Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2021 Dec 1.
Published in final edited form as: Curr Opin Behav Sci. 2020 Oct 27;36:115–119. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2020.09.004

Expectable Environments in Early Life

Kathryn L Humphreys 1, Virginia C Salo 1
PMCID: PMC7945685  NIHMSID: NIHMS1633588  PMID: 33718532

Abstract

Humans develop in the context of environmental information that can be considered either experience-expectant or experience-dependent. Though the exact timing of sensitive period closures and consequences of environmental experiences have not been well delineated, early life is a period of increased vulnerability. While some forms of care (e.g., institutional care for children; representing the absence of experience-expectant caregiving) are not present in the evolutionary history of humans, it is likely that what is considered significant hardship today may have been more typical experience-dependent environmental information in the evolutionary timescale. Thus, assumptions that threatening or neglectful experiences are unexpected for the human child may not fit well in the scope of the broader timescale of human history. We argue that it is important to consider early caregiving experiences from the context of what has been expected in our evolutionary past rather than what is expected in modern sociocultural terms.

Keywords: early experiences, adversity, threat, deprivation, caregiving


The environmental information incorporated into building the developing brain can be categorized as experience-expectant or experience-dependent [1]. Theories of early adversity have noted that deviations from the expectable environment can have significant impacts on development, with the most devastating consequences occurring when experience-expectant information is missing in early life [2]. Evidence from studies on the effects of severe deprivation provide insight into the developmental time scale of these effects (i.e., sensitive periods) and inform our understanding of what are the fundamental expectations for the newest members of the human species. Many expectations and associated responses develop over ontogenetic time as an infant experiences, and adjusts to, their unique environment. However, there is also an important set of shared expectations that have developed based on the environments humans were likely to encounter across phylogenetic time. While it is important to consider the precursors and foundations of adaptive functioning in today’s environment, current research places a disproportionate emphasis on the experiences of individuals from WEIRD (Western, educated, industrialized, rich, democratic) cultures. This research base likely results in characterizations of an expected environment that may be misaligned with (species-typical) expectations. This may be particularly true when considering expectations of caregivers. Current research on the neurobiological and behavioral consequences of adversity, in particular, would benefit from applying a phylogenetic – not just ontogenetic – stance.

Early adversity as experience-expectant and experience-dependent deviations

The brain is theorized to be built through a combination of a genetic blueprint and environmental information [1]. This environmental information can be further categorized as either that which is expected to be encountered by all members of our species (e.g., exposure to light) or that which is dependent on idiosyncratic variation (i.e., not shared by all members of our species). The brain requires environmental input to be built, but also to specialize in the context cued by information unique to one’s environment [3,4]. The idea that there is an expectable environment for human infants has been useful for providing a framework from which to consider the developmental impact of violations or deviations from what a human infant or young child may reasonably or typically experience [5]. For example, the insufficient care provided in orphanages (i.e., severe social neglect) is not typical for members of our species. Considering severe psychosocial deprivation as a deviation from the species-expectant environment has provided some utility for describing the severe impairments (i.e., reactive attachment disorder [6]) that appear to result from this type of experience, and highlights the experience of a responsive caregiving environment as species-expectant.

Variation in environmental information that is experience-dependent is also known to influence development. Both more common experiences of neglect (i.e., neglect not as severe as the severe psychosocial deprivation experienced in institutions), as well as physical abuse, are associated with a range of negative outcomes [7]. Theories on early adversity most commonly focus on these types of environmental input, with neglect considered a relative omission of appropriate care whereas abuse and other threatening experiences considered a commission of frightening/harmful care [8]. Typical processes related to learning about one’s environment through experiences are believed to govern these responses to adversity, though there is also evidence for specificity at both the neural and behavioral levels. The effects of deprivation appear to result in reduced synaptic proliferation (particularly in the cortex) and the experience of deprivation is associated with performance on cognitive control tasks. Whereas, the effects of threatening experiences appear to affect emotion regulation circuitry and emotion processing [9,10].

Yet, considering experience-dependent variation primarily from the perspective of adversity may artificially cut off what are likely to be continua by which to characterize individual environments. Research documenting the effects of variation in the early environment within the “normative” range suggests that even caregiving differences found among volunteer parents from the community may result in meaningful differences in child brain structure and function [1113]. Importantly, this challenges the idea that children simply require a species-expected “good enough” environment [14]. Multidimensional characterizations of environmental variation in children’s lives (e.g., considering both emotional and cognitive input on separate continua that range from neglect to enrichment, along with degree of sensitive caregiving received from one’s primary caregiver) may be fruitful in determining the overlap among and potential differences in metrics of environmental experiences in early life [15]. Further, this approach highlights that meaningful variation occurs not only with experiences of adversity, but also in the range considered “good enough” as well as highly enriched environments associated with human thriving.

What is the “expected” environment?

Understanding the likely environments of humans on the phylogenetic scale may be useful for those interested in human responses to early experiences of adversity. Indeed, the argument has been made that creating a “bridge” between evolutionary biology and developmental psychology can provide insight into potential natural selection pressures that might predispose us to exhibit certain behavioral responses observed today [16]. Most research linking adversity, broadly speaking, and later life outcomes has primarily taken a deficit approach. However, complementary approaches to examine potential strengths or “hidden talents” among individuals as adaptions following exposure to adversity are also gaining attention [1719]. It is possible, and even plausible, that exposure to adversity can confer advantages under certain circumstances [20,21]. This concept of potential adaptions to adversity that promote context-specific abilities is not an alternative to a deficit model and should be used as a complementary perspective in considering the effects of early experiences on functioning. Further, recent work found that the association between connectivity between specific brain networks and cognitive test performance was in opposite directions depending on whether children lived above or below the poverty line [22], suggesting the possibility of context-specific adaptations that predict functioning.

Given that the majority of research in developmental science is conduct with middle-class parent–child dyads from WEIRD societies [23], researchers obtain a limited view of the range of the early environment that may be expected by a member of our species. In other words, researchers considering variations in children’s experiences may be overexposed, in relative terms, to highly responsive and child-centered care valued in WEIRD cultures [24]. Adverse caregiving and other environmental adversity may be less likely seen as aberrations of the human childhood experience when considered in the context of our evolutionary history. Fossil records of prehistoric children provide support that children worked in grueling physical conditions [25]. Threats not only from other humans, but also natural predators, likely resulted in a typical child in our past being exposed to frightening situations with greater regularity and with more serious consequences [26]. Historical records indicate a 27% mortality rate for infants, and that only approximately half of children lived to adulthood [27]. Comparatively, recent data from the U.S. indicate an infant mortality rate of less than .01%, and less than .02% from birth to adulthood [28]. Considering childhood and its expected environment from a historical lens may change our perceptions about what members of our species may have evolved to prepare for.

Sensitive period(s) for environmental information

In terms of when information about our environment is expected, our limited evidence suggests that earlier, as compared to later, experiences of adversity have a more profound impact on developing brain circuitry [29]. During this time the brain is undergoing rapid development, not only of the limbic regions associated with emotion and memory, but also the prefrontal cortex, with evidence that experiences of both experience-expectant and experience-dependent deviations influence prefrontal cortex volume and connectivity (see [30]). One study found that experience of sexual abuse at ages 3–5 years was associated with smaller hippocampal volume, though no association was found for sexual abuse occurring between the ages of 6–8 years [31]. Another study found that severity of stress from birth to age 5 years was associated with smaller hippocampal volume in early adolescence [32], while no association was found between stressful experiences in later childhood and hippocampal volume. Importantly, a growing body of research indicates that humans respond to variations in the environment much earlier than birth [33], highlighting that both prenatal and postnatal experiences shape brain development and subsequent behaviors.

Expectations from the caregivers’ perspective

These findings on the possibility of a sensitive period highlight the observation that some degree of caregiver responsiveness in early life is an element of the evolved ‘expected’ environment from the infants’ perspective. This highlights the need to consider what that means for what is expected of caregivers. Caregivers, and in particular, mothers, face considerable demands in caring for infants and young children. While evolutionary developmental psychologists tend to operationalize the likely fitness of offspring as a function of tradeoffs regarding parental options to either reproduce or conserve energy for their own growth (see [34]), it is possible that conservation could be seen in part through one’s socioemotional expenditure. For a parent, determining the costs and benefits of being an involved caregiver for an infant or young child may differ as a function of likely child mortality. Responsive and attentive caregiving for young children requires effort. In fact, declines in maternal sensitivity have been observed over the course of just 10 minutes of play with one’s infant [35]. Reasonable concerns about the likelihood of loss of the child, paired with the considerable effort of attentive and responsive care, may promote strategies (whether conscious or unconscious) to limit emotional closeness in a manner that may be adaptive for caregivers (for modern examples see [36]). Psychological commitment to children is believed to be influenced by foster parents’ concern that their foster child will be reunited with the child’s biological parents [37]. Sociodemographic factors associated with the experience of stress and adversity (e.g., lower income, more children) are related to placing a relatively lower value on responsive and sensitive caregiving [38]. Thus, given closer consideration of our evolutionary timescale, in which loss and the experience of adversity were more prevalent, it may be that highly sensitive and responsive parenting is not the standard for our species. Assumptions that the natural tendency is for caregivers to be sensitive and the associated implicit expectations placed on caregivers may actually undermine prevention and intervention efforts aimed at improving the caregiving environment [39].

Conclusions

While non-human animal models can definitely assign causality in the relations between adversity and outcomes, a large body of longitudinal studies in humans indicate that early adverse experiences have widespread consequences on cognitive, social, and emotional functioning, with the most severe outcomes associated with severe deprivation that violates experience-expectant caregiving and which occur earliest in life. Future research will benefit from a careful consideration of the types of early adversity and the timing of these experiences so that potential sensitive periods may be identified. Further, the degree to which different types of human experiences, including exposure to stressful, even life-threatening experiences, as well as caregiver insensitivity, may be less likely to be seen as significant deviations of the human experience when placed in the broader evolutionary context. What we consider adverse by modern standards could reflect a shift in perspective and expectations.

In applying the evolutionary-informed framework to understanding the expected environment, future research may be usefully guided by three over-arching goals. First, with the understanding that our current conceptions of the “ideal” caregiving environment may not be either culturally or phylogenetically sensitive, one goal must be to identify how to better assess and quantify the characteristics of a sensitive, responsive, and reliable caregiving environment and how that may vary both within and across different developmental contexts. A second goal must be to integrate developmental timing of brain plasticity, historical survival rates, and expectations for independence in childhood into future theory and analyses. Lastly, it will be imperative to determine how best to support the kinds of caregiving environments, particularly during foundational periods of development, that today we have evidence are associated with thriving. Assuming that caregiver sensitivity is what is “natural” and expected may interfere with efforts to develop interventions to improve caregiving, and undermine those caregivers who might benefit most from intervention.

Highlights.

  • Environmental information is necessary for building the developing brain

  • Experience-expectant and experience-dependent information are likely to differ in their impact

  • Most forms of early adversity are experience-dependent variation, and are likely not species-atypical

  • Though human thriving is associated with sensitive and responsive caregiving, such caregiving environments may not be universally expected

Acknowledgements

Dr. Humphreys received support from the Jacobs Foundation Early Career Research Fellowship (2017-1261-05). Dr. Salo received support from the National Institutes of Health (F32HD100079).

Annotated bibliography

King LS, Humphreys KL, Gotlib IH: The neglect–enrichment continuum: Characterizing variation in early caregiving environments. Dev Rev 2019, 51:109–122.

Presents a new framework for conceptualizing the early caregiving environment along a continuum from severe neglect to environmental enrichment, and provides preliminary evidence for the separability of emotional and cognitive input as two key aspects of caregiving entichment.

McLaughlin KA, Weissman D, Bitrán D: Childhood Adversity and Neural

Development: A Systematic Review. Annu Rev Dev Psychol 2019, 1:277–312.

Provides a synthesis of the existing literature examining the link between early adversity and brain development, with a focus on those associations which are unique to either the experience of threat or deprivation.

Hodel AS: Rapid infant prefrontal cortex development and sensitivity to early environmental experience. Dev Rev 2018, 48:113–144.

Challenges previous characterisations of the prefrontal cortex as ‘late developing’, presenting a systematic review of the evidence that this region in fact exhibits rapid early development and is highly sensitive to variations in the early environment.

Humphreys KL, King LS, Sacchet MD, Camacho MC, Colich NL, Ordaz SJ, Ho TC, Gotlib IH: Evidence for a sensitive period in the effects of early life stress on hippocampal volume. Dev Sci 2019, 22:1–10.

Utilizing retrospective report, this empirical study provides evidence for a link between stress

and trauma experienced in earlier, as compared to later, childhood, and reduced hippocampal volume in adolescence.

Frankenhuis WE, Young ES, Ellis BJ: The hidden talents approach: Theoretical and methodological challenges. Trends Cogn Sci 2020, 24:569–581.

Reviews evidence of adaptions to adversity, including the potential for both cognitive deficits and advantages, depending on the context and specific cognitive skill in question.

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

Declarations of interest: none

References

  • *1.Greenough WT, Black JE, Wallace CS: Experience and Brain Development. Child Dev 1987, 58:539. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Nelson CA, Gabard-Durnam LJ: Early adversity and critical periods: Neurodevelopmental consequences of violating the expectable environment. Trends Neurosci 2020, doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2020.01.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Werker JF, Hensch TK: Critical Periods in Speech Perception: New Directions. Annu Rev Psychol 2015, 66:173–196. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Hensch TK: Critical Periods in Cortical Development. In The Neurobiology of Brain and Behavioral Development. . Elsevier Inc.; 2018:133–151. [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Humphreys KL, Zeanah CH: Deviations from the expectable environment in early childhood and emerging psychopathology. Neuropsychopharmacology 2015, 40:154–170. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Gleason MM, Fox NA, Drury S, Smyke AT, Egger HL, Nelson CA, Gregas MC, Zeanah CH: Validity of evidence-derived criteria for reactive attachment disorder: Indiscriminately social/disinhibited and emotionally withdrawn/inhibited types. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2011, 50:216–231. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Shonkoff JP, Garner AS: The lifelong effects of early childhood adversity and toxic stress. Pediatrics 2012, 129:e232–46. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Giovannoni J: Definitional issues in child maltreatment. In Child maltreatment: Theory and research on the causes and consequences of child abuse and neglect. Edited by Cicchetti D, Carlson V. Cambridge University Press; 1989:3–37. [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Machlin L, Miller AB, Snyder J, McLaughlin KA, Sheridan MA: Differential associations of deprivation and threat with cognitive control and fear conditioning in early childhood. Front Behav Neurosci 2019, 13. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • *10.McLaughlin KA, Weissman D, Bitrán D: Childhood Adversity and Neural Development: A Systematic Review. Annu Rev Dev Psychol 2019, 1:277–312. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Lee A, Poh JS, Wen DJ, Tan HM, Chong YS, Tan KH, Gluckman PD, Fortier MV., Rifkin-Graboi A, Qiu A: Maternal care in infancy and the course of limbic development. Dev Cogn Neurosci 2019, doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2019.100714. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Thijssen S, Muetzel RL, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Jaddoe VWV, Tiemeier H, Verhulst FC, White T, Van Ijzendoorn MH: Insensitive parenting may accelerate the development of the amygdala-medial prefrontal cortex circuit. Dev Psychopathol 2017, doi: 10.1017/S0954579417000141. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Kok R, Thijssen SJ, Bakermans-Kranenburg M, Jaddoe VW, Verhulst FC, White T, van IJzendoorn MH, Tiemeier H: Normal Variation in Early Parental Sensitivity Predicts Child Structural Brain Development. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2015, 54:824–831. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Winnicott DW: Primary maternal preoccupation. In The Maternal Lineage: Identification, Desire, and Transgenerational Issues. . 1956:59–66. [Google Scholar]
  • *15.King LS, Humphreys KL, Gotlib IH: The neglect–enrichment continuum: Characterizing variation in early caregiving environments. Dev Rev 2019, 51:109–122. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Frankenhuis WE, Tiokhin L: Bridging Evolutionary Biology and Developmental Psychology: Toward An Enduring Theoretical Infrastructure. Child Dev 2018, 89:2303–2306. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Frankenhuis WE, Panchanathan K, Nettle D: Cognition in harsh and unpredictable environments. Curr Opin Psychol 2016, 7:76–80. [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Frankenhuis WE, Nettle D: The strengths of people in poverty. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 2020, 29:096372141988115. [Google Scholar]
  • *19.Frankenhuis WE, Young ES, Ellis BJ: The hidden talents approach: Theoretical and methodological challenges. Trends Cogn Sci 2020, 24:569–581. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Frankenhuis WE, Vries SA, Bianchi J, Ellis BJ: Hidden talents in harsh conditions? A preregistered study of memory and reasoning about social dominance. Dev Sci 2019, doi: 10.1111/desc.12835. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Pollak SD, Sinha P: Effects of early experience on children’s recognition of facial displays of emotion. Dev Psychol 2002, 38:784–791. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Ellwood-Lowe ME, Whitfield-Gabrieli S, Bunge SA: What is an adaptive pattern of brain activity for a child? It depends on their environment. bioRxiv 2020, doi: 10.1101/2020.05.29.124297. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Nielsen M, Haun D, Kärtner J, Legare CH: The persistent sampling bias in developmental psychology: A call to action. J Exp Child Psychol 2017, 162:31–38. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Henrich J, Heine SJ, Norenzayan A: The weirdest people in the world? Behav Brain Sci 2010, doi: 10.1017/S0140525X0999152X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Pany-Kucera D, Kern A, Reschreiter H: Children in the mines? Tracing potential childhood labour in salt mines from the Early Iron Age in Hallstatt, Austria. Child Past 2019, doi: 10.1080/17585716.2019.1638554. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Hart D: Man the Hunted. In Man the Hunted. . 2018. [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Volk AA, Atkinson JA: Infant and child death in the human environment of evolutionary adaptation. Evol Hum Behav 2013, doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2012.11.007. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Thakrar AP, Forrest AD, Maltenfort MG, Forrest CB: Child Mortality In The US And 19 OECD Comparator Nations: A 50-Year Time-Trend Analysis. Health Aff 2018, 37:140–149. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Gee DG: Sensitive periods of emotion regulation: Influences of parental care on frontoamygdala circuitry and plasticity. In Maternal brain plasticity: Preclinical and human research and implications for intervention. Edited by Rutherford HJV, Mayes LC. 2016:87–110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • *30.Hodel AS: Rapid infant prefrontal cortex development and sensitivity to early environmental experience. Dev Rev 2018, 48:113–144. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Andersen SL, Tomada A, Vincow ES, Valente E, Polcari A, Teicher MH: Preliminary evidence for sensitive periods in the effect of childhood sexual abuse on regional brain development. J Neuropsychiatr 2008, 20:292–301. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • *32.Humphreys KL, King LS, Sacchet MD, Camacho MC, Colich NL, Ordaz SJ, Ho TC, Gotlib IH: Evidence for a sensitive period in the effects of early life stress on hippocampal volume. Dev Sci 2019, 22:1–10. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Hartman S, Belsky J: Prenatal programming of postnatal plasticity revisited - And extended. Dev Psychopathol 2018, 30:825–842. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Frankenhuis WE, Panchanathan K, Clark Barrett H: Bridging developmental systems theory and evolutionary psychology using dynamic optimization. Dev Sci 2013, 16:584–598. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.King LS, Rangel E, Simpson N, Tikotzky L, Manber R: Mothers’ postpartum sleep disturbance is associated with the ability to sustain sensitivity toward infants. Sleep Med 2020, doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2019.07.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Scheper-Hughes N: Culture, Scarcity, and Maternal Thinking: Maternal Detachment and Infant Survival in a Brazilian Shantytown. Ethos 1985, doi: 10.1525/eth.1985.13.4.02a00010. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Dozier M, Lindhiem O: This is my child: Differences among foster parents in commitment to their young children. Child Maltreat 2006, doi: 10.1177/1077559506291263. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Mesman J, van IJzendoorn M, Behrens K, Carbonell OA, Cárcamo R, Cohen-Paraira I, de la Harpe C, Ekmekçi H, Emmen R, Heidar J, et al. : Is the ideal mother a sensitive mother? Beliefs about early childhood parenting in mothers across the globe. Int J Behav Dev 2016, 40:385–397. [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Ganz Z: Attachment Theory’s Universality Hypothesis: Clinical Implications for Culturally Responsive Assessment. Smith Coll Stud Soc Work 2018, 88:262–281. [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES