Skip to main content
Neuronal Signaling logoLink to Neuronal Signaling
. 2021 Mar 12;5(1):NS20200011. doi: 10.1042/NS20200011

Signalling pathways contributing to learning and memory deficits in the Ts65Dn mouse model of Down syndrome

Aimée Freeburn 1, Robert Gordon Keith Munn 1,2,
PMCID: PMC7955101  PMID: 33763235

Abstract

Down syndrome (DS) is a genetic trisomic disorder that produces life-long changes in physiology and cognition. Many of the changes in learning and memory seen in DS are reminiscent of disorders involving the hippocampal/entorhinal circuit. Mouse models of DS typically involve trisomy of murine chromosome 16 is homologous for many of the genes triplicated in human trisomy 21, and provide us with good models of changes in, and potential pharmacotherapy for, human DS. Recent careful dissection of the Ts65Dn mouse model of DS has revealed differences in key signalling pathways from the basal forebrain to the hippocampus and associated rhinal cortices, as well as changes in the microstructure of the hippocampus itself. In vivo behavioural and electrophysiological studies have shown that Ts65Dn animals have difficulties in spatial memory that mirror hippocampal deficits, and have changes in hippocampal electrophysiological phenomenology that may explain these differences, and align with expectations generated from in vitro exploration of this model. Finally, given the existing data, we will examine the possibility for pharmacotherapy for DS, and outline the work that remains to be done to fully understand this system.

Keywords: Down Syndrome, GABA, Hippocampus, Learning and Memory, Ts65Dn

Cognitive changes in individuals with Down syndrome

On an average, individuals with Down syndrome (DS) on average account for approximately a third of cases of intellectual disability, and typically have lowered IQ scores in general, typically in the range of 30–70 which indicates moderate to mild impairment [1,2]. However, these changes are not uniform: full-scale IQ in individuals with DS can have a large range of scores, despite generally poorer performance on verbal and short-term memory components of these tests [3–8]. DS is often also comorbid with other disorders of executive function or attention such as attention-deficit disorder [6,7]. Adults with DS also often experience disorders of emotion such as depression [8–10], or anxiety disorders such as obsessive-compulsive disorder [11]. Individuals with DS typically have impaired episodic memory [12,13], with impairments in spatial memory of specific interest. Although this is typically assessed as deficits in spatial working memory using an object/location paradigm [14,15], there is evidence that DS is accompanied by poorer long-term spatial memory when allocentric navigation is involved [16]. This constellation of findings implicates impaired hippocampal function as the basis for memory disorder in DS.

The hippocampal/medial entorhinal cortex circuit and its role in memory and learning

Decades of research have centred the hippocampus and medial entorhinal cortex (MEC) as the neural substrates enabling episodic and spatial memory [17–28]. The discovery of spatially tuned cells in the hippocampus – dubbed ‘place’ cells due to the spatial specificity of their firing [17] – and then of the grid [18], border, direction [19–21], and speed cells [20,21] within MEC led to the idea that these regions function as a cognitive ‘map’ of space. Lesion of the hippocampus tends to lead to more profound spatial memory deficits, while lesion of entorhinal cortex leads to more subtle changes in spatial memory, such as destabilising place cell activity [22]. Inactivation of the hippocampus with GABA agonists does not abolish both places’ cells activity and grid periodicity in the MEC [23], suggesting a strong reciprocal connection between these regions.

Phasic organisation of information flow in hippocampus and MEC is critical to their function

Local field potential (LFP) in the hippocampus is dominated by the large, regular, 5–12 Hz theta oscillation seen as an animal moves through space. Theta has been demonstrated to be fundamental to normal hippocampal functioning theta in the hippocampus is central to learning and memory. First noted by Winson in 1978; the abolition of theta within the hippocampus is correlated with impairments in spatial navigation [24]. Reduction in hippocampal theta was then rapidly shown to correlate to imapirments in many tasks requiring memory, from spatial alternation [25,26] and reversal [27] to operant tasks [28]. Optogenetic disruption of hippocampal theta while maintaining innervation from the medial septum to the hippocampus produces impairments in spatial performance [29] demonstrating the functional relevance of theta itself to learning and memory.

Ongoing activity within the hippocampus is almost invariably organised relative to theta and is typically phasically entrained to theta rhythm. A core theta phase dependency is the phenomenon of place cell precession: as an animal moves through the firing field of the place cell, it tends to ‘precess’ on the underlying theta wave [30,31], linking spatial information (the location of the animal) to temporal information (the phase of theta). The separate phases of encoding and retrieval (SPEAR) model proposes that spiking of cells at different phases of theta segregates the resultant information flow into encoding and retrieval epochs [32]. Encoding of new associations occurs during the phase of theta during which input from the entorhinal cortex is strongest; while memory retrieval occurs when CA1 input is dominated by CA3 projections [33]. Theta is also a prominent rhythm within the MEC, which also arises from projections from medial septum [34–36]. Grid cells in the MEC have also been shown to precess through theta in each of their firing fields [37–40]. Likewise, reducing theta output from medial septum to MEC results in the regular hexagonal structure of grid cells in MEC degrading. Other cells in the MEC have been shown to be related to theta, with head direction cells being often strongly modulated by theta, and occasionally theta ‘skipping’, such that they are only active on every other theta oscillation [41,42]. Organisation on the theta wave is a clear motif organising the activity of the entire hippocampal/entorhinal circuit. Given the central role of phasic organisation of hippocampal activity in the successful encoding and retrieval information, it is reasonable expect changes in this organisation to be key feature of disorders of learning and memory such as DS. Understanding the flow of information within this circuit, as well as its major inputs, is central to understanding how differences in signalling within the circuit might produce learning and memory deficits.

Innervation of the hippocampus from medial septum and the origin of theta rhythm

The hippocampus receives input from a wide range of neocortical inputs. Perhaps the most well-known and well-studied projections to both the hippocampus and entorhinal cortex are the ascending excitatory and inhibitory projections from medial septum. Lesion work has demonstrated that hippocampal theta is conducted via the basal forebrain regions of the medial septum and diagonal band of broca [43–46], and damage to septum not only abolishes theta in the hippocampus, but produces profound spatial memory deficits similar to lesion of the hippocampus itself [24,47,48]. The septum contains cholinergic, GABAergic, and glutamatergic neurons, and all three types send long-range projections to the hippocampus. Several projections to the hippocampus underlie spatial processing (see Figure 1), at the core of which is a ‘trisynaptic loop’ from layer II of the MEC through to the dentate gyrus, CA3, and CA1 in addition to direct, unfiltered projections from MEC layers III/IV to CA1 (Figure 1). A major hippocampal input is comprised of the perforant pathway in which relatively few (∼200000 in rat) EC layer II cells diverge into relatively many ∼1000000 granule cells in the dentate gyrus, enabling the ‘pattern separation’ of memory into encoding cues such as ‘place’, ‘size’, and ‘colour’. Mossy fibres from the dentate gyrus then converge into ∼160000 pyramidal cells in the CA3 field, which are highly reciprocally connected. CA3 is where auto-associative ‘pattern completion’ occurs to translate representative cues into unique, stable episodic memories which can be easily recalled following future cues [49–52]. This information is fed forward to area CA1, in which stable place representations are formed, modified, and then fed out of the hippocampus.

Figure 1. Major connectivity within HPC and between HPC and MEC.

Figure 1

The most well-known and canonical loop within the hippocampus is the trisynaptic loop (orange) originating from layer II of MEC and travelling via the perforant path to synapse at neurons in the dentate gyrus. From there, the second synapses of the pathway are made at neurons in CA3, and the third at CA1, before the pathway travels back to the MEC layer III via the subiculum. However, another connection to CA3 directly from layer II exists (purple) [67,68], and MEC layer III synapses directly on to neurons in CA1 (the temporoammonic pathway, blue) [69]. Layer IV/V has a reciprocal connection with CA1 (yellow), and intrinsically connects with layers II and III (green) [70].

A major relay of hippocampal theta is the medial septal nucleus, which sends dense excitatory cholinergic [53] and glutamatergic [54,55] connections to the hippocampus and parahippocampal regions. It also sends inhibitory GABAergic projections to these regions [56]. While all three projections innervate the hippocampus, there are some fundamental differences in their patterns of connectivity: GABAergic projections from MS tend to target inhibitory interneurons in the hippocampus, particularly in area CA1 [57]. The phasic firing of interneurons within the hippocampus is thought to synchronise the firing of pyramidal cells [58]. Learning and spatial memory appear to critically depend on these GABAergic projections [59,60], while both cholinergic and GABAergic projections from the medial septum are essential for hippocampal theta rhythm [61]. However, the origins of theta rhythm do not lie in the medial septum, but rather information from various other brain nuclei coding for frequency and amplitude which are integrated in medial septum and relayed on. Careful dissection of this circuit has demonstrated that the frequency of hippocampal theta rhythm is dependent on the posterior hypothalamus, which is through to control the gain or amplitude of theta [62]. Input to the medial septum from the supramammiliary bodies [63,64] and the reticular formation [65,66] have also been implicated as important inputs in theta generation.

Animal models of DS

The pathogenesis of DS is poorly defined. DS is a contiguous gene syndrome that spans 35 Mb of the long arm of human chromosome 21 (Hsa21), resulting in increased expression of a subset of encoded genes. An extra copy of one or more genes or regulatory sequences on Hsa21 and associated underlie the DS phenotype [71]. A multiplicity of transgenic mouse models for candidate genes has been developed to study of the complex genotype–phenotype interactions, identification of dosage-sensitive genes and potential therapeutic targets (Figure 2). However, orthologues of Hsa21 map to differing segments across mouse chromosomes – primarily Mmu16, followed by internal segments of Mmu17 and Mmu10, which makes trisomy 21 difficult to model (Table 1). Illuminates the extant mouse models for DS and murine chromosome segments that are triplicated in each. While Hsa21 encodes for 161 protein-coding genes and 5 microRNAs, only 157 of these are conserved in the mouse [72]. Likewise, some murine genes do not have human homologues (i.e. Itg21 located within the Hsa21 homologous region) which may carry results in genetic modifications which may undermine accurate DS phenotyping [73].

Figure 2. Comparison of the genes involved in mouse models of DS.

Figure 2

Hsa21 comprises 552 genes in its long arm, and 166 of these (dashed line) are homologues to chromosomes found on murine (Mmu) chromosomes 10 (purple),16 (green), and 17 (red). Unlike other models of DS, the TS model recapitulates almost completely the anatomical, neurobiological, and behavioural phenotypes of human DS including deficits in cognition and memory, neuroanatomical characteristics, low life expectancy and age-related cognitive decline reminiscent of Alzheimer’s disease (AD) [75–77]. However, a drawback of the TS mouse is that it carries three extra copies of an extra segment with non-DS related genes arising from Mmu17, including ∼35 protein-coding genes. Various other mouse models have been developed that involve trisomy of chromosomes 10, 16, and 17 [78–81]. The Ts2Cje mouse involves the same portion of Mmu16 as is triplicated in the Ts65DN mouse, but this sequence is translocated to Mmu12. The TS mouse has relatively good construct validity, triplicating a large number of the same genes as seen in human DS, but its construct validity is not perfect due to incomplete triplication of all triplicated Hsa21 genes and its inclusion of non-triplicated genes in DS on the trisomic portion of Mmu17. This being the case, it has long been considered the best DS model available due to its extremely high, face validity, as both the structural and behavioural differences seen in this model map most closely to human DS (Tables 2 and 3). A more recently developed model combining the Dp(10)1Yey/+, Dp(16)1Yey/+, and Dp(17)1Yey/+ models has produced a promising alternative model to Ts65Dn, possessing very high construct validity, and the same learning and memory deficits seen in DS and the TS model [82].

Table 1. Mouse models that involve triplication of segments of specific murine chromosomes.

Triplicated chromosomes(s) Mmu 16 segment Mmu 17 segment Mmu 10 segment Mmu 16,17, and 10 segments
Mouse models Dp(16)1Yey/+
Ts65DN
Ts2Cje,
Ts1Cje
Ms1Ts65
Ts1Rhr
Ts1Rhr Dep(17)1Yey/+
Ts1Yah
Dp(10)1Yey/+ Dp(10)1Yey/+;
Dp(16)1Yey/+;
Dp(17)1Yey/+

Table 2. Comparison of neuronal changes observed in human DS and the Ts65DN mouse model.

DS (Human) Ts65DN
Brain volume
Neuronal density
Cerebellar volume
Neurogenesis Slow cell cycle Slow cell cycle
Impaired proliferation of neural precursors Impaired proliferation of neural precursors
Impaired differentiation Impaired differentiation
Dendrite morphology Reduced density Reduced density
Abnormal morphology Abnormal morphology
Electrophysiology EEG abnormalities
EEE coherence abnormalities
EEG abnormalities
EEG coherence abnormalities impaired long-term potentiation

Adapted from [83].

Table 3. Summary of the behavioural similarities between human DS and mouse models of DS.

DS (Human) Ts65Dn Dep(17)1Yey/+ Dep(10)1Yey/+ Ts1Yah
Spatial context discrimination = =
Spatial learning and memory = =
Working memory

Ts65Dn animals share many similar deficits (down arrow) in learning and memory with human DS compared with control, while some other mouse models are either unaffected (=) or enhanced (up arrow) compared with control. Adapted from [78].

The TS65Dn model was the first viable segmental trisomy model for DS [84], and involves trisomy of the distal portion of chromosome 16 in the TS model results in extra copies of ∼115 genes homologous to Hsa21. Among replicated genes is the amyloid precursor protein (APP) gene – a primary genetic substrate of early-onset Alzheimer’s disease (AD) and BFCN loss in DS patients [71,74].

Nevertheless, the Ts65Dn model remains the leading mouse model of DS, owing to its relatively uncomplicated genetic triplication, its wide availability, and its very high face and construct validity. In the following sections, as we consider changes in the Ts65Dn mouse in the context of human DS, it will become apparent that the central features of DS are well replicated in the Ts65Dn mouse, confirming its place as the eminent mouse model of DS.

Behavioural differences in the Ts65Dn model

In order to be a good model for human DS, a mouse model should not only triplicate candidate genes for the disorder, but should recapitulate the behavioural phenotype of human DS. One of the most striking differences in human DS, and perhaps one of the most problematic to those with DS, are difficulties with learning and memory [16,84–87]. Crucially, allocentric spatial memory, perhaps the primary measure used to study learning and memory in rodents, is impaired in individuals with DS, allowing us to easily probe the Ts65Dn model for similarities in behavioural output demonstrating impaired memory.

Learning and memory

Since the inception of the Ts65Dn model, it has been observed that the performance of TS animals in classic tests of spatial memory such as the Morris Water Maze is impaired [88–92]. In this test, depending on the exact protocol, animals must learn the position of a hidden platform in a water-filled maze, and then must remember the location of the platform on subsequent training days. Ts65Dn animals show impaired learning – they take longer to find the platform throughout training compared with control. A probe test with the platform absent tests recollection of the location of the platform. Ts65Dn animals spent less time searching the correct quadrant of the maze compared with control. Further investigation has demonstrated that Ts65Dn animals are impaired in a wide array of behavioural tests. They tend to perform poorly relative to non-trisomic animals in tests of context discrimination [93] in which animals remember a spatial location in which they previously received a footshock, as well as in tests of working memory [94] and novel object recognition [95,96]. Olfactory learning in Ts65Dn mice is also impaired [97]. Ts65Dn learned the radial arm maze significantly more slowly than control animals, and performed worse in a test of their delayed retention of this task [98], in a dry-land recapitulation of their learning and memory deficits uncovered in the Morris Water Maze. This constellation of behavioural deficits are classically reminiscent of those caused by hippocampal damage in both rodents and humans, and implicate the hippocampal-entorhinal region in the signalling abnormalities central to this model in specific, and DS in general. There is one important caveat to consider in the interpretation of memory deficits in the Ts65Dn model. In order for differences in behavioural tests of learning and memory to be correctly interpreted, other explanations for differences in behaviour between DS model animals and control animals must be ruled out. One of the most obvious potential causes of differences between groups in tests like the Morris Water Maze are differences in the motor output of the animals. Indeed, differences in nociception and proprioception also appear to be a central change in the Ts65Dn model. Ts65Dn animals show decreased responsiveness to behavioural tests of pain [99], and show a variety of subtle deficits in fine motor control, including abnormal paw-print placement that indicated less regularity in stride combined with shorter stride length [100,101]. Ts65Dn animals also spend less time balanced on the roto-rod, and slower and more variable swimming speed in the water maze [100]. Some of these differences in motor control may be due to altered muscle development in Ts65Dn [102,103], but this model also shows altered spinal nerve development [104]. It is also possible that these changes arise from divergence in the central generation of motor output and reception of the motor efferent copy. Whatever the cause, these behavioural differences must be considered when interpreting differences in task performance between Wildtype and Ts65Dn animals. In spite of these caveats, the array of different tests demonstrating clear impairments in memory in the Ts65Dn animal (some unlikely to be contaminated by the aforementioned differences) make it very unlikely that these changes in motor output can account for all of the deficits seen in Ts65Dn animals.

Neuronal pathways altered in the Ts65Dn model

The observation that individuals with DS develop Alzheimer’s-like pathology at rates far greater than the general population prompted much interest in putative differences in the neurotransmitter systems known to be involved in this pathology between DS and unaffected individuals. Cholinergic dysfunction is one of the central changes in AD. Individuals diagnosed with AD show a loss of cholinergic neurons in the basal forebrain [105–107]. In a similar manner to humans with AD-like pathology, the Ts65Dn mouse shows marked reduction in the number and size of cholinergic neurons in the basal forebrain [108–110]. thought to be the result of impaired retrograde transport of nerve growth factor (NGF) [111] As might be expected, the number of cholinergic neurons correlates with scores of attention [109]. Interestingly in this model, the number of cholinergic neurons in the basal forebrain decreased in with age in control animals, but the number of neurons did not decline further in the Ts65Dn animals as they aged, suggesting that the attentional deficits that appear with AD-like pathology are related directly to changes in basal cholinergic neurotransmission. This deficit in cholinergic transmission in the Ts65Dn model appears to be neurodevelopmental. Intraventricular delivery of NGF reverses the abnormal neuronal number of cholinergic neurons in the basal forebrain [111], and choline supplementation in utero appears to both normalise the number of cholinergic neurons in the basal forebrain in the resultant offspring in adulthood, and to some extent might rescue the learning and memory deficits associated with impaired cholinergic transmission [112]. Choline supplementation has been shown to affect the RNA expression in CA1 of key genes involved in calcium signalling and synaptic plasticity, indicating a long-term rescue by this intervention [113]. Recent evidence has emerged that suggests the picture of impaired cognition associated with reduced cholinergic signalling from the basal forebrain is more complicated. Pharmacological treatment with the acetylcholinesterase inhibitor Donepezil increases the level of acetylcholine, but does not affect learning and memory in the Ts65Dn model. By contrast, the GABA(A) antagonist Pentylenetrazole produced a robust increase in learning and memory performance by these animals [77]. Since the major outputs from the medial septum to learning and memory critical regions such as hippocampus and entorhinal cortex are a push-pull mix of excitatory cholinergic and inhibitory GABAergic projections (with a relatively recently uncovered sizable minority glutamatergic projection [55]), it seems likely that the ratio of cholinergic to GABAergic output is critical to maintaining normal activity within hippocampus. Indeed, cholinergic activation has been postulated to control the ‘gain’ or power of hippocampal theta rhythm through tonic activation of hippocampus, while GABAergic activity may control the frequency through inhibitory or disinhibitory action on hippocampal interneurons [114]. In addition to reduced cholinergic output from the basal forebrain in Ts65Dn animals, there is a marked up-regulation in choline transferase (CHAT) in regions receiving output from this region such as hippocampus and temporal cortex, suggesting a compensatory mechanism to reduced basal acetylcholine input [115].

Given the central involvement of output from medial septum in the generation of theta rhythm and the central alterations of key neurotransmission systems in septum in DS models, it is reasonable to expect changes in key aspects of hippocampal rhythmicity in DS models. This expectation is emerging as the case. Very recently theta power was shown to be increased in Ts65Dn mice during sleep [116]. The frequency of hippocampal theta and the phasic relationship of hippocampal theta to gamma oscillations has very recently been shown to be altered in the Ts65Dn model [117]. Moreover, the firing of CA1 place cells in the Ts65Dn model has been shown to be abnormally phase-locked to the trough of theta, in contrast with control cells, which precess normally through all phases of theta [117]. These changes may explain the mechanism of the learning and memory deficits in this model. The routing of information into encoding and retrieval epochs is thought to be tightly controlled by the phase of theta at which spiking occurs [32], and long-range communication between hippocampus and entorhinal regions is thought to depend on gamma oscillations, which are likewise phasically entrained to theta [118–121].

Changes in inhibitory GABAergic connections

A very recent finding shows that the phenomenology of interneurons in Ts65Dn mice are also changed; the inhibitory connections between somatostatin positive Martinotti cells and excitatory pyramidal cells are dramatically strengthened in the Ts65Dn animal, and parvalbumin positive interneurons become more excitable, but lose their tendency to enter fast-spiking epochs [122]. Critically, these changes were seen to increase the tendency of pyramidal cells to become phase-locked to network oscillations. Other work has shown an overall increase in inhibitory interneurons within the hippocampus; the changes again selectively sparing parvalbumin-positive interneurons [123]. Indeed, genetic down-regulation of GABA(A) receptors expressing the α-5 subunit, which are primarily found in the hippocampus [124,125]. improves both the structure and phenomenology of the hippocampus in Ts65Dn animals, it rectifies some of the behavioural differences in these animals [126]. Figure 3 summarises these changes in signalling.

Figure 3. Summary of theta generation mechanisms and known signalling changes in the Ts65Dn mouse (grey text).

Figure 3

The motor efferent copy arrives in the reticular formation (1) where it is relayed by excitatory projections from the medial supramammiliary nucleus and posterior hypothalamus to the medial septum [62,64,136–138]. The resulting theta rhythm is relayed to the hippocampal formation by GABAergic and cholinergic output from septum (3). In DS, GABAergic output from the suprachiasmatic nucleus to the medial septum is enhanced. (2) Cholinergic output from the basal forebrain (inc. medial septum) is diminished, and this tends to increase with age. This alters the excitatory (green)/inhibitory (red) output balance of the medial septum, resulting in excess inhibitory control. This is coupled (4) with increased connectivity of somatostatin-positive interneurons with hippocampal neurons and reduced dendritic arbourisation and cell loss among pyramidal neurons, which leads to abnormal theta rhythm within the hippocampus, and increased phase-locking of place cells to the ongoing theta wave.

In vitro signalling

Plasticity within the hippocampus in the form of long-term potentiation (LTP) and long-term depression (LTD) have (not uncontroversially) been mooted as neural substrates of learning and memory [127]. In the Ts65Dn mouse, LTP in hippocampus is dramatically reduced [128–130], while LTD is facilitated [131]. Once again, an excess of inhibition, or at least a change in the excitation/inhibition balance is central to these changes [132,133], inhibitory post-synaptic potentials mediated by both GABA(A) and GABA(B) are more efficient in the Ts65Dn model [134]. Inverse agonism of GABA receptors expressing the α-5 subunit reverses these changes [135]. Another study, while failing to find differences in LTP between trisomic and disomic animals using high-frequency stimulation found a profound reduction in LTP induced by theta-burst stimulation in Ts65Dn animals [129]. Theta-burst LTP induction is sensitive to picrotoxin, underlining the role of altered GABA signalling in the hippocampus in the changes in hippocampal phenomenology seen in the Ts65Dn mouse.

Based on the dual roles of altered cholinergic input to the hippocampus and GABA signalling within the hippocampus, both systems appear to be mutually synergistic in producing the cognitive and behavioural deficits in the Ts65Dn model. It seems likely that reduced cholinergic output from the basal forebrain alters the finely tuned excitatory/inhibitory balance upon which septal-generated theta depends, and this change alters the frequency of the oscillation being generated by septum. This frequency disturbance, in combination with changes of the activity of interneurons in the hippocampus then likely cause all aspects of hippocampal function that depend on theta to organise their information flow to now fire at the wrong phase, and hence route their information suboptimally. This reduction in computational efficiency then likely causes the impairments in learning and memory central to DS.

Structural changes in the brain of Ts65Dn mice

One of the earliest discovered neural correlates of intellectual disabilities in humans is a pronounced change in the dendritic arbourisation of excitatory pyramidal cells in the cortex; [139] their dendritic arbour tends to be diffusely abnormal, either abnormally long or short, or a marked reduction in number. These differences in connectivity between neurons suggests a difference in neuronal contact is a key difference in DS. Further studies imply that these differences in dendritic morphology represent a developmental disorder; normal neuronal development goes awry during postnatal development. There is either increased branching of, or no difference in [140] the dendrites of pyramidal cells in layer III of prefrontal cortex of 2.5-month-old infants, but infants older than 4 months show marked shortening of the dendrites in this area [141]. Interestingly, ablation of the excitatory input from entorhinal cortex to the hippocampus causes dendritic atrophy in the granule cells in dentate gyrus reminiscent of the overall changes in DS, [142] suggesting that the origin of the neuronal dendritic changes in DS is a damping of excitatory input. Indeed, the dendritic spine protein drebrin is reduced in human DS, and in another similarity with AD, in the brains of patients with Alzheimer’s [143]. One of the central hypotheses concerning these changes is that there are differences in cellular development in DS from very early in life that produce abnormalities in cell-to-cell connectivity through contact at dendritic spines. While this may or may not result in structural abnormalities in spine formation and appearance, the underlying differences in cell-to-cell signalling nevertheless eventually causes connective failure of the synapses between neurons and a negative feedback cycle producing retraction of the dentritic contact. In the case of DS, the reduced excitatory contact between cells due to reduced cholinergic signalling may be the root cause of this connective failure. This constellation of differences could account for the key changes in cell number, arbourisation, and cell-to-cell contact seen in the Ts65DN mouse, and may be the key neurodevelopmental change in human DS.

Differences in hippocampal signalling

While it is clear that DS in mice is associated with a marked loss of neurons in the basal forebrain, and specifically in the medial septal area, there are also changes in other regions. While organisation of activity in hippocampus and between hippocampus and associated regions is abnormal in the Ts65Dn model, the physical structure of the hippocampus is likewise altered in this model. While the overall hippocampal volume of Ts65Dn mice is largely similar to control, Ts65Dn mice have many fewer granule cells in the dentate gyrus of the hippocampus and more neurons in region CA3 [144]. Other brain regions show similar changes. Olfactory piriform cortex in Ts65Dn animals contains neurons that are not only less dendritically arbourised; they make more inhibitory and fewer excitatory connections [145]. In temporal cortex, the selective reduction in number of asymmetric synapses and the increased synaptic zone of contact in Ts65Dn animals suggests a specific reduction in excitatory connections, and an attempt by the system to compensate by increasing the contact area of each synapse [146]. More recently, reductions in neuron density have been seen throughout the hippocampus in the Ts65Dn model, with lower neuronal number in CA1, and reduced neuron-to-synapse ratio in regions CA1, CA3, and dentate gyrus. In a strikingly similar manner to the rest of temporal cortex, there is a selective reduction in asymmetric synapses, suggesting again a selective reduction in excitatory contact [147]. While it is possible that these changes result from the primary changes, which appear to be reductions in cholinergic input from the basal forebrain, changes in the dentate gyrus, at least, seem to be due to failed cell proliferation and survival during development [148]. Adrenergic agonism has been shown to be effective in supporting the proliferation of granule cells in the dentate [149–151]. Indeed, very recent data have shown that neonatally treating Ts65Dn mice with a β-2 adrenergic agonist successfully restores normal dendritic development in hippocampal granule cells [150], suggesting that a prolonged lack of normal connectivity produces the abnormal dendritic arbourisation. Interestingly, this prophylactic treatment does not rectify the abnormal neurogenesis in the Ts65Dn model, although β-2 agonism was shown to increase neural proliferation in vitro. This difference suggests a multifactorial etiology for abnormal neural development in DS, and a putative degenerative cycle – a reduction in excitatory input causes a reduction in dendritic arbourisation, which in turn further alters the excitatory/inhibitory balance.

Origins of abnormal excitatory input to the hippocampal formation

Novel object recognition memory is impaired in Ts65Dn mice, but is restored by lesion of the suprachiasmatic nucleus [152]. This surprising finding aligns with behavioural data in humans with DS that show dysfunctional sleep is a central aspect of the syndrome [153–156]. Surgical ablation of the suprachiasmatic nucleus of Siberian hamsters produces DS-like learning and memory deficits in object recognition memory [157]. Critically, treatment with a GABA(A) antagonist restores object recognition memory to SCN-ablated hamsters [157]. Cholinergic neurons in the basal forebrain project to the SCN [158–160], and loss of this cholinergic input in DS and AD is likely to underlie the sleep problems common to both disorders. Paradoxically, lesion of the SCN in Ts65Dn animals restores their object recognition memory [152]. Major, but somewhat overlooked, outputs of the SCN are the septal nuclei [161]. The output of the SCN is largely GABAergic. One of the major hypotheses that has emerged is that the origin of the cognitive changes in Ts65Dn animals is increased GABAergic input to MS from SCN, and in turn reduced cholinergic output to HPC and EC from MS [157,162–164]. This reduction in cholinergic output may then upset the excitatory/inhibitory balance critical to theta regulation, altering the frequency of theta, upsetting the phasic relationship of hippocampal information flow, and reducing the efficiency of the learning and memory system. Within the hippocampal formation specifically, this damping of cholinergic input causes a reduction in the dendritic arbourisation of granule cells in the dentate gyrus, which reduces the excitatory signal still further. The reduced excitatory input travelling along the trisynaptic loop to CA1 meets a normal amount of GABAergic inhibitory input from local interneurons, and the signal is damped still further. It is possible that these changes lead to plastic changes including the loss of neurons within the hippocampus, and that these changes lead to the difficulties in learning and memory seen in this disorder. If the central difference is an alteration of the excitatory/inhibitory balance, the question of whether pharmacological rectification of this imbalance can reverse these changes is of key importance.

The potential of pharmacotherapy for DS and future directions

The balance of evidence is that the deficits in learning and memory seen in DS result from an alteration of the excitatory/inhibitory output balance from medial septum to hippocampus and associated rhinal cortex regions, coupled with changes in the interneuron architecture within the hippocampus [130,132]. This observation suggests that pharmacotherapy addressing this altered balance may have therapeutic potential. Animal models have so far shown that a single bolus of GABA(A) antagonists such as picrotoxin and pentylenetetrazol can reverse deficits in learning and memory in the Ts65Dn mouse [77,95], and that these effects can last for a much longer time than the drug persistence, suggesting an intermediate-term neuroplastic change. Clinical use of GABA antagonists in humans remains problematic due to the lowering of the seizure threshold and increased risk of seizure that results from GABA antagonism. Given the central changes in DS seem to result from globally increased GABAergic signalling and a reduction in the excitation/inhibition ratio, however, this caveat may be less clinically relevant in a DS population. Newer drugs that are selective for α-5 subunit containing GABA receptors offer some clinical benefit, as this subclass of GABA receptors has been shown to be highly involved in the cognitive deficits in the Ts65Dn model [126]. It is possible that these agents could produce the pro-cognitive effects of GABA antagonism with less of the seizure risk [165] While antagonism of GABA(A) receptors shows clear pro-cognitive promise in DS, the Ts65Dn mouse model also shows enhanced GABA(B) transmission in both the dentate gyrus and area CA1 of the hippocampus [130,134,166], offering a potential novel mechanism to treat learning and memory deficits in DS without the seizure liability. Another potential treatment is the indirect modulation of GABA neurotransmission through modulation of the 5-HT system. Chronic serotonin-specific reuptake inhibitor (SSRI) treatment modulates GABA transmission [167], and in the Ts65Dn mouse model, treatment early in life with the SSRI fluoxetine can prevent abnormal development of the hippocampus, and normalises hippocampus-dependent memory [168]. However, other evidence demonstrates a lack of pro-cognitive effect of adult SSRI treatment in the Ts65Dn model, and an increase in seizures and mortality [169], suggesting that early-life treatment may prevent neurodevelopmental changes in the Ts65Dn model that, after they have occurred, are resistant to subtle or indirect changes in GABA signalling. Given the modulation of 5-HT by GABA [170] in a synergistic feedback loop, altered GABA neurotransmission in the Ts65Dn model early in life may also change the 5-HT system.

The balance of evidence regarding the signalling changes in DS that result in learning and memory deficits seem to be able to be summarised as a change in the excitatory/inhibitory balance that results in abnormal function of the hippocampus. These chronic changes in excitatory balance produce long-term developmental changes that result in a variety of morphological changes in the hippocampus; an increase in interneuronal contact, a reduction in the dendritic arbourisation of pyramidal neurons, and an overall reduction in cell number. There is preliminary behavioural evidence that correcting the excitatory/inhibitory balance corrects to some degree the learning and memory aspects of cognitive function in DS. What is currently unclear is whether this pharmacological therapy restores normal hippocampal function, or whether this treatment corrects the many neurodevelopmental differences we see in DS. Future experiments could clarify these questions, and could clarify to what extent the changes in DS are reversible, and whether these changes are all critical for the learning and memory deficits in DS to be seen. Future research could also clarify if it is only pharmacological damping of the GABA signal that is effective, or if other mechanisms of increasing excitation may also be therapeutically relevant, opening myriad new potential pharmacotherapies for DS.

Acknowledgements

We wish to acknowledge Scidraw.io for elements of the figures used in this review.

Abbreviations

5-HT

Serotonin

AD

Alzheimer’s disease

DS

Down syndrome

GABA

Gamma aminobutyric acid

HPC

Hippocampus

Hsa21

human chromosome 21

LTD

long-term depression

LTP

long-term potentiation

MEC

medial entorhinal cortex

NGF

nerve growth factor

SCN

Suprachiasmatic nucleus

SSRI

serotonin-specific reuptake inhibitor

Competing Interests

The authors declare that there are no competing interests associated with the manuscript.

References

  • 1.Weijerman M.E. and de Winter J.P. (2010) Clinical practice: the care of children with Down syndrome. Eur. J. Pediatr. 169, 1445–1452 10.1007/s00431-010-1253-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Reilly C. (2012) Behavioural phenotypes and special educational needs: is aetiology important in the classroom? J. Intell. Disabil. Res. 56, 929–946 10.1111/j.1365-2788.2012.01542.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Jarrold C., Baddeley A.D. and Phillips C. (2007) Long-term memory for verbal and visual information in down syndrome and williams syndrome: performance on the Doors and People Test. Cortex 43, 233–247 10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70478-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Crissman B.G., Worley G., Roizen N. and Kishnani P.S. (2006) Current perspectives on Down syndrome: Selected medical and social issues. Am. J. Med. Genet. 142C, 127–130 10.1002/ajmg.c.30099 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Roizen N.J. and Patterson D. (2003) Down’s syndrome. Lancet 361, 1281–1289 10.1016/S0140-6736(03)12987-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Martínez M.H., Duran X.P. and Navarro J.N. (2011) Attention deficit disorder with or without hyperactivity or impulsivity in children with Down's syndrome. Int. Med. Rev. Down Syndr. 15, 18–22 10.1016/S2171-9748(11)70006-X [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Ekstein S., Glick B., Weill M., Kay B. and Berger I. (2011) Down Syndrome and Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD). J. Child Neurol. 26, 1290–1295 10.1177/0883073811405201 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Peñuelas B.G. (2010) Depression and Down syndrome. Int. Medical Rev. Down Syndr. 14, 14–16 10.1016/S2171-9748(10)70004-0 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Collacott R.A., Cooper S.-A. and McGrother C. (1992) Differential rates of psychiatric disorders in adults with Down’s syndrome compared with other mentally handicapped adults. Br. J. Psychiatry 161, 671–674 10.1192/bjp.161.5.671 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Myers B.A. and Pueschel S.M. (1991) Psychiatric disorders in persons with Down Syndrome. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 179, 609–613 10.1097/00005053-199110000-00004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Prasher V.P. and Day S. (1995) Brief report: Obsessive-compulsive disorder in adults with Down’s Syndrome. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 25, 453–458 10.1007/BF02179379 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Pennington B.F., Moon J., Edgin J., Stedron J. and Nadel L. (2003) The neuropsychology of Down Syndrome: evidence for hippocampal dysfunction. Child Dev. 74, 75–93 10.1111/1467-8624.00522 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Spanò G. and Edgin J.O. (2016) Everyday memory in individuals with Down syndrome: validation of the Observer Memory Questionnaire – Parent Form. Child Neuropsychol. 23, 1–13 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Lanfranchi S., Mammarella I.C. and Carretti B. (2014) Spatial-simultaneous working memory and selective interference in Down syndrome. Child Neuropsychol. 21, 481–489 10.1080/09297049.2014.913557 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Carretti B. and Lanfranchi S. (2010) The effect of configuration on VSWM performance of Down syndrome individuals. J. Intell. Disabil. Res. 54, 1058–1066 10.1111/j.1365-2788.2010.01334.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Lavenex P.B.et al. (2015) Allocentric spatial learning and memory deficits in Down syndrome. Front. Psychol. 6, 62 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00062 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.O'Keefe J. and Dostrovsky J. (1971) The hippocampus as a spatial map. Preliminary evidence from unit activity in the freely-moving rat. Brain Res. 34, 171–175 10.1016/0006-8993(71)90358-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Hafting T., Fyhn M., Molden S., Moser M.-B. and Moser E.I. (2005) Microstructure of a spatial map in the entorhinal cortex. Nature 436, 801–806 10.1038/nature03721 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Sargolini F.et al. (2006) Conjunctive representation of position, direction, and velocity in entorhinal cortex. Science 312, 758–762 10.1126/science.1125572 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Hinman J.R., Brandon M.P., Climer J.R., Chapman W.G. and Hasselmo M.E. (2016) Multiple running speed signals in medial entorhinal cortex. Neuron 91, 666–679 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.06.027 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Kropff E., Carmichael J.E., Moser M.-B. and Moser E.I. (2015) Speed cells in the medial entorhinal cortex. Nature 523, 419–424 10.1038/nature14622 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Hales J.B.et al. (2014) Medial entorhinal cortex lesions only partially disrupt hippocampal place cells and hippocampus-dependent place memory. Cell Rep. 9, 893–901 10.1016/j.celrep.2014.10.009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Bonnevie T.et al. (2013) Grid cells require excitatory drive from the hippocampus. Nat. Neurosci. 16, 309–317 10.1038/nn.3311 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Winson J. (1978) Loss of hippocampal theta rhythm results in spatial memory deficit in the rat. Science 201, 160–163 10.1126/science.663646 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Givens B.S. and Olton D.S. (1990) Cholinergic and GABAergic modulation of medial septal area: Effect on working memory. Behav. Neurosci. 104, 849–855 10.1037/0735-7044.104.6.849 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Aggleton J.P., Neave N., Nagle S. and Hunt P.R. (1995) A comparison of the effects of anterior thalamic, mamillary body and fornix lesions on reinforced spatial alternation. Behav. Brain Res. 68, 91–101 10.1016/0166-4328(94)00163-A [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.M'Harzi M.et al. (1987) Effects of selective lesions of Fimbria-Fornix on learning set in the rat. Physiol. Behav. 40, 181–188 10.1016/0031-9384(87)90205-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Numan R. and Quaranta J.R. (1990) Effects of medial septal lesions on operant delayed alternation in rats. Brain Res. 531, 232–241 10.1016/0006-8993(90)90779-B [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Kloc M.L., Velasquez F., Niedecker R.W., Barry J.M. and Holmes G.L. (2020) Disruption of hippocampal rhythms via optogenetic stimulation during the critical period for memory development impairs spatial cognition. Brain Stimuli 13, 1535–1547 10.1016/j.brs.2020.08.011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Tsodyks M.V., Skaggs W.E., Sejnowski T.J. and McNaughton B.L. (1996) Population dynamics and theta rhythm phase precession of hippocampal place cell firing: a spiking neuron model. Hippocampus 6, 271–280 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Skaggs W.E., McNaughton B.L., Wilson M.A. and Barnes C.A. (1996) Theta phase precession in hippocampal neuronal populations and the compression of temporal sequences. Hippocampus 6, 149–172 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Manns J.R., Zilli E.A., Ong K.C., Hasselmo M.E. and Eichenbaum H. (2007) Hippocampal CA1 spiking during encoding and retrieval: Relation to theta phase. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 87, 9–20 10.1016/j.nlm.2006.05.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Hasselmo M.E., Bodelón C. and Wyble B.P. (2002) A proposed function for hippocampal theta rhythm: separate phases of encoding and retrieval enhance reversal of prior learning. Neural Comput. 14, 793–817 10.1162/089976602317318965 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Mitchell S.J. and Ranck J.B. (1980) Generation of theta rhythm in medial entorhinal cortex of freely moving rats. Brain Res. 189, 49–66 10.1016/0006-8993(80)90006-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Deshmukh S.S., Yoganarasimha D., Voicu H. and Knierim J.J. (2010) Theta modulation in the medial and the lateral entorhinal cortices. J. Neurophysiol. 104, 994–1006 10.1152/jn.01141.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Mitchell S., Rawlins J., Steward O. and Olton D. (1982) Medial septal area lesions disrupt theta rhythm and cholinergic staining in medial entorhinal cortex and produce impaired radial arm maze behavior in rats. J. Neurosci. 2, 292–302 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.02-03-00292.1982 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Reifenstein E.T., Kempter R., Schreiber S., Stemmler M.B. and Herz A.V. (2012) Grid cells in rat entorhinal cortex encode physical space with independent firing fields and phase precession at the single-trial level. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 109, 6301–6306 10.1073/pnas.1109599109 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Hafting T., Fyhn M., Bonnevie T., Moser M.-B. and Moser E.I. (2008) Hippocampus-independent phase precession in entorhinal grid cells. Nature 453, 1248–1252 10.1038/nature06957 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Climer J.R., Newman E.L. and Hasselmo M.E. (2013) Phase coding by grid cells in unconstrained environments: two‐dimensional phase precession. Eur. J. Neurosci. 38, 2526–2541 10.1111/ejn.12256 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Navratilova Z., Giocomo L.M., Fellous J., Hasselmo M.E. and McNaughton B.L. (2012) Phase precession and variable spatial scaling in a periodic attractor map model of medial entorhinal grid cells with realistic after‐spike dynamics. Hippocampus 22, 772–789 10.1002/hipo.20939 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Ebbesen C.L.et al. (2016) Cell type-specific differences in spike timing and spike shape in the rat parasubiculum and superficial medial entorhinal cortex. Cell Rep. 16, 1005–1015 10.1016/j.celrep.2016.06.057 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Brandon M.P., Bogaard A.R., Schultheiss N.W. and Hasselmo M.E. (2013) Segregation of cortical head direction cell assemblies on alternating theta cycles. Nat. Neurosci. 16, 739–748 10.1038/nn.3383 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Lee M.G., Chrobak J.J., Sik A., Wiley R.G. and Buzsáki G. (1994) Hippocampal theta activity following selective lesion of the septal cholinergic systeM. Neuroscience 62, 1033–1047 10.1016/0306-4522(94)90341-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Donovick P.J. (1968) Effects of localized septal lesions on hippocampal EEG activity and behavior in rats. J. Comp. Physiol. Psych. 66, 569–578 10.1037/h0026514 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Buzsáki G., Leung L.-W.S. and Vanderwolf C.H. (1983) Cellular bases of hippocampal EEG in the behaving rat. Brain Res. Rev. 6, 139–171 10.1016/0165-0173(83)90037-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Sainsbury R.S. and Bland B.H. (1981) The effects of selective septal lesions on theta production in CA1 and the dentate gyrus of the hippocampus. Physiol. Behav. 26, 1097–1101 10.1016/0031-9384(81)90214-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Leutgeb S. and Mizumori S.J. (1999) Excitotoxic septal lesions result in spatial memory deficits and altered flexibility of hippocampal single-unit representations. J. Neurosci. 19, 6661–6672 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-15-06661.1999 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Meck W., Church R., Wenk G. and Olton D. (1987) Nucleus basalis magnocellularis and medial septal area lesions differentially impair temporal memory. J. Neurosci. 7, 3505–3511 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.07-11-03505.1987 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Myers C.E. and Scharfman H.E. (2011) Pattern separation in the dentate gyrus: a role for the CA3 backprojection. Hippocampus 21, 1190–1215 10.1002/hipo.20828 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Kobayashi K. and Poo M. (2004) Spike train timing-dependent associative modification of hippocampal CA3 recurrent synapses by mossy fibers. Neuron 41, 445–454 10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00873-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Rolls E.T. (2013) A quantitative theory of the functions of the hippocampal CA3 network in memory. Front. Cell Neurosci. 7, 98 10.3389/fncel.2013.00098 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Rolls E.T. (2007) An attractor network in the hippocampus: theory and neurophysiology. Learn. Mem. 14, 714–731 10.1101/lm.631207 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Lewis P.R. and Shute C.C.D. (1967) The cholinergic limbic system: projections to hippocampal formation, medial cortex, nuclei of the ascending cholinergic reticular system, and the subfornical organ and supra-optic crest. Brain 90, 521–540 10.1093/brain/90.3.521 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Sotty F.et al. (2003) Distinct electrophysiological properties of glutamatergic, cholinergic and GABAergic rat septohippocampal neurons: novel implications for hippocampal rhythmicity. J. Physiol. 551, 927–943 10.1113/jphysiol.2003.046847 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Colom L.V., Castaneda M.T., Reyna T., Hernandez S. and Garrido‐sanabria E. (2005) Characterization of medial septal glutamatergic neurons and their projection to the hippocampus. Synapse 58, 151–164 10.1002/syn.20184 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Köhler C., Chan-Palay V. and Wu J.-Y. (1984) Septal neurons containing glutamic acid decarboxylase immunoreactivity project to the hippocampal region in the rat brain. Anat. Embryol. 169, 41–44 10.1007/BF00300585 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Unal G., Joshi A., Viney T.J., Kis V. and Somogyi P. (2015) Synaptic targets of medial septal projections in the hippocampus and extrahippocampal cortices of the mouse. J. Neurosci. 35, 15812–15826 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2639-15.2015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Cobb S., Buhl E., Halasy K., Paulsen O. and Somogyi P. (1995) Synchronization of neuronal activity in hippocampus by individual GABAergic interneurons. Nature 378, 75–78 10.1038/378075a0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Pang K.C.H., Jiao X., Sinha S., Beck K.D. and Servatius R.J. (2011) Damage of GABAergic neurons in the medial septum impairs spatial working memory and extinction of active avoidance: effects on proactive interference. Hippocampus 21, 835–846 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Smith H.R. and Pang K.C.H. (2005) Orexin-saporin lesions of the medial septum impair spatial memory. Neuroscience 132, 261–271 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2004.12.037 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Yoder R.M. and Pang K.C.H. (2005) Involvement of GABAergic and cholinergic medial septal neurons in hippocampal theta rhythm. Hippocampus 15, 381–392 10.1002/hipo.20062 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Oddie S.D., Kirk I.J., Whishaw I.Q. and Bland B.H. (1997) Hippocampal formation is involved in movement selection: evidence from medial septal cholinergic modulation and concurrent slow-wave (theta rhythm) recording. Behav. Brain Res. 88, 169–180 10.1016/S0166-4328(97)02290-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Pan W.-X. and McNaughton N. (1997) The medial supramammillary nucleus, spatial learning and the frequency of hippocampal theta activity. Brain Res. 764, 101–108 10.1016/S0006-8993(97)00431-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.McNaughton N.et al. (1995) Contribution of synapses in the medial supramammillary nucleus to the frequency of hippocampal theta rhythm in freely moving rats. Hippocampus 5, 534–545 10.1002/hipo.450050605 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.McNaughton N., Richardson J. and Gore C. (1986) Reticular elicitation of hippocampal slow waves: Common effects of some anxiolytic drugs. Neuroscience 19, 899–903 10.1016/0306-4522(86)90306-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.McNaughton N., Kocsis B. and Hajós M. (2007) Elicited hippocampal theta rhythm: a screen for anxiolytic and procognitive drugs through changes in hippocampal function? Behav. Pharmacol. 18, 329–346 10.1097/FBP.0b013e3282ee82e3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Liu Z., Peng C., Zhuang Y., Chen Y. and Behnisch T. (2020) Direct medial entorhinal cortex input to hippocampal CA3 is crucial for eEF2K inhibitor-induced neuronal oscillations in the mouse hippocampus. Front. Cell Neurosci. 14, 24 10.3389/fncel.2020.00024 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Witter M.P., Canto C.B., Couey J.J., Koganezawa N. and O'Reilly K.C. (2014) Architecture of spatial circuits in the hippocampal region. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 369, 20120515 10.1098/rstb.2012.0515 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Li Y.et al. (2017) A distinct entorhinal cortex to hippocampal CA1 direct circuit for olfactory associative learning. Nat. Neurosci. 20, 559–570 10.1038/nn.4517 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Ohara S.et al. (2018) Intrinsic projections of layer Vb neurons to layers Va, III, and II in the lateral and medial entorhinal cortex of the rat. Cell Rep. 24, 107–116 10.1016/j.celrep.2018.06.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Belichenko P.V., Kleschevnikov A.M., Salehi A., Epstein C.J. and Mobley W.C. (2007) Synaptic and cognitive abnormalities in mouse models of down syndrome: Exploring genotype‐phenotype relationships. J. Comp. Neurol. 504, 329–345 10.1002/cne.21433 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Busciglio J., Capone G., O’Bryan J., O’Byran J.P. and Gardiner K.J. (2013) Down syndrome: genes, model systems, and progress towards pharmacotherapies and clinical trials for cognitive deficits. Cytogenet. Genome Res. 141, 260–271 10.1159/000354306 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Herault Y.et al. (2017) Rodent models in Down syndrome research: impact and future opportunities. Dis. Model Mech. 10, 1165–1186 10.1242/dmm.029728 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Choi J.H.K.et al. (2009) Age‐dependent dysregulation of brain amyloid precursor protein in the Ts65Dn Down syndrome mouse model. J. Neurochem. 110, 1818–1827 10.1111/j.1471-4159.2009.06277.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Hyde L.A. and Crnic L.S. (2001) Age-related deficits in context discrimination learning in Ts65Dn mice that model Down syndrome and Alzheimer’s disease. Behav. Neurosci. 115, 1239–1246 10.1037/0735-7044.115.6.1239 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Olmos-Serrano J.L., Tyler W.A., Cabral H.J. and Haydar T.F. (2016) Longitudinal measures of cognition in the Ts65Dn mouse: refining windows and defining modalities for therapeutic intervention in Down syndrome. Exp. Neurol. 279, 40–56 10.1016/j.expneurol.2016.02.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Rueda N., Flórez J. and Martínez-Cué C. (2008) Chronic pentylenetetrazole but not donepezil treatment rescues spatial cognition in Ts65Dn mice, a model for Down syndrome. Neurosci. Lett. 433, 22–27 10.1016/j.neulet.2007.12.039 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Sago H.et al. (1998) Ts1Cje, a partial trisomy 16 mouse model for Down syndrome, exhibits learning and behavioral abnormalities. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 95, 6256–6261 10.1073/pnas.95.11.6256 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Pereira P.L.et al. (2009) A new mouse model for the trisomy of the Abcg1–U2af1 region reveals the complexity of the combinatorial genetic code of down syndrome. Hum. Mol. Genet. 18, 4756–4769 10.1093/hmg/ddp438 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Villar A.J.et al. (2005) Identification and characterization of a new Down syndrome model, Ts[Rb(12.1716)]2Cje, resulting from a spontaneous Robertsonian fusion between T(1716)65Dn and mouseChromosome 12. Mamm. Genome 16, 79–90 10.1007/s00335-004-2428-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Ahmed, Md. M.et al. (2013) Protein profiles in Tc1 mice implicate novel pathway perturbations in the Down syndrome brain. Hum. Mol. Genet. 22, 1709–1724 10.1093/hmg/ddt017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Yu T.et al. (2010) A mouse model of Down syndrome trisomic for all human chromosome 21 syntenic regions. Hum. Mol. Genet. 19, 2780–2791 10.1093/hmg/ddq179 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Rueda N., Flórez J. and Martínez-Cué C. (2012) Mouse models of down syndrome as a tool to unravel the causes of mental disabilities. Neural Plast. 2012, 584071 10.1155/2012/584071 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Carlesimo G.A., Marotta L. and Vicari S. (1997) Long-term memory in mental retardation: evidence for a specific impairment in subjects with Down’s syndrome. Neuropsychologia 35, 71–79 10.1016/S0028-3932(96)00055-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Vicari S. (2006) Motor development and neuropsychological patterns in persons with Down syndrome. Behav. Genet. 36, 355–364 10.1007/s10519-006-9057-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Costanzo F.et al. (2013) Executive functions in intellectual disabilities: a comparison between Williams syndrome and Down syndrome. Res. Dev. Disabil. 34, 1770–1780 10.1016/j.ridd.2013.01.024 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Chapman R. and Hesketh L. (2000) Behavioral phenotype of individuals with Down syndrome. Ment. Retard. Dev. D R [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Reeves R.H.et al. (1995) A mouse model for Down syndrome exhibits learning and behaviour deficits. Nat. Genet. 11, 177–184 10.1038/ng1095-177 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Escorihuela R.M.et al. (1998) Impaired short- and long-term memory in Ts65Dn mice, a model for Down syndrome. Neurosci. Lett. 247, 171–174 10.1016/S0304-3940(98)00317-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Holtzman D.M.et al. (1996) Developmental abnormalities and age-related neurodegeneration in a mouse model of Down syndrome. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 93, 13333–13338 10.1073/pnas.93.23.13333 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Moran T.H.et al. (2002) The effects of piracetam on cognitive performance in a mouse model of Down’s syndrome. Physiol. Behav. 77, 403–409 10.1016/S0031-9384(02)00873-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Sago H.et al. (2000) Genetic dissection of region associated with behavioral abnormalities in mouse models for Down syndrome. Pediatr. Res. 48, 606–613 10.1203/00006450-200011000-00009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Hyde L.A., Frisone D.F. and Crnic L.S. (2001) Ts65Dn mice, a model for Down syndrome, have deficits in context discrimination learning suggesting impaired hippocampal function. Behav. Brain Res. 118, 53–60 10.1016/S0166-4328(00)00313-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Hunter C.L., Bimonte H.A. and Granholm A.-C.E. (2003) Behavioral comparison of 4 and 6 month-old Ts65Dn mice: Age-related impairments in working and reference memory. Behav. Brain Res. 138, 121–131 10.1016/S0166-4328(02)00275-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Fernandez F.et al. (2007) Pharmacotherapy for cognitive impairment in a mouse model of Down syndrome. Nat. Neurosci. 10, 411–413 10.1038/nn1860 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 96.Colas D.et al. (2013) Short‐term treatment with the GABAA receptor antagonist pentylenetetrazole produces a sustained pro‐cognitive benefit in a mouse model of Down’s syndrome. Br. J. Pharmacol. 169, 963–973 10.1111/bph.12169 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 97.Souza F.M., de Busquet S., Blatner N., Maclean K.N. and Restrepo D. (2011) Galantamine improves olfactory learning in the Ts65Dn mouse model of Down syndrome. Sci. Rep. 1, 137 10.1038/srep00137 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 98.Demas G.E., Nelson R.J., Krueger B.K. and Yarowsky P.J. (1998) Impaired spatial working and reference memory in segmental trisomy (Ts65Dn) mice. Behav. Brain Res. 90, 199–201 10.1016/S0166-4328(97)00116-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 99.Martínez-Cué C.et al. (1999) A murine model for Down syndrome shows reduced responsiveness to pain. Neuroreport 10, 1119–1122 10.1097/00001756-199904060-00039 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 100.Costa A.C.S., Walsh K. and Davisson M.T. (1999) Motor dysfunction in a mouse model for Down syndrome. Physiol. Behav. 68, 211–220 10.1016/S0031-9384(99)00178-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 101.Roubertoux P.L.et al. (2017) Differential brain, cognitive and motor profiles associated with partial trisomy. Modeling Down syndrome in mice. Behav. Genet. 47, 305–322 10.1007/s10519-017-9835-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 102.Glass T.J. and Connor N.P. (2016) Digastric muscle phenotypes of the Ts65Dn mouse model of Down syndrome. PLoS ONE 11, e0158008 10.1371/journal.pone.0158008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 103.Cisterna B., Costanzo M., Scherini E., Zancanaro C. and Malatesta M. (2013) Ultrastructural features of skeletal muscle in adult and aging Ts65Dn mice, a murine model of Down syndrome. Muscles Ligaments Tendons J. 3, 287–294 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 104.Aziz N.M.et al. (2019) Spatiotemporal development of spinal neuronal and glial populations in the Ts65Dn mouse model of Down syndrome. J. Neurodev. Disord. 11, 35 10.1186/s11689-019-9294-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 105.Coyle J., Price D. and DeLong M. (1983) Alzheimer's disease: a disorder of cortical cholinergic innervation. Science 219, 1184–1190 10.1126/science.6338589 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 106.Kása P., Rakonczay Z. and Gulya K. (1997) The cholinergic system in Alzheimer’s disease. Prog. Neurobiol. 52, 511–535 10.1016/S0301-0082(97)00028-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 107.Whitehouse P.J., Price D.L., Clark A.W., Coyle J.T. and DeLong M.R. (1981) Alzheimer disease: evidence for selective loss of cholinergic neurons in the nucleus basalis. Ann. Neurol. 10, 122–126 10.1002/ana.410100203 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 108.Velazquez R.et al. (2012) Perinatal choline supplementation improves spatial learning and increases cholinergic neuron density in the medial septal nucleus in the Ts65Dn mouse model of Down syndrome. Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 34, 383 10.1016/j.ntt.2012.05.047 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 109.Powers B.E.et al. (2016) Attentional function and basal forebrain cholinergic neuron morphology during aging in the Ts65Dn mouse model of Down syndrome. Brain Struct Funct. 221, 4337–4352 10.1007/s00429-015-1164-y [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 110.Kelley C.M.et al. (2013) Sex differences in the cholinergic basal forebrain in the Ts65Dn mouse model of Down syndrome and Alzheimer’s disease. Brain Pathol. 24, 33–44 10.1111/bpa.12073 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 111.Cooper J.D.et al. (2001) Failed retrograde transport of NGF in a mouse model of Down’s syndrome: reversal of cholinergic neurodegenerative phenotypes following NGF infusion. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 98, 10439–10444 10.1073/pnas.181219298 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 112.Kelley C.M.et al. (2014) Maternal choline supplementation differentially alters the basal forebrain cholinergic system of young-adult Ts65Dn and disomic mice. J. Comp. Neurol. 522, 1390–1410 10.1002/cne.23492 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 113.Alldred M.J.et al. (2018) CA1 pyramidal neuron gene expression mosaics in the Ts65Dn murine model of Down syndrome and Alzheimer’s disease following maternal choline supplementation. Hippocampus 28, 251–268 10.1002/hipo.22832 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 114.Sil’kis I.G. (2019) Possible mechanisms of the complex effects of acetylcholine on theta activity, learning, and memory. Neurochem. J. 13, 121–136 10.1134/S1819712419020119 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 115.Contestabile A., Fila T., Bartesaghi R., Contestabile A. and Ciani E. (2006) Choline acetyltransferase activity at different ages in brain of Ts65Dn mice, an animal model for Down’s syndrome and related neurodegenerative diseases. J. Neurochem. 97, 515–526 10.1111/j.1471-4159.2006.03769.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 116.Colas D.et al. (2008) Sleep and EEG features in genetic models of Down syndrome. Neurobiol. Dis. 30, 1–7 10.1016/j.nbd.2007.07.014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 117.Heller H.C., Freeburn A., Finn D.P. and Munn R.G.K. (2020) Disordered phasic relationships between hippocampal place cells, theta, and gamma rhythms in the Ts65Dn mouse model of Down Syndrome. bioRxiv, 10.1101/2020.09.17.301432 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 118.Colgin L.L. (2015) Do slow and fast gamma rhythms correspond to distinct functional states in the hippocampal network? Brain Res. 1621, 309–315 10.1016/j.brainres.2015.01.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 119.Igarashi K.M., Lu L., Colgin L.L., Moser M.-B. and Moser E.I. (2014) Coordination of entorhinal-hippocampal ensemble activity during associative learning. Nature 510, 143–147 10.1038/nature13162 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 120.Colgin L.L. and Moser E.I. (2010) Gamma oscillations in the hippocampus. Physiology 25, 319–329 10.1152/physiol.00021.2010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 121.Colgin L.et al. (2009) Frequency of gamma oscillations routes flow of information in the hippocampus. Nature 462, 353 10.1038/nature08573 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 122.de San Martin J.Z.et al. (2020) Alterations of specific cortical GABAergic circuits underlie abnormal network activity in a mouse model of Down syndrome. eLife 9, e58731 10.7554/eLife.58731 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 123.Hernández-González S.et al. (2015) Altered distribution of hippocampal interneurons in the murine Down syndrome model Ts65Dn. Neurochem. Res. 40, 151–164 10.1007/s11064-014-1479-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 124.Laurie D., Wisden W. and Seeburg P. (1992) The distribution of thirteen GABAA receptor subunit mRNAs in the rat brain. III. Embryonic and postnatal development. J. Neurosci. 12, 4151–4172 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.12-11-04151.1992 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 125.Fritschy J. and Mohler H. (1995) GABAA‐receptor heterogeneity in the adult rat brain: Differential regional and cellular distribution of seven major subunits. J. Comp. Neurol. 359, 154–194 10.1002/cne.903590111 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 126.Vidal V.et al. (2018) Decreasing the expression of GABAA α5 subunit-containing receptors partially improves cognitive, electrophysiological, and morphological hippocampal defects in the Ts65Dn model of Down syndrome. Mol. Neurobiol. 55, 4745–4762 10.1007/s12035-017-0675-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 127.Baudry M., Bi X., Gall C. and Lynch G. (2011) The biochemistry of memory: The 26year journey of a ‘new and specific hypothesis’. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 95, 125–133 10.1016/j.nlm.2010.11.015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 128.Siarey R.J., Stoll J., Rapoport S.I. and Galdzicki Z. (1997) Altered long-term potentiation in the young and old Ts65Dn mouse, a model for down syndrome. Neuropharmacology 36, 1549–1554 10.1016/S0028-3908(97)00157-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 129.Costa A. and Grybko M.J. (2005) Deficits in hippocampal CA1 LTP induced by TBS but not HFS in the Ts65Dn mouse: A model of Down syndrome. Neurosci. Lett. 382, 317–322 10.1016/j.neulet.2005.03.031 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 130.Best T.K., Cramer N.P., Chakrabarti L., Haydar T.F. and Galdzicki Z. (2011) Dysfunctional hippocampal inhibition in the Ts65Dn mouse model of Down syndrome. Exp. Neurol. 233, 749–757 10.1016/j.expneurol.2011.11.033 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 131.Siarey R.J.et al. (1999) Increased synaptic depression in the Ts65Dn mouse, a model for mental retardation in Down syndrome. Neuropharmacology 38, 1917–1920 10.1016/S0028-3908(99)00083-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 132.Belichenko P.V.et al. (2009) Excitatory‐inhibitory relationship in the fascia dentata in the Ts65Dn mouse model of down syndrome. J. Comp. Neurol. 512, 453–466 10.1002/cne.21895 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 133.Mojabi F.S.et al. (2016) GABAergic hyperinnervation of dentate granule cells in the Ts65Dn mouse model of down syndrome: exploring the role of App. Hippocampus 26, 1641–1654 10.1002/hipo.22664 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 134.Kleschevnikov A.M.et al. (2011) Increased efficiency of the GABAA and GABAB receptor-mediated neurotransmission in the Ts65Dn mouse model of Down syndrome. Neurobiol. Dis. 45, 683–691 10.1016/j.nbd.2011.10.009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 135.Duchon A.et al. (2020) Long‐lasting correction of in vivo LTP and cognitive deficits of mice modelling Down syndrome with an α5‐selective GABAA inverse agonist. Br. J. Pharmacol. 177, 1106–1118 10.1111/bph.14903 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 136.Kirk I.J. (1998) Frequency modulation of hippocampal theta by the supramammillary nucleus, and other hypothalamo–hippocampal interactions: mechanisms and functional implications. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 22, 291–302 10.1016/S0149-7634(97)00015-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 137.Kirk I. and McNaughton N. (1991) Supramammillary cell firing and hippocampal rhythmical slow activity. Neuroreport 2, 723–725 10.1097/00001756-199111000-00023 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 138.Kirk I.J. (1997) Supramammillary neural discharge patterns and hippocampal EEG. Brain Res. Bull. 42, 23–26 10.1016/S0361-9230(96)00094-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 139.Marin‐Padilla M. (1976) Pyramidal cell abnormalities in the motor cortex of a child with Down’s syndrome. A Golgi study. J. Comp. Neurol. 167, 63–81 10.1002/cne.901670105 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 140.Vukšić M., Petanjek Z., Rašin M.R. and Kostović I. (2002) Perinatal growth of prefrontal layer III pyramids in down syndrome. Pediatr. Neurol. 27, 36–38 10.1016/S0887-8994(02)00380-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 141.Weitzdoerfer R., Dierssen M., Fountoulakis M. and Lubec G. (2001) Protein expression in Down syndrome brain. Amino Acids 21, 59–70 [Google Scholar]
  • 142.Vuksic M.et al. (2011) Unilateral entorhinal denervation leads to long-lasting dendritic alterations of mouse hippocampal granule cells. Exp. Neurol. 230, 176–185 10.1016/j.expneurol.2011.04.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 143.Shim K.S. and Lubec G. (2002) Drebrin, a dendritic spine protein, is manifold decreased in brains of patients with Alzheimer’s disease and Down syndrome. Neurosci. Lett. 324, 209–212 10.1016/S0304-3940(02)00210-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 144.Insausti A.et al. (1998) Hippocampal volume and neuronal number in Ts65Dn mice: a murine model of down syndrome. Neurosci. Lett. 253, 175–178 10.1016/S0304-3940(98)00641-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 145.Carbonell J., Blasco-Ibáñez J.M., Crespo C., Nácher J. and Varea E. (2020) Piriform cortex alterations in the ts65dn model for down syndrome. Brain Res. 1747, 147031 10.1016/j.brainres.2020.147031 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 146.Kurt M.A., Davies D.C., Kidd M., Dierssen M. and Flórez J. (2000) Synaptic deficit in the temporal cortex of partial trisomy 16 (Ts65Dn) mice. Brain Res. 858, 191–197 10.1016/S0006-8993(00)01984-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 147.Kurt M.A., Kafa M.I., Dierssen M. and Davies D.C. (2004) Deficits of neuronal density in CA1 and synaptic density in the dentate gyrus, CA3 and CA1, in a mouse model of Down syndrome. Brain Res. 1022, 101–109 10.1016/j.brainres.2004.06.075 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 148.Contestabile A.et al. (2007) Cell cycle alteration and decreased cell proliferation in the hippocampal dentate gyrus and in the neocortical germinal matrix of fetuses with down syndrome and in Ts65Dn mice. Hippocampus 17, 665–678 10.1002/hipo.20308 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 149.Masuda T.et al. (2012) Noradrenaline increases neural precursor cells derived from adult rat dentate gyrus through beta2 receptor. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 36, 44–51 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2011.08.019 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 150.Emili M.et al. (2020) Neonatal therapy with clenbuterol and salmeterol restores spinogenesis and dendritic complexity in the dentate gyrus of the Ts65Dn model of Down syndrome. Neurobiol. Dis. 140, 104874 10.1016/j.nbd.2020.104874 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 151.Bortolotto V.et al. (2019) Salmeterol, a β2 adrenergic agonist, promotes adult hippocampal neurogenesis in a region-specific manner. Front. Pharmacol. 10, 1000 10.3389/fphar.2019.01000 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 152.Chuluun B.et al. (2020) Suprachiasmatic lesions restore object recognition in Down syndrome model mice. Neurobiol. Sleep Circad. Rhythm 8, 100049 10.1016/j.nbscr.2020.100049 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 153.Fernandez F.et al. (2017) Young children with Down syndrome show normal development of circadian rhythms, but poor sleep efficiency: a cross-sectional study across the first 60 months of life. Sleep Med. 33, 134–144 10.1016/j.sleep.2016.12.026 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 154.Lukowski A.F., Slonecker E.M. and Milojevich H.M. (2020) Sleep problems and recall memory in children with Down syndrome and typically developing controls. Res. Dev. Disabil. 96, 103512 10.1016/j.ridd.2019.103512 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 155.Horne R.S.C., Wijayaratne P., Nixon G.M. and Walter L.M. (2018) Sleep and sleep disordered breathing in children with Down syndrome: effects on behaviour, neurocognition and the cardiovascular system. Sleep Med. Rev. 44, 1–11 10.1016/j.smrv.2018.11.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 156.Bassell J.L., Phan H., Leu R., Kronk R. and Visootsak J. (2015) Sleep profiles in children with down syndrome. Am. J. Med. Genet. A 167, 1830–1835 10.1002/ajmg.a.37096 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 157.Ruby N.F.et al. (2008) Hippocampal-dependent learning requires a functional circadian system. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 105, 15593–15598 10.1073/pnas.0808259105 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 158.Bina K.G., Rusak B. and Semba K. (1993) Localization of cholinergic neurons in the forebrain and brainstem that project to the suprachiasmatic nucleus of the hypothalamus in rat. J. Comp. Neurol. 335, 295–307 10.1002/cne.903350212 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 159.Bina K.G., Rusak B. and Semba K. (1997) Sources of p75-nerve growth factor receptor-like immunoreactivity in the rat suprachiasmatic nucleus. Neuroscience 77, 461–472 10.1016/S0306-4522(96)00496-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 160.Madeira M.D., Pereira P.A., Silva S.M., Cadete-Leite A. and Paula-Barbosa M.M. (2004) Basal forebrain neurons modulate the synthesis and expression of neuropeptides in the rat suprachiasmatic nucleus. Neuroscience 125, 889–901 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2004.03.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 161.Watts A.G., Swanson L.W. and Sanchez‐Watts G. (1987) Efferent projections of the suprachiasmatic nucleus: I. Studies using anterograde transport of Phaseolus vulgaris leucoagglutinin in the rat. J. Comp. Neurol. 258, 204–229 10.1002/cne.902580204 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 162.Ruby N.F.et al. (2010) Circadian locomotor rhythms are normal in Ts65Dn “Down Syndrome” mice and unaffected by pentylenetetrazole. J. Biol. Rhythm. 25, 63–66 10.1177/0748730409356202 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 163.Ruby N.F.et al. (2017) Scheduled feeding restores memory and modulates c-Fos expression in the suprachiasmatic nucleus and septohippocampal complex. Sci. Rep. 7, 6755 10.1038/s41598-017-06963-w [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 164.Ruby N.F.et al. (2013) Spatial memory and long-term object recognition are impaired by circadian arrhythmia and restored by the GABAA antagonist pentylenetetrazole. PLoS ONE 8, e72433 10.1371/journal.pone.0072433 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 165.Potier M.-C., Braudeau J., Dauphinot L. and Delatour B. (2014) Reducing GABAergic inhibition restores cognitive functions in a mouse model of Down syndrome. CNS Neurol. Disord. Drug Targets 13, 8–15 10.2174/18715273113126660185 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 166.Best T.K., Siarey R.J. and Galdzicki Z. (2007) Ts65Dn, a mouse model of down syndrome, exhibits increased GABA B -induced potassium current. J. Neurophysiol. 97, 892–900 10.1152/jn.00626.2006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 167.Zhong P. and Yan Z. (2004) Chronic antidepressant treatment alters serotonergic regulation of GABA transmission in prefrontal cortical pyramidal neurons. Neuroscience 129, 65–73 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2004.06.072 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 168.Bianchi P.et al. (2010) Early pharmacotherapy restores neurogenesis and cognitive performance in the Ts65Dn mouse model for Down Syndrome. J. Neurosci. 30, 8769–8779 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0534-10.2010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 169.Heinen M.et al. (2012) Adult-onset fluoxetine treatment does not improve behavioral impairments and may have adverse effects on the Ts65Dn mouse model of Down syndrome. Neural Plast. 2012, 467251 10.1155/2012/467251 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 170.Judge S.J., Ingram C.D. and Gartside S.E. (2004) GABA receptor modulation of 5-HT neuronal firing: characterization and effect of moderate in vivo variations in glucocorticoid levels. Neurochem. Int. 45, 1057–1065 10.1016/j.neuint.2004.05.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Neuronal Signaling are provided here courtesy of Portland Press Ltd

RESOURCES