Abstract
Social characteristics are prominent factors in mate selection, but they can be risk factors for intimate partner violence. Yet this prior work is limited, as it largely focuses on demographic differences (or asymmetries) between intimate partners. In addition to demographic asymmetries, we explored how differences in relational and risk behaviors were associated with intimate partner violence. Examining data from the Toledo Adolescent Relationships Study (TARS) (n=828), we found that either partner’s unemployment is associated with greater frequency of relationship violence; and, compared to same-race relationships, interracial relationships reported higher frequency of partner violence. Additionally, relationships in which men compared to their partners were more invested, were characterized by lower frequency of violence. Relationships characterized by any asymmetry in power, compared to those in which both partners held equal power, were also characterized by higher frequency of partner violence. Relationships in which the female partner was more successful at school or work compared to the male partner were associated with more frequent partner violence. Lastly, compared to relationships in which neither partner engaged in antisocial/criminal activity, those in which the male, but not female partner was antisocial/criminal and those in which both partners engaged in antisocial/criminal activity reported greater frequency of partner violence. These findings highlighted the importance of considering different types of asymmetries for understanding intimate partner violence in young adult relationships.
Keywords: asymmetries, homogamy, intimate relationships, young adulthood
Individuals tend to choose intimate partners who have similar social characteristics or backgrounds. Referred to as homophily or homogamy, shared social characteristics are prominent factors in mate selection, but also play an important role in the quality and functioning of intimate relationships (Blackwell & Lichter, 2004; Schwartz & Mare, 2005). Mäenpää1 and Jalovaara (2014), for example, have argued that intimate partner similarities “…foster value consensus between partners on basic life goals and practices, ensure a common basis of conversation, and reduce friction that may arise from dissimilarity in tastes and worldviews (p. 1771). Indeed, researchers have demonstrated that intimate relationships that are characterized by homophily are often the most successful, with higher levels of satisfaction (Bertrand, Kamenica, & Hohmann-Marriot, 2013; Zhang & Van Hook, 2009), less conflict (Atkinson, Greenstein, & Lang, 2005; Kaukinen, 2004) and greater stability (Schwartz & Han, 2014). However, this prior work has tended to conceptualize homogamy in demographic terms, focusing on partner similarity in socioeconomic status, race/ethnicity, and age or as differences in levels of commitment (Stanley, Rhoades, Kelmer, Scott, Markman, & Fincham, 2019). In this paper, we have argued that partner asymmetries with respect to investing in the relationship, job and school success, and power dynamics, as well as differences in engagement in risky antisocial behavior may have negative implications for relationship quality and functioning. These asymmetries signal differences in views about and experiences within intimate relationships that have the potential to lead to problematic outcomes.
Drawing on data from the fifth interview of the Toledo Adolescent Relationships Study (TARS), we explored whether demographic, relational, and risk asymmetries are associated with the frequency of intimate partner violence (IPV) in young adulthood. Extending prior research, our study made three contributions to the wider family literature. First, we considered a broader range of asymmetries, as opposed to solely focusing on demographic similarities and dissimilarities. Second, we examined the relative importance of each type of asymmetry to determine which had the strongest association with self-reported partner violence. Finally, we assessed whether these patterns were similar or different for men and women. Our research highlighted the importance of accounting for various types of asymmetries, provided new insight into the correlates of intimate partner violence, and showed the consequences of asymmetries for the quality of young adults’ intimate relationships.
Background
Intimate Relationships in Young Adulthood
Most young adults are involved in intimate relationships. Estimates have suggested that the majority of young adults are in dating relationships followed by cohabiting and martial unions, respectively (Child Trends, 2013; Sorgi, Chen, Dean, Halpern, & Harris, 2016). Regardless of union type, many young adults report poor relationship functioning. Researchers examining data from the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent to Adult Health (Add Health) (n=4,134) (Halpern, Spriggs, Martin, & Kupper, 2009), as well as TARS (Johnson, Giordano, Manning, & Longmore, 2015), have shown that violence characterizes many young adults’ relationships. Halpern and colleagues (2009) have demonstrated that nearly a quarter of young adults experienced intimate partner violence victimization in the past year. Johnson et al. (2015) found that violence perpetration peaks in early young adulthood (age 20) and declines in subsequent years. Although these studies are important for providing a descriptive overview of partner violence, and much prior work has considered background factors, it is also important to consider couple-level dynamics. Within the realm of dyadic concerns, few investigators have considered the role of asymmetries, particularly if we move beyond those in the demographic realm.
Demographic Asymmetries and Partner Violence
Educational and socioeconomic asymmetries.
Historically, the notion of a marriage gradient suggests that husbands tend to be better educated and earn higher incomes than their wives. Thus, the meaning of masculinity, as well as the male identity, has been associated with the ‘breadwinner’ role (Anderson, 1997). However, increased numbers of women in the paid labor force (U.S. Census Bureau, 2016) and the reversal of the gender gap in education (Schwartz & Han, 2014) have led to shifts in the educational and employment composition of many couples. These shifts might suggest that relationships characterized by educational and socioeconomic asymmetries that favor the female partner may be characterized by poorer relationship functioning because such differences challenge the gendered expectations that men should be the financial providers (even in the context of increased female labor force participation). Researchers (Atkinson et al., 2005; Kaukinen, 2004) examining survey data have found that relationships characterized by educational, employment, and income asymmetries that favor the female partner are associated with greater frequency of partner violence.
Racial asymmetries.
Estimates suggest that there has been a steady increase in interracial relationships since the mid-1960s (Hattery & Smith, 2009; Livingston & Brown, 2017). Although the rising trends in interracial relationships implies that the social barriers that deterred the formation and maintenance of such relationships in the past are diminishing (Rosenfeld & Kim, 2005), some researchers (Chartier & Caetano, 2013; Fusco, 2010; Martin, Cui, Ueno, & Fincham, 2013) have suggested that interracial couples experience greater frequency and more severe acts of partner violence.
Higher levels and greater severity in partner violence may be the function of interracial couples’ exposure to social marginalization, including experiencing a lack of support for their relationships from family members, peers, and community members (Miller, Olson, & Fazio, 2004), a hesitancy to engage in public and private displays of affection (Vaquera & Kao, 2005), or their reluctance to share experiences of discrimination with each other (Killian, 2001). Thus, previous research has provided evidence suggesting that interracial relationships experience unique challenges that may cause greater interpersonal stress and conflict between partners (Martin et al., 2013). This lends support for the view that racial homogamy may be important for some aspects of relationship functioning, despite the increase in and growing acceptance of interracial relationships.
Age asymmetries.
Another common source of homogamy between partners is age. Estimates from the Add Health have shown that nearly half (44%) of young adult women and a quarter of young adult men are involved in intimate relationships characterized by age asymmetries—or age differences of four or more years (Scott, Steward-Streng, Manlove, Schelar, & Cui, 2011). Previous findings on the association between age asymmetries and intimate partner violence are not only varied, but inconclusive. For instance, some researchers (e.g., Peters, Shackelford, & Buss, 2002; Catallozi, Simon, Davidson, Breitbart, & Rickert, 2011) have reported a positive association between age asymmetries and partner violence, while others (e.g., Agurcia, Rickert, Berenson, Volk, & Wiemann, 2001; Harner, 2004; Volpe, Hardie, Cerulli, Sommers, & Morrison-Beedy, 2013) have not found significant associations.
One explanation for these disparate findings on age asymmetries and partner violence may be due to sample differences. Some studies (e.g., Agurcia et al., 2001; Harner, 2004) have examined adolescent mothers who were partnered with adult men, and other studies focused on dating relationships (e.g., Volpe et al., 2011), and no studies have included dating, cohabiting, and marital unions among young adults. Nevertheless, consistent with the homogamy argument, we expect that age asymmetries would be associated with higher odds of intimate partner violence.
Relational Asymmetries and Partner Violence
Prior research that has examined associations between asymmetries and partner violence has focused almost exclusively on demographic asymmetries. Thus, our understanding of the association between relational asymmetries and intimate partner violence is limited. Despite a lack of empirical research, the broader theoretical framework of attachment theory sheds light on how relational asymmetries may be associated with partner violence in young adulthood.
In brief, attachment theory is concerned with the dynamics of personal relationships and seeks to explain how individuals respond when they feel distant from the individuals with whom they should feel closest or when they believe their relationship is being threatened. (Feeney & Noller, 1990; Finkel & Slotter, 2007). According to Finkel and Slotter (2007) “…attachment bonds in intimate relationships are threatened by diverse circumstances, including the perception that one’s partner has become detached or is attracted to somebody else” (p. 902). With respect to partner violence, scholars have argued that individuals who feel that they are being treated unfairly within their intimate relationships will feel angry, which in turn may lead to the perpetration of violence towards their partners (Finkel & Slotter, 2007). Thus, relationships characterized by relational asymmetries, such as those in influence and investment, may be more susceptible to partner violence because they signal differences in partners’ approaches to the relationships. Historically, the field has focused on asymmetries of power (e.g., Jackson & Leone, 2005) as a general heuristic for understanding violence, but little research has examined the idea of a relational mismatch (e.g., one person more invested) or how differential perceptions about success in the larger arenas of career/employment goals may impact intimate partner violence. Three asymmetries that we examined included (1) investment in the relationship – that is, who seems more invested in the relationship; (2) perceptions of power – which partner seems to have more power in the relationship, and (3) perceptions of being successful with respect to career/employment goals. We expected that these relational asymmetries would be associated with higher odds of intimate partner violence.
Risky Antisocial Behavior Asymmetries and Partner Violence
Although the role of relational asymmetries and partner violence is expected to mirror that of demographic asymmetries, the association between asymmetries in risky antisocial or criminal behavior and intimate partner violence may operate differently. Examining TARS, Alvira-Hammond and colleagues’ (2014) found that, compared to relationships in which neither partner engaged in antisocial activities, relationships characterized by any antisocial behavior were associated with greater frequency of partner violence. Yet, Alvira-Hammond et al. did not control for demographic and relational asymmetries. We expected that relationships in which either partner engaged in risk behaviors would be associated with poor relationship functioning and higher levels of conflict, but test whether risk asymmetries were also associated with greater frequency of intimate partner violence.
Current Investigation
Using data from TARS, we examined whether demographic, relational, and risk asymmetries were associated with intimate partner violence. Next, we assessed the relative importance of demographic, relational, and risk asymmetries to determine which type of asymmetry was more consequential to partner violence in young adulthood. Extending prior research, we considered several types of asymmetries and accounted for variation in intimate partner violence experiences. Additionally, we assessed whether the association between asymmetries and partner violence in young adulthood differed for men and women. We hypothesized that asymmetries in demographic characteristics (education, employment, race/ethnicity, and age) and relational dynamics (invested in the relationship, relationship power, and success in employment/work) would be associated with greater frequency of partner violence. We also expected that relationships in which any partner (male, female, or both) engaged in risky antisocial or criminal behavior would be associated with greater frequency of partner violence.
In assessing associations between sociodemographic, relational, and risky behavior asymmetries and intimate partner violence, we controlled for a number of key correlates of intimate partner violence. Such correlates included gender (e.g., Johnson et al., 2015), race (e.g., Sampson, Morenoff, & Raudenbush, 2005), age (e.g., Johnson et al., 2015), union status (e.g., Brown & Bulanda, 2008, Manning, Longmore, & Giordano, 2018), relationship duration (e.g., Giordano, Johnson, Manning, & Longmore, 2014), presence of children (e.g., Vest, Caitlin, Chen, & Brownson, 2002), and parental and friend support of the relationship (Zhang & Van Hook, 2009). By controlling for these correlates, we attempted to account for factors that may potentially confound the association between asymmetries and partner violence.
Method
Data and Sample
We used data from the fifth interview of TARS, a longitudinal study based on a stratified (gender, age, and race/ethnicity) random sample of adolescents registered for the 7th, 9th, and 11th grades in Lucas, County, Ohio during the fall of 2000. Made available through Ohio’s Freedom of Information Act, the initial TARS sampling frame, which was developed by the National Opinion Research Center (NORC), drew from student rosters from 62 schools across seven different school districts and included over-samples of Black and Hispanic adolescents. Although the sampling frame of the TARS data was school-based, school attendance was not required for inclusion in the sample. The first interview of TARS occurred in 2001 and the fifth interview in 2012. The initial sample included 1,321 respondents, and we retained 1,021 valid respondents, or 77% of the initial sample. Compared with descriptive data from the 2011 American Community Survey (ACS), the TARS sample is demographically similar to young adults living in the U.S. in terms of gender, race, educational attainment, employment status, and union status.
The analytic sample (n=828) consisted of all respondents from the fifth interview with three exclusions. We excluded respondents who did not report a current or most recent intimate relationship (n=71) at the fifth interview. Additionally, due to small sample sizes, we excluded respondents who reported a same-sex intimate relationship (n=24) at the fifth interview and respondents who did not report their race/ethnicity or their partner’s race/ethnicity as White, Black, or Hispanic (n=47 and n=57, respectively). We used mean and modal imputation to account for missing data.
Measures
Dependent Variables
Intimate partner violence, measured with twenty-four items from the Revised Conflict Tactics Scale (CTS2) (Straus, Hamby, Boney-McCoy, & Sugarman, 1996), referred to both victimization and perpetration. We asked respondents the following, “During this relationship, how often have [Partner/you] done the following (not because you were playing around)”: (1) “thrown something,” (2) “twisted arm or hair,” (3) “used a knife or gun,” (4) “punched or hit with something that could hurt,” (5) “choked,” (6) “slammed against a wall,” (7) “beat,” (8) “burned or scalded on purpose,” (9) “kicked,” (10) “pushed, shoved, or grabbed,” (11) “slapped in the face or head with an open head,” and (12) “hit.” Responses ranged from 1 (never) to 5 (very often). The non-logged scale ranged from 0 to 24 (α= .95). Due to skewness, we analyzed intimate partner violence as a logged, summed scale.
Independent Variables
Educational asymmetries referred to partners’ differences in educational levels, which we measured with two questions: (1) “How far have you gone in school,” and (2) “What is [Partner’s] highest grade level?” Responses ranged from “dropped out of high school” to “graduate or professional school.” We recoded responses into four categories: less than high school, high school, some college, and college or more. We created three dichotomous variables reflecting educational asymmetries: male partner more educated, educational homogamy (reference group), and female partner more educated.
Employment asymmetries reflected responses to the following two questions: (1) “Are you currently working for pay for at least 10 hours a week,” and (2) “Does [Partner] currently have a job?” Responses were “yes” and “no.” We created the following four dichotomous variables: (1) male partner employed, female partner not employed, (2) both male and female partners employed (reference group), (3) both male and female partners not employed, and (4) female partner employed, male partner not employed.
Race asymmetries were measured with two questions: “What is your racial/ethnic background,” and “What is [Partner’s] racial/ethnic background?” We recoded responses as White, Black, and Hispanic. We created two dichotomous variables reflecting race asymmetries: (1)same-race (reference group) and (2) interracial.
Age asymmetries were measured with the question, “How old are/is [you/Partner]?” We took the difference between the respondents’ and partners’ ages. Since the average age difference between young adult partners is three years (Scott et al. 2011), we used three years to assess age asymmetry. We created three dichotomous variables reflecting age asymmetries: (1) male older, (2) age homogamy (reference group), and (3) female older.
Investment asymmetries were measured using the following prompt: “In many relationships, one person is more ‘into’ the relationship than the other. Would you say…” Responses included, “you are more into it,” “[Partner] is more into it,” and “you are about the same.” We combined responses with the respondent’s gender to create gender specific asymmetries in relationship investment: (1) male more invested, (2) investment homogamy (reference group), and (3) female more invested.
Power asymmetries were measured with the following statement: “Thinking about your relationship overall, who would you say has the most power in your relationship?” Responses included: “[Partner] does,” “I do,” “We generally both have an equal say,” and “We have not disagreed.” We combined responses with the respondent’s gender to create gender specific power asymmetries: (1) male partner has more power, (2) power homogamy, and (3) female partner has more power.
Success in job/school asymmetries were measured by asking respondents, “Which of the following best describes you and [Partner]?” Responses included: “[Partner] is not as successful in (job, school) as I am,” “We are equally successful,” and “[Partner] is more successful than I am at this point in our lives.” We combined responses with the respondent’s gender to create three asymmetries: (1) male partner more successful, (2) success homogamy, and (3) female partner more successful.
Antisocial behavior asymmetries referred to respondents’ reports of the frequency of their own and partners’ antisocial behavior. We used 9 items from Elliott and Ageton’s (1980) antisocial behavior scale. Respondents were asked: “In the past 24 months (or 2 years), how often have you/[Partner]: (1)”drunk alcohol,” (2) “stolen (or tried to steal) things worth $5 or less,” (3) “carried a hidden weapon other than a plain pocket knife,” (4) “damaged or destroyed property on purpose,” (5) “stolen (or tried to steal something worth more than $50),” (6) “attacked someone with the idea of seriously hurting him/her,” (7) “sold drugs,” (8) “broken into a building or vehicle (or tried to break in) to steal something or just to look around,” and (9) “used drugs to get high.” Response ranged from 1 (never) to 9 (more than once a day) (respondent: α= .73, partner: α= .75). We considered individuals who scored at least one standard deviation above the mean as antisocial. We created four dichotomous variables: (1) male partner antisocial, (2) both partners antisocial, (3) neither partner antisocial, and (4) female partner antisocial.
Control Variables
We included several sociodemographic variables in the multivariate analyses. Gender (female is reference category), race/ethnicity (White reference group, Black, and Hispanic/Latino), and age, were self- reported at the fifth interview. Current relationship referred to whether the respondent reported on a current (reference category) or most recent relationship. Union status included three dichotomous variables: dating (reference category), cohabiting, and married. We calculated relationship duration based on the number of months and years that respondents have been in their relationship. Presence of children referred to whether the respondent lived with children (reference category).
We measured parental approval of the relationship with the item, “How often do your parents express disapproval of your relationship with your partner (dating, living with, or married to)?” Responses ranged from 1 (never) to 6 (two or more times a week). We measured friends ’ approval of the relationship with the following statement: “My friends approve of my relationship with [Partner].” Responses ranged from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree).
Analytic Strategy
We used ordinary least squares regression to examine the association between demographic, relational, and antisocial behavior asymmetries and frequency of intimate partner violence in young adulthood. In the multivariate analyses, Model 1 (baseline model) showed the association between demographic asymmetries and intimate partner violence. Model 2 added relational asymmetries to the baseline model and Model 3 included demographic, relational, and antisocial behavior asymmetries. Model 4 added the control variables to the third model and examined the extent to which asymmetries have an independent effect on IPV. Standardized coefficients from Model 4 were assessed to determine the relative importance of demographic, relational, and risk asymmetries.
Results
Descriptive Results
In Table 1, the average level of frequency of partner violence was 1.44 (σ=3.67), suggesting that respondents reported low frequency of relationship violence. Consistent with Halpern and colleagues’ (2009) work using the Add Health data, about 23% of respondents reported any violence in their current or most recent intimate relationships.
Table 1.
Full Sample (n=828) |
IPV (n=189) |
No IPV (n=639) |
|
---|---|---|---|
Dependent Variables | |||
Intimate Partner Violence (range =0-24) | 1.4 (3.7) | - | - |
Independent Variables | |||
Demographic Asymmetries | |||
Educational Asymmetries | |||
Male more educated | 18.1% | 19.6% | 17.7% |
Education homogamy | 49.2% | 46.0% | 50.1% |
Female more educated | 32.7% | 34.4% | 32.2% |
Employment Asymmetries | |||
Male employed, female unemployed | 15.6% | 12.7% | 14.1% |
Both partners employed | 65.0 % | 45.0% | 70.9% |
Both partners unemployed | 6.8% | 13.3% | 4.9% |
Female employed, male unemployed | 12.7% | 21.2% | 10.2% |
Race Asymmetries | |||
Same-race | 80.9% | 69.3% | 84.4% |
Interracial | 19.1% | 30.7% | 15.7% |
Age Asymmetries | |||
Male older | 23.3% | 29.1% | 21.6% |
Age homogamy | 71.3% | 65.6% | 72.9% |
Female older | 5.4% | 5.3% | 15.7% |
Relational Asymmetries | |||
Asymmetries in Investment | |||
Male more invested | 8.7% | 9.0% | 8.6% |
Investment homogamy | 71.0% | 57.1% | 75.1% |
Female more invested | 20.3% | 66.1% | 16.3% |
Asymmetries in Power | |||
Male more influence | 8.5% | 18.5% | 5.5% |
Influence homogamy | 74.0% | 55.6% | 79.5% |
Female more influence | 17.5% | 25.9% | 15.0% |
Asymmetries in Success | |||
Male more successful | 11.6% | 10.6% | 11.9% |
Success homogamy | 62.4% | 50.3% | 66.0% |
Female more successful | 26. 0% | 39.2% | 22.1% |
Risk Asymmetries | |||
Asymmetries in Antisocial Behavior | |||
Male, but not female antisocial | 8.3% | 16.9% | 3.6% |
Both partners antisocial | 4.7% | 5.8% | 0.63% |
Neither partner antisocial | 81.5% | 72.0% | 93.9% |
Female, not male antisocial | 5.4% | 6.0% | 1.9% |
Control Variables | |||
Respondent Characteristics | |||
Gender | |||
Male | 45.8% | 46.0% | 45.7% |
Female | 54.2% | 54.0% | 54.3% |
Race/ethnicity | |||
White | 69.3% | 54.5% | 73.7% |
Black | 20.4% | 30.2% | 17.5% |
Hispanic | 10.3% | 15.3% | 8.8% |
Age (range = 22-29) | 25.4 (1.9) | 25.4 (1.8) | 25.5 (1.9) |
Relationship Characteristics | |||
Current Relationship | |||
Current relationship | 80.7% | 78.8% | 81.2% |
Most recent relationship | 19.3% | 21.2% | 18.8% |
Union Status | |||
Dating | 44.0% | 34.4% | 46.8% |
Cohabiting | 31.5% | 41.8% | 28.5% |
Married | 24.5% | 23.8% | 24.7% |
Relationship Duration (range = 0-13 years) | 3.4 (2.9) | 4.0 (2.8) | 3.3 (2.9) |
Presence of Children | |||
Lives with children | 35.3% | 49.7% | 69.2% |
Does not live with or have children | 64.7% | 50.3% | 30.8% |
Parental Approval of Relationship (range = 1-5) | 4.0 (1.1) | 3.4 (1.2) | 4.2 (.93) |
Friend Approval of Relationship (range = 1-5) | 3.9 (1.0) | 3.4 (1.2) | 4.1 (.93) |
Source: Toledo Adolescent Relationships Study
Note: Standard deviations are indicated under mean values and ranges are listed with continuous variables
About half (49 %) of relationships reflected educational homogamy, whereas in one-third (33%) of the relationships women reported higher educational levels, and one-fifth (18%) were characterized by greater male education. The majority (65%) of relationships had employment homogamy in which both partners were employed. Unemployment homogamy characterized nearly seven percent of relationships. Approximately 16% of relationships were characterized by male, but not female, employment whereas 13% were characterized by female, but not male, employment. Most relationships (81%) were characterized by racial/ethnic homogamy whereas a fifth (19%) were interracial relationships. About three-quarters (71 %) of relationships were characterized by age homogamy whereas a quarter (23%) of relationships were characterized by an older male partner. Relationships in which the female partner was older than the male partner were least common, accounting for about six percent of the sample. Collectively, demographic homogamy characterized many intimate relationships in young adulthood, but this varied when examining the subsamples of relationships reporting intimate partner violence. That is, relationships with no reports of partner violence appeared to be more demographically homogamous than relationships with any report of violence.
Regarding relational asymmetries, over two-thirds (71%) of relationships involved both partners being equally invested, one-fifth (20%) were characterized by greater investment by the female partners, and less than one-tenth (9%) involved greater investment by the male partners. Similarly, the majority (71%) of relationships were characterized by both partners being equally influential whereas nearly one-third (29%) were characterized by asymmetries in power. The least common asymmetry in power was found in relationships in which the male partner held more power than the female partner. Over three-fifths (62%) of relationships involved partners who were equally as successful at work or school. Approximately 12% of relationships were characterized by the male partner being more successful and in 26% of relationships the female partner was more successful. Finally, in most relationships (82%), neither partner engaged in antisocial or criminal behavior. This was followed by relationships in which the male, but not female, partner was antisocial (8%). Relationships in which the female, but not male, partner was antisocial accounted for about five percent of the sample whereas relationships in which both partners engaged in antisocial behavior was the least common. Relationships with no reports of partner violence appeared to be more homogamous in relational and risk behaviors than relationships with any report of violence.
Regarding the characteristics of the respondents, as well as their relationships, a slight majority were female (54%). Additionally, over two-thirds of respondents were White, followed by Black (20%) and Hispanic (10%), and the average age of respondents was 25 (σ=1.85). Relationship duration, on average, was 3.5 years (σ=2.87), with the majority of respondents in dating relationships (44%), followed by 30% and 25% of respondents in cohabiting and marital unions, respectively. Additionally, the majority (65%) of respondents did not have children living in the household. Finally, average levels of parental and friend support were high (μ=4.00 and μ=3,92, respectively), suggesting that most relationships were supported by the respondents’ parents and friends.
Multivariate Results
In Table 2, we showed the OLS regression of partner violence on demographic asymmetries, relational asymmetries, antisocial behavior asymmetries, and the control variables. Focusing on demographic asymmetries, Model 1 demonstrated that relationships in which both partners were unemployed reported significantly higher frequency of partner violence than relationships in which both partners were employed. Additionally, relationships characterized by asymmetries in employment reported greater frequency of partner violence than relationships in which both partners were employed. Thus, Model 1 showed that any unemployment is associated with greater relationship violence. Model 1 also demonstrated that compared to same-race relationships, interracial relationships reported higher frequency of partner violence. Educational and age asymmetries were not associated with relationship violence in the baseline model.
Table 2.
Model 1 | Model 2 | Model 3 | Model 4a | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Demographic Asymmetries | ||||
Education (Educational homogamy) | ||||
Male more educated | −0.02 | 0.02 | 0.03 | −0.01 |
Female more educated | −0.01 | −0.06 | −0.06 | −0.10 |
Employment (Both partners employed) | ||||
Male employed, female unemployed | 0.28 *** | 0.29 *** | 0.27 *** | 0.17 * |
Both partners unemployed | 0.59 *** | 0.53 *** | 0.49 *** | 0.32 ** |
Female employed, male unemployed | 0.46 *** | 0.35 *** | 0.33 *** | 0.20 * |
Race (Same-race) Interracial | 0.29 *** | 0.24 *** | 0.24 ** | 0.20 ** |
Age (Age homogamy) | ||||
Male older | 0.12 | 0.10 | 0.11 | 0.13 * |
Female older | −0.03 | −0.10 | −0.11 | −0.07 |
Relational Asymmetries | ||||
Investment (Investment homogamy) | ||||
Male more invested | −0.13 | −0.13 | −0.24 * | |
Female more invested Power (Influence homogamy) |
0.24 *** | 0.20 ** | 0.13 | |
Male more influence than female | 0.51 *** | 0.43 *** | 0.28 ** | |
Female more influence than male | 0.24 ** | 0.22 ** | 0.19 ** | |
Success (Success homogamy) | ||||
Male more successful | 0.00 | 0.03 | 0.03 | |
Female more successful | 0.23 *** | 0.17 ** | 0.13 * | |
Risk Asymmetries | ||||
Antisocial Behavior (Neither partner antisocial) | ||||
Male, but not female, antisocial | 0.43 *** | 0.40 *** | ||
Both partners antisocial | 0.57 *** | 0.51 *** | ||
Female, but not male, antisocial | 0.14 | 0.18 |
Source: Toledo Adolescent Relationships Study
Note:
p<.05
p<.01
p<.001
Model 4 included all control variables, but due to spacing, the control variables are not presented in the table.
Model 2 included both demographic and relational asymmetries. Compared to relationships characterized by homogamy in investment, those in which the female, but not male, partner was more invested reported greater frequency of partner violence. No significant differences were found between relationships characterized by homogamy in investment and those in which the male, compared with the female, partner was more invested. Asymmetries in power were associated with partner violence, such that relationships characterized by any asymmetry in influence experienced higher frequency of partner violence than relationships in which both partners were equally as influential. Additionally, compared to relationships in which partners were equally successful in their jobs or school, those in which the female partner was more successful than the male partner reported more frequent partner violence. No significant differences were found between relationships characterized by homogamy in success and those characterized by greater male success. The findings from the first model remained significant in the second model, net of relational asymmetries.
Model 3 examined the association between asymmetries in antisocial behavior and intimate partner violence, controlling for demographic and relational asymmetries. No significant differences were found between relationships in which neither partner was engaged in antisocial behavior and in which the female, but not male, partner was antisocial. Consistent with Model 2, demographic asymmetries in employment and race, as well as relational asymmetries in investment, power, and success in work or school, were associated with intimate partner violence in young adulthood.
Model 4 included demographic, relational, and risk asymmetries, net of the control variables. This model corroborated prior models and showed that any unemployment, whether the female, male, or both partners were unemployed, was associated with greater frequency of violence. Additionally, interracial relationships compared with same-race relationships were characterized by higher frequency of violence. Contrary to prior models, asymmetries in age were associated with partner violence in the full model, demonstrating that relationships with an older male partner reported greater frequency of partner violence than relationships characterized by age homogamy.
In terms of relationship asymmetries, this model also showed that compared to relationships in which both partners were equally invested, those with greater male investment reported lower frequency of violence. No significant differences were found between relationships characterized by investment homogamy and relationships with greater female investment. Additionally, relationships with asymmetries in power were associated with higher frequency of intimate partner violence than those in which both partners wielded comparable power. Relationships characterized by greater female success reported high frequency of intimate partner violence compared to relationships characterized by success homogamy. Relationships with greater male success and those in which both partners were equally as successful did not significantly differ.
Finally, relationships in which both partners were involved in antisocial behavior had greater frequency of partner violence than relationships in which neither partner engaged in antisocial behavior. Furthermore, compared to relationships in which neither partner was involved in antisocial behavior, violence frequency was higher among relationships in which the female, but not male, partner was antisocial. Standardized coefficients (available from authors) showed that relational and risk asymmetries are more consequential to partner violence than demographic asymmetries.
To simplify the table presentation, we did not display the full roster of control variables. These variables operated in the expected manner (available from authors). Black, compared to White, respondents reported greater frequency of partner violence and compared to daters, cohabiting unions reported higher frequency of violence. Greater frequency of partner violence was also found in relationships longer in duration and those in which children were present in the respondents’ households. Finally, parental and friend support were associated negatively with intimate partner violence, suggesting that relationships receiving greater support from parents and friends experienced lower frequency of partner violence. Standardized coefficients (available from authors) suggested that out of these control variables lack of parental and friend support of the relationship, as well as relationship duration, have the greatest effects on partner violence frequency.
Discussion
Summarizing the key findings associated with demographic asymmetries, employment and race were both associated with greater frequency of violence. Specifically, either partner’s unemployment is associated with greater relationship violence; and, compared to same-race relationships, interracial relationships reported higher frequency of partner violence. Regarding relational asymmetries, differences in investment, power, and success in jobs or school were associated with violence. Relationships in which men were more invested, self-reported violence was lower. Relationships characterized by any asymmetry in power were also characterized by higher frequency of partner violence compared to relationships in which both partners held equal power. Relationships in which the female partner was more successful at school or work compared to the male partner reported more frequent partner violence. Lastly, compared to relationships in which neither partner engaged in antisocial behavior, those in which the male, but not female partner was antisocial and those in which both partners engaged in antisocial behavior reported greater frequency of partner violence.
Although we have covered new terrain regarding asymmetries and intimate relationship functioning, our study has some limitations. First, we relied on individual-level data that consisted of the respondents’ reports of their partners’ demographic characteristics, relational attributes, and involvement in antisocial behaviors. It is possible that respondents may have provided inaccurate information about their intimate partners and over- or understated their partners’ behaviors within the relationship, as well as their involvement in antisocial behaviors. Future research may benefit from using couple-level data to provide a more comprehensive and dyadic approach of the association between asymmetries and IPV in young adulthood. Second, we did not explore the underlying mechanisms through which these associations operated. The associations between asymmetries and partner violence may operate indirectly through other processes, such as relationship stress or poor communication between partners. Thus, future research should consider the possible mediating mechanisms through which the associations between asymmetries and partner violence operate. Finally, although the TARS sample shares similar sociodemographic characteristics with national averages, it is a regional sample of young adults. Data permitting, future research would benefit from using nationally representative data that is generalizable to the U.S. population of young adults.
The present study contributes to the literature on intimate relationships in several important ways. First, given the nature of the TARS data, which provides comprehensive measures of relationship dynamics and characteristics, we assessed a wider scope of asymmetries than many prior studies. As such, we were able to perform a more thorough investigation of the role of asymmetries in intimate partner violence and further, discern the relative importance of demographic, relational, and risky behavior asymmetries. Second, given the variation in union experiences, we considered a broad range of relationships in young adulthood, including dating, cohabiting, and marital unions. In doing so, we were able to directly assess the influence that asymmetries have on intimate partner violence during young adulthood. Finally, our analyses of gender specific couple-level indicators allow us to discern the gender patterns underlying intimate partner violence.
Young adult relationships are shifting as individuals wait longer to marry, and increasingly are entering cohabiting relationships (Lamadi & Manning, 2016). By taking a more dyadic approach in understanding the etiology of intimate partner violence, this research has important implications with regard to partner violence prevention and intervention efforts targeted toward young adults. Specifically, as women make gains in education and employment it appears there are some sources of strain. The education and employment composition of couples does not appear to be the source of negative ramifications, but instead, the perceptions of success may have negative implications. When women are more successful in employment or school there appears to be more frequent intimate partner violence, but the same is not true when men are more successful. This suggests that the disruption of traditional gender role expectations may have implications for the health and well-being of young adult couples. Additionally, antisocial/criminal activity is associated with increased frequency of intimate partner violence. As such, our findings are relevant to practitioners – what Hawkins refers to as “theory illuminating practice.” Healthy relationship programs often have been provided as interventions after the occurrence of intimate partner violence (Antle, Karam, Christensen, Baarbee, & Sar, 2011). Yet a complimentary practice is to provide healthy education programs to individuals and couples to reduce their likelihood of engaging in intimate partner violence by teaching healthy relationship practices, conflict strategies, and exit strategies. We argue that such programs may be ideal for discussing the challenges and issues that are common among contemporary couples, including the role that differences or asymmetries may play with respect to conflict. With this knowledge, perhaps individuals can learn to communicate more positively (Hawkins, 2017; Stanley, Markman, & Whitton, 2002; Stanley et al. 2019), which can have a preventative effect. Lastly, as relationship education begins to shift from a focus on marriage to dating and cohabiting partnerships (Hawkins, 2017), it is imperative that theory-based findings be used to help young people make appropriate choices including understanding risk factors associated with relationship violence.
Acknowledgments
This project was supported by Award Nos. 2009-IJ-CX-0503 and 2010-MU-MU-0031, awarded by the National Institute of Justice, Office of Justice Programs, U. S. Department of Justice, and in part by the Center for Family and Demographic Research, Bowling Green State University, which has core funding from The Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (P2CHD050959). The opinions, findings, and conclusions or recommendations expressed in this publication/program/exhibition are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect those of the Department of Justice.
References
- Alvira, Hammond M, , Longmore MA, Manning WD, & Giordano PC (2014). Gainful activity and intimate partner aggression in emerging adulthood. Emerging Adulthood, 2, 116–127. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Anderson KL (1997). Gender, status, and domestic violence: An integration of feminist and family violence approaches. Journal of Marriage and Family, 59(3), 655–669. [Google Scholar]
- Antle BF, Karam E, Christensen DN, Barbee AP, & Sar BK (2011). An evaluation of health relationship education to reduce intimate partner violence. Journal of Family Social Work, 14, 387–406. [Google Scholar]
- Atkinson MP, Greenstein TN, & Lang MM (2005). “For Women, Breadwinning can be Dangerous: Gendered Resource Theory and Wife Abuse.” Journal of Marriage and Family 67: 1137–1148. [Google Scholar]
- Agurcia CA, Rickert VI, Berenson AB, Volk RJ, & Wiemann CM (2001). The behavioral risks and life circumstances of adolescent mothers involved with older adult partners. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine, 155(7), 822–830. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bertrand M, Kamenica E, & Pan J (2015). Gender identity and relative income within households. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 130(2). [Google Scholar]
- Blackwell DL, & Lichter DT (2004). Homogamy among dating, cohabiting, and married couples. The Sociological Quarterly 45(4), 719–737. [Google Scholar]
- Brown SB, & Bulanda JR (2008). Relationship violence in young adulthood: A comparison of daters, cohabitors, and marrieds. Social Science Research, 37(1), 73–87. [Google Scholar]
- Catallozzi M, Simon PJ, Davidson LL, Breitbart V, & Rickert VI (2011). Understanding control in adolescent and young adult relationships. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine, 165, 313–319. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chartier KG & Caetano R (2012). Intimate partner violence and alcohol problems in interethnic and intra-ethnic Couples. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 27, 1780–1801. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Child Trends, (2013, December). The dynamics of young adult romantic relationships. Bethesda, MD: Child Trends. Retrieved from https://www.childtrends.org/wp-content/uploads/2013/12/2013-37DynamicsYoungAdultRomanticRelationships.pdf. [Google Scholar]
- Feeney JA, & Noller P (1990). Attachment style as a predictor of adult romantic relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58(2), 281–291. [Google Scholar]
- Finkel EJ, & Slotter EB (2007). An attachment theory perspective on the perpetration of intimate partner violence. DePaul Law Review, 56, 895–907. [Google Scholar]
- Fusco RA (2010). Intimate partner violence in interracial couples: a comparison to white and ethnic minority monoracial couples.” Journal of Interpersonal Violence 25(10): 1785–1800. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Giordano PC, Johnson WL, Manning WD, & Longmore MA (2014). Parenting in Adolescence and Young Adult Intimate Partner Violence. Journal of Family Issues, DOI: 10.1177/0192513X13520156 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Halpern CT, Spriggs AL, Martin SL, & Kupper LL (2009). Patterns of intimate partner violence victimization from adolescence to young adulthood in a nationally representative sample. Journal of Adolescent Health, 45(5), 508–516. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harner HM (2004). Domestic violence and trauma care in teenage pregnancy: Does paternal age make a difference? Journal of Obstetric Gynecologic, & Neonatal Nursing, 33, 312–319. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hawkins AJ (2017). Shifting the relationship education field to prioritize youth relationship education. Journal of Couple & Relationship Therapy, DOI: 10.1080/15332691.2017.1341355 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Hawkins AJ (2019). Theory illuminating practice: One researcher’s journey through the strange new world of policy change. Journal of Family Theory and Review, 11, 161–164. [Google Scholar]
- Johnson MP, & Leone JM (2005). The differential effects of intimate terrorism and situational couple violence: Findings from the National Violence Against Women Survey. Journal of Family Issues, 26(3), 322–349. [Google Scholar]
- Johnson WL, Giordano PC, Manning WD, & Longmore MA (2015). The age-IPV curve: Changes in the perpetration of intimate partner violence during adolescence and young adulthood. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 4, 708–726. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaukinen C (2004). Status compatibility, physical violence, and emotional abuse in intimate relationships. Journal of Marriage and Family, 66, 452–471. [Google Scholar]
- Killian KD (2001). Reconstituting racial histories and identities: The narratives of interracial couples. Journal of Marital and Family Therapy, 27, 27–42. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Livingston G, & Brown A (2017). Intermarriage in the U.S. 50 years after Loving v. Virginia. Washington D.C.: Pew Research Center Social & Demographic Trends. Retrieved from http://assets.pewresearch.org/wp-content/uploads/sites/3/2017/05/19102233/Intermarriage-May-2017-Full-Report.pdf. [Google Scholar]
- MacMillan R & Gartner R (1999). When she brings home the bacon: Labor-force participation and the risk of spousal violence against women.” Journal of Marriage and Family, 61, 947–958. [Google Scholar]
- Mäenpääl E, & Jalovaara M (2014). Homogamy in socio-economic background and education, and the dissolution of cohabiting unions. Demographic Research, 30(65), 1769–1792. [Google Scholar]
- Manlove JS, Terry-Humen E, Ikramulla EN, & Moore KA (2006). The role of parent religiosity in teens’ transitions to sex and contraception. Journal of Adolescent Health, 39(4), 578–587. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Manning WD, Longmore MA, & Giordano PC (2018). Cohabitation and intimate partner violence during emerging adulthood: High constraints and low commitment. Journal of Family Issues, 39(4), 1030–1055. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martin BA, Cui M, Ueno K, & Fincham FD (2013). Intimate partner violence in interracial and monoracial couples. Family Relations, 61, 202–211. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miller SC, Olson MA, & Fazio RH (2004). Perceived reactions to interracial romantic relationships: When race is used as a cue to status.” Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 7, 354–369. [Google Scholar]
- Peters J, Shackelford TK, & Buss DM (2002). Understanding domestic violence against women: Using evolutionary psychology to extend the feminist functional analysis. Violence and Victims, 17, 255–264. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sampson RJ, Morenoff JD, & Raudenbush S (2005). Social anatomy of racial and ethnic disparities in violence. American Journal of Public Health, 95(2), 224–232. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schwartz CR, & Han H (2014). The reversal of the gender gap in education and trends in marital dissolution. American Sociological Review, 79(4), 605–629. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schwartz CR, & Mare RD (2005). Trends in educational assortative mating from 1940 to 2003. Demography, 42(4), 621–646. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scott ME, Steward-Streng NR, Manlove J, Schelar E, & Cui C (2011). Characteristics of young adult sexual relationships: Diverse, sometimes violent, often loving. Child Trends Research Brief, 1, 1–8. [Google Scholar]
- Smith E, & Hattery AJ 2009. Interracial relationships in the 21st century. Durham, NC: Carolina Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
- Sorgi A, Chen P, Dean SC, Halpern CT, & Halpern K (2016). Characteristics of young adult relationships: The National Longitudinal study of Adolescent to Adult Health. Research Brief, No. 3. Chapel Hill, NC: Carolina Population Center, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill. http://www.cpc.unc.edu/projects/addhealth/documentation/ResearchBrief3.pdf [Google Scholar]
- Stanley SM, Rhoades GK, Kelmer G, Scott SB, Markman HJ, & Fincham FD (2018). Unequally into “Us”: Characteristics of individuals in asymmetrically committed relationships. Family Process, 58(1), 214–231. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Straus MA, Hamby SL, Boney-McCoy S, & Sugarman DB (1996). The revised conflict tactics scales (CTS2): Development and preliminary psychometric data. Journal of Family Issues, 17, 283–316. [Google Scholar]
- U.S. Bureau of Labor Statistics. 2016. “Women in the labor force: A databook.” Retrieved from (https://www.bls.gov/opub/reports/womens-databook/archive/women-in-the-labor-force-a-databook-2015.pdf)
- Vaquera E & Kao G (2005). Private and public displays of affection among interracial and intra-racial adolescent couples. Social Science Quarterly, 86, 484–508. [Google Scholar]
- Vest JR, Caitlin TK, Chen JJ, & Brownson RC (2002). Multistate analysis of factors associated with intimate partner violence.” American Journal of Preventative Medicine, 22, 156–164. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Volpe EM, Hardie TL, Cerulli C, Sommers MS, & Morrison-Beedy D (2013). Age got to do with it? Partner age difference, power, intimate partner violence, and sexual risk in urban adolescents. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 28, 2068–2087. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhang Y & Van Hook J (2009). Marital dissolution among interracial couples. Journal of Marriage and Family, 71, 95–107 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]