016/j.cobeha.2016.06.009) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Bentley K, et al. 2014. The role of differential VE-cadherin dynamics in cell rearrangement during angiogenesis. Nat. Cell Biol. 16, 309-321. ( 10.1038/ncb2926) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Bentley K, Gerhardt H, Bates PA. 2008. Agent-based simulation of Notch-mediated tip cell selection in angiogenic sprout initialisation. J. Theor. Biol. 250, 25-36. ( 10.1016/j.jtbi.2007.09.015) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Bentley K, Mariggi G, Gerhardt H, Bates PA. 2009. Tipping the balance: robustness of tip cell selection, migration and fusion in angiogenesis. PLoS Comput. Biol. 5, e1000549. ( 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000549) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Cruys B, et al. 2016. Glycolytic regulation of cell rearrangement in angiogenesis. Nat. Commun. 7, 1-15. ( 10.1038/ncomms12240) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Costa G, Harrington KI, Lovegrove HE, Page DJ, Chakravartula S, Bentley K, Herbert SP. 2016. Asymmetric division coordinates collective cell migration in angiogenesis. Nat. Cell Biol. 18, 1292-1301. ( 10.1038/ncb3443) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Mullard A. 2007. Notch mediates the sprouting of tip cells. Nat. Rev. Mol. Cell Biol. 8, 183-183. ( 10.1038/nrm2136) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Hellstrom M, et al. 2007. Dll4 signalling through Notch1 regulates formation of tip cells during angiogenesis. Nature 445, 776-780. ( 10.1038/nature05571) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Siekmann AF, Lawson ND. 2007. Notch signalling limits angiogenic cell behaviour in developing zebrafish arteries. Nature 445, 781-784. ( 10.1038/nature05577) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Carmeliet P, et al. 1996. Abnormal blood vessel development and lethality in embryos lacking a single VEGF allele. Nature 380, 435-439. ( 10.1038/380435a0) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Ferrara N, Carver-Moore K, Chen H, Dowd M, Lu L, O'Shea KS, Powell-Braxton L, Hillan KJ, Moore MW. 1996. Heterozygous embryonic lethality induced by targeted inactivation of the VEGF gene. Nature 380, 439-442. ( 10.1038/380439a0) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Venkatraman L, Regan ER, Bentley K. 2016. Time to decide? Dynamical analysis predicts partial tip/stalk patterning states arise during angiogenesis. PLoS ONE 11, e0166489. ( 10.1371/journal.pone.0166489) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Arima S, et al. 2011. Angiogenic morphogenesis driven by dynamic and heterogeneous collective endothelial cell movement. Development 138, 4763-4776. ( 10.1242/dev.068023) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Jakobsson L, et al. 2010. Endothelial cells dynamically compete for the tip cell position during angiogenic sprouting. Nat. Cell Biol. 12, 943-953. ( 10.1038/ncb2103) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Ubezio B, Blanco RA, Geudens I, Stanchi F, Mathivet T, Jones ML, Ragab A, Bentley K, Gerhardt H. 2016. Synchronization of endothelial Dll4-Notch dynamics switch blood vessels from branching to expansion. Elife 5, e12167. ( 10.7554/eLife.12167) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Leslie JD, Ariza-McNaughton L, Bermange AL, McAdow R, Johnson SL, Lewis J. 2007. Endothelial signalling by the Notch ligand Delta-like 4 restricts angiogenesis. Development 134, 839-844. ( 10.1242/dev.003244) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Jacquemet G, Hamidi H, Ivaska J. 2015. Filopodia in cell adhesion, 3D migration and cancer cell invasion. Curr. Opin. Cell Biol. 36, 23-31. ( 10.1016/j.ceb.2015.06.007) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Mattila PK, Lappalainen P. 2008. Filopodia: molecular architecture and cellular functions. Nat. Rev. Mol. Cell Biol. 9, 446-454. ( 10.1038/nrm2406) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Suchting S, Freitas C, le Noble F, Benedito R, Bréant C, Duarte A, Eichmann A. 2007. The Notch ligand Delta-like 4 negatively regulates endothelial tip cell formation and vessel branching. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 104, 3225-3230. ( 10.1073/pnas.0611177104) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Hellström M, Phng L.-K, Gerhardt H. 2007. VEGF and Notch signaling. Cell Adhesion Migration 1, 133-136. ( 10.4161/cam.1.3.4978) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Phng L-K, Gerhardt H. 2009. Angiogenesis: a team effort coordinated by Notch. Dev. Cell 16, 196-208. ( 10.1016/j.devcel.2009.01.015) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Gerhardt H, et al. 2003. VEGF guides angiogenic sprouting utilizing endothelial tip cell filopodia. J. Cell Biol. 161, 1163-1177. ( 10.1083/jcb.200302047) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Phng L-K, Stanchi F, Gerhardt H. 2013. Filopodia are dispensable for endothelial tip cell guidance. Development 140, 4031-4040. ( 10.1242/dev.097352) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.De Smet F, Segura I, De Bock K, Hohensinner PJ, Carmeliet P. 2009. Mechanisms of vessel branching: filopodia on endothelial tip cells lead the way. Arterioscler. Thromb. Vasc. Biol. 29, 639-649. ( 10.1161/ATVBAHA.109.185165) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Steketee MB, Tosney KW. 2002. Three functionally distinct adhesions in filopodia: shaft adhesions control lamellar extension. J. Neurosci. 22, 8071-8083. ( 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-18-08071.2002) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Lenard A, Ellertsdottir E, Herwig L, Krudewig A, Sauteur L, Belting H-G, Affolter M. 2013. In vivo analysis reveals a highly stereotypic morphogenetic pathway of vascular anastomosis. Dev. Cell 25, 492-506. ( 10.1016/j.devcel.2013.05.010) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Marín-Padilla M, Howard L. 2019. Endothelial cells filopodia in the anastomosis of CNS capillaries. Front. Neuroanatomy 13, 49. ( 10.3389/fnana.2019.00049) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Cohen M, Georgiou M, Stevenson NL, Miodownik M, Baum B. 2010. Dynamic filopodia transmit intermittent Delta-Notch signaling to drive pattern refinement during lateral inhibition. Dev. Cell 19, 78-89. ( 10.1016/j.devcel.2010.06.006) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Dewey J. 1896. The reflex arc concept in psychology. Psychol. Rev. 3, 357. ( 10.1037/h0070405) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Gibson JJ. 2014. The ecological approach to visual perception: classic edition. Hove, UK: Psychology Press. [Google Scholar]
- 52.Gibson JJ. 1966. The senses considered as perceptual systems. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.
- 53.Gibson JJ. 1950. The perception of the visual world. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.
- 54.Pulvermüller F, Fadiga L. 2010. Active perception: sensorimotor circuits as a cortical basis for language. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 11, 351-360. ( 10.1038/nrn2811) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Yang SC-H, Lengyel M, Wolpert DM. 2016. Active sensing in the categorization of visual patterns. Elife 5, e12215. ( 10.7554/eLife.12215) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Long J, Zucker SW, Emonet T. 2017. Feedback between motion and sensation provides nonlinear boost in run-and-tumble navigation. PLoS Comput. Biol. 13, 1-25. ( 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1005429) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Calhoun AJ, Chalasani SH, Sharpee TO. 2014. Maximally informative foraging by Caenorhabditis elegans. Elife 3, e04220. ( 10.7554/eLife.04220) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Berg HC, Purcell EM. 1977. Physics of chemoreception. Biophys. J. 20, 193-219. ( 10.1016/S0006-3495(77)85544-6) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Sourjik V, Wingreen NS. 2012. Responding to chemical gradients: bacterial chemotaxis. Curr. Opin. Cell Biol. 24, 262-268. ( 10.1016/j.ceb.2011.11.008) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Bialek W, Setayeshgar S. 2005. Physical limits to biochemical signaling. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 102, 10 040-10 045. ( 10.1073/pnas.0504321102) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Bajcsy R. 1988. Active perception. Proc. IEEE76, 966–1005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 62.Tenenbaum JM. 1970. Accommodation in computer vision. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Department of Computer Science. [Google Scholar]
- 63.Pfeifer R, Lungarella M, Iida F. 2007. Self-organization, embodiment, and biologically inspired robotics. Science 318, 1088-1093. ( 10.1126/science.1145803) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Lungarella M, Sporns O. 2006. Mapping information flow in sensorimotor networks. PLoS Comput. Biol. 2, e0020144. ( 10.1371/journal.pcbi.0020144) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Lungarella M, Sporns O. 2005. Information self-structuring: key principle for learning and development. In Proc. the 4th Int. Conf. Development and Learning, Osaka, Japan, 19–21 July 2005, pp. 25-30. New York, NY: IEEE. [Google Scholar]
- 66.Bizzarri M, Brash DE, Briscoe J, Grieneisen VA, Stern CD, Levin M. 2019. A call for a better understanding of causation in cell biology. Nat. Rev. Mol. Cell Biol. 20, 261-262. ( 10.1038/s41580-019-0127-1) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Levin M. 2020. The biophysics of regenerative repair suggests new perspectives on biological causation. Bioessays 42, 1900146. ( 10.1002/bies.201900146) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Nurse P. 2008. Life, logic and information. Nature 454, 424-426. ( 10.1038/454424a) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Manicka S, Levin M. 2019. The Cognitive Lens: a primer on conceptual tools for analysing information processing in developmental and regenerative morphogenesis. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 374, 20180369. ( 10.1098/rstb.2018.0369) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Villefranc JA, Nicoli S, Bentley K, Jeltsch M, Zarkada G, Moore JC, Gerhardt H, Alitalo K, Lawson ND. 2013. A truncation allele in vascular endothelial growth factor c reveals distinct modes of signaling during lymphatic and vascular development. Development 140, 1497-1506. ( 10.1242/dev.084152) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Chávez MN, Aedo G, Fierro FA, Allende ML, Egaña JT. 2016. Zebrafish as an emerging model organism to study angiogenesis in development and regeneration. Front. Physiol. 7, 56. ( 10.3389/fphys.2016.00056) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Shin M, Beane TJ, Quillien A, Male I, Zhu LJ, Lawson ND. 2016. Vegfa signals through ERK to promote angiogenesis, but not artery differentiation. Development 143, 3796-3805. ( 10.1242/dev.137919) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Yokota Y, Nakajima H, Wakayama Y, Muto A, Kawakami K, Fukuhara S, Mochizuki N. 2015. Endothelial Ca2+ oscillations reflect VEGFR signaling-regulated angiogenic capacity in vivo. elife 4, e08817. ( 10.7554/eLife.08817) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.VanderPlas JT. 2018. Understanding the Lomb–Scargle periodogram. Astrophys. J. Suppl. Ser. 236, 16. ( 10.3847/1538-4365/aab766) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Angeli D, Ferrell JE, Sontag ED. 2004. Detection of multistability, bifurcations, and hysteresis in a large class of biological positive-feedback systems. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 101, 1822-1827. ( 10.1073/pnas.0308265100) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Cherry JL, Adler FR. 2000. How to make a biological switch. J. Theor. Biol. 203, 117-133. ( 10.1006/jtbi.2000.1068) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Gardner TS, Cantor CR, Collins JJ. 2000. Construction of a genetic toggle switch in Escherichia coli. Nature 403, 339-342. ( 10.1038/35002131) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Ferrell JE Jr. 2002. Self-perpetuating states in signal transduction: positive feedback, double-negative feedback and bistability. Curr. Opin. Cell Biol. 14, 140-148. ( 10.1016/S0955-0674(02)00314-9) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Bentley KHKI, Regan ER. 2014. Can active perception generate bistability? Heterogeneous collective dynamics and vascular patterning. In ALIFE 14: Proc. 14th Int. Conf. on the Synthesis and Simulation of Living Systems, New York, NY, 30 July–2 August 2014, pp. 328-335. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
- 80.Lungarella M, Pegors T, Bulwinkle D, Sporns O. 2005. Methods for quantifying the informational structure of sensory and motor data. Neuroinformatics 3, 243-262. ( 10.1385/NI:3:3:243) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Sporns O, Pegors TK. 2004. Information-theoretical aspects of embodied artificial intelligence. In Embodied artificial intelligence (eds F Iida, R Pfeifer, L Steels, Y Kuniyoshi), pp. 74-85. Berlin, Germany: Springer. [Google Scholar]
- 82.MacKay DJ. 2003. Information theory, inference and learning algorithm. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- 83.Bentley K, Philippides A, Regan ER. 2014. Do endothelial cells dream of eclectic shape? Dev. Cell 29, 146-158. ( 10.1016/j.devcel.2014.03.019) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Wang JH.-C, Thampatty BP. 2006. An introductory review of cell mechanobiology. Biomech. Model. Mechanobiol. 5, 1-16. ( 10.1007/s10237-005-0012-z) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Schmick M, Bastiaens PI. 2014. The interdependence of membrane shape and cellular signal processing. Cell 156, 1132-1138. ( 10.1016/j.cell.2014.02.007) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Zhao H, Pykäläinen A, Lappalainen P. 2011. I-BAR domain proteins: linking actin and plasma membrane dynamics. Curr. Opin. Cell Biol. 23, 14-21. ( 10.1016/j.ceb.2010.10.005) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Rangamani P, et al. 2013. Decoding information in cell shape. Cell 154, 1356-1369. ( 10.1016/j.cell.2013.08.026) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Goetz JG, et al. 2014. Endothelial cilia mediate low flow sensing during zebrafish vascular development. Cell Rep. 6, 799-808. ( 10.1016/j.celrep.2014.01.032) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Horsthemke M, et al. 2017. Multiple roles of filopodial dynamics in particle capture and phagocytosis and phenotypes of Cdc42 and Myo10 deletion. J. Biol. Chem. 292, 7258-7273. ( 10.1074/jbc.M116.766923) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Dorland YL, et al. 2016. The F-BAR protein pacsin2 inhibits asymmetric VE-cadherin internalization from tensile adherens junctions. Nat. Commun. 7, 1-18. ( 10.1038/ncomms12210) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Zukauskas A, Merley A, Li D, Ang L., Sciuto TE, Salman S, Dvorak AM, Dvorak HF, Jaminet S-CS. 2011. TM4SF1: a tetraspanin-like protein necessary for nanopodia formation and endothelial cell migration. Angiogenesis 14, 345-354. ( 10.1007/s10456-011-9218-0) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Boureux A, Vignal E, Faure S, Fort P. 2007. Evolution of the Rho family of ras-like GTPases in eukaryotes. Mol. Biol. Evol. 24, 203-216. ( 10.1093/molbev/msl145) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Melendez J, Grogg M, Zheng Y. 2011. Signaling role of Cdc42 in regulating mammalian physiology. J. Biol. Chem. 286, 2375-2381. ( 10.1074/jbc.R110.200329) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94.Melendez J, Stengel K, Zhou X, Chauhan BK, Debidda M, Andreassen P, Lang RA, Zheng Y. 2011. RhoA GTPase is dispensable for actomyosin regulation but is essential for mitosis in primary mouse embryonic fibroblasts. J. Biol. Chem. 286, 15 132-15 137. ( 10.1074/jbc.C111.229336) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95.Jaffe AB, Hall A. 2005. Rho GTPases: biochemistry and biology. Annu. Rev. Cell Dev. Biol. 21, 247-269. ( 10.1146/annurev.cellbio.21.020604.150721) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96.Laviña B, et al. 2018. Defective endothelial cell migration in the absence of Cdc42 leads to capillary-venous malformations. Development 145, dev161182. ( 10.1242/dev.161182) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97.Körding KP, Wolpert DM. 2006. Bayesian decision theory in sensorimotor control. Trends Cogn. Sci. 10, 319-326. ( 10.1016/j.tics.2006.05.003) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98.Rosenblueth A, Wiener N, Bigelow J. 1943. Behavior, purpose and teleology. Philos. Sci. 10, 18-24. ( 10.1086/286788) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 99.Schreiber T. 2000. Measuring information transfer. Phys. Rev. Lett. 85, 461. ( 10.1103/PhysRevLett.85.461) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 100.Blum Y, Belting H-G, Ellertsdottir E, Herwig L, Lüders F, Affolter, M. 2008. Complex cell rearrangements during intersegmental vessel sprouting and vessel fusion in the zebrafish embryo. Dev. Biol. 316, 312-322. ( 10.1016/j.ydbio.2008.01.038) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101.Chi NC, Shaw RM, De Val S, Kang G, Jan LY, Black BL, Stainier DY. 2008. Foxn4 directly regulates tbx2b expression and atrioventricular canal formation. Genes Dev. 22, 734-739. ( 10.1101/gad.1629408) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
The code has been fully open sourced and can be found here: https://github.com/Bentley-Cellular-Adaptive-Behaviour-Lab/mem-agent-spring-model-activeperception.