Skip to main content
Frontiers in Neurology logoLink to Frontiers in Neurology
. 2021 Mar 1;12:629251. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2021.629251

Association of Leisure Activities With Cognitive Impairment and Dementia in Older Adults in Colombia: A SABE-Based Study

Alejandra Guerrero Barragán 1,2,3,4,*, Diego Lucumí 3, Brian Lawlor 1,2,5
PMCID: PMC7956952  PMID: 33732207

Abstract

Observational and interventional studies suggest that participation in leisure activities may help protect against cognitive decline in older people. This study aimed to examine the association between participation in leisure activities and cognitive impairment in older adults in Colombia. Data for this study were derived from the Colombian National Survey of Aging (SABE 2015), a cross-sectional survey with a sample size of 23,694 older adults representing the total population (mean age, 70.8 years; 57.3% females). Cognitive impairment was classified as cognitive impairment without dementia (CIWD) and dementia, according to the revised version of the Folstein Mini-Mental State Examination and the Lawton and Brody functional scale. Leisure activities were evaluated using six items of a questionnaire. Sex-stratified multinomial regression models were used to analyze the association of leisure activities with CIWD and dementia after adjusting for educational attainment, literacy, and other potential confounders. In adjusted models for men, leisure activities in later life were associated with a decreased risk of CIWD (odds ratio [OR], 0.73; 95% confidence interval [CI], 0.68–0.78) and dementia (OR, 0,52; 95% CI, 0.48–0.58). For women, leisure activities in later life were associated with a decreased risk of CIWD (OR, 0.72; 95% CI, 0.66–0.78) and dementia (OR, 0.48; 95% CI, 0.43–0.53). The findings suggest that greater participation in leisure activities in later life may act as a protective factor against CIWD and dementia among older adults in Colombia, independent of educational attainment and literacy.

Keywords: prevention, cognitive reserve, leisure activities, dementia, cognitive impairment

Introduction

With a rise in global life expectancy, the prevalence of dementia is rising. A new diagnosis of dementia is reported every 3 s. Currently, the disease affects more than 50 million people globally (1), 50% of those living in middle- and low-income countries. It is expected that this number will rise to 63% in 2030 and 68% in 2050 (2), with the Americas being one of the most affected regions with increasing prevalence and incidence rates of dementia (3).

Several protective factors for dementia have been described in the literature (4). Leisure activities, a term that refers to a range of tasks and activities outside work-related activities is a promising factor for dementia prevention (5). Leisure activities can have a multidimensional profile, manifesting mental, social, and physical involvement (6). These activities are attractive, pleasant, and motivating to the individual and are more likely to be sustained over time (7, 8). Leisure activities usually constitute a relatively large part of daily life post-retirement and may provide mental stimulation, social engagement, and physical activity (6). Systematic reviews have found evidence suggesting that participation in leisure activities might significantly contribute to the prevention of later-life cognitive decline as a risk-preventing factor (5, 9). The most common categories associated with the preventive effect of leisure activities are cognitive, physical, and social activities (5, 1012). Despite these benefits, there is a lack of standard definitions, measures, and methods for studying the role of leisure activities and their protective effects.

The most common explanatory mechanism for the preventive effect of leisure activities is that of the cognitive reserve theory (5, 13). The cognitive reserve theory suggests that innate intelligence or life experiences such as educational or occupational attainments may supply a reserve in the form of skill sets or repertoires that empower the brain to tolerate atrophies and insults and, as a result, allow some people to cope with progressing dementia better than others and delay symptom onset (14, 15). The cognitive reserve cannot be observed or directly measured, and proxy measures such as education, premorbid intelligence (IQ), linguistic ability, and occupational complexity are often used (14). Several studies have proposed that engagement in leisure activities may result in more functionally efficient or resilient cognitive networks or in recruiting alternate networks, providing a cognitive reserve that prevents or delays the onset of dementia (1618). In this regard, it is unclear whether a high cognitive reserve delays symptoms onset, but any noticeable delay, for example, 1 or 2 years, would translate into tremendous public health benefits by reducing the prevalence of dementia in the population (14). While characteristics such as education and intelligence are relatively stable from young adulthood, there is increasing interest in the role of leisure activities in building up a cognitive reserve (14).

Although maintaining or building a cognitive reserve is a possible mechanism for dementia prevention (5, 9, 14), culture, poverty, and inequality are key obstacles to, and drivers of, the need for changes concerning the cognitive reserve. Sections of the society that are most deprived of basic needs, like low and middle income countries (LMIC), require these changes and will greatly benefit from them (4). It has been suggested that a low socioeconomic status (SES) is associated with less access to physical (19) and cognitive stimulating activities (20). In Latin America, low levels of education, high rates of brain injury, poor diet, a sedentary lifestyle, and a high risk of cardiovascular diseases are among the main risk factors behind the rapid growth in the number of people with dementia (21); however, there is a high potential for dementia prevention in the region (4, 22). Thus, there is an urgent need for dementia prevention policies in Latin America to reduce the burden and economic costs of dementia in the region (2325).

In this study, we detail the association between participation in leisure activities and the risk of developing cognitive impairment without dementia (CIWD), or dementia, using the data from the Colombian National Survey of Aging (SABE 2015) (26). There are few papers focusing on cognitive outcomes from Colombian SABE, the effect of education in early life and the probability of cognitive impairment in later life in Colombia has been recently published (27) as well as the role of gait speed in dementia (28) and the mediating effect of physical fitness on cognitive functioning (29). To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study to analyze this topic in Colombia using this dataset.

Materials and Methods

We conducted a secondary data analysis of a cross-sectional study using data from SABE Colombia 2015. SABE Colombia 2015 is the first study in Colombia representative of the national population of those aged at least 60 years. Individuals for SABE Colombia were selected following a multistage area probability sample design with a total sample size of 23,694 participants from 244 municipalities (urban and rural). Data collection took place between April and September 2015 (26). This study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of Universidad de los Andes, code ID 1114/2019.

The dependent variable was cognitive impairment, as measured in SABE 2015. This survey used the revised version of the Folstein Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE), a validated international scale translated to Spanish (30). A cut-off point of 12 or less indicated cognitive impairment, while a score of 13 or above was normal (26). For dementia, functional impairment was evaluated using four items of the Lawton and Brody functional scale (31): phone use, transport use, handling medicines, and management of money. The lack of functionality for doing at least two of the four activities was defined as dependence and indication of dementia (32). If functional impairment was not detected, participants were classified as CIWD.

Independent variables were educational attainment, literacy (reading and writing), and participation in leisure activities. Educational attainment was established based on the 11 categories reported by SABE 2015. We classified participants into three groups in accordance with their educational qualifications: those who had an educational qualification lower than completion of primary school, those who had completed primary school, and those who had sought further qualifications after completion of high school. Literacy was reported using a yes/no question. Using an 18-item questionnaire reported by SABE 2015, we constructed a 6-item continuous variable for participation in leisure activities, among which five items were considered cognitively challenging (reading, solving math problems, solving puzzles, tabletop games, attending classes or courses) and one item that included physical activity.

Confounding variables included were age in years, area of residence (urban, rural), marital status (married/with a partner, separated/widower, single), living alone (yes/no), health insurance (subsidized, contribute, no affiliation), antecedent of forced displacement during life (yes/no), country region (residence in one of the six geographic areas of Colombia: Atlantic, Oriental, Orinoquia and Amazonia, Bogotá, Central, Pacific), skin color (light, medium, dark), pension (yes/no), lifetime occupation (manual or dependent worker, boss or independent worker, none), salary (<1 minimum wage, between 1 and 2 minimum wages, more than 2 minimum wages), and physical capital tercile (13).

Inclusion and Exclusion Criteria

The present study included the total SABE survey sample: participants aged 60 years and above, non-institutionalized, capable of communicating with the research team and able to provide written informed consent. If the MMSE was below 13 a proxy interview was developed (26).

Participants with missing data for leisure activities participation were excluded from this study.

Statistical Analyses

Descriptive analyses of data were conducted using frequencies and percentages for qualitative variables and means and standard deviations for quantitative variables. Unadjusted odds ratio (OR) was calculated using multinomial regression logistic analyses for dependent and independent variables. Multicollinearity was test using variance inflation factor (VIF). Finally, sex-stratified multinomial regression models were used to examine the association of leisure activities with CI and dementia after adjusting for educational attainment, literacy, and other potential confounders. Results are presented as OR with 95% confidence intervals. All analyses were performed using STATA release 16® (StataCorp LP, College Station, USA).

Results

The sample size of the survey was 23,694 participants, 31 participants with missing data for leisure activities were excluded from this study, the final sample size was 23.663 with a mean age of 70.82 years (standard deviation [SD] 8.20); 57.3% were women. Most participants had an educational qualification lower than completion of primary school (62.6%), followed by those who had completed primary school (26.8%) and sought further qualifications after completion of high school (10.5%). Literacy was reported as 78.2 and 77.1% for reading and writing, respectively. The prevalence of CIWD was 8.9 and 10.8% for dementia (Table 1). The mean number of leisure activities participated in was 2.02 (SD 1.59) by those with normal cognition, 0.93 (SD 1.17) by those with CIWD, and 0.51 (SD 0.86) by those with dementia (Figure 1).

Table 1.

Descriptive analyses.

Variable n %
Cognition
Normal 19.004 80.2
Cognitive Impairment Without Dementia (CIWD) 2.109 8.9
Dementia 2.581 10.8
Residence Area
Urban 17.189 72.5
Rural 6.505 27.4
Country Region
Atlantic 6.202 26.1
Oriental 3.583 15.1
Orinoquia and Amazonia 1.394 5.8
Bogotá 2.003 8.45
Central 6.351 26.8
Pacific 4.161 17.5
Sex
Men 10.112 42.6
Women 13.582 57.3
Marriage Status
Married/With a Partner 12.557 53
Separated/Widower 8.456 35.7
Single 2.671 11.2
Living Alone
Yes 2.201 9.2
No 21.493 90.7
Health Affiliation
Subsidized 14.160 59.82
Contribute 8.998 38.01
No affiliation 512 2.16
Skin Color
Light 11.465 48.3
Medium 8.706 36.7
Dark 3.523 14.8
Pension
Yes 1.589 6.7
No 21.97 93.2
Lifetime Occupation
Manual or dependent worker 15.061 64.89
Boss or independent worker 5.131 22.11
None 3.018 13
Income
<1 minimum wage 13.468 68.7
Between 1 and 2 minimum wage 5074 25.8
More than 2 minimum wage 1.061 5.4
Physical Capital Tercile
1 10.062 42.5
2 7.482 31.6
3 6.15 25.9
Educational Attainment
Less than primary school 14.778 62.6
Completed primary school 6.325 26.8
More than high school 2.498 10.5
Literacy (Writing)
No 18.523 78.2
Yes 5.149 21.7
Literacy (Reading)
No 18.264 77.1
Yes 5.407 22.8

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Mean leisure activities participation. CIWD, Cognitive impairment without dementia.

Unadjusted OR estimation showed that people who had an educational qualification lower than the completion of primary school were more likely to develop CIWD (OR 10.18; CI 7.33–14.15) and dementia (OR 8.54; CI 6.52–11.17) than those who had sought further educational qualifications after high school. Reading and writing illiteracy were associated with a higher likelihood of having either CIWD (OR 4.70, CI 4.28–5.17; OR 4.57, CI 4.16–5.02, respectively), or dementia (OR 6.43, CI 5.90–7.02; OR 6.48, CI 5.95–7.07, respectively). Participation in leisure activities was a protective factor against CIWD and dementia (OR 0.56, CI 0.54–0.59; OR 0.36, CI 0.34–0.38, respectively) (Table 2).

Table 2.

Non-adjusted odds ratio for independent variables.

Variable Cognitive Impairment Dementia
OR (Confidence Interval) OR (Confidence Interval)
Educational Attainment
Less than primary school 10.18 (7.33–14.15)*** 8.54 (6.52–11.17)***
Completed primary school 3.95 (2.80–5.56)*** 2.42 (1.81–3.22)***
More than high school REF REF
Literacy (Writing) 4.57 (4.16–5.02)*** 6.48 (5.95–7.07)***
Literacy (Reading) 4.70 (4.28–5.17)*** 6.43 (5.90–7.02)***
Leisure Activities 0.56 (0.54–0.59)*** 0.36 (0.34–0.38)***
*

p < 0.05,

**

p < 0.01,

***

p < 0.001.

Statistical Significance.

Analysis of multicollinearity showed high correlation between reading and writing literacy, the former was removed from the multinomial regression models. Table 3 shows the results for sex-stratified multinomial regression models adjusted for potential confounders. In model 1, those who had an educational qualification lower than the completion of primary school (OR 7.27, CI 4.19–12.61) as well as those who had completed primary school (OR 3.02, CI 1.73–5.30) were significantly more likely to have CIWD than their counterparts with higher educational qualifications. Similar results were found for the association between those who had an educational qualification lower than the completion of primary school (OR 3.30, CI 2.06–5.28) and dementia in comparison with those who had the highest level of education. Model 2 for CIWD persisted with the association for those who had an educational qualification lower than the completion of primary school (OR 3.70, CI 2.12–6.47) and those who had completed primary school (OR 2.65, CI 1.51–4.64). Leisure activities were associated with a decreased risk of CIWD (OR 0.73, CI 0.68–0.78). For dementia, in model 2, the effect of educational attainment disappeared, and inability to read was significantly associated with greater risk (OR 2.77, CI 2.29–3.34). Participation in leisure activities decreased the risk of dementia (OR 0.52, CI 0.48– 0.58).

Table 3.

Multinomial sex-stratified regression models.

Women Men
Variable Cognitive impairment without dementia Dementia Cognitive impairment without dementia Dementia
Model 1# Model 2# Model 1# Model 2# Model 1# Model 2# Model 1# Model 2#
OR CI 95% OR CI 95% OR CI 95% OR CI 95% OR CI 95% OR CI 95% OR CI 95% OR CI 95%
Educational Attainment
Less than primary school 7.27***
(4.19–12.61)
3.70***
(2.12–6.47)
3.30***
(2.06–5.28)
1.23 (0.74–2.03) 4.57***
(2.71–7.70)
2.42**
(1.41–4.14)
3.46***
(1.97–6.07)
1.53 (0.83–2.83)
Completed primary school 3.02***
(1.73–5.30)
2.65**
(1.51–4.64)
1.15
(0.71–1.87)
0.98 (0.59–1.64) 2.47** (1.45–4.21) 2.20**
(1.28–3.77)
1.52 (0.84–2.73) 1.31 (0.70–2.45)
More than high school REF REF REF REF REF REF REF REF
Literacy (Reading)
No 2.28***
(1.90–2.73)
2.77**
(2.29 −3.34)
1.94***
(1.57–2.41)
1.68***
(1.34–2.11)
Yes REF REF REF REF
Leisure Activities 0.73***
(0.68–0.78)
0.52***
(0.48–0.58)
0.72***
(0.66–0.78)
0.48***
(0.43–0.53)
#

Adjusted for age, area, marital status, living partners, health affiliation, displacement, country region, race, occupation, salary, and physical assets.

*

p < 0.05,

**

p < 0.01,

***

p < 0.001.

CI, Confidence Interval.

In adjusted models for men, model 1 for CIWD showed significant results for those who had an educational qualification lower than the completion of primary school (OR 4.57, CI 2.71–7.70) and those who had completed primary school (OR 2.47, CI 1.45–4.21). For dementia, results were significant for those who had an educational qualification lower than the completion of primary school (OR 3.46, CI 1.97–6.07). Model 2 for CIWD persisted with the association for those who had an educational qualification lower than the completion of primary school (OR 2.42, CI 1.41–4.14) and those who had completed primary school (OR 2.20, CI 1.28–3.77) and was significant for the inability to read (OR 1.94, CI 1.57–2.41). Leisure activities were associated with a decreased risk of CIWD (OR 0.72, CI 0.66–0.78). For dementia, in model 2, the effect of educational attainment disappeared; results for reading literacy were significant (OR 1.68, CI 1.34–2.11). Leisure activities decreased the risk of dementia (OR 0.48, CI 0.43–0.53).

Discussion

This study's main objective was to investigate the effect of participation in leisure activities on reducing the risk of developing CIWD or dementia in Colombian older adults. We found that for CIWD, participation in leisure activities is linked with educational attainment as well as the ability to read. For dementia, participation in leisure activities in later life might impart a protective effect independent of educational attainment for men and women.

In this sample, more than two-thirds of the participants had low educational attainment. Low educational attainment has been associated with a higher risk of dementia and CIWD (33, 34). Our results suggest that when adjusted for leisure activities, the effect of education is significant for CIWD but not for dementia. Education attainment is an essential factor for cognitive reserve building (35); nonetheless, cognitive reserve is not only determined by education but also by other lifestyle factors in which leisure activities are included (4, 36). In individuals with CIWD, educational attainment has been shown to affect intellectual development during the entire adult life span. However, it was not associated with the cognitive decline rate, in contrast with mid/late-life cognitive activities, which was beneficial and delayed the onset of dementia (37). Previous studies had shown the protective effect of cognitive reserve factors such as education and literacy in LMIC (38). Our study contributes with evidence from this region on the potential role of leisure activities on cognitive reserve building, but further research is needed to clarify this association.

Illiteracy was associated as an independent risk factor in non-adjusted analysis for both CIWD and dementia in previous findings (39). Our study included illiteracy while evaluating writing and reading abilities separately; even though the effect of this difference is not known, it is presumed that acquiring one aspect of literacy over the other could lead to differential dementia risks (39). The ability to read provides the means to acquire and structure new knowledge for language skills and reinforces working memory, visual memory, visuospatial processing, and visuomotor skills (4042). Our results indicate that an inability to read was associated with a higher risk of dementia independent of educational attainment for women; contradictory results have been found in the literature (39, 43).

The study findings suggest that greater participation in leisure activities in later life may be a protective factor against CIWD and dementia among older adults in Colombia after adjusting for education attainment, illiteracy, and other socioeconomic factors covariates. Previous studies have shown the protective effect of participation in leisure activities with a reduced risk of CIWD and dementia (9) and that the relationship between leisure activities and cognition is not driven by educational attainment (44) or SES (45). In contrast to these findings, a recent study based on longitudinal follow-up failed to support that leisure activity participation can lower the risk of dementia but instead suggested that reduction in activity participation indicates possible prodromal dementia (46).

This study has several limitations. First, the study was based on a yes/no self-report questionnaire and frequency, intensity, or quality of the activities was measured. Thus, it is not possible to elucidate the underlying mechanisms for the protective effect described here (12). A study found that participation in several activities with varying cognitive complexity levels was a better predictor of cognitive impairment than increased activity frequency (47). We need to know the amount and frequency that generates a protective effect to be able to design a suitable intervention. Second, although gender, demographic, and SES covariates were adjusted, other known risk factors for dementia such as hypertension, diabetes, stroke, and depression were not included (48). In a cohort of Swedish older adults, moderate to high engagement levels in mental, social, and physical leisure activities were associated with a dramatically decreased risk of dementia in people with diabetes (49). Third, education was measured by years of school completed, and there was no measure of the quality of education. Higher literacy may be a more sensitive marker of cognitive reserve than higher education (50); thus we cannot rule out the possibility that leisure activities' protective effect is dependent on education. Further research is needed to clarify the association between education, leisure activities, and cognitive impairment. Furthermore, data from this study comes from a cross-sectional survey; long term, population-based or representative cohort studies are needed to estimate with more precision the role of lifestyle choices like leisure activities might play in reducing dementia risk (51).

Despite these limitations, this is the first study that demonstrates the protective effect of leisure activities using a survey representative of the Colombian population to the best of our knowledge. The public health implications from this finding are related to the importance of increasing late-life participation in leisure activities, their potential role in late-life cognition, and its benefits in people with and without cognitive impairment (52). Nevertheless, further research is needed to determine the amount of exposure, intervention period, and frequency for designing an effective intervention (53). The available research evidence suggests that it is not too late to increase physical and cognitive activity in old age (14) and policymaking for dementia primary prevention needs evidence for non-pharmacological interventions aiming to increase cognitive reserve (54). More research is required to stablish more reliable conclusions for dementia preventive factors and potential interventions.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets presented in this article are not readily available because the user agreement does not permit sharing the data directly. Requests to access the datasets should be directed to the Colombian Ministry of Health (repositorio@minsalud.gov.co).

Ethics Statement

This study was a secondary data analysis approved by the Institutional Review Board of Universidad de los Andes, code ID 1114/2019. And it was consider an investigation without risk according to Colombian Ministry of Health laws.

Author Contributions

AG and DL were responsible for the study design and data analyses. AG was responsible for drafting the manuscript and interpretation of the findings. DL and BL provided critical feedback on drafts, and approved the final manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Footnotes

Funding. This work was supported by the Alzheimer's Association and Alzheimer's Society (GBHI ALZ UK-20-640663). AG is an Atlantic Fellow for Equity in Brain Health and received fellowships from the Global Brain Health Institute.

References

  • 1.Alzheimer's Disease International . From Plan to Impact: Progress Towards Targets of the Global Action Plan on Dementia. London: Alzheimer's Disease International; (2018). [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Alzheimer's Disease International . World Alzheimer Report 2015- The Global Impact of Dementia: An Analysis of Prevalence, Incidence, Cost and Trends. London: Alzheimer's Disease International; (2015). [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Organizacion Panamericana de la Salud ADI . Demencia: Una Prioridad de Salud Publica. Washington: Organizacion Panamericana de la Salud; (2013). [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Livingston G, Huntley J, Sommerlad A, Ames D, Ballard C, Banerjee S, et al. Dementia prevention, intervention, and care: 2020 report of the Lancet Commission. Lancet. (2020) 396:413–46. 10.1016/S0140-6736(20)30367-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Fallahpour M, Borell L, Luborsky M, Nygård L. Leisure-activity participation to prevent later-life cognitive decline: a systematic review. Scand J Occup Ther. (2015) 23:162–97. 10.3109/11038128.2015.1102320 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Karp A, Paillard-Borg S, Wang HX, Silverstein M, Winblad B, Fratiglioni L. Mental, physical and social components in leisure activities equally contribute to decrease dementia risk. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. (2006) 21:65–73. 10.1159/000089919 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Wa Lam LC, Cheng ST. Maintaining long-term adherence to lifestyle interventions for cognitive health in late life. Int Psychogeriatr. (2013) 25:171–3. 10.1017/S1041610212001603 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Cheng ST, Chow PK, Yu ECS, Chan ACM. Leisure activities alleviate depressive symptoms in nursing home residents with very mild or mild dementia. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. (2012) 20:904–8. 10.1097/JGP.0b013e3182423988 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Yates LA, Ziser S, Spector A, Orrell M. Cognitive leisure activities and future risk of cognitive impairment and dementia: systematic review and meta-analysis. Int Psychogeriatr. (2016) 28:1791–806. 10.1017/S1041610216001137 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Tolppanen AM, Solomon A, Kulmala J, Kåreholt I, Ngandu T, Rusanen M, et al. Leisure-time physical activity from mid- to late life, body mass index, and risk of dementia. Alzheimers Dement. (2015) 11:434–43.e6. 10.1016/j.jalz.2014.01.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Akbaraly TN, Portet F, Fustinoni S, Dartigues JF, Artero S, Rouaud O, et al. Leisure activities and the risk of dementia in the elderly: results from the three-city study. Neurology. (2009) 73:854–61. 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181b7849b [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Sajeev G, Weuve J, Jackson JW, Vanderweele TJ, Bennett DA, Grodstein F, et al. Late-life cognitive activity and dementia. Epidemiology. (2016) 27:732–42. 10.1097/EDE.0000000000000513 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Scarmeas N, Stern Y. Cognitive reserve and lifestyle. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. (2003) 25:625–33. 10.1076/jcen.25.5.625.14576 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Cheng ST. Cognitive reserve and the prevention of dementia: the role of physical and cognitive activities. Curr Psychiatry Rep. (2016) 18:85. 10.1007/s11920-016-0721-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Stern Y. Cognitive reserve and Alzheimer disease. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. (2006) 20:112–7. 10.1097/01.wad.0000213815.20177.19 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Scarmeas N, Levy G, Tang M-X, Manly J, Stern Y. Influence of leisure activity on the incidence of Alzheimer's Disease. Neurology. (2001) 57:2236–42. 10.1212/WNL.57.12.2236 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Crowe M, Andel R, Pedersen NL, Johansson B, Gatz M. Does participation in leisure activities lead to reduced risk of Alzheimer's disease? A prospective study of Swedish twins. J Gerontol Ser B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. (2003) 58:249–55. 10.1093/geronb/58.5.P249 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Richards M, Hardy R, Wadsworth MEJ. Does active leisure protect cognition? Evidence from a national birth cohort. Soc Sci Med. (2003) 56:785–92. 10.1016/S0277-9536(02)00075-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Cerin E, Leslie E. How socio-economic status contributes to participation in leisure-time physical activity. Soc Sci Med. (2008) 66:2596–609. 10.1016/j.socscimed.2008.02.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Cassarino M, Setti A. Environment as ‘Brain Training': a review of geographical and physical environmental influences on cognitive ageing. Ageing Res Rev. (2015) 23:167–82. 10.1016/j.arr.2015.06.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Gonzalez FJ, Gaona C, Quintero M, Chavez CA, Selga J, Maestre GE. Building capacity for dementia care in Latin America and the Caribbean Francisco. Dement e Neuropsychol. (2014) 8:310–6. 10.1590/S1980-57642014DN84000002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Mukadam N, Sommerlad A, Huntley J, Livingston G. Population attributable fractions for risk factors for dementia in low-income and middle-income countries: an analysis using cross-sectional survey data. Lancet Glob Heal. (2019) 7:e596–603. 10.1016/S2214-109X(19)30074-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Custodio N, Wheelock A, Thumala D, Slachevsky A. Dementia in Latin America: epidemiological evidence and implications for public policy. Front Aging Neurosci. (2017) 9:221. 10.3389/fnagi.2017.00221 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Baez S, Ibáñez A. Dementia in Latin America: an emergent silent tsunami. Front Aging Neurosci. (2016) 8:253. 10.3389/fnagi.2016.00253 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Parra MA, Baez S, Allegri R, Nitrini R, Lopera F, Slachevsky A, et al. Dementia in Latin America Assessing the present and envisioning the future. Neurology. (2018) 90:222–31. 10.1212/WNL.0000000000004897 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Gomez F, Corchuelo J, Curcio CL, Calzada MT, Mendez F. SABE Colombia: survey on health, well-being, and aging in Colombia - study design and protocol. Curr Gerontol Geriatr Res. (2016) 2016:7910205. 10.1155/2016/7910205 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Donovan GO, Hamer M, Sarmiento OL, Hessel P. Education in early life markedly reduces the probability of cognitive impairment in later life in Colombia. Sci Rep. (2020) 10:17685. 10.1038/s41598-020-74822-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Garcia-Cifuentes E, Márquez I, Vasquez D, Aguillon D, Borda MG, Lopera F, et al. The role of gait speed in dementia: a secondary analysis from the SABE Colombia study. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. (2020) 1–8. 10.1159/000510494 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Pérez-Sousa MÁ, del Pozo-Cruz J, Olivares PR, Cano-Gutiérrez CA, Izquierdo M, Ramírez-Vélez R. Role for physical fitness in the association between age and cognitive function in older adults: a mediation analysis of the SABE Colombia study. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2021) 18:751. 10.3390/ijerph18020751 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Icaza MG, Albala C. Minimental State Examinations (MMSE) del Estudio de Demencia en Chile: Analisis Estadistico. Washington, DC: OPS Investig en Salud Pública Doc Técnicos. (1999) p. 7. [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Luck T, Luppa M, Wiese B, Maier W, Den Bussche H Van, Eisele M, et al. Prediction of incident dementia: Impact of impairment in instrumental activities of daily living and mild cognitive impairment-results from the german study on ageing, cognition, and dementia in primary care patients. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. (2012) 20:943–54. 10.1097/JGP.0b013e31825c09bc [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Ministerio de Salud y Protección Social . Oficina de Protección Social-MINSALUD, Departamento Administrativo de ciencia tecnología e innovación-C-. SABE Colombia 2015: Estudio Nacional de Salud, Bienestar y Envejecimiento. Bogotá: Minsalud. (2015). [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Caamaño-Isorna F, Corral M, Montes-Martínez A, Takkouche B. Education and dementia: a meta-analytic study. Neuroepidemiology. (2006) 26:226–32. 10.1159/000093378 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Sharp ES, Gatz M. Relationship between education and dementia an updated systematic review. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. (2011) 25:289–304. 10.1097/WAD.0b013e318211c83c [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Whalley LJ, Deary IJ, Appleton CL, Starr JM. Cognitive reserve and the neurobiology of cognitive aging. Ageing Res Rev. (2004) 3:369–82. 10.1016/j.arr.2004.05.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Polidori MC, Nelles G, Pientka L. Prevention of dementia: focus on lifestyle. Int J Alzheimers Dis. (2010) 2010:393579. 10.4061/2010/393579 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Vemuri P, Lesnick TG, Przybelski SA, Machulda M, Knopman DS, Mielke MM, et al. Association of lifetime intellectual enrichment with cognitive decline in the older population. JAMA Neurol. (2014) 71:1017–24. 10.1001/jamaneurol.2014.963 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Prince M, Acosta D, Ferri CP, Guerra M, Huang Y, Rodriguez JJL, et al. Dementia incidence and mortality in middle-income countries, and associations with indicators of cognitive reserve: a 10/66 Dementia Research Group population-based cohort study. Lancet. (2012) 380:50–8. 10.1016/S0140-6736(12)60399-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Arce Rentería M, Vonk JMJ, Felix G, Avila JF, Zahodne LB, Dalchand E, et al. Illiteracy, dementia risk, and cognitive trajectories among older adults with low education. Neurology. (2019) 93:E2247–56. 10.1212/WNL.0000000000008587 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Bramão I, Mendonça A, Faísca L, Ingvar M, Peterson KM, Reis A. The impact of reading and writing skills on a visuo-motor integration task: a comparison between illiterate and literate subjects. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. (2007) 13:359–64. 10.1017/S1355617707070440 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Petersson KM, Reis A, Ingvar M. Cognitive processing in literate and illiterate subjects: a review of some recent behavioral and functional neuroimaging data. Scand J Psychol. (2001) 42:251–67. 10.1111/1467-9450.00235 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Kosmidis MH, Zafiri M, Politimou N. Literacy versus formal schooling: influence. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. (2011) 26:575–82. 10.1093/arclin/acr063 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Zhang M, Katzman R, Salmon D, Jin H, Cai G, Wang Z, et al. The prevalence of dementia and Alzheimer's disease in Shanghai, China: impact of age, gender, and education. Ann Neurol. (1990) 27:428–37. 10.1002/ana.410270412 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Peterson RL, Gilsanz P, George KM, Ackley S, Glymour MM, Mungas DM, et al. Differences in association of leisure time activities and cognition in a racially/ethnically diverse cohort of older adults: findings from the KHANDLE study. Alzheimers Dement Transl Res Clin Interv. (2020) 6:1–9. 10.1002/trc2.12047 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Singh-Manoux A, Richards M, Marmot M. Leisure activities and cognitive function in middle age: Evidence from the Whitehall II study. J Epidemiol Community Health. (2003) 57:907–13. 10.1136/jech.57.11.907 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Sommerlad A, Sabia S, Livingston G, Kivimäki M, Lewis G, Singh-Manoux A. Leisure activity participation and risk of dementia: 18 year follow-up of the Whitehall II Study. Neurology. (2020) 95:e2803–15. 10.1212/WNL.0000000000010966 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Carlson MC, Parisi JM, Xia J, Xue QL, Rebok GW, Bandeen-Roche K, et al. Lifestyle activities and memory: variety may be the spice of life the women's health and aging study II. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. (2012) 18:286–94. 10.1017/S135561771100169X [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Frankish H, Horton R. Prevention and management of dementia: a priority for public health. Lancet. (2017) 390:2614–5. 10.1016/S0140-6736(17)31756-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Marseglia A, Wang HX, Rizzuto D, Fratiglioni L, Xu W. Participating in mental, social, and physical leisure activities and having a rich social network reduce the incidence of diabetes-related dementia in a cohort of Swedish older adults. Diabetes Care. (2019) 42:232–9. 10.2337/dc18-1428 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Kaup AR, Simonsick EM, Harris TB, Satterfield S, Metti AL, Ayonayon HN, et al. Older adults with limited literacy are at increased risk for likely dementia. J Gerontol Ser A Biol Sci Med Sci. (2014) 69:900–6. 10.1093/gerona/glt176 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Henderson VW, Elias MF. Leisure activity for dementia prevention: more work to be done. Neurology. (2020) 95:895–6. 10.1212/WNL.0000000000010962 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Wells RE, Kerr C, Dossett ML, Danhauer SC, Sohl SJ, Sachs BC, et al. Can adults with mild cognitive impairment build cognitive reserve and learn mindfulness meditation? Qualitative theme analyses from a small pilot study. J Alzheimers Dis. (2019) 70:825–42. 10.3233/JAD-190191 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Iizuka A, Suzuki H, Ogawa S, Kobayashi-Cuya KE, Kobayashi M, Takebayashi T, et al. Can cognitive leisure activity prevent cognitive decline in older adults? A systematic review of intervention studies. Geriatr Gerontol Int. (2019) 19:469–82. 10.1111/ggi.13671 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Wu YT, Fratiglioni L, Matthews FE, Lobo A, Breteler MMB, Skoog I, et al. Dementia in western Europe: epidemiological evidence and implications for policy making. Lancet Neurol. (2016) 15:116–24. 10.1016/S1474-4422(15)00092-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets presented in this article are not readily available because the user agreement does not permit sharing the data directly. Requests to access the datasets should be directed to the Colombian Ministry of Health (repositorio@minsalud.gov.co).


Articles from Frontiers in Neurology are provided here courtesy of Frontiers Media SA

RESOURCES