Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2021 Feb 22;10:e63819. doi: 10.7554/eLife.63819

Incomplete removal of extracellular glutamate controls synaptic transmission and integration at a cerebellar synapse

Timothy S Balmer 1,, Carolina Borges-Merjane 1,2,, Laurence O Trussell 1,
Editors: Katalin Toth3, John R Huguenard4
PMCID: PMC7935485  PMID: 33616036

Abstract

Synapses of glutamatergic mossy fibers (MFs) onto cerebellar unipolar brush cells (UBCs) generate slow excitatory (ON) or inhibitory (OFF) postsynaptic responses dependent on the complement of glutamate receptors expressed on the UBC’s large dendritic brush. Using mouse brain slice recording and computational modeling of synaptic transmission, we found that substantial glutamate is maintained in the UBC synaptic cleft, sufficient to modify spontaneous firing in OFF UBCs and tonically desensitize AMPARs of ON UBCs. The source of this ambient glutamate was spontaneous, spike-independent exocytosis from the MF terminal, and its level was dependent on activity of glutamate transporters EAAT1–2. Increasing levels of ambient glutamate shifted the polarity of evoked synaptic responses in ON UBCs and altered the phase of responses to in vivo-like synaptic activity. Unlike classical fast synapses, receptors at the UBC synapse are virtually always exposed to a significant level of glutamate, which varies in a graded manner during transmission.

Research organism: Mouse

Introduction

The neurotransmitter glutamate initiates phasic postsynaptic currents following its release from synaptic vesicles and binding to ionotropic postsynaptic receptors. Glutamate is then cleared within milliseconds from the synaptic cleft by diffusion and the activity of excitatory amino acid transporters (EAATs). Rapid glutamate removal is essential to maintain brief and reliable transmission. By contrast, unipolar brush cells (UBCs) of the cerebellar cortex transform brief mossy fiber (MF) excitation into a prolonged postsynaptic current due to the persistence of glutamate in the synapse for hundreds of milliseconds after synaptic release (Lu et al., 2017; Rossi et al., 1995). The unusually long lifetime of glutamate at this synapse may be due to a barrier to diffusion caused by the convoluted dendritic brush (Rossi et al., 1995), perhaps combined with a relatively low expression of EAATs. Due to these constraints, removal of glutamate is not only slow, but might also be incomplete. If so, the mechanism of synaptic transmission would be quite unique at this synapse, with transmission being mediated by elevations in free glutamate above a significant baseline level, while always maintaining a near equilibrium of transmitter with receptors.

At some synapses, ambient neurotransmitter can induce tonic currents (also called background or standing currents). For example, cerebellar granule cells have a tonic inhibitory current due to ambient GABA-activating extrasynaptic GABAA receptors (Brickley et al., 1996; Kaneda et al., 1995). At other synapses, tonic activation of high-affinity NMDARs (Sah et al., 1989) and metabotropic receptors Bandrowski et al., 2003 have been reported. We investigated whether ambient glutamate and tonic currents are a prominent feature of the MF to UBC synapse, given the unusual time course of glutamate signaling that characterizes this synapse.

This question was of particular interest because tonic glutamate receptor currents could have profound effects on cerebellar activity. UBCs are spontaneously active in vivo (Ruigrok et al., 2011; Simpson et al., 2005), and their axons terminate in multiple MFs that likely coordinate the firing of numerous granule cells and their parallel fibers. Thus, in addition to more typical intrinsic mechanisms (Diana et al., 2007; Russo et al., 2007), tonic glutamate currents at a single UBC could control the UBCs’ spontaneous firing rate and therefore the activity of downstream neurons.

Here we report that a physiologically significant level of ambient glutamate is indeed present at the MF to UBC synapse, where it controls spontaneous firing by tonic mGluR2 activation and regulates phasic postsynaptic currents by partially desensitizing AMPA receptors (AMPARs). The source of the ambient glutamate is not action potential-evoked release, but is due to spontaneous vesicle fusion, and its level is regulated by EAATs. Indeed, stimulating the MF to UBC synapse with in vivo-like frequency patterns shows that EAATs control the phase delay of firing between MF input and UBC output that likely coordinates the activity of hundreds of parallel fibers and may play a role in cerebellar learning.

Results

Ambient glutamate is regulated by EAATs

EAATs play an important role in synaptic transmission by removing glutamate from the synapse after vesicular release. At the MF to granule cell synapse, EAATs limit the exposure to glutamate spillover (DiGregorio et al., 2002; Overstreet et al., 1999; Xu-Friedman and Regehr, 2003). At the same cerebellar glomeruli, MFs release glutamate onto ON UBCs and OFF UBCs, which, respectively, give a net inward and outward current response (Borges-Merjane and Trussell, 2015), and EAATs limit the duration of excitatory postsynaptic currents (EPSCs) and reduce AMPAR desensitization (Lu et al., 2017). The outward current results from mGluR2-activated K+ conductance while inward current results from AMPAR. We tested the hypothesis that EAATs control ambient glutamate levels at the MF to UBC synapse by making whole-cell recordings from UBCs in lobe X (nodulus) of mouse cerebellum. ON or OFF UBC subtypes were differentiated by their response to pressure ejection of glutamate (1 mM, puff) via a second pipette or electrical stimulation of presynaptic MFs. In all experiments, GABAA, glycine and mGluR1 receptors were blocked (see Materials and methods). In these and some previous experiments (Borges-Merjane and Trussell, 2015), we did not observe synaptic activation of mGluR1, despite the sensitivity of the cells to agonists of these receptors.

In the first set of experiments, we reasoned that blockade of EAATs should elevate ambient glutamate, leading to tonic inward or outward currents, depending on the UBC subtype and its complement of receptors. In OFF UBCs, bath application of EAAT inhibitor DL-TBOA (50 µM) caused an outward shift in holding current that was blocked by mGluR2 antagonist LY341495 (1 µM) (Figure 1A,B), consistent with EAATs limiting ambient glutamate and thus tonic mGluR2 receptor activation. Antagonist application made the holding current inward relative to control, presumably because of a tonic AMPAR-mediated current. In addition to an outward shift in holding current, the mGluR2-mediated outward currents became smaller in the presence of DL-TBOA, suggesting that ambient glutamate activated synaptic receptors (Figure 1C).

Figure 1. EAATs prevent glutamate accumulation at rest.

(A) OFF UBC responding to 50 Hz 10 × 10 V electrical stimulation of white matter every 20 s. The upward deflections are mGluR2-mediated outward currents typical of OFF UBC responses to synaptic stimulation (arrow points to the first). DL-TBOA caused outward shift in tonic current, blocked by mGluR2 antagonist LY341495. (B) Effect of DL-TBOA on OFF UBCs. Paired t-tests, baseline vs DL-TBOA, p=0.008, n = 10; DL-TBOA vs DL-TBOA+LY34, p=0.006, n = 5. (C) Synaptic currents evoked by electrical stimulation. Same cell as in (A). The holding current shifted outward (upward arrow) when EAATs were blocked by DL-TBOA. (D) ON UBC example. DL-TBOA caused an increase in tonic inward current, blocked by AMPAR antagonist GYKI. (E) Effect of DL-TBOA on ON UBCs. Paired t-tests, baseline vs DL-TBOA, p=0.0003, n = 13; DL-TBOA vs DL-TBOA+GYKI, p=0.007, n = 9. (F) ON UBC synaptic currents evoked by 50 Hz 10× electrical stimulation. DL-TBOA caused the holding current to shift inward (downward arrow) and the slow rebound EPSC became slower to rise and decay. (Fi) Steady-state current during stimulation, with the tonic current subtracted to show that in DL-TBOA the steady-state current is outward relative to the tonic current. Scale as in (F). (Fii) Phasic EPSCs with tonic currents subtracted. The first EPSC became smaller due to increased tonic AMPAR desensitization. The holding potential was maintained at −70 mV, and thus increasing AMPAR currents are inward and increasing mGluR2 responses are outward.

Figure 1.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1. EAAT1–2 contribute to glutamate uptake at MF-UBC synapse.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1.

AMPAR-mediated currents were isolated by blocking mGluR2, mGluR1 and NMDA currents with LY341495, JNJ16259685 and MK-801. In both sets of experiments, the EC50 value for the EAAT subtype with a lower affinity for the drug was used as the first concentration, in order to fully block high-affinity EAATs while preserving partial activity of the lower affinity EAATs. Then by subsequently increasing the drug concentration the contributions of EAATs with differing affinities for the antagonists could be separated. For example, a low concentration of TFB-TBOA (0.3 µM) blocks EAAT1 and 2 completely, while a higher concentration will additionally block EAAT3 completely (Shimamoto et al., 2004). (A) Application of 0.3 µM TFB-TBOA, which likely blocks EAAT1 and EAAT2 completely, increased the tonic inward current. Raising the concentration to 2 µM to additionally block EAAT3 had no significant effect on the holding current. Application of GYKI blocked the tonic inward current induced by TFB-TBOA. n = 6. The data are relative to the application of the drug indicated by the arrowheads, with the break in data correcting for the variable time between drug applications. (B) Effect of TFB-TBOA on currents evoked by 50 Hz 10 X synaptic stimulation. There was no significant change in the synaptic currents between the two concentrations of TFB-TBOA. The synaptic currents evoked by 50 Hz stimulation were almost entirely outward in both concentrations of TFB-TBOA, relative to the tonic current. This reversal of the direction of the current is due to synaptically released glutamate desensitizing AMPARs that are open at rest due to their activation by ambient glutamate. (C) Summary of the effects of low and high concentrations of TFB-TBOA and DL-TBOA. The low concentration of TFB-TBOA that likely blocked EAAT1 and EAAT2 accounted for ~93% of the shift in holding current. Increasing the TFB-TBOA concentration to additionally block EAAT3 did not have a significant effect (p=0.970). (D) DL-TBOA will block EAAT2 and 3 completely at 50 µM, but only completely block EAAT1 at a higher concentration (Shimamoto et al., 1998). Both concentrations of DL-TBOA increased the inward holding current, which suggests that EAAT1 and EAAT2 both contribute to glutamate uptake at this synapse. Application of 50 µM DL-TBOA, which likely blocks EAAT2 and EAAT3 completely, had a large effect on the tonic current. Raising the concentration to 300 µM to additionally block EAAT1 also increased the tonic inward current. The inward current induced by DL-TBOA was carried by AMPARs, as it was blocked by GYKI n = 5. The plots are relative to the application of the drug indicated by the arrowheads. (E) Effect of DL-TBOA on currents evoked by 50 Hz 10X synaptic stimulation. (F) The low concentration of DL-TBOA which is presumed to block EAAT2 and EAAT3 only accounted for 59% of the total shift in holding current. Increasing the DL-TBOA concentration to additionally block EAAT1 caused a significant shift in holding current consistent with EAAT1 contributing to glutamate uptake at this synapse. Repeated measures ANOVA followed by Tukey's multiple comparisons tests. (p>0.05), (*) (p≤0.05), (**) (p≤0.01), (***) (p≤0.001), (****) (p≤0.0001).
Figure 1—figure supplement 2. AMPAR kinetic model predicts glutamate transients that produce slow EPSC.

Figure 1—figure supplement 2.

(A) The kinetic AMPAR model contained four binding sites. Each bound state (C) can either open (O) or desensitize (D). Conductance values increase with number of binding sites occupied: 5, 10, 15, and 20 pS. (B) Simulation of currents evoked by application of glutamate to AMPAR kinetic model. One millimolar glutamate gave a larger peak current, but a smaller steady-state current than 30 µM. (C) Simulated dose–response curves of peak and steady-state currents. The dots in the right panel are from experiments reported in Lu et al., 2017 to show that the simulated steady-state currents represented by the line reproduce the experimental data. (D) Glutamate concentration transients (top trace) were generated that evoked modeled AMPAR currents (lower black trace) that approximated the experimental data (red) using a 3D diffusion equation. Inset: Magnification of the glutamate transients and simulated currents during the first five release events. (E) The glutamate transient at the end of the train plotted on a semilog scale. The blue circle indicates where the transient decays to 1 mM and the red circle indicates where the transient decays to 32 µM, the concentration that evoked a peak steady-state conductance in the model. The slow EPSC (below) is generated as the glutamate concentration decreases along the bell-shaped dose–response curve. (F) Without ambient glutamate, predicted currents driven by modeled glutamate transients cannot reproduce experimental data; the steady-state currents were too large and the slow EPSCs were too small. (G) Summary of AMPAR number and ambient glutamate concentration that produced currents that fit data from 14 UBCs. The models predicted an ambient glutamate level of 4.7 ± 2.7 µM and 190.1 ± 82.6 AMPARs (mean ± SD) each having a maximal conductance of 20 pS with all four sites bound. (H) Same cell as (DE) after block of EAATs with DL-TBOA, the evoked currents from 5/9 cells could be reproduced by the model by simply increasing the ambient glutamate concentration and the molecules of glutamate released in the 3D diffusion equation used to generate the glutamate transient waveform. (I) The glutamate transient in DL-TBOA is much longer, reaches the concentration that evoked the peak conductance (32 µM) later, and decays slower than under control conditions.
Figure 1—figure supplement 2—source data 1. The parameters of the AMPA receptor kinetic model were adjusted to give incomplete desensitization and to fit experimental data from Lu et al., 2017.
Figure 1—figure supplement 3. Some AMPAR currents recorded in DL-TBOA required increasing the maximal conductance to produce a good fit, suggesting that DL-TBOA allows spillover onto additional receptors not activated when glutamate transporters are functioning.

Figure 1—figure supplement 3.

(A) In the control solution, the 3D diffusion equation was able to produce glutamate transients (top black trace) that evoked simulated EPSCs (bottom black trace) that fit the experimental data well (lower red trace). This example required 300 AMPA receptors (maximal conductance of synapse: 6141 pS) and an ambient glutamate concentration of 3.96 µM. (B) AMPAR currents recorded from the same cell as in A in 50 µM DL-TBOA were markedly different (lower red trace), with a much larger slow EPSC in amplitude and charge. In order to fit this current, the model required 500 receptors (maximal conductance of synapse: 10,033 pS), in addition to a 4.0-fold increase in the number of molecules released. Note that the first fast EPSC was much too large in the modeled current. The red arrowhead indicates the peak of the fast EPSC in the experimental data. A model incorporating changes in the amount of glutamate released across the train could improve the fit to the fast EPSCs.

To directly test for tonic AMPAR current, ON UBC responses were recorded in LY341495. DL-TBOA now caused an inward shift in holding current, blocked by the AMPAR antagonist GYKI53655 (GYKI, 50 µM) (Figure 1D–F). In addition to tonic inward AMPAR current, increased ambient glutamate caused three other effects, as previously described (Lu et al., 2017): (1) the steady-state inward current during synaptic stimulation decreased and in many cases shifted outward, as in the example in Figure 1F, (2) the slow EPSC, after stimulation lengthened in duration (Figure 1Fi), and (3) the fast (phasic) EPSC amplitudes were reduced (Figure 1Fii). Therefore, EAATs not only limit glutamate accumulation at rest and hasten its removal after release, but shape the time course and amplitudes of synaptic currents.

To address which subtypes of EAATs contribute to glutamate uptake at this synapse we pharmacologically isolated AMPAR current and utilized the differing affinities of EAAT1-3 for antagonists DL-TBOA and TFB-TBOA (Figure 1—figure supplement 1; Shimamoto et al., 1998; Shimamoto et al., 2004). A low concentration of TFB-TBOA, which blocks EAAT1-2, generated a tonic inward current (Figure 1—figure supplement 1A–C). Increasing the concentration to additionally block EAAT3 had no effect. The inward current induced by the drug was caused by an increase in ambient glutamate that activated AMPARs, as it was blocked by GYKI. By contrast, a low concentration of DL-TBOA, which blocks EAAT2-3, and a high concentration that additionally blocks EAAT1, both shifted the holding current inward (Figure 1—figure supplement 1D,E). Thus, both EAAT1 and EAAT2 contributed to glutamate uptake at this synapse.

Prediction of ambient and evoked glutamate concentration profiles

To explore how EAATs contribute to the generation of ultra-slow signaling through AMPARs at the UBC synapse, a 13-state kinetic model was constructed that had five closed states representing occupation of 0–4 binding sites (C0–C4) and transitions from these closed-bound states to four open (O1–O4) and four desensitized states (D1–D4) (Figure 1—figure supplement 2A). Rates were adjusted to give incomplete desensitization, such that receptors with three or four glutamates bound had lower equilibrium current than receptors with one or two glutamates bound (Figure 1—figure supplement 2—source data 1). The model generated currents (Figure 2B) that fit the experimental data from Lu et al., 2017 (dots on Figure 1—figure supplement 2C), reproducing the essential features of the steady-state dose–response curves (Figure 1—figure supplement 2C).

Figure 2. mGluR2-mediated tonic outward current in OFF UBCs, but not ON UBCs.

(A) Example bath application of mGluR2 antagonist LY341495 on OFF UBC (in the presence of 5 μM NBQX, 1 μM JNJ16259685). Synaptic stimulation (100 Hz, 20×, 10 V, 15 s intervals, indicated by dots below trace) evokes an outward current, blocked by LY341495. A ~20 pA standing outward current is also blocked. (B) UBCs identified as OFF UBCs based on synaptic stimulation or glutamate puff had an mGluR2 standing current, blocked by LY341495. Paired t-test, p<0.001, n = 22. (C) UBCs identified as ON UBCs based on synaptic stimulation or glutamate puff did not have an mGluR2 standing current. Paired t-test, p=0.243, n = 9. (D) Summary of mGluR2 standing currents. The tonic mGluR2 current was larger in OFF UBCs than ON UBCs. Student's t-test, p=0.021, n = 31. (E) Example OFF UBCs in current-clamp mode before and after LY341495, showing increase in spike rate after blocking tonic mGluR2-mediated current. (F) Summary showing significant increase in spike rate across OFF UBCs. Paired t-test, p=0.025, n = 5.

Figure 2.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1. Widely utilized mGluR1 antagonist LY367385 also acts as mGluR2 agonist.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1.

(A) Example OFF UBC responding to 50 ms puff of metabotropic glutamate receptor group II agonist LY354740 with a slow outward current (baseline – black). Administering mGluR1 antagonist LY367385 caused a large increase in outward holding current (to prevent hyperpolarization of the cell) and reduced the evoked outward current, presumably reaching a saturating level of activation (red). Subsequent addition of highly selective group II antagonist LY341495 revealed that the increase in holding outward current was due to activation of a group II receptor (mGluR2), as it reversed the LY354740 induced outward shift in the holding current. As expected, it also blocked the puff evoked outward mGluR2-mediated current (green). Right: −10 mV test pulse shows change in input resistance consistent with an increase in the number of open channels in LY367385 (red) that was reversed by LY341495 (green). (B) Example OFF UBC responding to 50 ms puff of metabotropic glutamate receptor group II agonist LY354740 with a slow outward current (black). This current was blocked by group II antagonist LY341495 (green). The effect of the mGluR1 antagonist LY367385 on the holding current was blocked by the presence of mGluR2 antagonist LY341495 (red). (C) In all UBCs tested, LY367385 increased the outward holding current and subsequent LY341495 reversed the effect. Paired t-tests, baseline vs LY367385, p=0.022, n = 5; LY367385 vs LY341495, p=0.011, n = 5. Right: When LY341495 was applied first, the effect of subsequent LY367385 was absent. (D) The outward current induced by LY367385 was dose dependent and apparent at micromolar concentrations. Right: Input resistance was reduced by LY367385 in a dose-dependent manner. LY367385 used in these studies were from different lots purchased from two different suppliers (Tocris and Abcam). Other drugs are known to act as agonists of group II and antagonists of group I metabotropic glutamate receptors including phenylglycine derivatives such as (S)−4-carboxyphenylglycine (Hayashi et al., 1994).
Figure 2—figure supplement 2. Spontaneous activity and ON and OFF responses in cell-attached recordings.

Figure 2—figure supplement 2.

(A) Top panel shows cell-attached recordings of OFF UBC with regular spontaneous firing; bottom panel shows a prolonged pause in firing in response to brief (15–40 ms) puff application of glutamate. (B) Summary of the firing frequency of OFF UBCs before and during the 2 s period immediately following glutamate puff. (C) Top three panels show the irregularity of spontaneous firing of ON UBCs; bottom panel shows a prolonged increase in firing in response to brief application of glutamate. (D) Summary of firing frequency of ON UBCs and the increase in frequency during the 2 s period following glutamate application. (E) To show the difference in regularity of firing, inter-spike interval (ISI) data of all N’s for ON (light gray) and OFF (dark gray) cells were pooled and normalized to maximum number of events. OFF cells show a narrow peak, while ON cells display a broad distribution indicative of a larger variation in ISIs. (F) Summary of the mean ISI of each subtype (OFF cells n = 8; ON cells n = 6) and (G) Graphic representation of the coefficient of variation of both ON and OFF cells. Black arrows in (A) and (C) show puff of 1 mM glutamate. Error bars show ± SEM, and significance-level symbols are as follows: ns, non-significant or (p>0.05), (*) (p≤0.05), (**) (p≤0.01), (***) (p≤0.001), (****) (p≤0.0001).
Figure 2—figure supplement 3. Tonic AMPAR current in ON UBCs, but not OFF UBCs.

Figure 2—figure supplement 3.

(A) Glutamate puffs indicated by dots below trace cause inward currents typical of an ON UBC. Application of NBQX shifted the holding current outward and then blocked the glutamate response. In some cases, ON or OFF UBCs were identified with synaptic stimulation instead of glutamate puffs. (B) There was no apparent effect of 5 µM NBQX (open circles) or 50 µM GYKI53655 (filled circles) on OFF UBC holding current. Thus, OFF UBCs had no tonic AMPAR inward current. Paired t-test, p=0.109, n = 10. (C) Small but significant effect of 5 µM NBQX (open circles) or 50 µM GYKI53655 (filled circles) on ON UBC holding current. Wilcoxon signed rank test, p=0.003, n = 16. This small tonic inward current was significant for either NBQX or GYKI53655 experiments analyzed separately as well. (D) ON UBCs had significantly different response to AMPAR antagonists compared to OFF UBCs, Mann–Whitney test, p=0.0006, n = 26. (E) In some experiments, NBQX reduced the spontaneous firing rate of ON UBCs. (F) This effect was not significant across the population, which was not surprising given the small size of the current.

The model was then used to predict the glutamate concentration at synaptic AMPARs receptors at rest and during synaptic transmission. Trains of glutamate transients were generated using a 3D diffusion equation (see Materials and methods; Balmer and Trussell, 2021) and applied to the model AMPARs. This equation was simpler to implement than sums of exponential decays; both approaches could account well for an initial fast decay of transmitter followed by a very slow decay expected from large synaptic spaces. The resulting current was compared to data recorded from UBCs, and parameters of the diffusion equation were then varied until an excellent fit between model and data was obtained for both fast and slow components of the synaptic response (Figure 1—figure supplement 2D). This approach revealed that the slow EPSC at the end of the train increased as the glutamate concentration at the receptors fell from >1 mM to the peak of the steady-state dose–response curve (32 µM), and then decayed slowly over hundreds of milliseconds as the concentration gradually decreased below 32 µM (Figure 1—figure supplement 2E). Importantly, the glutamate transients could not generate currents that fit the data recorded in the absence of EAAT blockers unless we incorporated a low level of ambient glutamate into the model (Figure 1—figure supplement 2F). The error values of the fits (square norm between the fit and the data) were reduced 44.5 ± 24.0% (mean ± SD) by the addition of the ambient glutamate parameter and produced currents that fit the data significantly better (paired t-test, p<0.001). Simulations lacking ambient glutamate consistently showed relatively small slow currents as compared to the experimental records, despite matching the dose–response for exogenous application of glutamate. The model predicts that in order for the characteristic slow EPSC to be produced, not only must glutamate persist at the synapse for hundreds of milliseconds after synaptic release, but a background level of ~5 µM glutamate must be present prior to synaptic stimulation (Figure 1—figure supplement 2G).

Having established a glutamate time course that accounts for control data, we then explored how the time course was changed by DL-TBOA. Synaptic responses were recorded in nine UBCs, before and after application of DL-TBOA. For all data, the model was first fit to control data to establish the glutamate transients predicted with EAATs active. Then, responses in DL-TBOA from these same cells were fit by varying only two parameters, the ambient glutamate concentration and the amount of glutamate released. It should be emphasized that the number of glutamate molecules ‘released’ refers to the number available for diffusion and binding to receptors; EAATs capable of rapidly buffering glutamate will reduce the effective amount of release, as previously described (Diamond and Jahr, 1997). For five of the cells, simply increasing ambient glutamate concentration and glutamate released generated simulated AMPAR currents that reproduced the major effects of DL-TBOA in our experiments, including the outward shift of the steady-state current during the stimulation train and a longer duration slow EPSC at the cessation of stimulation (Figure 1—figure supplement 2H,I). For these cases, ambient glutamate was increased 2.4-fold, from 4.7 ± 2.7 µM to 11.2 ± 7.6 µM (mean ± SD) and glutamate molecules released were increased 2.3-fold. In three other cells, increasing maximum conductance (i.e. number of receptors) 1.7 ± 0.5 fold (mean ± SD) was also required to modify the glutamate transient to produce a current that fit the DL-TBOA condition (Figure 1—figure supplement 3). This suggests that in some cases, glutamate released does not, under normal conditions, reach all receptors on the cell and that when EAATs are blocked, spillover to additional receptors may occur.

The model thus predicts that EAATs do not fully remove transmitter from the synapse at rest, but instead reduce ambient transmitter to a low micromolar level. Based on the estimate of 5 µM ambient glutamate, only 54.2% of AMPAR are free of agonist prior to spike-driven release from the MF terminal, with the remainder bound to at least one agonist molecule. Following exocytosis, EAATs restrict the amount of glutamate that reaches AMPARs. When EAATs are blocked, there is a large increase in ambient glutamate and in glutamate available from exocytosis.

Ambient glutamate causes a tonic outward current in OFF UBCs

The model predicted that micromolar levels of ambient glutamate persist and shape the synaptic currents in ON UBCs. This persistence of glutamate is surprising given that studies documented only low nanomolar levels at other synapses, due to the efficacy of EAAT activity (Herman and Jahr, 2007). We therefore tested the model’s prediction by investigating whether ambient glutamate is present at the UBC synapse in the absence of EAAT blockers. For OFF UBCs, where mGluR2 receptors activate outward currents, ambient glutamate would be expected if application of an mGluR2 antagonist shifted the holding current inward. To isolate the mGluR2-mediated current, these experiments were done in the presence of a selective non-competitive antagonist of mGluR1 (JNJ16259685). A commonly used antagonist of mGluR1 (LY367385) was not used, as it was found to additionally act as an agonist of mGluR2 (Figure 2—figure supplement 1).

Figure 2A shows a trace recorded from a voltage-clamped OFF UBC; outward deflections are inhibitory responses to MF stimulation. As expected, LY341495 not only blocked the synaptically evoked outward currents, but also shifted the holding current inward, consistent with blockade of a tonic outward current mediated by mGluR2 receptors (Figure 2A). Only OFF UBCs had such a tonic outward current (Figure 2B–D), despite both UBC subtypes’ expression of the inhibitory mGluR2 (Jaarsma et al., 1995), presumably because ON UBCs have significantly less mGluR2-GIRK-mediated current than OFF UBCs (Borges-Merjane and Trussell, 2015). Current clamp was then used to characterize the effects of tonic outward current on firing activity in OFF UBCs. UBCs are spontaneously active in vivo and in vitro, firing spikes without synaptic input (Kim et al., 2012; Ruigrok et al., 2011), and glutamate released from MF serves to either increase or decrease this spontaneous activity (Figure 2—figure supplement 2). In the presence of the mGluR2 antagonist, the frequency of spontaneous firing increased significantly (Figure 2E,F). Therefore, ambient glutamate at OFF UBC synapses tonically activates mGluR2 receptors and reduces spontaneous spike rate, presumably lowering transmitter release at UBC axon terminals onto granule cells.

Small tonic AMPAR currents in ON UBCs caused by ambient glutamate

We next investigated the effect of ambient glutamate on AMPARs. In the experiment with an ON UBC (Figure 2—figure supplement 3A), bath application of AMPAR antagonist NBQX blocked the evoked inward current and shifted the baseline current outward, consistent with blockade of a tonic inward current. In OFF UBCs, there was no effect of AMPAR antagonists on baseline current (Figure 2—figure supplement 3B). Across the population of ON UBCs, there was a small, but significant effect of the antagonist (Figure 2—figure supplement 3B,C). However, the illustrated example was an uncommonly large effect; the tonic AMPAR current was typically just a few picoamperes. This small current may be an underestimate, given that during these relatively long recordings UBCs typically become increasingly leaky, which would oppose the blocking effect of NBQX on a small inward current. However, in current-clamp recordings, the spike rate was not consistently affected by the AMPAR antagonist (Figure 2—figure supplement 3E,F), unlike the case for mGluR2 block in OFF UBCs. It is possible that ambient glutamate at the ON UBC synapse is low compared to OFF UBCs, despite the predictions of the model. Alternatively, however, it may be that micromolar levels of ambient glutamate were present, but did not conduct significant tonic inward current because of partial AMPAR desensitization.

AMPAR desensitization caused by ambient glutamate

Cyclothiazide (CTZ), an allosteric modulator of AMPARs that inhibits desensitization (Partin et al., 1993), was used to test whether ambient glutamate leads to tonic AMPAR desensitization. If true, CTZ application would shift the baseline current inward as receptors transition from the desensitized state to the open state. Indeed, in both ON and OFF UBCs, 100 µM CTZ caused a striking inward shift in baseline current that was blocked by NBQX (Figure 3A–C). The current was larger and noisier during CTZ than control and was blocked NBQX (Figure 4A), consistent with increased channel activity caused by CTZ relieving AMPARs from their desensitized state. CTZ may have presynaptic effects that could induce glutamate release (Diamond and Jahr, 1995). Accordingly, the same experiment was done with cerebellar granule cells, which receive the same MF inputs as UBCs (Balmer and Trussell, 2019). Despite sharing MF presynaptic input and having similar AMPAR subtypes (Kinney et al., 1997), an effect of CTZ on baseline current was not observed in granule cells (Figure 3D,E), and thus the effect of CTZ on UBCs is unlikely to be due to the drug’s effect on the MF terminal. The absence of an effect of CTZ on granule cells suggests that ambient glutamate is cell specific, rather than pervasive through the brain slice.

Figure 3. Tonic AMPAR desensitization in UBCs, but not granule cells.

Figure 3.

(A) 100 µM cyclothiazide (CTZ) bath application increases standing inward current of this OFF UBC and is blocked by 5 µM NBQX. Note increase in noise in CTZ, presumably due to increased AMPAR activity. (B) CTZ increased inward current in OFF UBCs, indicating ambient level of AMPAR desensitization. Wilcoxon signed rank: control vs CTZ, p=0.014, n = 16; paired t-test: CTZ vs CTZ+NBQX, p=0.025, n = 11. (C) CTZ increased inward current in ON UBCs. Paired t-tests: control vs CTZ, p=0.0001, n = 16; CTZ vs CTZ+NBQX, p=0.001, n = 12. (D) There was no significant effect of CTZ on the holding current of granule cells: Friedman test: p=0.264, n = 12. (E) The effect of CTZ on OFF and ON UBCs was not different, but both were significantly different than on GrCs. Mann–Whitney test: OFF UBCs vs GrCs, p<0.0001, n = 29; unpaired t-tests: ON UBCs vs GrCs, p=0.001, n = 29. OFF UBCs vs ON UBCs, p=0.809, n = 32. (F) ON UBC synaptic responses to 50 Hz, 10× electrical stimulation and response after CTZ. Right: EPSCs evoked during 50 Hz stimulation, magnified. The first EPSC became much larger, indicating that ambient glutamate desensitizes AMPARs. (G) CTZ increased the EPSC amplitude 2.3 ± 0.8 fold (mean ± SD), paired t-test, p=0.006, n = 8. The charge during the 50 Hz, 10× stimulation, and the long inward tail increased 9.3 ± 7.0 fold (mean ± SD). Paired t-test, p<0.0001, n = 8.

Figure 4. The source of ambient glutamate is not action potential-evoked release.

Figure 4.

(A) Example tonic currents in OFF UBC. DL-TBOA increased tonic outward current in all OFF UBCs. Addition of TTX did not reverse this effect. Addition of mGluR2 antagonist LY341495 (1 µM) blocked the outward current, shifting the tonic current inward. (B) Summary of the effect of each additional agent in seven OFF UBCs. On average, there was no effect of TTX on the tonic outward current of OFF UBCs. In some cases, GYKI53655 (50 µM) was applied before DL-TBOA (unfilled circles), but this had no effect and were therefore pooled with examples in the absence of GYKI53655 (filled circles). Paired t-tests: Baseline vs DL-TBOA, p=0.0011, n = 11; DL-TBOA vs DL-TBOA+TTX, p=0.674, n = 11; DL-TBOA+TTX vs DL-TBOA+TTX+LY341495, p=0.0013, n = 9. (C) Example tonic currents in ON UBC. After determination of ON or OFF UBC type by synaptic stimulation or glutamate puff, LY341495 was added before DL-TBOA. DL-TBOA increased tonic inward current. TTX reduced inward current. GYKI further reduced the inward current. (D) Summary of the effect of agents in seven ON UBCs. Paired t-tests: baseline vs DL-TBOA, p=0.033, n = 7; DL-TBOA vs DL-TBOA+TTX, p=0.004, n = 7; DL-TBOA+TTX vs DL-TBOA+TTX+GYKI53655, p=0.002. (E) To test whether TTX was blocking a persistent sodium current that is known to be present in UBCs, GYKI was applied first. Any effect of TTX after GYKI was interpreted as block of persistent sodium current. This example ON UBC had a persistent sodium current that was blocked. (F) Summary of the effect of GYKI in the presence or absence of TTX. GYKI blocked the same amount of tonic AMPA current whether or not action potentials were blocked by TTX, indicating that the sole effect of TTX was to block a persistent Na+ current. Unpaired t-test: p=0.486, n = 21. (G) TTX had little effect on the response of OFF UBCs to 10 ms 1 mM glutamate puffs. Right- average response before (black) and after (gray) TTX. (H) In the ON UBC example, TTX blocked a small tonic inward current but had little effect on the 10 ms 1 mM glutamate puff-evoked current. Right: Average response before (black) and after TTX (gray).

If ambient glutamate desensitized AMPARs, it might also reduce synaptically evoked EPSCs. Indeed, synaptic currents in UBCs were significantly larger after relief of desensitization by CTZ, in some cases strikingly so (Figure 3F–G). On average, the amplitude of the first EPSC in the train doubled, and the charge evoked by the train increased ninefold, principally due to increase in current between each EPSC (Figure 3F), and the decay constant of the slow EPSC lengthened from 265 ± 72 ms to 416 ± 110 ms (mean ± SD, paired t-test, p=0.002, n = 6). The broadened EPSCs evoked by a brief stimulation indicates that elevated glutamate persists at the synapse for seconds, but does not typically evoke a current because AMPARs return to their tonic desensitized state. The slow decay in CTZ is consistent with the prolonged time course for recovery from AMPAR desensitization following transmitter release at the UBC synapse, assayed with either paired synaptic stimuli or glutamate uncaging (Lu et al., 2017), confirming that glutamate lingers in the synaptic cleft for hundreds of milliseconds. Thus, tonic desensitization by ambient glutamate constrains the amplitude and duration of synaptic excitation in UBCs.

The source of ambient glutamate is not action potential-evoked exocytosis

What is the source of ambient glutamate at the MF–UBC synapse? Spontaneous firing of MFs only rarely occurs in acute slices, so action potential-evoked release of glutamate from the MF input to the UBC can be ruled out as the source of ambient glutamate. To test whether spillover into the UBC brush from other spontaneously active neurons in the slice contributes to the tonic glutamate current, TTX was used to block action potential-evoked transmitter release, with the expectation that this manipulation should then reduce the size of the tonic current. TBOA was first applied to increase the ambient glutamate to enhance sensitivity of the test. These experiments were conducted in the presence of strychnine, SR99531, JNJ16259685, and MK-801 to isolate mGluR2 and AMPAR currents.

In OFF UBCs, application of 1 µM TTX had no effect on the tonic current, and 1 µm LY341495 blocked an ambient mGluR2 current, similar to its effects in controls (Figure 4A,B). Thus, the tonic current in OFF UBCs is not dependent of action potential firing. By contrast, in ON UBCs, TTX did block a tonic inward current (Figure 4C,D). However, UBCs have a persistent Na+ current that could underlie the effect of TTX (Afshari et al., 2004; Russo et al., 2007). GYKI53655 (50 µM) was initially applied to block AMPARs prior to applying TTX in order to test whether TTX had a direct effect on a Na+ channel conducting a tonic inward current. We found that application of TTX blocked an inward current whose amplitude was the same whether or not GYKI was present (Figure 4E,F), indicating that the sole effect of TTX was to block a persistent Na+ current. Moreover, TTX had little effect on glutamate puff-evoked mGluR2 currents in OFF UBCs (Figure 4G) or AMPAR currents in ON UBCs (Figure 4H). Taken together, these results indicate that ambient glutamate is specific to the UBC and not to neighboring granule cells and is independent of action potential firing in the slice. A likely source for this glutamate is therefore from spontaneous exocytic events.

Spontaneous exocytosis is a source of ambient glutamate

Spike-independent, spontaneous exocytosis of glutamate generates miniature excitatory synaptic currents (mEPSCs) in most central neurons. UBCs were recorded in the presence of TTX in order to isolate mEPSCs. mEPSCs in UBCs were rare and unusually small and slow (Figure 5A,B). Five examples are shown in comparison to a typical granule cell mEPSC (Figure 5B). mEPSCs were confirmed to be AMPAR-mediated because they were blocked by AMPAR antagonists GYKI or NBQX (n = 6) (Figure 5C). When mEPSCs were present in voltage clamp, their block with GYKI reduced spontaneous EPSPs measured in current clamp (Figure 5D). UBC mEPSC amplitudes were 7.20 ± 3.47 pA (mean ± SD, n = 11), less than half the size of mEPSCs recorded from granule cells under the same conditions (18.96 pA ±11.95 pA, n = 8; UBC vs GrC, Mann–Whitney test, p=0.0008, Figure 5E). Double exponential functions were fit to the mEPSCs to characterize the decay time of the currents. Both the fast and slow time constants of the fits to mEPSCs from UBCs were over 10-fold longer than those of GrCs (fast tau, UBC: 4.76 ± 5.11 ms, GrC: 0.41 ± 0.07 ms; slow tau, UBC: 57.23 ± 53.55 ms, GrC: 3.54 ± 1.40 ms. Mann–Whitney test, fast tau, p<0.0001, Mann–Whitney test, slow tau, p=0.0008, Figure 5E). In cases where synaptic stimulation was used to identify ON or OFF subtype, only ON UBCs had mEPSCs. The paucity of these events is consistent with the hypothesis that many AMPARs at the ON UBC synapse are displaced from sites of exocytosis, and only sense glutamate from spillover between sites (Zampini et al., 2016). Nevertheless, these results confirm that such spontaneous exocytosis occurs.

Figure 5. mEPSCs suggest vesicular source of ambient glutamate.

Figure 5.

(A) In the presence of TTX, 4 of 15 UBC had mEPSCs that were confidently resolvable above the noise. Each trace is a different cell. Traces are low-pass filtered at 100 Hz, which had a small effect on the amplitude, but was necessary to resolve mEPSCs. (B) Example minis indicated by numbers in (A). Traces were filtered 2.5 kHz and smoothed (10-point binomial smoothing) to avoid distortion of the peak of the mEPSCs. Note the long duration relative to the granule cell example recorded and filtered in the same way. (C) mEPSCs (left) are blocked by GYKI (50 µM) (right). (D) The same cell as in (D) in current clamp. EPSPs that occasionally reached spike threshold (left) were blocked by GYKI (right). (E) mEPSCs from UBCs were smaller in amplitude and slower to decay, quantified by the time constants of double exponential fits. Mann–Whitney tests, n = 11 UBCs, n = 8 GrCs.

We tested the hypothesis that vesicular glutamate is the source of glutamate that generates the tonic current by depleting vesicles of glutamate using bafilomycin-A1 (BafA1, 2 µM), which inhibits vacuolar H+ ATPase, preventing loading of synaptic vesicles (Cavelier and Attwell, 2007; Crider et al., 1994). After slice preparation, the first slice was used for control recordings, while subsequent slices were incubated in 2 µM BafA1 for >1 hr. Because mEPSCs are small in UBCs, the large, frequent mEPSCs of molecular layer stellate cells (Carter and Regehr, 2002) were used as positive controls to verify that the BafA1 treatment reduces vesicular glutamate (Figure 6—figure supplement 1).

We focused on the tonic inward current generated by AMPAR, as block of desensitization with CTZ would result in a reliable increase in inward tonic current, with the prediction that the enhanced current induced by CTZ would be abolished if vesicular glutamate concentration was depleted. CTZ (100 μM) was applied to controls slices and led to a significant increase in the tonic inward current (−24.3 ± 5.1 pA; p=0.01, n = 12, Figure 6A,B), as predicted. Subsequent application of the AMPAR antagonist NBQX (5 µM) blocked the enhanced current. Upon repeating this procedure in ON UBCs in slices incubated in 2 μM BafA1, CTZ still had an effect (−4.16 ± 1.64; p=0.016, n = 13), but nearly sixfold smaller (effect of CTZ in control vs BafA1, p=0.002; n = 25, Figure 6C). Thus, the majority of the glutamate driving the tonic current is from synaptic vesicles, likely from the MF terminal.

Figure 6. Depletion of glutamate from synaptic vesicles confirms vesicular source of ambient glutamate.

(A) Example recordings of the baseline current of an ON UBC in control (left panel) and of an ON UBC after BafA1 treatment (right), showing the decrease in the current revealed by 100 µM CTZ after treatment with BafA1, with subsequent block by 5 µM NBQX. (B) Left: After incubation with ACSF as a control, CTZ increased the inward holding current significantly, which was reversed by NBQX. Friedman test, p<0.0001, n = 12; Dunn's multiple comparisons tests, baseline vs CTZ, p=0.01, CTZ vs CTZ+NBQX, p<0.001. Right- Incubation in BafA1 reduced the effect of CTZ. Friedman test, p=0.0169, n = 12; Dunn's multiple comparisons tests, Baseline vs CTZ, p=0.459; CTZ vs CTZ+NBQX, p=0.0128. (C) The effect of CTZ was significantly reduced by incubation in BafA1, unpaired t-test, p=0.002, n = 25.

Figure 6.

Figure 6—figure supplement 1. Control for recordings in the presence of bafilomycin A1.

Figure 6—figure supplement 1.

(A) Example traces of a control stellate cell with evoked (left) and spontaneous (right) EPSCs. (B) Example traces from a stellate cell after BafA1 treatment and reduction of vesicular glutamate concentration. Evoked EPSCs and spontaneous EPSCs were absent. Far right trace shows a reliable response to puff application of glutamate demonstrating that postsynaptic receptors were present and functioning. Stellate cells (n = 3) were filled with Alexa 488, and the dendrite locations were easily identified by fluorescence for targeted puff. Black arrows in (A) and (B) show onset of a single electrical stimulus and in (B) black arrowhead indicates onset of a brief puff (10–15 ms) of glutamate. Stimulus artifact was removed for clarity.

Control of synaptic integration by EAATs

UBCs typically respond with an all-or-nothing synaptic response upon electrical stimulation of the white matter of lobe X, despite synchronously activating numerous MF axons, suggesting that a single MF signal is processed by each UBC. What prevents UBCs from sensing nearby MF inputs? In order to address this question, we examined the effect of EAAT blockade on the number of synaptic inputs, as measured by changes in response amplitude following increments in stimulus voltage. A challenge in the experiment is that slow ON or OFF currents are best detected after train stimulation, and at intermediate stimulus voltages, each axon might not be consistently activated during the train. Therefore, single stimuli were delivered after broadening presynaptic action potentials and increasing transmitter release with 1 mM TEA in the bath to block a portion of voltage-gated K-channels (Ritzau-Jost et al., 2014). To focus on the response of AMPARs, all other synaptic receptors present in UBCs were blocked and QX314 was added to the pipette solution to prevent postsynaptic spikes.

Of eight UBCs, seven had only a single, sharp increment in EPSC amplitude upon increase in stimulation intensity, suggesting that most ON UBCs only respond to the glutamate released from a single MF (Figure 7A). In five of seven UBCs, DL-TBOA revealed a slow spillover EPSC at stimulation intensities below threshold for the fast EPSC that was not apparent before blocking glutamate removal by EAATs (Figure 7B,C). After adding DL-TBOA, not only was the response tremendously prolonged, but also the majority of this slow component could be evoked without an accompanying fast EPSC, suggesting that in DL-TBOA, glutamate from a second, more distant MF activates AMPAR on this UBC. Thus, EAATs isolate UBCs from spillover from neighboring MF terminals (Figure 7D). By extension, it can be inferred that ambient glutamate is both produced and sensed by a given terminal and is restricted to that synaptic region by EAATs.

Figure 7. EAATs regulate synaptic integration by UBCs.

Figure 7.

(A) Example of AMPAR-mediated EPSC in 1 mM TEA. Top: Single EPSC size (all-or-nothing) at the first stimulation intensity that evoked a current (black). Two stimulation intensities below threshold (light and dark gray) had no EPSC response. Bottom: The same trace with a longer timebase showing the all-or-nothing slow EPSC that occurred at the same stimulation intensity as the fast EPSC. (B) Top: Application of DL-TBOA to the same cell as in (A) reveals slow EPSCs at stimulation intensities below the level that evoked the fast EPSC (red). Note the reduction in EPSC amplitude due to tonic desensitization. Bottom: In DL-TBOA, the slow EPSCs are much longer than in TEA alone. Without DL-TBOA, only 1 of 11 cells had multiple EPSC levels. In DL-TBOA, five of seven cells had at least two different EPSC sizes, indicating that EAATs prevent AMPARs from detecting multiple mossy fibers during strong synaptic stimulation. (C) Top: Fast EPSC amplitudes (relative to the maximal fast EPSC under the same bath conditions) plotted as a function of synaptic stimulation intensity (relative to the lowest intensity that evoked the fast EPSC under the same bath conditions). Both before and after DL-TBOA, the fast EPSC is all-or-nothing. Bottom: Same as above, but showing slow EPSCs. In DL-TBOA, there are multiple amplitude levels. The slow EPSC amplitudes are relative to the maximal slow EPSC in the same bath conditions and the x-axis is relative to the lowest stimulation intensity that evoked the maximal slow EPSC. Error bars mean and SEM. Pink and gray lines are individual experiments. (D) Interpretation of these results. In control conditions (left) glutamate is released from the mossy fiber (MF), detected by the receptors on the UBC, then removed from the synaptic cleft by astrocytes (A). When glutamate transporters are inhibited, not only does ambient glutamate build up (blue background), but glutamate released from neighboring synapses is sensed and integrated as slow rising and slow decaying synaptic currents. Thus, glutamate transporters prevent cross-talk between glomeruli and therefore underlie the ability of glomeruli to function as discrete processing units.

EAAT activity determines the phase delay of the spiking response

The vestibular nerve projecting to lobe X terminates as primary afferent MFs which synapse onto ON UBCs and granule cells (Balmer and Trussell, 2019). These MFs fire spontaneously in vivo, and their firing rate is modulated by head motion (Goldberg and Fernandez, 1971a; Goldberg and Fernandez, 1971b). We developed a stimulation protocol based on Zampini et al., 2016 to test how ON UBCs respond to realistic synaptic input patterns and how glutamate accumulation could affect the resulting spiking responses. After a 10 s, 26 Hz conditioning stimulus, MFs were stimulated at sinusoidally modulated frequencies that approximated the signals they would likely receive during head movement in one direction and then back, with a 1 s cycle (Figure 8A). Only cells that responded to every synaptic stimulation without failures were used.

Figure 8. Ambient glutamate determines the phase of the spiking response to frequency-modulated synaptic stimulation.

Figure 8.

(A) Example of 1 Hz frequency-modulated synaptic stimulation protocol. Five seconds of no stimulation, followed by 10 s, 26 Hz stimulation, followed by 10 s of modulation with a 40 Hz depth at a modulation rate of 1 Hz. Raster plot shows time of stimuli plotted against instantaneous rate. Below is the stimulus train that was delivered to white matter. Evoked currents are shown for baseline condition (black) and after bath application of DL-TBOA in the same cell (red). DL-TBOA increased inward holding current. Note that during the modulated stimulation the evoked outward current does not go beyond the baseline holding current. Spike trains recorded in current clamp are shown below. Bias current was applied to offset the tonic currents induced by DL-TBOA and to maintain the resting membrane potential at −70 mV. The phase of spiking during the modulated stimulation shifts after block of EAATs. (B) Expanded view of the first five pulses of the 26 Hz stimulation. Currents are baseline subtracted. In DL-TBOA, the first EPSC in the train is smaller, indicating tonic desensitization of AMPARs by ambient glutamate. Below, current clamp shows delayed spiking in presence of DL-TBOA. (C) Expanded view of two cycles of the frequency modulated stimulation. The currents are zeroed right before the increase in stimulation rate. In control, the currents remain net inward during the increased rate of stimulation. In DL-TBOA, desensitization caused a net outward current during the increased stimulation rate. Below, in current clamp, in control conditions, the highest rate of synaptic stimulation caused spiking. In DL-TBOA, the highest rate of stimulation caused hyperpolarization that causes the cell to stop spiking. (D) Raster plot showing the application of DL-TBOA starting at sweep 6 (red). Note the shift in timing of the spikes. (E) PSTHs during the frequency modulated stimulation were made over several sweeps and cycles. Sine waves were fit to the PSTHs, and their period was used to calculate the phase shift caused by DL-TBOA. In this example the UBC fired during increasing and decreasing stimulus rates. In DL-TBOA, the peak of firing was shifted towards 270° and was continuous from the offset of stimulation until the stimulation rate increases. (F) In all cases, DL-TBOA shifted the offset firing response toward the lower stimulation rates, shifting the phase later. The phase of the offset response in control ACSF is represented as a black dot and the phase of the offset response in DL-TBOA of the same ON UBC is represented by the red arrowhead. Firing rate is indicated on the radius. The curves between control and DL-TBOA are arbitrary. These experiments included 5 µM MK-801 in addition to strychnine and SR-95531.

Figure 8—source code 1. Python script to produce a sinusoidally modulated pattern of TTL pulses that was used to approximate in vivo-like activity patterns.
The script creates a text file that can be imported into pClamp and used as a stimulus output pattern.

EPSCs in control artificial cerebral spinal fluid (ACSF) (black) showed synaptic depression and a steady-state inward current during the 26 Hz stimulus (Figure 8A,B). During frequency-modulated stimulation, EPSCs were apparent during the higher frequency part of the cycle, and these recovered to baseline during the lower frequency part of the cycle (Figure 8C). DL-TBOA was applied to increase ambient glutamate by blocking EAATs. After application of DL-TBOA (red), the holding current shifted inward, due to increased tonic AMPAR current (Figure 8A). During the 26 Hz stimulus, the steady-state current depressed to a level that was slightly outward relative to pre-stimulus baseline current (Figure 8B). During the frequency-modulated stimuli, the current in DL-TBOA was further desensitized, becoming outward during the highest frequencies of the cycle.

In current clamp, DL-TBOA shifted the timing of spiking during the modulated frequency stimulation. In the illustrated example in control ACSF, the cell fired at the beginning of each cycle after the first couple stimuli, slowed during the fastest stimulus rates, and then began firing again as the rate decreased in an offset response (Figure 8C,D). In DL-TBOA, the cessation of firing during the frequency increase was nearly complete, picking up markedly when the frequency was lowest (Figure 8C,D). To quantify the phase of the offset response, sine wave functions were fit to peristimulus time histograms (PSTHs) generated over cycles of the frequency modulated stimulus (Figure 8E). The peak rate of the stimulus was defined as 90°, and the slowest rate was 270°. In control conditions, ON UBCs typically fired both at onset and offset of the stimulus cycle. Because there were two response periods per stimulus cycle, the onset and offset, the sine function fit to the PSTH in control conditions had close to two cycles per stimulus period (1.70 ± 0.29 cycles [mean ± SD]; note the two peaks in the fit to the control PSTH in Figure 8E). In DL-TBOA, the sine function fit had closer to one cycle per stimulus period (1.10 ± 0.13 cycles), a significant change due to the cell firing during the entire slow stimulation period (paired t-test, p<0.0001, n = 9). Moreover, the peak of the offset response shifted toward the lowest stimulation rate (control: 205.9 ± 15.6° [mean ± SD], DL-TBOA: 282.9 ± 21.5°, paired t-test, p<0.0001, n = 9, Figure 8F). This mean 77° shift represents a 214 ms delay. Thus, EAATs controlled the timing of the offset responses of ON UBCs.

Discussion

Role of synaptic structure in generating tonic currents and shaping evoked currents

MF terminals form glomerular synapses with dendrites of granule cells, UBCs, and Golgi cells. Glomerular structure and relatively low expression of EAATs allow glutamate spillover in granule cells (DiGregorio et al., 2002), slow-rising AMPAR EPSCs (Nielsen et al., 2004), and desensitization of AMPARs (DiGregorio et al., 2007; Xu-Friedman and Regehr, 2003). However, the anatomical and molecular composition of glomeruli differs among target cells, being particularly suited to glutamate accumulation in UBCs. For example, very slow AMPAR EPSCs are a distinctive feature of UBCs as opposed to other MF targets (Lu et al., 2017). Accordingly, we observed the tonic glutamate current in UBCs, but not in granule cells. Even mEPSCs were smaller and slower in UBCs vs granule cells. One possibility is that the ultrastructure of synapses formed between MF terminals and granule cells vs UBCs might underlie the observed physiological differences. Electron microscopy reveals very different ultrastructure of these synaptic contacts. The distance between the release sites and the postsynaptic receptors of UBCs may also be greater than granule cells (Diño et al., 2000a; Diño et al., 2000b; Rossi et al., 1995; Xu-Friedman and Regehr, 2003). The location of glial cell processes that position EAATs near synapses must almost certainly differ between the cell types, given their different dendritic morphologies. Finally, diffusion barriers specific to the convolutions of the dendritic brush or to extracellular matrix molecules could slow diffusion and increase ambient glutamate.

It is surprising that, despite having enormous postsynaptic densities, UBCs have small-amplitude MF-evoked EPSCs (Mugnaini et al., 1994). Our data reveal that one reason for small EPSCs is tonic desensitization caused by ambient glutamate. Relieving desensitization with CTZ increased the fast EPSC amplitude more than twofold. Notably, CTZ also causes an increase in the AMPAR-mediated EPSC of granule cells, without causing a tonic inward current and this difference may be due to the UBC having a dendritic structure that is more restrictive to glutamate diffusion than the granule cell dendrites.

MFs as the source of ambient glutamate

In the cerebellar granular layer, the only glutamatergic axon terminals are MFs (whether extrinsic or ‘intrinsic’ MF terminals of the UBCs themselves). The source of ambient glutamate can be most clearly attributed to MF release in experiments where mEPSCs were detected, as they are clearly the result of vesicles being released directly onto postsynaptic AMPA receptors of the recorded UBC. Exocytic glutamate release from astrocytes remains a possibility, although a controversial one (Bazargani and Attwell, 2016). Postsynaptic release of glutamate from the dendrites of UBCs has not been previously described, although the presence synaptic vesicles in UBC dendrites has been reported (Mugnaini et al., 1994), and if functional, could provide an additional minor source of glutamate. It should be noted that in our recordings the postsynaptic cell was dialyzed with a glutamate-free solution, which may limit availability of postsynaptic glutamate for vesicles. For these reasons, we favor the simplest interpretation that MFs are the main source of glutamate.

What concentration of glutamate produces tonic current?

Glutamate receptors vary in their affinities: mGluRs can be activated by relatively low concentrations of glutamate (low micromolar range) (Conn and Pin, 1997) and AMPARs, which mediate the majority of phasic excitatory transmission in the CNS, require higher concentrations for maximal activation (~32 μM) (Lu et al., 2017; Raman and Trussell, 1992). Consistent with their high affinity for glutamate, mGluR2 were tonically activated by ambient glutamate. Indeed, we found that OFF UBCs, whose synaptic responses are dominated by mGluR2 currents, showed tonic inhibition by ambient glutamate.

By contrast, AMPARs desensitize and therefore contribute less to tonic current. ON UBCs had a ~3 pA AMPAR-mediated tonic current, consistent with a ~5 µM steady-state glutamate concentration predicted by our model. Notably, ON UBCs receive primary vestibular afferents that fire constantly at high rates (Balmer and Trussell, 2019; Goldberg and Fernandez, 1971b). Thus, with a higher level of MF activity, the ambient glutamate concentration may be even higher in vivo.

Graded transmission

Together with previous studies, our current results suggest that the MF to ON UBC synapse operates quite differently from classical glutamatergic synapses with respect to the buildup and removal of glutamate during synaptic transmission. At conventional glutamatergic synapses, exocytosis raises glutamate levels in the cleft rapidly, and this elevated glutamate is quickly removed by diffusion and uptake or buffering by EAATs (Clements et al., 1992; Diamond and Jahr, 1997). Thus, the typical postsynaptic cell integrates these extremely brief chemical events to produce stable ongoing responses by generating long-lasting synaptic currents or potentials. As with most synapses, action potentials at presynaptic MF terminals elicit exocytosis and a sharp rise in glutamate at subsynaptic membrane. However, several features of the ON UBC synapse lead to an AMPAR response dictated more by slowly changing levels of transmitter than by postsynaptic integration of brief pulses of glutamate. These features are as follows: (1) persistence of transmitter, (2) receptors not localized immediately beneath the release sites (Zampini et al., 2016), (3) cleft structure and EAAT localization favoring accumulation of glutamate and its gradual removal, (4) the entry and recovery of AMPARs from desensitization tracks nonlinearly these gradual changes in transmitter. Thus, postsynaptic currents slowly increase and decay in proportion to changes in the frequency of presynaptic spikes in a nonlinear manner that is governed in part by the activity of EAATs.

Astrocytes may control the excitability of UBCs by regulating EAATs

Control of ambient glutamate would be expected to profoundly regulate synaptic transmission. One such control point could be EAATs, which are extremely dense on the processes of astrocytes near synapses (Tzingounis and Wadiche, 2007). EAAT1–2 contribute to the uptake of glutamate at the UBC synapse, and both are presumed to be expressed by glial cells and not neurons (Rothstein et al., 1994). The MF-UBC synapse is exquisitely sensitive to levels of ambient glutamate dictated by EAAT function. By controlling the density or activity of EAATs, astrocytes could regulate both tonic and phasic glutamate currents at this synapse. Astrocytic regulation of EAATs could be a mechanism by which glial cells influence synaptic transmission that does not require release of a transmitter.

Our study also highlighted an additional important role of EAATs besides limiting ambient glutamate and controlling decay time of synaptic responses. In the absence of EAAT activity, spillover between nerve terminals was great enough to produce ‘extra’ EPSCs in a given neuron. While each UBC had a primary, fast, and slow EPSC from its innervating mossy terminal, blockade of EAATs resulted in additional slow EPSCs likely from transmitter released from terminals on neighboring UBCs or granule cells (Figure 7). These data suggest the possibility that regulation of EAAT expression might alter the effective convergence ratio to each UBC, that is the number of axons producing functional postsynaptic responses for that cell. Such changes could alter multimodal integration of signals in the cerebellum, dependent on the information carried by adjacent nerve terminals.

It should be noted that the data presented here that demonstrate the influence of ambient glutamate on synaptic integration and spiking activity utilized the ability to experimentally increase ambient glutamate. To further explore the role of ambient glutamate at this synapse, technical advances that would allow the experimental reduction of ambient glutamate are warranted, such as over-expression of glutamate transporters or buffers.

Responses of UBCs to vestibular-like synaptic input

One can predict how UBCs might respond to head movement by examining their firing response to synaptic input that mimics in vivo MF activity. MFs code the velocity of back-and-forth head movements by increasing and decreasing firing rate (Arenz et al., 2008; Goldberg and Fernandez, 1971a). We find that ON UBCs increase their firing in response to both increases and decreases of the rate of synaptic input and therefore may code for velocity of the head in either direction. In contrast, granule cells receiving the same input would be expected to maintain the velocity signal by increasing and decreasing firing along with the changing rate of the input MF. In addition, the offset response of ON UBCs, which is sensitive to EAAT activity, may delay the parallel fiber vestibular signals in order to coordinate their activity with delayed climbing fiber error signals (Raymond and Lisberger, 1998; Suvrathan et al., 2016).

Materials and methods

Key resources table.

Reagent type (species) or resource Designation Source or reference Identifiers Additional information
Genetic reagent (M. musculus) C57BL/6J Jackson Laboratory RRID: IMSR_JAX:000664
Genetic reagent (M. musculus) B6.TgN(grm2-IL2RA/GFP)1kyo Dr. Robert Duvoisin (OHSU)
PMID:9778244
RBRC: RBRC01194
Chemical compound, drug Alexa Fluor 488 hydrazide sodium salt ThermoFisher Scientific Cat # A10436
Chemical compound, drug Alexa Fluor 594 hydrazide sodium salt ThermoFisher Scientific Cat # A10438
Chemical compound, drug GYKI-53655 Tocris Cat # 2555
Chemical compound, drug JNJ-16259685 Tocris Cat # 2333
Chemical compound, drug LY-341495 Tocris Cat # 1209
Chemical compound, drug (+)-MK-801 hydrogen maleate Sigma Cat # M107
Chemical compound, drug (R)-CPP Abcam Cat # ab120159
Chemical compound, drug Strychnine hydrochloride Sigma Cat # S8753
Chemical compound, drug SR-95531 hydrobromide Tocris Cat # 1262
Chemical compound, drug Tetraethylammonium chloride Sigma Cat # 86614
Chemical compound, drug NBQX disodium salt Abcam Cat # ab120046
Chemical compound, drug Cyclothiazide Tocris Cat # 0713
Chemical compound, drug Bafilomycin A1 InvivoGen Cat # tlrl-baf1
Chemical compound, drug DL-TBOA Tocris Cat # 1223
Chemical compound, drug TFB-TBOA Tocris Cat # 2532
Chemical compound, drug Tetrodotoxin citrate Tocris Cat # 1069
Software, algorithm pClamp 10 Molecular Devices RRID:SCR_011323
Software, algorithm Igor Pro 8 WaveMetrics RRID:SCR_000325
Software, algorithm Prism 8 GraphPad RRID:SCR_002798
Software, algorithm Excel Microsoft RRID:SCR_016137
Software, algorithm Axograph X Axograph RRID:SCR_014284
Software, algorithm NEURON PMID:9248061 RRID:SCR_005393
Software, algorithm Affinity Designer Serif RRID:SCR_016952
Software, algorithm Python python.org RRID:SCR_008394
Software, algorithm NumPy numpy.org RRID:SCR_008633
Software, algorithm MatPlotLib matplotlib.org RRID:SCR_008624

Animals

Animals used in this study were primarily C57BL/6J-TgN(grm2-IL2RA/GFP)1kyo line. In this mouse line, GFP is tagged to human interleukin-2 receptor α subunit with expression driven by an mGluR2 promoter (Watanabe et al., 1998). Both ON and OFF UBCs express mGluR2, thus both subsets are labeled in this line (Nunzi et al., 2002), with GFP targeted to the plasma membrane. They were bred in a colony maintained in the animal facility managed by the Department of Comparative Medicine, and all procedures were approved by the Oregon Health and Science University's Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee. All experiments were performed in brain sections from males and females, postnatal days 21–35 (P21–P35). Synapse formation between the presynaptic MF terminal and the postsynaptic dendritic brush structure of the UBCs is mature in animals older than P21 (Hámori and Somogyi, 1983; Morin et al., 2001). Transgenic mice were phenotyped by light at P0–P3.

Brain slice preparation

Animals were anesthetized with isoflurane, decapitated, and the cerebellum was dissected from the skull under ice-cold high-sucrose ACSF solution containing the following (in mM): 87 NaCl, 75 sucrose, 25 NaHCO3, 25 glucose, 2.5 KCl, 1.25 NaH2PO4, 0.4 Na-ascorbate, 2 Na-pyruvate, 0.5 CaCl2, 7 MgCl2, and 5 μm R-CPP, bubbled with 5% CO2/95% O2 (Bischofberger et al., 2006). Parasagittal cerebellum sections 300 μm thick were cut with a vibratome (VT1200S, Leica or 7000smz-2, Campden Instruments) in ice-cold high-sucrose ACSF. Slices recovered at 35°C for 30–40 min, in ACSF containing the following (in mM): 130 NaCl, 2.1 KCl, 1.2 KH2PO4, 3 Na-HEPES, 10 glucose, 20 NaHCO3, 0.4 Na-ascorbate, 2 Na-pyruvate, 2 CaCl2, 1 MgSO4, bubbled with 5% CO2/95% O(300–305 mOsm). In the experiments that attempted to approximate in vivo-like stimulation, 1.2 mM Ca was used. In some experiments, 5 μm R-CPP or MK-801 was included in high-sucrose ACSF. Slices were kept at room temperature (~23°C) until recording. Recordings were performed from cerebellar lobe X, within 6 hr of preparation.

Electrophysiological recordings

During recordings, slices were perfused with recording ACSF using a peristaltic pump (Ismatec) at 2–3 ml/min and maintained at ~34°C with an inline heater (SH-27B, Warner Instruments). All experiments were done in the presence of 5 μM SR-95531 and 0.5 μM strychnine. The recording set up was composed of a Zeiss Axioskop 2 FS Plus microscope with Dodt gradient contrast optics (Dodt et al., 2002), with 10× and 60× (water immersion) Olympus objectives. UBCs were initially identified by soma size or GFP fluorescence, but intracellular recording solution always contained 5 μM Alexa Fluor 594 hydrazide sodium salt (Life Technologies). Thus, in whole-cell mode, UBCs were easily identifiable by morphology after dye dialysis. Patch electrodes were pulled with borosilicate glass capillaries (OD 1.2 mm and ID 0.68 mm, World Precision Instruments), with an upright puller (PC10, Narishige).

For whole-cell recordings, intracellular recording solution contained (mM): 113 K-gluconate, 9 HEPES, 4.5 MgCl2, 0.1 EGTA, 14 Tris–phosphocreatine, 4 Na2-ATP, 0.3 Tris–GTP (adjusted to 290 mOsm with sucrose), pH 7.2–7.25. All recordings were corrected for a −10 mV junction potential. For data acquisition, we used a Multiclamp 700B amplifier and pClamp nine software (Molecular Devices). Signals were sampled at 20–50 kHz using a Digidata (1440A, Molecular Devices) analog–digital converter board. Current signals in voltage clamp were acquired with 5–10× gain and low-pass filtered at 10 kHz, with further filtering applied offline. Stimulation artifacts have been removed for clarity. Patch pipette tip resistance was 5–8 MΩ; series resistance was compensated with correction 20–40% and prediction 50–70%, bandwidth 2 kHz. Membrane potential was held constant at −70 mV in voltage-clamp experiments.

Electrical stimulation was performed using a concentric bipolar electrode (CBBPC75, FHC) placed in the white matter of the sagittal cerebellar slice. Some fraction of synaptic responses may be due to the activation of MF terminals of UBCs’ axons, in addition to the extrinsic MFs that make up the majority of the white matter. Stimuli were evoked using a stimulus isolation unit (Iso Flex, A.M.P.I.) delivering 100–250 μs duration pulses of 0–90 V. Frequency-modulated stimulation protocols were generated using custom Python scripts (Figure 8—source code 1) approximating those used by Zampini et al., 2016. The stimulation train consisted of a 10 s 26 Hz train, followed by a 10 s train with a modulation frequency of 0.3, 1, or 3 Hz and a modulation depth of 6, 40, or 120, respectively. The stimulus was calculated by:

sin2π*carrier*t+modulator*(d/f)

where modulator = sin2πft*d, carrier = 26 Hz, f is modulation frequency, d is modulation depth, and t is time. As the purpose of these experiments was to determine the effect of ambient glutamate on synaptic currents and resulting spiking patterns, bias current was injected to maintain the resting membrane potential around −70 mV throughout the experiment.

Puff application of agonists and antagonists was delivered through a Picospritzer II (Parker Instrumentation), at 5–10 psi, with borosilicate glass capillaries. Glutamate applications were at 1 mM and 7–10 ms in duration and were always used prior to recordings for subtype identification. Puff application of control solution without drugs ruled out puff artifacts. The puff pipette was kept >20 μm away from the dendritic brush and soma to avoid mechanical disturbance of the cell.

AMPA receptor kinetic model

The AMPAR kinetic model was constructed by fitting the reaction rates of a 13-state scheme to AMPAR currents from whole-cell recordings of dissociated UBCs (Lu et al., 2017) using Axograph X software (RRID:SCR_014284). Parameters (Figure 1—figure supplement 2—source data 1) were adjusted to account for the steady-state dose–response curve, rate of decay of the fast initial EPSC in a train, the rate of decline of the amplitude of fast EPSCs during the train, the relative amplitude of the peak fast EPSC vs the peak slow current after the train, and the rise and decay time of the slow current. The key feature yielding a non-monotonic dose–response relationship was the differences in the rates governing entry and exit of D1-2 vs D3-4. Alterations in rates of opening and closing of the different open states could also generate a fall-off in current at high agonist concentrations, but also distorted fast deactivation to an unacceptable degree.

The AMPAR kinetic model was implemented using NMDL in NEURON (Balmer and Trussell, 2021; Carnevale and Hines, 2006; Hines and Carnevale, 2000) (RRID:SCR_005393). AMPAR conductance (g) was calculated from the occupancy of four open states (Figure 1—figure supplement 2—source data 1). AMPAR currents were calculated at a holding potential (v) of −70 mV and a reversal potential (Erev) of 0 mV to match experimental conditions, using the following equation:

AMPAcurrent=g(vErev)

In order to use the model that was derived from recordings made at room temperature to approximate synaptic currents recorded in slices at near physiological temperatures, the rates were adjusted. We chose to simply apply a Q10 (temperature coefficient) of 2.4 for all rates in the kinetic model for the following reasons: Multiplication by a single factor that equally affected all transition rates between states of an AMPAR model was sufficient to fit temperature-dependent changes of AMPAR currents at the Calyx of Held (Postlethwaite et al., 2007). Association and dissociation rate constants are temperature independent for acetylcholine receptors, which are similar in structure to AMPARs (Gupta and Auerbach, 2011). By increasing both the binding and unbinding constants by the same value, the association and dissociation rate constants remain unchanged, but the transitions between states are sped. This increased the rise and decay rates of fast synaptic currents and had no effect on the dose–response curves.

Glutamate transient prediction

Glutamate time courses were generated and used to stimulate the model synapse. The resulting currents were fit to experimentally isolate AMPA currents from whole-cell recordings of UBCs in brain slices. This approach predicts the glutamate concentration profiles that may have occurred at the AMPARs at the UBC synapse. Glutamate transients were generated using a 3D diffusion equation:

glu=M8απD/λ2t3/2exp-r24D/λ2t+ambient

where glutamate concentration [glu] evolves over time, t. Moles of glutamate released, M, and 3D distance between release site and receptors, r, and the level of ambient glutamate (ambient) were varied across fitting trials. The other variables remained constant: volume fraction (α = 0.21) (Nicholson and Phillips, 1981), diffusion coefficient (D = 0.33 cm2/s) (Nielsen et al., 2004), and tortuosity (λ = 1.55) (Barbour and Häusser, 1997; Nicholson and Phillips, 1981). The diffusion profile generated by this equation has a slowly decaying tail that is characteristic of a hyperbolic curve, which in this application approximates the slow exit of glutamate out of the synaptic cleft. Although the equation produced synaptic current models that fit the experimental data well, it remains a rough approximation due to assumptions of a single point of release and single point of detection (see Balmer and Trussell, 2021 for model code). Glutamate time courses from individual release events were added linearly to generate trains. No presynaptic depression was assumed. The other parameter of the model that was varied to optimize the fit was the maximal conductance of the AMPARs, which approximates the number of receptors present. The fitting procedure returned an average value for r of 1.23 ± 0.36 μm, which includes receptors located both near sites of exocytosis and also far (and therefore only activated by pooling of transmitter). This result is consistent with the proposal of Zampini et al., 2016 that many receptors at this synapse are activated by glutamate diffusing some distance from sites of release.

The leak current (baseline) of the experimentally recorded trace was also adjusted to correct for leak channels not implemented in the model that may have been active in the cell, as well as other typical sources of leak that accompany whole-cell recordings. Experimental traces were averaged across 5–10 trials, low-pass filtered (1 kHz), and the stimulation artifacts were blanked. Simulated glutamate transients were optimized by least-square fitting the simulated synaptic current to experimentally isolated AMPAR currents using NEURON’s Praxis package (RRID:SCR_005393).

Acknowledgements

This study was funded by National Institutes of Health (NIH) Grants NS028901, R35NS116798, and DC004450 (PI: LOT); F31 DC012454 and NL Tartar Trust Fellowship (PI: CBM); F32 DC014878, K99 DC016905, and Hearing Health Foundation Emerging Research Grant (PI: TSB); and P30 DC005983 (PI: Gillespie, Peter). We would like to thank Dr. Craig Jahr and members of the Trussell Lab for helpful discussions.

Funding Statement

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Contributor Information

Laurence O Trussell, Email: trussell@ohsu.edu.

Katalin Toth, University of Ottawa, Canada.

John R Huguenard, Stanford University School of Medicine, United States.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grants:

  • National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke NS028901 to Laurence O Trussell.

  • National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke NS116798 to Laurence O Trussell.

  • National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders DC004450 to Laurence O Trussell.

  • National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders DC016905 to Timothy S Balmer.

  • National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders DC014878 to Timothy S Balmer.

  • National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders DC012454 to Carolina Borges-Merjane.

Additional information

Competing interests

No competing interests declared.

Author contributions

Conceptualization, Data curation, Software, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Validation, Investigation, Visualization, Writing - original draft, Writing - review and editing.

Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing - review and editing.

Conceptualization, Resources, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Writing - original draft, Project administration, Writing - review and editing.

Ethics

Animal experimentation: All experiments were performed under the approval of the institutional animal care and use committee (IACUC) of Oregon Health and Science University, assurance #A3304-01.

Additional files

Transparent reporting form

Data availability

All data generated or analysed during this study are included in the manuscript and supporting files.

References

  1. Afshari FS, Ptak K, Khaliq ZM, Grieco TM, Slater NT, McCrimmon DR, Raman IM. Resurgent na currents in four classes of neurons of the cerebellum. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2004;92:2831–2843. doi: 10.1152/jn.00261.2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Arenz A, Silver RA, Schaefer AT, Margrie TW. The contribution of single synapses to sensory representation in vivo. Science. 2008;321:977–980. doi: 10.1126/science.1158391. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Balmer TS, Trussell LO. Selective targeting of unipolar brush cell subtypes by cerebellar mossy fibers. eLife. 2019;8:e44964. doi: 10.7554/eLife.44964. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Balmer TS, Trussell LO. Ambient Glutamate Shapes AMPA Receptor Responses to Simulated Transients. ModelDB; 2021. http://modeldb.yale.edu/266925 [Google Scholar]
  5. Bandrowski AE, Huguenard JR, Prince DA. Baseline glutamate levels affect group I and II mGluRs in layer V pyramidal neurons of rat sensorimotor cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2003;89:1308–1316. doi: 10.1152/jn.00644.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Barbour B, Häusser M. Intersynaptic diffusion of neurotransmitter. Trends in Neurosciences. 1997;20:377–384. doi: 10.1016/s0166-2236(96)20050-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bazargani N, Attwell D. Astrocyte calcium signaling: the third wave. Nature Neuroscience. 2016;19:182–189. doi: 10.1038/nn.4201. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Bischofberger J, Engel D, Li L, Geiger JR, Jonas P. Patch-clamp recording from mossy fiber terminals in hippocampal slices. Nature Protocols. 2006;1:2075–2081. doi: 10.1038/nprot.2006.312. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Borges-Merjane C, Trussell LO. ON and OFF unipolar brush cells transform multisensory inputs to the auditory system. Neuron. 2015;85:1029–1042. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.02.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Brickley SG, Cull-Candy SG, Farrant M. Development of a tonic form of synaptic inhibition in rat cerebellar granule cells resulting from persistent activation of GABAA receptors. The Journal of Physiology. 1996;497 ( Pt 3:753–759. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1996.sp021806. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Carnevale NT, Hines ML. The NEURON Book. Cambridge University Press; 2006. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  12. Carter AG, Regehr WG. Quantal events shape cerebellar interneuron firing. Nature Neuroscience. 2002;5:1309–1318. doi: 10.1038/nn970. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Cavelier P, Attwell D. Neurotransmitter depletion by bafilomycin is promoted by vesicle turnover. Neuroscience Letters. 2007;412:95–100. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2006.10.040. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Clements JD, Lester RA, Tong G, Jahr CE, Westbrook GL. The time course of glutamate in the synaptic cleft. Science. 1992;258:1498–1501. doi: 10.1126/science.1359647. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Conn PJ, Pin JP. Pharmacology and functions of metabotropic glutamate receptors. Annual Review of Pharmacology and Toxicology. 1997;37:205–237. doi: 10.1146/annurev.pharmtox.37.1.205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Crider BP, Xie XS, Stone DK. Bafilomycin inhibits proton flow through the H+ channel of vacuolar proton pumps. Journal of Biological Chemistry. 1994;269:17379–17381. doi: 10.1016/S0021-9258(17)32447-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Diamond JS, Jahr CE. Asynchronous release of synaptic vesicles determines the time course of the AMPA receptor-mediated EPSC. Neuron. 1995;15:1097–1107. doi: 10.1016/0896-6273(95)90098-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Diamond JS, Jahr CE. Transporters buffer synaptically released glutamate on a submillisecond time scale. The Journal of Neuroscience. 1997;17:4672–4687. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-12-04672.1997. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Diana MA, Otsu Y, Maton G, Collin T, Chat M, Dieudonné S. T-type and L-type Ca2+ conductances define and encode the bimodal firing pattern of vestibulocerebellar unipolar brush cells. Journal of Neuroscience. 2007;27:3823–3838. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4719-06.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. DiGregorio DA, Nusser Z, Silver RA. Spillover of glutamate onto synaptic AMPA receptors enhances fast transmission at a cerebellar synapse. Neuron. 2002;35:521–533. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00787-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. DiGregorio DA, Rothman JS, Nielsen TA, Silver RA. Desensitization properties of AMPA receptors at the cerebellar mossy fiber granule cell synapse. Journal of Neuroscience. 2007;27:8344–8357. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2399-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Diño MR, Nunzi MG, Anelli R, Mugnaini E. Unipolar brush cells of the vestibulocerebellum: afferents and targets. In: Gerrits T. J, editor. Progress in Brain Research. Elsevier; 2000a. pp. 123–137. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Diño MR, Schuerger RJ, Liu Y, Slater NT, Mugnaini E. Unipolar brush cell: a potential feedforward excitatory interneuron of the cerebellum. Neuroscience. 2000b;98:625–636. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4522(00)00123-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Dodt H-U, Eder M, Schierloh A, Zieglgansberger W. Infrared-Guided laser stimulation of neurons in brain slices. Science Signaling. 2002;2002:pl2. doi: 10.1126/stke.2002.120.pl2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Goldberg JM, Fernandez C. Physiology of peripheral neurons innervating semicircular canals of the squirrel monkey. 3. variations among units in their discharge properties. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1971a;34:676–684. doi: 10.1152/jn.1971.34.4.676. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Goldberg JM, Fernandez C. Physiology of peripheral neurons innervating semicircular canals of the squirrel monkey. I. resting discharge and response to constant angular accelerations. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1971b;34:635–660. doi: 10.1152/jn.1971.34.4.635. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Gupta S, Auerbach A. Temperature dependence of acetylcholine receptor channels activated by different agonists. Biophysical Journal. 2011;100:895–903. doi: 10.1016/j.bpj.2010.12.3727. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Hámori J, Somogyi J. Differentiation of cerebellar mossy fiber synapses in the rat: a quantitative electron microscope study. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 1983;220:365–377. doi: 10.1002/cne.902200402. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Hayashi Y, Sekiyama N, Nakanishi S, Jane DE, Sunter DC, Birse EF, Udvarhelyi PM, Watkins JC. Analysis of agonist and antagonist activities of phenylglycine derivatives for different cloned metabotropic glutamate receptor subtypes. The Journal of Neuroscience. 1994;14:3370–3377. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.14-05-03370.1994. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Herman MA, Jahr CE. Extracellular glutamate concentration in hippocampal slice. Journal of Neuroscience. 2007;27:9736–9741. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3009-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Hines ML, Carnevale NT. Expanding NEURON's repertoire of mechanisms with NMODL. Neural Computation. 2000;12:995–1007. doi: 10.1162/089976600300015475. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Jaarsma D, Wenthold RJ, Mugnaini E. Glutamate receptor subunits at mossy fiber-unipolar brush cell synapses: light and electron microscopic immunocytochemical study in cerebellar cortex of rat and cat. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 1995;357:145–160. doi: 10.1002/cne.903570113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Kaneda M, Farrant M, Cull-Candy SG. Whole-cell and single-channel currents activated by GABA and glycine in granule cells of the rat cerebellum. The Journal of Physiology. 1995;485 ( Pt 2:419–435. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1995.sp020739. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Kim JA, Sekerková G, Mugnaini E, Martina M. Electrophysiological, morphological, and topological properties of two histochemically distinct subpopulations of cerebellar unipolar brush cells. The Cerebellum. 2012;11:1012–1025. doi: 10.1007/s12311-012-0380-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Kinney GA, Overstreet LS, Slater NT. Prolonged Physiological Entrapment of Glutamate in the Synaptic Cleft of Cerebellar Unipolar Brush Cells. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1997;78:1320–1333. doi: 10.1152/jn.1997.78.3.1320. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Lu H-W, Balmer TS, Romero GE, Trussell LO. Slow AMPAR synaptic transmission is determined by stargazin and glutamate transporters. Neuron. 2017;96:e74. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.08.043. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Morin F, Diño MR, Mugnaini E. Postnatal differentiation of unipolar brush cells and mossy fiber-unipolar brush cell synapses in rat cerebellum. Neuroscience. 2001;104:1127–1139. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4522(01)00144-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Mugnaini E, Floris A, Wright-Goss M. Extraordinary synapses of the unipolar brush cell: An electron microscopic study in the rat cerebellum. Synapse. 1994;16:284–311. doi: 10.1002/syn.890160406. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Nicholson C, Phillips JM. Ion diffusion modified by tortuosity and volume fraction in the extracellular microenvironment of the rat cerebellum. The Journal of Physiology. 1981;321:225–257. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1981.sp013981. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Nielsen TA, DiGregorio DA, Silver RA. Modulation of glutamate mobility reveals the mechanism underlying slow-rising AMPAR EPSCs and the diffusion coefficient in the synaptic cleft. Neuron. 2004;42:757–771. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.04.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Nunzi MG, Shigemoto R, Mugnaini E. Differential expression of calretinin and metabotropic glutamate receptor mGluR1alpha defines subsets of unipolar brush cells in mouse cerebellum. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 2002;451:189–199. doi: 10.1002/cne.10344. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Overstreet LS, Kinney GA, Liu YB, Billups D, Slater NT. Glutamate transporters contribute to the time course of synaptic transmission in cerebellar granule cells. The Journal of Neuroscience. 1999;19:9663–9673. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-21-09663.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Partin KM, Patneau DK, Winters CA, Mayer ML, Buonanno A. Selective modulation of desensitization at AMPA versus kainate receptors by cyclothiazide and concanavalin A. Neuron. 1993;11:1069–1082. doi: 10.1016/0896-6273(93)90220-L. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Postlethwaite M, Hennig MH, Steinert JR, Graham BP, Forsythe ID. Acceleration of AMPA receptor kinetics underlies temperature-dependent changes in synaptic strength at the rat calyx of Held. The Journal of Physiology. 2007;579:69–84. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2006.123612. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Raman IM, Trussell LO. The kinetics of the response to glutamate and kainate in neurons of the avian cochlear nucleus. Neuron. 1992;9:173–186. doi: 10.1016/0896-6273(92)90232-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Raymond JL, Lisberger SG. Neural learning rules for the vestibulo-ocular reflex. The Journal of Neuroscience. 1998;18:9112–9129. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-21-09112.1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Ritzau-Jost A, Delvendahl I, Rings A, Byczkowicz N, Harada H, Shigemoto R, Hirrlinger J, Eilers J, Hallermann S. Ultrafast action potentials mediate kilohertz signaling at a central synapse. Neuron. 2014;84:152–163. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.08.036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Rossi DJ, Alford S, Mugnaini E, Slater NT. Properties of transmission at a giant glutamatergic synapse in cerebellum: the mossy fiber-unipolar brush cell synapse. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1995;74:24–42. doi: 10.1152/jn.1995.74.1.24. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Rothstein JD, Martin L, Levey AI, Dykes-Hoberg M, Jin L, Wu D, Nash N, Kuncl RW. Localization of neuronal and glial glutamate transporters. Neuron. 1994;13:713–725. doi: 10.1016/0896-6273(94)90038-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Ruigrok TJ, Hensbroek RA, Simpson JI. Spontaneous activity signatures of morphologically identified interneurons in the vestibulocerebellum. Journal of Neuroscience. 2011;31:712–724. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1959-10.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Russo MJ, Mugnaini E, Martina M. Intrinsic properties and mechanisms of spontaneous firing in mouse cerebellar unipolar brush cells. The Journal of Physiology. 2007;581:709–724. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2007.129106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Sah P, Hestrin S, Nicoll RA. Tonic activation of NMDA receptors by ambient glutamate enhances excitability of neurons. Science. 1989;246:815–818. doi: 10.1126/science.2573153. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Shimamoto K, Lebrun B, Yasuda-Kamatani Y, Sakaitani M, Shigeri Y, Yumoto N, Nakajima T. DL-threo-beta-benzyloxyaspartate, a potent blocker of excitatory amino acid transporters. Molecular Pharmacology. 1998;53:195–201. doi: 10.1124/mol.53.2.195. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Shimamoto K, Sakai R, Takaoka K, Yumoto N, Nakajima T, Amara SG, Shigeri Y. Characterization of novel L-threo-beta-benzyloxyaspartate derivatives, potent blockers of the glutamate transporters. Molecular Pharmacology. 2004;65:1008–1015. doi: 10.1124/mol.65.4.1008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Simpson JI, Hulscher HC, Sabel-Goedknegt E, Ruigrok TJ. Between in and out: linking morphology and physiology of cerebellar cortical interneurons. Progress in Brain Research. 2005;148:329–340. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(04)48026-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Suvrathan A, Payne HL, Raymond JL. Timing rules for synaptic plasticity matched to behavioral function. Neuron. 2016;92:959–967. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.10.022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Tzingounis AV, Wadiche JI. Glutamate transporters: confining runaway excitation by shaping synaptic transmission. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2007;8:935–947. doi: 10.1038/nrn2274. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Watanabe D, Inokawa H, Hashimoto K, Suzuki N, Kano M, Shigemoto R, Hirano T, Toyama K, Kaneko S, Yokoi M, Moriyoshi K, Suzuki M, Kobayashi K, Nagatsu T, Kreitman RJ, Pastan I, Nakanishi S. Ablation of cerebellar golgi cells disrupts synaptic integration involving GABA inhibition and NMDA receptor activation in motor coordination. Cell. 1998;95:17–27. doi: 10.1016/S0092-8674(00)81779-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Xu-Friedman MA, Regehr WG. Ultrastructural contributions to desensitization at cerebellar mossy fiber to granule cell synapses. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2003;23:2182–2192. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-06-02182.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Zampini V, Liu JK, Diana MA, Maldonado PP, Brunel N, Dieudonné S. Mechanisms and functional roles of glutamatergic synapse diversity in a cerebellar circuit. eLife. 2016;5:e15872. doi: 10.7554/eLife.15872. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision letter

Editor: Katalin Toth1
Reviewed by: Alain Marty2

In the interests of transparency, eLife publishes the most substantive revision requests and the accompanying author responses.

Acceptance summary:

This paper provides convincing evidence that a physiologically significant level of ambient glutamate is present at a synapse in the cerebellum, where it controls spontaneous firing and regulates phasic postsynaptic currents. The ambient glutamate is the result of spontaneous vesicle fusion and not action potential-evoked release. These synapses are virtually always exposed to a significant level of glutamate unlike classical fast synapses, which has a powerful impact on neuronal firing patterns. This regulatory mechanism endows these synapses with unique functional properties.

Decision letter after peer review:

Thank you for submitting your article "Incomplete removal of extracellular glutamate controls synaptic transmission and integration at a cerebellar synapse" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by three peer reviewers, one of whom is a member of our Board of Reviewing Editors, and the evaluation has been overseen by John Huguenard as the Senior Editor. The following individual involved in review of your submission has agreed to reveal their identity: Alain Marty (Reviewer #2).

The reviewers have discussed the reviews with one another and the Reviewing Editor has drafted this decision to help you prepare a revised submission.

We would like to draw your attention to changes in our revision policy that we have made in response to COVID-19 (https://elifesciences.org/articles/57162). Specifically, we are asking editors to accept without delay manuscripts, like yours, that they judge can stand as eLife papers without additional data, even if they feel that they would make the manuscript stronger. Thus the revisions requested below only address clarity and presentation.

In the current study Balmer et al. showed that ambient glutamate in the synaptic cleft tonically activates ionotropic and metabotropic receptors in UBC cells in the cerebellum. This is quite different from the typical glutamatergic synapse where synaptic responses rapidly rise and fall. Ambient glutamate responses described in the current manuscript exist in the absence of action potentials in the presynaptic neurons and result from spontaneous vesicular glutamate release. The level of ambient glutamate, which is set by glutamate transporters in the synaptic cleft, endows the neurons with a complex signalling mechanism via the regulation of the length and polarity of postsynaptic responses. The observed change in postsynaptic currents is much slower than changes in the frequency of presynaptic spikes. The authors illustrate the functional complexity of this synaptic reposes by showing that ambient glutamate can shift the phase of responsiveness of UBC cells to periodic stimulation.

As you will see in the detailed reviews below, all three reviewers were unanimous to praise the rigorous experimental design and the high quality of the experimental data. The authors' conclusions are solidly built on their findings and fully supported by the data presented. While there is general enthusiasm for this study, the modelling data presented on Figure 2. was identified as the weakest point of the manuscript. Reviewers raised several critical points related to model. They also find that this is not a critical element of the study and suggest that the authors either move this figure to a supplementary figure or remove it completely.

Reviewer #1:

In this manuscript Balmer et al. investigates the effect of extracellularly accumulating glutamate at MF-UBC synapses in the cerebellum. The authors found that glutamate accumulating in the synaptic cleft influenced the spontaneous firing rate of OFF cells and desensitized AMPA receptors in ON cells. They identified the source of ambient glutamate as spontaneous, AP-independent release from MF terminals. mGluR2-medated tonic current was present in OFF cells, and tonic desensitization of AMPA currents was observed in ON cells.

The study investigates differences in ON and OFF cells and identifies dominant glutamate mediated tonic currents in both groups suggesting the presence of ambient glutamate at these synapses. The authors also show that these tonic currents could alter firing properties and hence could significantly impact downstream network function.

The study offers novel insights into the presence of tonic currents in UBCs mediated by ambient glutamate. This is an important component of the functional landscape shaping firing patterns in this network. The study is carefully planned and executed.

The focus of the study is the presence and role of tonic currents that are mediated by ambient glutamate that is the result of AP-independent release. There is an inherent logical difficulty here since the presence of these tonic currents have been convincingly demonstrated, but the “ideal” experiments investigating the role of these currents (i.e. isolating and selectively removing tonic currents) in synaptic integration and spiking is technically very difficult. The authors use the TBOA experiments in their functional experiments, but these experiments will only allow them to draw conclusions about the role of EAATs in signal integration without having direct insights into the role of ambient glutamate in the cleft. This disconnect is expected, but the manuscript could be improved to make this issue clearer to the reader.

The study is a logical continuation of the authors previous papers (Balmer et al., 2017 and Borges-Merjane and Trussel, 2015). This means that there is some unavoidable overlap between the experiments even if the focus is quite distinct here. The Discussion could benefit from a more in-depth investigation of how the current and previous findings combine at the functional level. For example, it would be interesting to see a detailed discussion on the relationship between the CTZ experiments presented here on Figure 4 and uncaging experiments on Figure 3 of Balmer et al.

The authors suggest that the most likely source of glutamate driving the tonic currents is from MF terminals. The authors should elaborate on the level of certainty they have in this and outline the alternatives.

UBCs terminate not only on granule cells but also on other UBCs, potentially forming a chain of interconnected UBCs (Dorp and De Zeeux, 2015). Could the synaptic inputs investigated by the authors be disynaptic?

Figure 3A shows an extreme example, the average tonic AMPA current is ~3pA and this small current is explained by AMPA-R desensitization. One of the authors' point in the Discussion is about how desensitization leads to a smaller tonic current. May be a more typical example in Figure 3A would better illustrate the main findings.

Figure 8 is focused on ON cells. In these cells tonic currents seems to be rather small due to AMPA-R desensitization, while the mGluR2-medated tonic outward current in OFF cells is larger. What happens in OFF cells?

Figure 8 The firing of the neuron is a result of interactions between synaptic and intrinsic conductance. Blockade of EAATs with TBOA (and therefore increase in ambient glutamate) affects both a tonic current and the evoked synaptic current. Can the authors tease apart these two effects? In presence of TBOA, the baseline firing of the neuron in absence of synaptic stimulation is increased due to augmented inward tonic current. First, it would be important to know how the baseline membrane potential was adjusted (or were recordings performed at resting Vm?). Second, how much did the baseline firing change after TBOA application, due to the increased tonic inward current? Third, and most importantly, the authors should test whether artificially increasing the baseline firing of the neuron through somatic current injection (and recapitulating the effects of TBOA on basal firing rates) can mimic the effect of TBOA on the firing rate during the synaptic stimulation protocol. Overall, this would provide reassurance that all effects can be attributed to EAATs blockade as the authors suggest.

Figure 6F, right panel: the NBQX trace is almost completely hides the control trace. Perhaps use the same indicators [(1) control, (2) CTZ, (3) NBQX] as on the left panel.

Reviewer #2:

In recent years the Trussell group has produced several interesting papers examining the mechanisms of synaptic transmission in MF-UBC synapses. In the present work, it is shown that under basal conditions, there is enough ambiant glutamate to significantly activate ionotropic and metabotropic receptors at these synapses. Furthermore, the authors convincingly show that the basal glutamate is provided by the continuous supply linked to spontaneous vesicular release from resting MF terminals. Finally, in a spectacular final figure, the authors present intriguing data suggesting that ambient glutamate may shift the phase of responsiveness of the UBC cells to periodic stimulation. This leads to the very interesting suggestion that the UBC to GC relay may shift the phase of the incoming cerebellar sensory input in a flexible manner.

The design and execution of the experiments are excellent, and the conclusions of the paper are both novel and justified.

My only substantial criticism concerns the treatment of glutamate diffusion in the glomerulus. In both Introduction and Discussion, the authors say that glutamate escape out of the glomerulus may occur slowly, on a time scale of 100s of ms. But their actual diffusion model, that is used in the Results section, does not incorporate a separate, slow phase representing glutamate exit out of the glomerulus. Rather, it treats glutamate diffusion as a single process that is homogeneous in space, with global slowing factors reflecting volume fraction and tortuosity. This results in an apparent contradiction that should somehow be addressed, at least with a caveat sentence when presenting the diffusion model, saying that this model is only a rough approximation.

I have listed below specific suggestions that should be taken into consideration when revising the manuscript.

1) Glutamate diffusion model:

Whereas the AMPAR activation is modelled in detail (Figure 2A), the diffusion of glutamate is described by a highly simplified model (diffusion equation). This model does not attempt to incorporate the complicated MF terminal geometry, which is understandable. However, it may be oversimplified, as all glutamate sources are considered equivalent, and the diffusion process is homogeneous in space.

Incorporating crevices in the model, as was done by Nielsen et al. (2004), results in the prediction of two phases of glutamate concentration decay, with a slow phase reflecting glutamate escape once the glutamate concentration has equilibrated within the glomerulus. A biphasic glutamate decay model was likewise developed by Barbour et al. (1994) at PF-PC synapses. Such a biphasic decay could help interpreting the time course of mEPSCs. Specifically, it is possible that the slow time constant of mEPSC decay could reflect a slow component of glutamate concentration decay (escape out of the glomerulus), rather than a low value for channel resensitization rates, as it presently appears from Supplemental Table 1.

The possibility of a slow glutamate escape would give a likely mechanistic interpretation for the finding of a significant basal glutamate concentration inside the synaptic cleft. By contrast, this conclusion of the paper is presumably difficult to reconcile with the monotonic glutamate decay assumed, given the scarcity of recorded mEPSCs.

Again, I do not suggest developing a realistic diffusion model based on glomerulus morphology: this would be a huge task that would require the introduction of many geometric parameters that are presently unknown. But the authors should acknowledge in their modelling section the possibility that glutamate concentration decay could be biphasic, and that this could be an explanation for the slow phase of mEPSC decay.

2) mGluR1 activation by basal glutamate?

When reading the section on effects of basal glutamate in ON UBCs, the question arises as to whether mGluR1s would be activated in addition to AMPARs. Was this examined? Even if the results were negative, it would be good to mention them for completeness.

3) Figure 6:

This figure has two parts: panels A-E describe mEPSCs in UBCs, and F-H describe combined bafilomycin/CTZ suggesting that the source of ambient glutamate is vesicular. It is the second part which is most directly relevant to the paper. While describing mEPSCs is valuable -and may yield important clues on glutamate diffusion, see above-, the fact that mEPSCs are present in these cells does not represent a surprise. In view of the above the authors should consider keeping only parts F-H as main text figure, and downgrading parts A-E as an additional supplementary figure.

4) Model of Figure 8:

In the analysis of Figure 8E-F, the control response is modelled as a second harmonic of the MF spike modulation, while the response in TBOA is modelled as a first harmonic. But looking at the data in D-E, it appears that modelling the control response as a second harmonic is rather arbitrary. The amplitude of the second wave is much smaller than that of the first one, indicating that the true period remains the driving period. Looking at the raw data in D, it appears that the black spikes are roughly synchronized with the driving signal, except for a small delay, whereas the red spikes are almost opposite in phase with respect to the driving signal. This interpretation could be conveyed by performing a first harmonic fit to the black data as well as to the red data in E. Fitting the black histogram in E with a first harmonic would considerably simplify the presentation of the next panel (F). Presently panel F is almost impossible to understand. It is unclear what the radius in F represents (probably the firing rate for the second harmonic was doubled; but this is hard to justify). It is also unclear how the arc circles joining black and red dots were obtained. And it is unclear why the phase chosen for the black dots corresponds to the second peak in E rather than to the first one.

Reviewer #3:

This study shows how glutamate transporters limit activation of glutamate receptors at ON and OFF UBCs. The experiments show quite convincingly that glutamate transporters regulate ambient glutamate levels and synaptic glutamate diffusion dynamics. ON and OFF UBCs serve as controls for each other in various experiments, enabling the authors to distinguish effects on ambient and evoked transmitter levels. Further experiments show that ambient glutamate is due to spontaneous vesicle release and that transporters influence physiologically relevant UBC signaling. The experiments appear rigorously performed and are clearly presented. My primary concern is with the kinetic model used to estimate the glutamate concentration.

The authors estimate the glutamate concentration time course using a kinetic model of AMPA receptors. The experimental bases for model parameters are not described thoroughly and raise many questions. The authors note that parameters were tuned to match whole-cell responses recorded from dissociated UBCs (Lu, et al., 2014), experiments in which glutamate was applied to the whole cell with relatively slow exchange times (estimated by the authors to be 50 ms). These methods are likely inadequate to constrain many of the parameters in the model. As just one example, the authors determine that the glutamate binding rate at room temperature to a single binding site is 2×108 s-1M-1s-1, which is certainly within theoretical diffusion limits but, as far as I know, is 10 times faster than any previous estimates and cannot be derived reliably from the cited whole-cell data. This is likely to be an important issue, because a higher binding rate would confer higher sensitivity in non-equilibrium conditions. I do not suggest that the authors undertake an entire kinetic analysis of UBC AMPA receptors, but I would ask that they add more detail supporting the rate constants and how much the model's behavior (beyond the non-monotonic dose-response) relies on specific values. I would also suggest that the model constitutes the weakest part of the paper and distracts somewhat from an otherwise pleasing flow of nice physiological experiments. I would suggest that the model is not necessary and, if retained, might function better as a discussion point at the end.

eLife. 2021 Feb 22;10:e63819. doi: 10.7554/eLife.63819.sa2

Author response


As you will see in the detailed reviews below, all three reviewers were unanimous to praise the rigorous experimental design and the high quality of the experimental data. The authors' conclusions are solidly built on their findings and fully supported by the data presented. While there is general enthusiasm for this study, the modelling data presented on Figure 2. was identified as the weakest point of the manuscript. Reviewers raised several critical points related to model. They also find that this is not a critical element of the study and suggest that the authors either move this figure to a supplementary figure or remove it completely.

We have opted to move Figure 2 to a supplemental figure. While the points the reviewers made are well taken, we feel the modeling approach was key. Notably, we are not using the model as a confirmation of experiments, but rather to generate experimental testable hypotheses. The model suggested the presence of ambient glutamate and the paper then goes on to confirm the model in that regard. We hope that by positioning it as an early supplement, we honor both the reviewers concerns and our own intentions.

Reviewer #1:

[…] The authors use the TBOA experiments in their functional experiments, but these experiments will only allow them to draw conclusions about the role of EAATs in signal integration without having direct insights into the role of ambient glutamate in the cleft. This disconnect is expected, but the manuscript could be improved to make this issue clearer to the reader.

This is a good point. We have added a caveat in the text.

The study is a logical continuation of the authors previous papers (Balmer et al., 2017 and Borges-Merjane and Trussel, 2015). This means that there is some unavoidable overlap between the experiments even if the focus is quite distinct here. The Discussion could benefit from a more in-depth investigation of how the current and previous findings combine at the functional level. For example, it would be interesting to see a detailed discussion on the relationship between the CTZ experiments presented here on Figure 4 and uncaging experiments on Figure 3 of Balmer et al.

We appreciate that the reviewer has recognized the continuity of these works and have added text, to succinctly connect the present work to the conclusions of the uncaging study.

The authors suggest that the most likely source of glutamate driving the tonic currents is from MF terminals. The authors should elaborate on the level of certainty they have in this and outline the alternatives.

We have added an exploration of the alternatives to the Discussion section.

UBCs terminate not only on granule cells but also on other UBCs, potentially forming a chain of interconnected UBCs (Dorp and De Zeeux, 2015). Could the synaptic inputs investigated by the authors be disynaptic?

The reviewer makes an interesting point. We doubt the dataset we show here includes such cases. Where disynaptic responses were apparent, they were characterized by longer delays and jitter in the onset of the fast AMPA EPSC. If the stimulus electrode in the white matter was activating UBC soma, the trains of responses would almost certainly trigger spike bursts, and therefore bursts of EPSCs. One possibility is that the electrode stimulated UBC axons that had been severed from their soma, but these are not so likely to populate the white matter containing cerebellar input fibers. We make a note about this now in the Materials and methods.

Figure 3A shows an extreme example, the average tonic AMPA current is ~3pA and this small current is explained by AMPA-R desensitization. One of the authors' point in the Discussion is about how desensitization leads to a smaller tonic current. May be a more typical example in Figure 3A would better illustrate the main findings.

Since a more typical response is very small and noisy, we chose to show an outlier and label it as such.

Figure 8 is focused on ON cells. In these cells tonic currents seems to be rather small due to AMPA-R desensitization, while the mGluR2-medated tonic outward current in OFF cells is larger. What happens in OFF cells?

We chose to focus on ON cells because in the rare cases where we had an OFF cell for this kind of experiment, the results were frankly not very interesting: the cell remained off for most of the during of stimulation, particularly in TBOA.

Figure 8 The firing of the neuron is a result of interactions between synaptic and intrinsic conductance. Blockade of EAATs with TBOA (and therefore increase in ambient glutamate) affects both a tonic current and the evoked synaptic current. Can the authors tease apart these two effects? In presence of TBOA, the baseline firing of the neuron in absence of synaptic stimulation is increased due to augmented inward tonic current. First, it would be important to know how the baseline membrane potential was adjusted (or were recordings performed at resting Vm?). Second, how much did the baseline firing change after TBOA application, due to the increased tonic inward current? Third, and most importantly, the authors should test whether artificially increasing the baseline firing of the neuron through somatic current injection (and recapitulating the effects of TBOA on basal firing rates) can mimic the effect of TBOA on the firing rate during the synaptic stimulation protocol. Overall, this would provide reassurance that all effects can be attributed to EAATs blockade as the authors suggest.

“Can the authors tease apart these two effects?”. These two are manifestations of a single effect, which is activation of the receptors by accumulated glutamate. Thus, they are not separable. For mGluR2, the receptors are tonically activated thus generating steady current and in parallel reducing the fraction available for activation by the evoked response. For AMPAR, activation and desensitization.

“…know how the baseline membrane potential was adjusted”. We added a sentence to Materials and methods to indicate that bias current was applied to keep the resting membrane potential constant. Small currents, on the order of 20 pA would be sufficient to lead to depolarization block. We did not do the experiment suggested, to inject current to mimic TBOA, but it should be clear that since bias current was injected, the effects we described must be related to the nonlinear responses of the AMPAR to ambient glutamate and not merely a steady current buildup.

Figure 6F, right panel: the NBQX trace is almost completely hides the control trace. Perhaps use the same indicators [(1) control, (2) CTZ, (3) NBQX] as on the left panel.

Thanks, this is corrected.

Reviewer #2:

In recent years the Trussell group has produced several interesting papers examining the mechanisms of synaptic transmission in MF-UBC synapses. In the present work, it is shown that under basal conditions, there is enough ambiant glutamate to significantly activate ionotropic and metabotropic receptors at these synapses. Furthermore, the authors convincingly show that the basal glutamate is provided by the continuous supply linked to spontaneous vesicular release from resting MF terminals. Finally, in a spectacular final figure, the authors present intriguing data suggesting that ambient glutamate may shift the phase of responsiveness of the UBC cells to periodic stimulation. This leads to the very interesting suggestion that the UBC to GC relay may shift the phase of the incoming cerebellar sensory input in a flexible manner.

The design and execution of the experiments are excellent, and the conclusions of the paper are both novel and justified.

My only substantial criticism concerns the treatment of glutamate diffusion in the glomerulus. In both Introduction and Discussion, the authors say that glutamate escape out of the glomerulus may occur slowly, on a time scale of 100s of ms. But their actual diffusion model, that is used in the Results section, does not incorporate a separate, slow phase representing glutamate exit out of the glomerulus. Rather, it treats glutamate diffusion as a single process that is homogeneous in space, with global slowing factors reflecting volume fraction and tortuosity. This results in an apparent contradiction that should somehow be addressed, at least with a caveat sentence when presenting the diffusion model, saying that this model is only a rough approximation.

We have now added caveats in the Materials and methods to acknowledge just this point.

I have listed below specific suggestions that should be taken into consideration when revising the manuscript.

1) Glutamate diffusion model:

Whereas the AMPAR activation is modelled in detail (Figure 2A), the diffusion of glutamate is described by a highly simplified model (diffusion equation). This model does not attempt to incorporate the complicated MF terminal geometry, which is understandable. However, it may be oversimplified, as all glutamate sources are considered equivalent, and the diffusion process is homogeneous in space.

Incorporating crevices in the model, as was done by Nielsen et al. (2004), results in the prediction of two phases of glutamate concentration decay, with a slow phase reflecting glutamate escape once the glutamate concentration has equilibrated within the glomerulus. A biphasic glutamate decay model was likewise developed by Barbour et al. (1994) at PF-PC synapses. Such a biphasic decay could help interpreting the time course of mEPSCs. Specifically, it is possible that the slow time constant of mEPSC decay could reflect a slow component of glutamate concentration decay (escape out of the glomerulus), rather than a low value for channel resensitization rates, as it presently appears from Supplemental Table 1.

The situation is not so much “rather than” as “both”: The slow clearance of glutamate slows the EPSC and slows re-sensitization (since receptors cannot re-sensitize until transmitter levels fall). This is implicit in the model, and seems inescapable give the data shown here and in Lu et al., 2017.

The possibility of a slow glutamate escape would give a likely mechanistic interpretation for the finding of a significant basal glutamate concentration inside the synaptic cleft. By contrast, this conclusion of the paper is presumably difficult to reconcile with the monotonic glutamate decay assumed, given the scarcity of recorded mEPSCs.

The glutamate transient we assumed is monotonic in that it continuously decreases after the peak, but it does not decrease monophasically. The equations predict a significant slow tail. This slow phase is larger when there is multivesicular release within the “confines” of the MF-UBC synaptic cleft area, but even the recorded mEPSCs shows evidence of this slow phase.

Again, I do not suggest developing a realistic diffusion model based on glomerulus morphology: this would be a huge task that would require the introduction of many geometric parameters that are presently unknown. But the authors should acknowledge in their modelling section the possibility that glutamate concentration decay could be biphasic, and that this could be an explanation for the slow phase of mEPSC decay.

Again, the concentration decay is “biphasic” in the model. In the modeling figure panels E and I, for example, the predicted glutamate profile is plotted on a log scale and is clearly nonlinear on that axis. The slow “phase” lasts for hundreds of milliseconds. We have also explored models using multiexponential decays, which give very similar results. We have uploaded all of these models (kinetic, 3D and exponential) into Neuron Modeldb, and this link is cited in the paper. The conclusions are robust.

2) mGluR1 activation by basal glutamate?

When reading the section on effects of basal glutamate in ON UBCs, the question arises as to whether mGluR1s would be activated in addition to AMPARs. Was this examined? Even if the results were negative, it would be good to mention them for completeness.

As requested, we have added such a sentence to the Results.

3) Figure 6:

This figure has two parts: panels A-E describe mEPSCs in UBCs, and F-H describe combined bafilomycin/CTZ suggesting that the source of ambient glutamate is vesicular. It is the second part which is most directly relevant to the paper. While describing mEPSCs is valuable -and may yield important clues on glutamate diffusion, see above-, the fact that mEPSCs are present in these cells does not represent a surprise. In view of the above the authors should consider keeping only parts F-H as main text figure, and downgrading parts A-E as an additional supplementary figure.

We agree that these two parts of the figure describe somewhat different topics and so have split them into two figures. Regarding the “surprise” element, while we agree that mEPSCs are expected in most neurons, we were actually quite surprised to see them in UBCs: such events have not been reported in UBCs, and our initial efforts did not reveal any. Only when we realized how small and how incredibly slow they are (for mEPSCs) did we re-examine the datasets with greater filtering than typical for whole-cell recordings and then realized they were there all along. Since their presence is part of the logic of our proposal that spontaneous exocytosis supplies ambient glutamate, we prefer to let them stay in the paper.

4) Model of Figure 8:

In the analysis of Figure 8E-F, the control response is modelled as a second harmonic of the MF spike modulation, while the response in TBOA is modelled as a first harmonic. But looking at the data in D-E, it appears that modelling the control response as a second harmonic is rather arbitrary. The amplitude of the second wave is much smaller than that of the first one, indicating that the true period remains the driving period. Looking at the raw data in D, it appears that the black spikes are roughly synchronized with the driving signal, except for a small delay, whereas the red spikes are almost opposite in phase with respect to the driving signal.

This is what we were are trying to convey.

This interpretation could be conveyed by performing a first harmonic fit to the black data as well as to the red data in E. Fitting the black histogram in E with a first harmonic would considerably simplify the presentation of the next panel (F).

We fit with a simple sine wave with a variable period. It is not clear how a first harmonic would be any different.

Presently panel F is almost impossible to understand. It is unclear what the radius in F represents (probably the firing rate for the second harmonic was doubled; but this is hard to justify). It is also unclear how the arc circles joining black and red dots were obtained. And it is unclear why the phase chosen for the black dots corresponds to the second peak in E rather than to the first one.

We have tried to explain this further in the legend and with explanatory changes in the figure panels E and F, emphasizing that we are plotting the phase of offset responses with and without TBOA and showing their accompanying firing rates.

Reviewer #3:

This study shows how glutamate transporters limit activation of glutamate receptors at ON and OFF UBCs. The experiments show quite convincingly that glutamate transporters regulate ambient glutamate levels and synaptic glutamate diffusion dynamics. ON and OFF UBCs serve as controls for each other in various experiments, enabling the authors to distinguish effects on ambient and evoked transmitter levels. Further experiments show that ambient glutamate is due to spontaneous vesicle release and that transporters influence physiologically relevant UBC signaling. The experiments appear rigorously performed and are clearly presented. My primary concern is with the kinetic model used to estimate the glutamate concentration.

The authors estimate the glutamate concentration time course using a kinetic model of AMPA receptors. The experimental bases for model parameters are not described thoroughly and raise many questions. The authors note that parameters were tuned to match whole-cell responses recorded from dissociated UBCs (Lu, et al., 2014), experiments in which glutamate was applied to the whole cell with relatively slow exchange times (estimated by the authors to be 50 ms). These methods are likely inadequate to constrain many of the parameters in the model. As just one example, the authors determine that the glutamate binding rate at room temperature to a single binding site is 2×108 s-1M-1s-1, which is certainly within theoretical diffusion limits but, as far as I know, is 10 times faster than any previous estimates and cannot be derived reliably from the cited whole-cell data. This is likely to be an important issue, because a higher binding rate would confer higher sensitivity in non-equilibrium conditions.

We repeated simulations and found that a 10-fold reduction in binding rate, accompanied by parallel reduction in dissociation, had almost no effect on the predicted responses. This is expected given that the “meat” of this work is in responses to slow glutamate transients. We note that association rates assumed for receptors are often in the 107 / M.s range as the reviewer notes, but in many cases these are not actually measured. Measured values for ligand binding to ligand gated channels varies across the 107-108 range. Given all this, we feel comfortable that values utilized here do not weaken our conclusions.

I do not suggest that the authors undertake an entire kinetic analysis of UBC AMPA receptors, but I would ask that they add more detail supporting the rate constants and how much the model's behavior (beyond the non-monotonic dose-response) relies on specific values. I would also suggest that the model constitutes the weakest part of the paper and distracts somewhat from an otherwise pleasing flow of nice physiological experiments. I would suggest that the model is not necessary and, if retained, might function better as a discussion point at the end.

In response to the options offered by the editors we have moved the model to a supplemental figure. But to move it to a discussion point at the end of the paper would up-end the point of the model which was to make a testable prediction about ambient glutamate. Without that, the flow of the paper would consist of an investigation into a rather random query about the possibility of ambient glutamate. We note also that the model values chosen mimic extraordinarily diverse aspects of the receptors during fast and slow glutamate transients: the amplitudes of the fast and slow EPSCs and their time courses are almost perfectly fit by the model. The excellence of the fits across diverse time scales (ms to seconds), we believe, is supportive of the parameters of the kinetic model.

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    Figure 1—figure supplement 2—source data 1. The parameters of the AMPA receptor kinetic model were adjusted to give incomplete desensitization and to fit experimental data from Lu et al., 2017.
    Figure 8—source code 1. Python script to produce a sinusoidally modulated pattern of TTL pulses that was used to approximate in vivo-like activity patterns.

    The script creates a text file that can be imported into pClamp and used as a stimulus output pattern.

    Transparent reporting form

    Data Availability Statement

    All data generated or analysed during this study are included in the manuscript and supporting files.


    Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

    RESOURCES