Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2021 Mar 4;10:e59612. doi: 10.7554/eLife.59612

NMDA receptors control development of somatosensory callosal axonal projections

Jing Zhou 1,2, Yong Lin 1,3, Trung Huynh 1,2, Hirofumi Noguchi 1,2, Jeffrey O Bush 4,5, Samuel J Pleasure 1,2,6,
Editors: Carol A Mason7, Jonathan A Cooper8
PMCID: PMC7959694  PMID: 33661095

Abstract

Callosal projections from primary somatosensory cortex (S1) are key for processing somatosensory inputs and integrating sensory-motor information. How the callosal innervation pattern in S1 is formed during early postnatal development is not clear. We found that the normal termination pattern of these callosal projections is disrupted in cortex specific NMDAR mutants. Rather than projecting selectively to the primary/secondary somatosensory cortex (S1/S2) border, axons were uniformly distributed throughout S1. In addition, the density of this projection increased over postnatal life until the mice died by P30. By combining genetic and antibody-mediated loss of function, we demonstrated that it is GluN2B-containing NMDA receptors in target S1 that mediate this guidance phenotype, thus playing a central role in interhemispheric connectivity. Furthermore, we found that this function of NMDA receptors in callosal circuit formation is independent of ion channel function and works with the EPHRIN-B/EPHB system. Thus, NMDAR in target S1 cortex regulates the formation callosal circuits perhaps by modulating EPH-dependent repulsion.

Research organism: Mouse

Introduction

Synaptic connections between neurons form circuits that can convey neural information. Abnormalities at any stage of synaptic circuit development can result in neuropsychiatric pathology. The corpus callosum (CC) is the largest interhemispheric commissural circuit in mammals. The connectivity of the CC is essential for coordinated sensory-motor function and for many higher cognitive processes, and CC pathology is implicated in a variety of developmental disorders (Paul, 2011).

Callosal projections originate from pyramidal neurons located in layers II/III, V, and VI and traverse the CC to form synapses with neurons in contralateral homotopic or heterotopic cortical areas. We previously showed (Zhou et al., 2013) that the medial-lateral topography of callosal neurons in the cortex is tightly constrained by the dorsal-ventral (D-V) position of axons within the CC. The axon position within the CC determines its terminal location in the contralateral cortex, with dorsally located axons projecting medially and ventrally located axons projecting laterally. As such, the spatial organization of topographically represented information from one hemisphere is preserved as it is projected onto the contralateral hemisphere. However, the molecular determinants regulating proper targeting of commissural projections remain unknown.

In vivo Ca2+ imaging and multiunit recordings show distinct patterns of neural activity in the cortex of newborn mice (Adelsberger et al., 2005; Khazipov and Luhmann, 2006; Khazipov et al., 2004). These activity patterns synchronize spatially and temporally distinct neural networks and may play important roles in wiring cortical maps (Allène et al., 2008; Golshani et al., 2009; Yang et al., 2009). Suppressing endogenous neural activity by overexpressing the inward rectifying potassium channel Kir2.1 in callosal neurons delays axon growth and results ultimately in layer-targeting defects in visual cortex and somatosensory cortex (Mizuno et al., 2007; Rodríguez-Tornos et al., 2016; Wang et al., 2007). Sensory deprivation by ablating whiskers or transecting the infraorbital nerve (ION) before P5 blocks sensory activity to the trigeminal nerve and disrupts barrel field formation in primary somatosensory cortex (S1) with secondary disruption of callosal innervation at the S1/S2 border (Huang et al., 2013; Suárez et al., 2014). These studies show that directly reducing neural activity or blocking ascending sensory inputs to callosal neurons affects callosal targeting and map formation. However, the molecular mechanisms governing these events are not clear.

Neural activity is generally propagated from the axons of presynaptic neurons to the dendrites of postsynaptic neurons by the secretion of neurotransmitters. Neurotransmitter receptors located on the postsynaptic neuron regulate synaptic transmission. The NMDA receptor (NMDAR) is a glutamatergic neurotransmitter receptor located at the synapses that mediates the vast majority of excitatory neurotransmission in the cortex (Traynelis et al., 2010). NMDAR mediated synaptic transmission is important in generating synchronized activity patterns in immature cortex, suggesting that NMDAR Ca2+ channel may be involved in neural circuit formation.

In this study we examined the role of NMDAR in the formation of callosal circuitry. Initially, our hypothesis was that NMDARs would be important modulators of callosal circuit formation and that this would be mediated through the ion channel function by regulating neural activity. Indeed, we did find a crucial role for NMDAR in regulating callosal innervation patterns. To our surprise, this was not mediated by the ion channel function of NMDAR. Rather, there was a specific role for the GluN2B-containing NMDAR and loss of NMDAR resulted in changes in EPHB2 expression. EPHB2 is known to be necessary for localization of NMDAR to synapses (Nolt et al., 2011); however, we found that this requirement was reciprocal – when NMDARs are lost, EPHB2 protein expression during development is lost as well. Most importantly, this is the first demonstration that interactions between NMDAR and EPHRIN-B/EPHB are required for neural circuit formation during development.

Results

Postnatal development of the S1 callosal projection

The development of commissural S1 projections serves as an ideal model to study interhemispheric circuit development. To understand normal development of the S1 callosal projection, we labeled the progenitor cells for layer II/III neurons with enhanced green fluorescent protein (EGFP) by in utero electroporation at embryonic day (E) 15.5 and examined callosal development at four critical time points (Figure 1). At postnatal day (P) 5, the callosal axons from S1 had reached the white matter underneath contralateral S1 (Figure 1B). At P8, the callosal axons were diffusely distributed in contralateral S1 (Figure 1C). By P12, pruning of excess projections led to a refined innervation pattern with a narrow band limited to the S1/S2 border (Figure 1D). After P12, the pattern is generally stable, as shown at P30 (Figure 1E).

Figure 1. The callosal somatosensory innervation pattern was disrupted in Emx1cre/+; Grin1fl/fl mice.

(A–E) Postnatal development of callosal projection in S1. (A) EGFP plasmid injected into lateral ventricle of embryo at embryonic day 15.5 (E15.5) and electrical pulse given to enable the plasmid to enter cortical progenitor cells of layer II/III in the ventricular zone. (B, B’) At postnatal day 5 (P5), the callosal axons from S1 had reached the white matter underneath contralateral S1. (C, C’) At P8, the callosal axons were diffusely distributed in contralateral S1. (D, D’) By P12, pruning of excess projections led to a refined innervation pattern with a narrow band limited to the S1/S2 border. (E, E’) After P12, the pattern was stable as observed at P30. (F) In P14 control mice (Emx1cre/+; Grin1fl/wt), the callosal innervation pattern of S1 of the contralateral cortex is well differentiated with a dense innervation at S1/S2 border. The pattern persists to P30 (J). (G) In GluN1 KO mice (Emx1cre/+; Grin1fl/fl), the innervation pattern was disrupted and projections were extremely diffused which also persisted to P30 (K). (H) Quantification of fluorescent intensity across the medial to lateral extent of the S1. (I, L) Quantification of fluorescence density of S1 region of control vs. GluN1 KO mice at P14 (I, p=0.002) and P30 (L, p=0.0003). Scale bar: 500 μm for all images. S1: primary somatosensory cortex; S2: secondary somatosensory cortex.

Figure 1.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1. The expression of NMDAR in cortex was reduced in Emx1cre/+; Grin1fl/fl mice.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1.

Examples of 12 µm coronal brain sections from P8 Emx1cre/+; Grin1wt/wt (A) and Emx1cre/+; Grin1fl/fl (B) of the same litter. Immunostaining of vesicular glutamate transporter 2 (VGult2) showed thalamocortical barrels in Layer IV of S1 which are pointed out by arrows. The VGlut2 staining in Emx1cre/+; Grin1wt/wt mice revealed a clear barrel pattern (Aa). However, the barrel pattern in Emx1cre/+; Grin1fl/fl mice was disrupted and less distinct (Ba). The GluN1 staining in Emx1cre/+; Grin1wt/wt mice were dense and strong in cortex (Ab, Ac). However, the staining in Emx1cre/+; Grin1fl/fl mice was less bright and apparently reduced in Layer V and VI (Bb, Bc). (C) Western blot of cortical protein extracts from P8 S1. Relative to the loading control GAPDH, the protein levels of GluN1 were greatly reduced in the four samples of Emx1cre/+; Grin1fl/fl mice compared to the four samples of controls. (D) Quantification of protein levels of GluN1 relative to GAPDH. p=0.0002. Scale bar: 100 μm for Ac and Bc; 500 μm for rest of images.

GluN1 knock-out (KO) mice have disrupted callosal innervation

NMDARs are heteromeric receptor channel complexes that differ in subunit composition. To date, seven different subunits have been identified: the GluN1 subunit, four distinct NR2 subunits (A–D), and a pair of GluN3 subunits. The GluN1 subunit is the essential subunit of NMDARs (Dingledine et al., 1999; Paoletti et al., 2013). Global GluN1 knockout mice die within a few hours of birth (Forrest et al., 1994). To explore the role of NMDAR in callosal development, we generated cortex-specific GluN1 knock-out (KO) mice by crossing a floxed GluN1 allele mice (Grin1fl/fl) with Emx1-Cre recombinase (Cre) mice (Emx1cre/+) thereby selectively deleting GluN1 in excitatory cortical projection neurons. Since GluN1 is essential, GluN1 deletion results in loss of functional NMDARs in cortical excitatory neurons. Immunostaining and western blot at P8 showed that expression of cortical GluN1 was greatly diminished (Figure 1—figure supplement 1) in GluN1 KO mice. Consistent with previous studies (Iwasato et al., 2000; Lo et al., 2013), the organization of thalamocortical barrels in Layer IV of S1 was somewhat disrupted but still apparent in GluN1 KO mice at P8, as revealed by vesicular glutamate transporter 2 (VGlut2) immunostaining (Figure 1—figure supplement 1).

To investigate whether NMDAR plays a role in the targeting of callosal projections to contralateral cortex, we first examined the callosal innervation pattern at P14, by which point the mature pattern has formed. In littermate controls (Emx1cre/+; Grin1fl/wt), a dense area of innervation was formed at the S1/S2 border (Figure 1F) and the overall pattern was the same as wild-type controls in Figure 1D. However, in GluN1 KO mice (Emx1cre/+; Grin1fl/fl mice), the normal restricted pattern of callosal targeting to the S1/S2 border was absent, and callosal axons were uniformly distributed throughout the contralateral somatosensory cortex (Figure 1G). Quantitative fluorescence intensity analysis showed that the axon distribution pattern in S1 was statistically significantly different between littermate control and GluN1 KO mice (Figure 1H). Additionally, fluorescence density analysis showed that GluN1 KO mice also had more overall callosal axons innervating S1 at P14 (Figure 1I). This result suggests that the NMDAR plays a role in callosal circuit formation. In the absence of NMDAR, the targeted callosal innervation of the S1/S2 border was lost, and the overall callosal innervation in S1 was significantly increased. GluN1 KO mice were smaller than littermate controls after P5 (data not shown) and rarely survived past P30 so we chose P30 as the last time point to determine if this defect persists. We found that this phenotype worsens after P14 (Figure 1J–L).

GluN1 KO mice prematurely innervate S1

To determine when this prominent targeting defect can first be detected, we examined different time points (P0, P3, P5, and P6) corresponding to critical phases of initial CC circuit formation – initial axon extension to ipsilateral CC (P0), axons crossing the midline (P3), axons reaching the white matter underneath contralateral S1 (P5), and axons starting to innervate S1 (P6). GluN1 KO mice showed no differences with littermate controls at P0, P3, and P5 (Figure 2—figure supplement 1) indicating no difference in the overall rate of axon growth. However, GluN1 KO mice showed earlier and increased innervation of S1 at P6, when callosal axons start entering the contralateral cortex (Figure 2B and E). In control littermates, at P6, callosal axons were gathered into a bundle under contralateral S1 with few axons penetrating into S1. The penetrating axons in S1 were distributed across layer VI (‘*’ in S1 in Figure 2A), and layer V (arrows in S1 in Figure 2A) but failed to penetrate to more superficial layers. In contrast, axons targeting S1 in GluN1 KO mice at P6 had extended past layer V, with many axons terminating in layer II/III (arrows in S1 in Figure 2B). Interestingly, while control mice showed equal degrees of callosal innervation from medial to lateral S1 (see arrows in S1 in Figure 2A), GluN1 KO mice showed preferential early callosal innervation of medial S1 (see arrows in S1 in Figure 2B), and this was even more apparent at P8 (Figure 2C and D).

Figure 2. The callosal innervation defect was first detected at P6 in Emx1cre/+; Grin1fl/fl mice.

(A, A’) At P6, most axons in control grew into deeper layer VI of S1 (see ‘*’); a few axons grew to layer V from medial to lateral S1 (see arrows). However, axons projecting to lateral S2 had grown to layer IV which was apparently faster than the axons in S1 (see arrows). (B, B’) In GluN1 KO mice, most axons had grown to layer V and some even grew to layer I (see arrows) at P6. (C, D) At P8, axons in control and mutant mice had grown to the superficial layer of cortex. However, the innervation patterns were different. Controls showed more axon innervation in the lateral S1 with dense callosal innervation at S1/S2 border (C). Mutants showed slightly more axon innervation in the medial S1 (D). (E) The fluorescence density of mutant mice in S1 was significantly higher than in control mice at P6 which suggested that the mutants had increased axon innervation in contralateral S1 at P6. p=0.003. Scale bar: 500 μm for all images. The square brackets in all images outline the S1. The arrow heads in all images outline the S1/S2 border. White lines outline different layers in the cortex of A–D. M: medial; L: lateral.

Figure 2.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1. There was no difference between Emx1cre/+; Grin1fl/wt and Emx1cre/+; Grin1fl/fl mice during axonal extension into the ipsilateral CC (P0) and to the contralateral CC (P5).

Figure 2—figure supplement 1.

(A, B) The callosal axons in S1 formed a bundle and grew into the ipsilateral CC at P0 in control and GluN1 KO littermates (Emx1cre/+; Grin1fl/wt and Emx1cre/+; Grin1fl/fl mice). The arrows show the extent of axon growth into the CC. By P3, the callosal axons crossed the midline (C, D) and by P5, the callosal axons have grown to underneath the contralateral S1 (E, F). Scale bar: 500 μm for all images.
Figure 2—figure supplement 2. No increased cell death in S1 of Emx1cre/+; Grin1fl/fl mice at P6.

Figure 2—figure supplement 2.

(A) In control mice (Emx1cre/+; Grin1fl/wt), cleaved caspase-3+ cells were mostly detected in layer II/III of M1 (A’), only rare cell death was observed in other cortical regions, such as S1 (A’’). (B) Compared with controls, there was increased cell death in layer II/III of motor cortex in mutant mice (Emx1cre/+; Grin1fl/fl) (B’). However, compared with controls, there was no increased cell death in other cortical regions in mutant mice, such as S1 (B’’). Scale bar: 500 μm for A and B; 200 μm for A’A’’, B’, and B’’.
Figure 2—figure supplement 3. Callosal axon density analysis by Image J.

Figure 2—figure supplement 3.

(A) This picture was an 8-bit image. (B) Fluorescence signals within threshold in image A turned to red after setting up threshold range. Box I was drawn to encompass only the S1. Box II was used to measure axon density in the midline.

Because this defect was first detected at P6, we wondered whether increased cell death of target cortical neurons could account for the mis-innervation of S1. We used active (cleaved) caspase-3 as a marker of cell death in control and GluN1 KO mice at P6. In control mice, cleaved caspase-3+ cells were mostly detected in layer II/III of primary motor cortex (M1), and rarely were observed in other cortical regions (Figure 2—figure supplement 2A). Mutants did have increased cell death in layer II/III of M1 (Figure 2—figure supplement 2B’); however, there was no increase in cell death in other cortical regions including S1 (Figure 2—figure supplement 2B’’).

Taken together, these data indicate that the deletion of NMDAR in excitatory cortical neurons leads to premature and disrupted callosal innervation of contralateral S1. Furthermore, this excessive innervation persists and is not corrected by pruning of mistargeted axons at later developmental stages.

The NMDAR is required in target neurons for normal callosal innervation

In GluN1 KO mice, the NMDAR is deleted from both projecting (presynaptic) and target (postsynaptic) neurons. To examine the role of NMDAR in presynaptic projecting callosal neurons, we electroporated vectors encoding Cre and EGFP into S1 of homozygous floxed GluN1 mice at E15.5 (Figure 3A). Compared with littermate controls (Figure 3B), presynaptic deletion of GluN1 (ipsiS1-/-) from neurons had no discernible effect on callosal innervation at P14 (Figure 3C). This suggests that NMDAR is not required for projecting neurons to properly target S1. To determine whether GluN1 is required in the S1 target neurons, we deleted NMDAR in contralateral target neurons by in utero electroporation. To delete NMDAR in all cortical layers of the contralateral cortex, a Cre vector was electroporated into Grin1fl/fl; Rosa26fs-tdTomato mice in the contralateral/target S1 at E12.5 followed by a second electroporation of EGFP on E15.5 to label ipsilateral/projecting S1 (Figure 3E). Rosa26fs-tdTomato mice express the fluorescent protein tdTomato after Cre-mediated recombination, and therefore labeled cells where Cre-mediated excision occurred and thus GluN1 should be deleted (Madisen et al., 2010). Control mice (Grin1wt/wt; Rosa26fs-tdTomato mice) after double electroporation were analyzed at P14 (Figure 3F) and showed no defects. However, selective deletion of postsynaptic GluN1 in experimental mice strikingly increased callosal S1 innervation (Figure 3G and H, Figure 3—figure supplement 1) similar to the deletion of GluN1 in all excitatory cortical neurons (Figure 1G). These results suggest that NMDAR is required in the target neurons only for normal callosal circuit formation.

Figure 3. NMDAR is required in target neurons for normal callosal innervation.

(A–D) Deleting NMDAR specifically in projecting neurons. Vectors expressing Cre-recombinase (Cre) and EGFP were delivered into S1 of floxed GluN1 mice (Grin1fl/wt x Grin1fl/wt) by in utero electroporation at E15.5 (A). Callosal innervation patterns at P14 in control Grin1+/+ mice (B) and mice after ipsilateral deletion (C). (D) Quantification of fluorescence density. p=0.317. (E–H) Deleting NMDAR specifically in target neurons. GluN1 was deleted in target contralateral S1 by in utero electroporation of Cre at E12.5 in Grin1fl/fl; Rosa26fs-tdTomato mice, the ipsilateral projecting neurons were labeled by EGFP at E15.5 (E). Compared with control Grin1wt/wt; Rosa26fs-tdTomato (F), Grin1fl/fl; Rosa26fs-tdTomato mice which specifically deleted GluN1 in target S1 showed increased callosal innervation in S1 as ‘*’ shows (G). (H) Quantification of fluorescence density. p=0.002. Scale bar: 500 μm for all images. R26tdT: Rosa26fs-tdTomato.

Figure 3.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1. Deleting NMDAR specifically in target neurons.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1.

GluN1 was deleted in target contralateral S1 by in utero electroporation of Cre at E12.5 in Grin1fl/fl; Rosa26fs-tdTomato mice, the ipsilateral projecting neurons were labeled by EGFP at E15.5. This figure shows images from all of the animals that were analyzed for the statistical analysis. Scale bar: 500 μm for all images. R26tdT: Rosa26fs-tdTomato.

Increased callosal innervation in S1 after contralateral injection of anti-NMDAR antibodies

NMDAR antibodies directed to an extracellular domain of GluN1 are known to downregulate the numbers of surface NMDARs. To further examine that it is the contralateral/target expression of NMDAR in deep cortical layers is required for callosal axon targeting, we decided to use infusions of anti-GluN1 antibodies to block NMDAR function and expression in a temporally specific manner. We chose a commercial antibody against the amino acid residues 385–399 in the extracellular N-terminal domain of GluN1. It has been shown that this antibody can alter the surface distribution and dynamics of NMDAR (Dupuis et al., 2014). To examine the efficiency of injection and whether the injection itself can cause brain damage, we injected the anti-GluN1 antibodies into the lateral ventricle from P2 to P8 and perfused the mice 3 hr after the last injection. The distribution of anti-GluN1 antibodies was most abundant in deep cortical layers of the ipsilateral injected hemisphere (Figure 4—figure supplement 1). Thus, presumably due to the widespread high level of expression of GluN1, the antibodies are not distributed widely in the cortex, but rather remain predominantly on the side of injection.

Next, we injected the anti-GluN1 antibodies into the lateral ventricle from P2 to P12 either ipsilateral or contralateral to the origin of EGFP labeled callosal neurons and examined callosal innervation patterns at P14. The fluorescence density analysis at P14 showed that the contralateral (Figure 4E–H) but not ipsilateral antibody injections (Figure 4A–D) led to more overall callosal innervation in S1 at P14. This further supports our genetic data that contralateral/target expression of NMDAR in deep cortical layers is required for callosal axon targeting.

Figure 4. Increased callosal innervation in S1 after contralateral but not ipsilateral injection of anti-NMDAR antibodies from P2 to P12.

(A–D) Anti-GluN1 antibodies were injected into the lateral ventricle from P2 to P12 in ipsilateral cortex. RbIgG served as control. Compared with control (B), antibody injection in mice did not show increased callosal innervation in S1 at P14 (C). (D) Quantification of fluorescence density. p=0.94. (E–H) Anti-GluN1 antibodies were injected into the lateral ventricle from P2 to P12 in contralateral cortex. Compared with control (F), antibody injection in mice showed increased callosal innervation in S1 at P14 (see ‘*', G). (H) Quantification of fluorescence density. p=0.0002. Scale bar: 500 μm for all images.

Figure 4.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1. The efficiency of intraventricular antibody injection and the distribution territory in the cortex after 3 hr of last injection.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1.

(A) Anti-GluN1 antibodies were injected into the lateral ventricle from P2 to P8 and mice were perfused 3 hr later after last injection. Rabbit IgG served as control. Mouse brains then were stained with anti-Rabbit secondary coupled with Alexa594. The red fluorophore of Alexa594 indicated where the antibodies had distributed to. Scale bar: 500 μm for all images. (B, B’) In control, the fluorescence signals were mostly detected in the cortex of the ipsilateral injection side, and few in the contralateral cortex. In the ipsilateral injection side, the signals were detected in all the cortical layers, but most strongly in the pia, layer I, layer V, layer VI, cingulum, and corpus callosum (see arrows). The signals were also detected in the hippocampus and contralateral motor cortex (see arrows). (C, C’) The general antibody distribution pattern was similar as seen in control. Moreover, the anti-GluN1 antibody can bind to NMDAR on the cell membranes, which thus showing beautiful cell membrane staining (see arrows in C’). Scale bar: 500 μm for B, C; 200 μm for B’, C’. CC: corpus callosum; cg: cingulum; Hip: hippocampus; M: motor cortex; S1: primary somatosensory cortex; S2: secondary somatosensory cortex.

NMDAR are required specifically during callosal axon growth into contralateral S1

The antibody injections from P2 to P12 cover all the critical postnatal phases of CC development – (a) axons crossing midline (P3); (b) axons reaching the CC underneath contralateral S1 (P5); (c) growth into S1 (P6–P8); and (d) the refinement of projections (P8–P12). Thus, it is difficult to be certain when during this time the antibodies are acting to cause the observed increased callosal innervation. Our genetic data indicates the role of NMDAR in callosal development is first seen during the process of callosal axon growth into S1. However, the callosal innervation pattern in S1 does not only include the process of projecting into S1 but also the later refinement of projection. To address the temporal role of NMDAR in callosal development, we injected antibodies either from P4 to P8 or from P8 to P14 and examined the callosal innervation pattern at P14. We found that antibody injections from P4 to P8 had increased callosal axonal growth into S1 similar to that observed in NMDAR genetic deletion mice (Figure 5A–D). However, antibody injections only from P8 to P14 had no effect on the overall callosal innervation pattern and we saw no increased callosal innervation in S1 (Figure 5E–H). As mentioned before, the GluN1 KO mice showed earlier and increased innervation of S1 when the callosal axons started entering target cortex at P6 (Figure 2B), but there was no difference between GluN1 KO and littermate controls before P6 (Figure 2—figure supplement 1). Thus, taking the genetic and antibody injection data together suggests that the crucial effect of NMDAR on callosal circuit formation is primarily during callosal projection into the cortex (P6–P8).

Figure 5. Contralateral injection of anti-NMDAR antibodies from P4 to P8 but not P8 to P14 had increased callosal innervation in S1.

Figure 5.

(A–D) Anti-GluN1 antibodies were injected into the lateral ventricle from P4 to P8 in contralateral cortex. RbIgG served as control. Compared with control (B), antibody injection in mice show increased callosal innervation in S1 at P14 (C). (D) Quantification of fluorescence density. p=0.004. (E–H) Anti-GluN1 antibodies were injected into the lateral ventricle from P8 to P14 in contralateral cortex. Compared with control (F), antibody injection in mice did not show increased callosal innervation in S1 at P14 (G). (H) Quantification of fluorescence density. p=0.69. Scale bar: 500 μm for all images.

GluN2B, but not GluN2A, is required for callosal axon targeting

GluN1 is an obligatory component of tetrameric NMDA receptors and is required for assembly of functional NMDAR. Therefore, NMDARs were entirely absent from excitatory cortical projection neurons in our GluN1 KO mice. In the forebrain, GluN1 primarily assembles with GluN2A and GluN2B to form functional NMDARs. GluN2A- and GluN2B-containing NMDARs are functionally distinct (Kutsuwada et al., 1992; Loftis and Janowsky, 2003) and follow different developmental expression trajectories with GluN2B as the major NR2 subunit during the first postnatal week and GluN2A expression present but increasing thereafter (Liu et al., 2004; Monyer et al., 1994; Sans et al., 2000; Sheng et al., 1994). We wondered whether GluN2A-containing or GluN2B-containing NMDARs play different roles in S1 callosal development. Thus, we crossed Emx1cre/+ mice with Grin2afl/fl mice (GluN2A KO) and Grin2bfl/fl mice (GluN2B KO) (Gray et al., 2011). To address previous studies that found that GluN2A was expressed at lower levels during development, we checked the expression of GluN2A in S1 at P8 and found that it was dramatically diminished in Emx1cre/+; Grin2afl/fl mice (Figure 6—figure supplement 2A and B), while the expression of GluN2B was the same in control and Emx1cre/+; Grin2afl/fl mice (Figure 6—figure supplement 2A and C), and similarly GluN2A expression was unchanged in Emx1cre/+; Grin2bfl/fl mice (data not shown).

In GluN2A KO mice, the overall callosal innervation pattern was similar to control mice, although there was increased callosal innervation at the M1/S1 border at P14 (Figure 6A–C). The body size of GluN2A KO mice was not significantly different from littermate controls, and they survived to adulthood. At P30, the general callosal innervation pattern of GluN2A KO mice was similar to littermate controls; however, the increased callosal innervation at the M1/S1 border persisted (Figure 6—figure supplement 1A–C). In contrast, GluN2B deletion phenocopied GluN1 deletion with increased S1 innervation and loss of targeted innervation of the S1/S2 border at P14 (Figure 6D–F). Similar to GluN1 KO mice, GluN2B KO mice were smaller than littermate controls after P5 and rarely survived past P30. Like GluN1 KO mice, this phenotype continued to worsen after P14 (Figure 6—figure supplement 1D–F). Either GluN2A-containing or GluN2B-containing NMDAR is channel competent and GluN2A-containing NMDARs are present in the cortex of GluN2B KO mice, thus it seems possible that NMDAR’s role in callosal circuit development may be separable from its channel activity.

Figure 6. Emx1cre/+; Grin2bfl/fl but not Emx1cre/+; Grin2afl/fl mice had the same disrupted callosal innervation patterns as Emx1cre/+; Grin1fl/fl at P14.

Callosal innervation patterns in control Emx1cre/+; Grin2afl/wt (A) and Emx1cre/+; Grin2afl/fl mice (B) at P14. ‘*’ points out M1/S1 border. (C) Quantification of fluorescence density. p=0.392. Callosal innervation patterns in control Emx1cre/+; Grin2bfl/wt (D) and Emx1cre/+; Grin2bfl/fl mice (E) at P14. (F) Quantification of fluorescence density. p=0.03. Scale bar: 500 μm for all images.

Figure 6.

Figure 6—figure supplement 1. NR2B (Emx1cre/+; Grin2bfl/fl) but not NR2A (Emx1cre/+; Grin2afl/fl) mice had same disrupted callosal innervation patterns as Emx1cre/+; Grin1fl/fl at P30.

Figure 6—figure supplement 1.

(A) The callosal innervation pattern in S1 at P30 in control mice (Emx1cre/+; Grin2afl/wt) is similar as the pattern in P14 WT control mice, with few axons in S1 but a dense innervation at S1/S2 border. (B) In the mutant mice (Emx1cre/+; Grin2afl/fl), the general innervation pattern was as same as control. However, the increased callosal innervation at the border of M1 and S1 was persistent at P30 (see ‘*’ in B’). (C) Quantification of fluorescence density. p=0.63. (D) In control Emx1cre/+; Grin2bfl/wt mice, the callosal innervation pattern at P30 was as normal as WT control. (E) However, the increased callosal innervation in Emx1cre/+; Grin2bfl/fl mice lasted at least to P30 as we observed in Emx1cre/+; Grin1fl/fl mice at P30. (F) Quantification of fluorescence density. p=0.007. Scale bar: 500 μm for all images.
Figure 6—figure supplement 2. The protein expression level of GluN2A and GluN2B in S1 at P8.

Figure 6—figure supplement 2.

(A) Western blot analysis of cortical protein extracts from P8 S1 showed that GluN2A protein has been expressed in S1 at P8. Relative to the loading control GAPDH, the protein levels of GluN2A were greatly reduced in the five samples of Emx1cre/+; Grin2afl/fl mice compared to the five samples of controls. However, the protein levels of GluN2B were same between the five samples of Emx1cre/+; Grin2afl/fl mice and the five samples of controls. (B) Quantification of protein levels of GluN2A relative to GAPDH. p=0.007. (C) Quantification of protein levels of GluN2B relative to GAPDH. p=0.5.

NMDAR regulates callosal circuit development independent of NMDAR channel activity

Ca2+ influx through NMDARs is essential for synaptogenesis, experience-dependent synaptic remodeling, and long-lasting changes in synaptic efficacy such as long-term potentiation (LTP) and long-term depression (LTD) (Collingridge et al., 2004; Lau and Zukin, 2007). However, accumulating evidence shows that there are NMDAR functions independent of its ion-influx (Kessels et al., 2013; Nabavi et al., 2013), such as a use-dependent tyrosine dephosphorylation of NMDA receptors is independent of ion flux (Vissel et al., 2001), and NMDAR-dependent LTD which can be induced independent of Ca2+ influx (Dore et al., 2016). In models of ischemic stroke, neuronal death caused by overactivation of NMDAR is also independent of Ca2+ influx, but dependent on signaling complexes formed by NMDARs, Src kinase, and Panx1 (Weilinger et al., 2016). To address whether channel activity of the NMDAR is required in callosal targeting, we systemically injected MK-801, a non-competitive NMDAR antagonist during CC development. MK-801 enters the open NMDAR channel and binds to the ‘blocking site’ located deep in the pore, blocking Ca2+ influx through NMDAR (Huettner and Bean, 1988Figure 7A). Based on previous literature, a single dose of MK-801 for acute i.p. administration is up to 1–10 mg/kg (Foster et al., 1988; Mitrovic et al., 1996); the daily dose of MK-801 for chronic i.p. administration is around 0.3–0.6 mg/kg (Nilsson et al., 1997; Uttl et al., 2018; Zuo et al., 2006). Previous studies showed that blocking NMDAR with a daily dose of 0.4 mg/kg MK-801 in ferret pups between P14 and P21 disrupted axonal pattern formation by retinal afferents in the lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN) (Hahm et al., 1999). We identified a dose of 1 mg/kg MK-801 (see Materials and methods for detail) to block Ca2+ influx of NMDAR from P4 to P12 and examined the callosal innervation pattern at P14 (Figure 7A–D). Compared with saline control, MK801-treated pups gained weight slowly and developed opisthotonic posturing of limbs and head (data not shown), suggesting the channel function of NMDAR had been blocked. However, the normal callosal innervation pattern in these pups was similar to saline-treated controls (Figure 7B–D). Similar weight loss and abnormal behaviors have been reported in neonatal mice after MK-801 administration previously (Facchinetti et al., 1993; Griesbach and Amsel, 1998; Wu et al., 2005). We also performed similar experiments using D(-)−2-amino-5-phosphonopentanoic acid (D-APV), which competitively blocks the ligand (glutamate) binding site to NMDAR and inhibits channel opening, thereby preventing Ca2+ influx (Morris, 1989Figure 7E). In a previous study, a one-time infusion of 5 μg D-APV into the basolateral amygdala of adult rat (~250 g) caused memory deficit persistent for at least 4 weeks (Milton et al., 2008). Also, blocking NMDARs by intracerebral infusion of 0.05 μg D-APV into P7 mouse pup reduced somatic calcium transients in pyramidal cells evoked by lateral olfactory tract stimulation, and caused memory deficits both in short-term (3 hr) and long-term (24 hr) odor preference memory (Mukherjee and Yuan, 2016). We injected D-APV (5 μg/μl, 0.8–1 μl/injection, see Materials and Methods for detail) into the lateral ventricle of the contralateral S1 twice-daily from P4 to P12 and examined the callosal innervation pattern at P14 (Figure 7E–H). Compared with saline control, D-APV-treated pups developed unilateral muscle contractions in limbs on the opposite side of the injection (data not shown), suggesting the channel function of NMDAR had been blocked. However, the callosal innervation pattern in these pups was again similar to saline controls (Figure 7F–H). Taken together, these results indicate that NMDAR function in callosal targeting is independent of its channel activity.

Figure 7. NMDAR regulates callosal circuit development independent of NMDAR channel activity.

Figure 7.

(A–D) Blocking Ca2+ influx through NMDAR by MK-801. MK-801 enters the open NMDAR channel and binds to the ‘blocking site’ located deep in the pore (A). Callosal innervation patterns in Saline (B) and MK-801 (C) injected mice at P14. (D) Quantification of fluorescence density. p=0.91. (E–H) Blocking NMDAR channel opening by D-APV. D-APV competitively inhibits glutamate binding site to NMDAR (E). Callosal innervation patterns in saline (F) and D-APV (G) injected mice at P14. (H) Quantification of fluorescence density. p=0.93. Scale bar: 500 μm for all images.

EPHB2 protein expression is decreased in GluN1 KO mice

Our studies to this point led us to consider whether the NMDAR may mediate callosal axon targeting via interaction with other guidance signaling systems. Previous studies have shown that NMDAR subunits bind directly to EPHB axon guidance receptors (Dalva et al., 2000). EPHB receptor tyrosine kinases and their transmembrane-ligands, the EPHRIN-B family, mediate short-distance cell–cell signaling and thus regulate many developmental processes at the interface between pattern formation and morphogenesis, including ordered neural maps (Kania and Klein, 2016; Niethamer and Bush, 2019). Further, several members of the EPH/EPHRIN family, including EPHRIN-B1 and EPHB2, are involved in earlier stages of CC midline axon crossing, strengthening their relevance in this context (Bush and Soriano, 2009; Orioli et al., 1996; Robichaux et al., 2016). EPHB2 and NMDARs colocalize at postsynaptic dendrites, and the extracellular domain of NMDAR interacts directly with EPHB2, an interaction driven by activation of EPHB2 by clustered EPHRIN-B1 expressed in presynaptic axon terminals (Dalva et al., 2000; Nolt et al., 2011; Palmer and Klein, 2003). Mice lacking EPHB2 have reduced levels of NMDARs at synapses in the hippocampus and cortex (Henkemeyer et al., 2003; Sheffler-Collins and Dalva, 2012), suggesting coordinated localization. EPHB2 also preferentially decreases Ca2+-dependent inactivation of GluN2B-containing NMDARs but not GluN2A-containing NMDARs at synapses of mature neurons (Nolt et al., 2011). In addition, EPHB2 signaling leads to phosphorylation of GluN2B at tyrosine residue 1472 preventing clathrin-dependent endocytosis, and increasing the surface retention of GluN2B-containing NMDARs (Chen and Roche, 2007; Nolt et al., 2011; Takasu et al., 2002). Taken together, these pieces of evidence suggested us that NMDAR may cooperate with EPHRIN-B/EPHB signaling during initial circuit formation.

We thus examined expression of EPHB2 in NMDAR KO mice by immunostaining. At P5, EPHB2 was found in both cortex and CC of controls (Figure 8—figure supplement 1A). However, in GluN1 KO mice, EPHB2 expression in the cortex was reduced (Figure 8—figure supplement 1B). Western blots also confirmed that protein levels of EPHB2 were reduced at least 30% in GluN1 KO mice (Figure 8—figure supplement 1C–E) while mRNA levels of EPHB2 were unchanged (Figure 8—figure supplement 1F). As discussed above, in Emx1cre/+; Grin1fl/fl mice, GluN1 was selectively deleted in excitatory neurons. Based on previous studies, the expression of GluN1 in interneurons (Korotkova et al., 2010), oligodendrocytes, and oligodendrocyte precursor cells (Káradóttir et al., 2005) in the cortex should not be affected in Emx1cre/+; Grin1fl/fl mice. To examine the expression of EPHB2 specifically in GluN1 KO cells, we crossed Emx1cre/+; Grin1fl/fl mice with Cre-reporter mice – Rosa26fs-tdTomato mice that exhibit tdTomato expression after Cre-mediated recombination. Supporting our hypothesis that loss of GluN1 leads to loss of EPHB2, the punctate staining of EPHB2 on cell membranes was completely lost in red (recombined) cells of Emx1cre/+; Grin1fl/fl; Rosa26fs-tdTomato brain sections but not red cells of Emx1cre/+; Grin1wt/wt; Rosa26fs-tdTomato brain sections (Figure 8—figure supplement 2). Taken together, we demonstrated that the loss of NMDAR caused the loss of EPHB2 selectively on cells that lack GluN1 after excision, thus explaining the 30% reduction in EPHB2 protein expression. Given the known physical association between NMDAR and EPHB2, these data suggest reciprocity in this stabilizing interaction and dendritic localization.

NMDAR cooperates with EPHRIN-B/EPHB in controlling axon targeting in S1

EPHB2 and NMDARs colocalize at postsynaptic dendrites, and the extracellular domain of NMDAR interacts directly with EPHB2, an interaction driven by activation of EPHB2 by clustered EPHRIN-B1 expressed in presynaptic axon terminals (Dalva et al., 2000). This is consistent with the possibility that EPHRIN-B1, expressed by the projecting neuronal axons, signals through EPHB2 and NMDAR, located on the target neurons, to regulate axon extension in the contralateral cortex (Figure 8A). To test this prospect, we deleted EPHRIN-B1 in projecting neurons by electroporating vectors of Cre and EGFP at E15.5 in Efnb1fl/fl mice and examined the initial callosal targeting at P6 (Figure 8B and C). In projecting neurons lacking EPHRIN-B1, callosal axons extended into the cortex past layer V, and many axons terminated in layer II/III (arrows in Figure 8C), similar to that observed in GluN1 KO mice at P6 (Figure 2B).

Figure 8. NMDARs cooperate with EPHRIN-B/EPHB in controlling axon targeting in S1.

(A) EPHRIN-B1, expressed by the projecting neuronal axons, signals through EPHB2 and NMDAR, located on the target neurons, regulates axon extension in contralateral cortex. (B, C) Deleting EPHRIN-B1 in projecting neurons in Efnb1fl/fl mice. Vectors expressing Cre and EGFP were delivered into S1 of all pups from Efnb1fl/wt × Efnb1fl/wt crosses by in utero electroporation at E15.5. Compared with control mice (B), ipsilaterally deleted mice showed earlier callosal innervation at P6 (C). (D, E) Blocking NMDAR function in target neurons by intraventricular injection of GluN1Ab in contralateral cortex, from P3 to P6. Compared with control RbIgG injected mice (D), GluN1Ab injected mice showed earlier callosal innervation at P6 (E). (F, G) Blocking EPHB function in target neurons by intraventricular injection of EPHRIN-B1-Fc in contralateral cortex, from P3 to P6. Compared with control Hum Fc injected mice, EPHRIN-B1-Fc injected mice showed earlier callosal innervation at P6 (G). (H, I) Blocking NMDAR and EPHB function simultaneously by GluN1Ab and EPHRIN-B1-Fc in contralateral cortex, from P3 to P6. Compared with control RbIgG + Hum IgG injected mice (H), GluN1Ab + EPHRIN-B1-Fc injected mice showed earlier callosal innervation at P6 (I). (J) Quantification of fluorescence density. Scale bar: 500 μm for all images. Arrows pointed out axon terminals in the target cortex.

Figure 8.

Figure 8—figure supplement 1. The protein but not RNA level of EPHB2 was reduced in Emx1cre/+; Grin1fl/fl mice at P8.

Figure 8—figure supplement 1.

(A, B) EPHB2 protein expression is decreased in Emx1cre/+; Grin1fl/fl mice at P5. In control Emx1cre/+; Grin1wt/wt mice, EPHB2 was expressed both in CC and cortex (A). EPHB2 in Emx1cre/+; Grin1fl/fl mice was decreased in cortex (B). (C) Western blot analysis of cortical protein extracts from P8 S1 showed that, relative to the loading control beta-tubulin (β-Tub) and GAPDH, lower levels of EPHB2 were observed in the five samples of Emx1cre/+; Grin1fl/fl mice compared to the five samples of controls. (D) Quantification of protein levels relative to β-Tub. p=0.001. (E) Quantification of protein levels relative to GAPDH. p<0.0001. (F) The quantitative polymerase chain reaction (qPCR) analysis showed no expression difference of EPHB2 between Emx1cre/+; Grin1fl/fl mice and controls. Scale bar: 500 μm for A, B.
Figure 8—figure supplement 2. The cell membrane expression of EPHB2 was absent in Emx1cre/+; Grin1fl/fl; Rosa26fs-tdTomato positive cells.

Figure 8—figure supplement 2.

(A) We crossed Emx1cre/+; Grin1fl/fl mice with Cre-reporter Rosa26fs-tdTomato mice to produce GluN1 knockout cells labeled with red fluorescence. (B) 12 µm coronal brain sections from P8 Emx1cre/+; Grin1wt/wt; Rosa26fs-tdTomato and Emx1cre/+; Grin1fl/fl; Rosa26fs-tdTomato mice of the same litter. Immunostaining of EPHB2 was done in all brain sections. Broad arrowheads pointed the cells with Cre recombination. Triangle arrowheads pointed to cells with Cre recombination. V shaped arrowheads pointed to cells without Cre recombination. In control Emx1cre/+; Grin1wt/wt; Rosa26fs-tdTomato mice, EPHB2 signals were detected on both Cre recombination and non-Cre recombination cells. However, in Emx1cre/+; Grin1fl/fl; Rosa26fs-tdTomato mice, EPHB2 signals were only detected on non-Cre recombination cells, but absent on Cre recombination cells with deletion of GluN1. (C) Quantification of fluorescence density of EPHB2 immunostaining for each cell. Compared to control (Emx1cre/+; Grin1wt/wt; Rosa26fs-tdTomato mice), the protein expression of EPHB2 in Cre recombination cells in Emx1cre/+; Grin1fl/fl; Rosa26fs-tdTomato mice was dramatically decreased. p<0.0001. Scale bar: 10 μm for all images in B. R26tdT: Rosa26fs-tdTomato.

Since this manipulation does not take into account potential compensation by other EPHRIN-B ligands, we blocked all EPHB2 signaling in the contralateral cortex by intraventricular injection of soluble EPHRIN-B1-Fc, from P3 to P6 and examined the initial callosal targeting at P6 (Figure 8F and G). EPHRINs have to be clustered in a cell membrane or artificially with, for example, antibodies to efficiently activate EPH receptors (Davis et al., 1994). Unclustered soluble EPHRINs bind EPH receptors but act as antagonists (Vearing and Lackmann, 2005). Compared with littermate controls, contralateral injection of soluble EPHRIN-B1-Fc led to increased callosal innervation at P6 with axons penetrating to superficial layers (Figure 8F, G, and J). These results support the idea that EPHRIN-B1-EPHB2 play important roles in controlling callosal axon penetration into the contralateral S1 cortex.

To further determine whether there is collaboration between NMDAR and EPH-B signaling, we developed a way to assess blockade of both systems and determine if this blockade was additive, synergistic or likely acting in the same pathway. We blocked NMDAR in contralateral/target cortex by intraventricular injection of anti-GluN1 antibodies, from P3 to P6, and examined the initial callosal targeting at P6 (Figure 8D and E). Compared with controls, contralateral injection of anti-GluN1 antibodies caused increased callosal innervation at P6 and penetration to superficial layers of cortex (Figure 8D, E, and J). Critically, blocking NMDAR and EPHRIN-B1-EPHB2 signaling simultaneously in contralateral cortex by injecting both anti-GluN1 antibody and EPHRIN-B1-Fc did not show any additive or synergistic effects (Figure 8H–J). This suggests that in fact NMDAR and EPHRIN-B1-EPHB2 are in the same pathway in regulating callosal targeting in S1 and is consistent with our other findings.

Discussion

In this study, we found that NMDARs cooperate with EPHRIN-B/EPHB in controlling callosal circuit formation and demonstrated that it is GluN2B-containing NMDARs in target S1 cortex that regulate callosal axon targeting in S1. In mutants where either GluN2B or GluN1 are disrupted, there is excess axonal growth throughout S1. Either genetic or antibody-mediated loss of NMDAR specifically in the target hemisphere disrupted this projection. We found that this begins at P6 when axons that should terminate in deep cortical layers of target S1 instead project more superficially. Once this targeting defect begins, it continues to worsen. We believe this phenotype is most consistent with a loss of a repellant activity that normally restricts commissural axonal projections to the S1 cortex at initial stages of this process (Figure 2). Once these axons aberrantly penetrate through the deeper cortical layers of S1 in the GluN1 mutants, this phenotype continues to worsen. A systematic role for NMDAR in this process was a surprise and has significant implications for disorders where NMDAR function is lost due to mutations or due to antibody-mediated disruption of NMDAR surface distribution. We predict this would be a potent disruptor of circuit formation during development certainly in this callosal circuit, but likely others as well.

Mechanisms for the formation of homotopic callosal projection during development

In previous studies (Zhou et al., 2013), we showed that the medial-lateral topography of callosal neurons is tightly constrained by the D-V position of their axons within the CC. The axon position within the CC determines the contralateral cortical projection, with more dorsally located axons projecting medially and ventrally located axons projecting laterally. The complementary expression of chemotropic guidance cue Semaphorin3A (Sem3A) and its receptor Neuropilin-1 (Nrp1) contributes to this axonal order within the CC. The studies of genetic manipulations showed that Sema3A/Nrp1 signaling contributes to the topographic order of axons within the CC but is not involved in the axon position-dependent projection to the contralateral target cortex. Here, in this study, we demonstrated that contralateral/target expression of NMDAR controls the pattern of callosal projections to contralateral target S1. Interestingly, despite the disruption of targeting within S1 in mice with disrupted NMDAR, there is little evidence of ectopic projection to other cortical areas. This indicates that there are still other cues that regulate the generic projection to S1 but that the mechanisms we have uncovered help govern the distribution of these axons within S1.

NMDARs cooperate with EPHRIN-B/EPHB to regulate axon extension into the cortex

Given the longstanding understanding of important roles for neural activity in the wiring of circuits and the generation of cortical maps, we expected that the function of NMDAR in this process would be due to the critical role of NMDAR in excitatory activity in the developing brain as synapses form. However, our data show that NMDAR ion channel function is not necessary for its role in somatosensory callosal targeting. These data are consistent with the idea that NMDAR protein complexes containing GluN1 and GluN2B are critical for commissural projection. Our findings suggest that without NMDAR containing GluN1 and GluN2B in the target cortex, there is earlier and increased callosal innervation of S1 starting at P6, when callosal axons start entering the contralateral cortex. We believe that this observation indicates that either loss of NMDAR leads to an increase in the attractiveness of S1 cortex or that loss of NMDAR leads to loss of a repellant activity in the cortex that normally limits axon growth into superficial layers of S1, until axons reach the S1/S2 border. This realization led us to consider whether there might be a role for NMDAR complexes in collaborating with already known axon guidance mechanisms.

EPHRIN-B/EPHB signals are well-known repulsive axon guidance cues. Because EPHB2 is necessary for localization of NMDAR to postsynaptic terminals, we wondered if this requirement was reciprocal and found indeed that it is – when NMDARs are lost, membrane based EPHB2 protein expression in target neurons is lost as well (Figure 8—figure supplement 2). This result suggests that NMDAR reciprocally regulates the localization of EPHB2. In addition, the NMDAR interaction with EPHB2 is driven by the stimulation of EPHB2 by clustered EPHRIN-B1 expressed in presynaptic axon terminals, indicating that the interaction may regulate axon guidance and termination. When we deleted EPHRIN-B1 only in projecting callosal axons, this also led to excess ingrowth into the S1 cortex visible from P6 to P8. That this phenotype is less severe than the NMDAR phenotype is most likely due to redundancy with other EPHRIN-B ligands also expressed in projecting axons. To globally disrupt EPHB-EPHRIN-B interactions we injected EPHRIN-B1-Fc fragments and found that these also disrupted the somatosensory callosal targeting as efficiently as anti-NMDAR antibodies; furthermore, these two treatments were neither additive nor synergistic, implying they act through a common pathway. Further studies will examine whether this phenotype is due to loss of repulsive activity of EPHRIN-B/EPHB signals, and whether it is through forward EPHB or reverse EPHRIN-B signaling or both.

The refinement of callosal innervation pattern

The production of transient, exuberant axons, and axonal branches is a general phenomenon in development across species and systems (De León Reyes et al., 2019; Fenlon et al., 2017; Hand et al., 2015; Innocenti, 2020; Innocenti and Price, 2005; Luo and O'Leary, 2005; Ribeiro Gomes et al., 2020). Our data indicate that NMDAR are required to collaborate with EPHRIN-B/EPHB in target cortex, and the most parsimonious conclusion is that this acts as a repellant to limit early growth of callosal axons into S1 cortex. These phenotypes are seen first at P6 when axons inappropriately enter superficial cortex in S1 so that by P30 (when the animals die) there is a nearly sixfold increase in axonal extension into S1. This is consistent with a role for NMDAR in controlling initial projection into S1 but probably also later regulating refinement of these inappropriate projections by pruning – both core functions of axonal repellants. Previous studies have observed the role of NMDAR in axon elimination (Colonnese and Constantine-Paton, 2006; Henson et al., 2017; Personius et al., 2016; Rabacchi et al., 1992; Rajan et al., 1999; Zhang et al., 2013). In vivo time-lapse images of retinal axons from albino Xenopus tadpoles show that correlated neural activity influences axon branch dynamics of retinal axons in the optic tectum (Ruthazer et al., 2003). The axon branches whose activity is not correlated with that of their neighbors are selectively eliminated. However, the selective elimination can be abolished by blocking the Ca2+ influx of NMDAR by its antagonist MK-801. When we blocked the Ca2+ influx of NMDAR by MK-801 or D-APV, even though the overall callosal innervation pattern of somatosensory cortex was not affected, there were some subtle, local laminar-specific innervation differences at the S1/S2 border, suggesting that the refinement of axon arborization may rely on the Ca2+ influx-mediated activity changes through NMDAR.

Regarding previous roles of EPHRIN-B/EPHB signaling in pruning, this pathway was previously shown to be involved in infrapyramidal bundle pruning in dentate gyrus (Xu and Henkemeyer, 2009) and it is known that activation of EPHB by EPHRIN-B potentiates Ca2+ influx of NMDA receptor (Takasu et al., 2002). These lines of evidence suggest that NMDAR and EPHRIN-B/EPHB signaling may cooperate in the refinement of the callosal innervation pattern and perhaps in other circuits. Further studies will examine how and whether NMDAR regulates EPHRIN-B/EPHB signaling directly and the detailed molecular mechanism underlying their collaboration to regulate callosal innervation patterns in the primary somatosensory cortex.

Materials and methods

Experimental model and subject details

All animal protocols were in accordance with the regulations of the National Institute of Health and approved by the University of California San Francisco Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (IACUC). Floxed GluN1Grin1 allele (Stock #005246), EMX1-Cre (Stock #005628) and Rosa26<fs-tdTomato> Cre reporter allele (Stock # 007914) were obtained from Jackson Laboratories (Bar Harbor, ME, USA). Floxed Grin2a and Grin2b alleles were provided by the laboratory of Prof. Roger Nicoll. Floxed EphrinB1 allele was provided by the laboratory of Prof. Jeffrey Bush. Wild-type CD1 mice were obtained from Charles River Laboratories. Male and female embryos at embryonic (E) 12.5 and E15.5 were used for the in utero electroporation, and pups between postnatal day 0 (P0) to 30 (P30) for the experiments.

In utero electroporation

DNA solution including the plasmid and 0.04% fast green was injected into the medial region of the lateral ventricle of the embryonic brain with a glass micropipette. Electrical pulses then were delivered to embryos by electrodes connected to a square-pulse generator (ECM830, BTX). For each electroporation, five 35 V pulses of 50 ms were applied at 1 s intervals. After the electroporation, the uterus was returned to the abdominal cavity, followed by suturing of the abdominal wall and skin. Mice were perfused at different postnatal stages using 4% paraformaldehyde followed by post-fixed overnight and incubation in 30% sucrose at 4°C. 35 μm-thick coronal sections were obtained using cryostat sectioning.

Plasmid

Plasmid of pCAGGS-Cre and pCAGGS-CreERT was obtained from Addgene. The ubiquitin-EGFP plasmid used was from a previous study (Zhou et al., 2013).

Antibodies: Antibodies for intraventricular injection

Commercial anti-NMDAR antibody is against amino acid residues 385–399 in the N-terminus of GluN1 and was made in Rabbit (AGC-001, Alomone labs). Rabbit IgG (#31235, Invitrogen) served as control. Recombinant Mouse EPHRIN-B1 Fc Chimera Protein was obtained from R and D (473-EB). Recombinant Human IgG1 Fc Protein (110-HG, R and D) served as control. Antibodies for immunostaining: Rabbit anti-GluN1 (1:500, AGC-001, Alomone labs), anti-vGlut2 (1:200, AB2251, Millipore), goat anti-EphB2 (1:50, AF467, R and D), anti-cleaved caspase-3 (#9661S, Cell Signaling), anti-Rabbit 594 (#711-585-152, Jackson ImmunoResearch), and anti-guinea pig 488 (A-11073, Invitrogen). Antibodies for western blot: rabbit anti-GluN1 (AB9864, Millipore), rabbit anti-GluN2A (#4205, Cell signaling), rabbit anti-GluN2B (#4212, Cell signaling), goat anti-EPHB2 (1:1000, AF467, R and D), rabbit anti-GAPDH (1:5000, #2118, Cell Signaling), rabbit anti-β tubulin (1:5000, #2128, Cell Signaling), IRDye 680RD Donkey anti-Goat IgG(H+L) Ab (1:10,000, #926–68074, Li-Cor), and IRDye 800CW Goat × Rabbit IgG(H+L) Ab (1:20,000, #925–32211, Li-Cor).

Intraventricular injection

Antibodies/Fc-fragment was injected to lateral ventricular of pups by glass pipette with a sharp bevel at 45° (BV-10 Micropipette Beveler, Sutter instrument). The diameter of pipette tip was ~40–80 μm (Vogt et al., 2015). The concentrations for antibody injections were 3.2 μg/μl for the commercial anti-GluN1 antibody and Rabbit IgG. The concentrations for Fc injections were 2 μg/μl for EPHRIN-B1 Fc and Human IgG1 Fc. Antibodies/Fc-fragment was injected twice-daily and the injection volume was 0.8–1 μl for each injection.

MK-801 systemic injection

EGFP positive pups were given intraperitoneal (i.p.) injection of MK-801 (1 mg/kg, M107-25MG, Sigma) or equivalent volume of 0.9% saline once-daily. See the following for the criteria of selecting effective dose of MK-801. Based on literature, the single dose of MK-801 for acute i.p. administration is up to 1–10 mg/kg (Foster et al., 1988); the daily dose of MK-801 for chronic i.p. administration is around 0.3–0.6 mg/kg (Nilsson et al., 1997; Uttl et al., 2018; Zuo et al., 2006). To optimize the dose for chronic administration, MK-801 was given to three groups of pups (each group had four pups with mixed genders) from P4 to P9 with the doses of 1 mg/kg, 10 mg/kg, and 20 mg/kg, respectively. Saline was given to four pups of the same litter as control. We measured body weights of all pups every day before i.p. administration. The pups in saline group always had abundant milk in their stomachs and gained weight rapidly. The pups with the dose of 1 mg/kg had milk in their stomachs and gained weight, but very slowly. Some even lost weights. All MK-801-treated pups developed abnormal behavior-opisthotonic posturing for heads and four limbs, similar to the abnormal postures in children with anti-NMDAR encephalitis (Florance et al., 2009). The doses of 10 mg/kg and 20 mg/kg were too close to lethal dose and the pups lost weight and died within 2–3 days. Thus, we chose 1 mg/kg for our experiment. The weight loss and abnormal behavior had been reported in neonatal mice after MK-801 administration (Facchinetti et al., 1993; Griesbach and Amsel, 1998; Wu et al., 2005).

D(-)−2-amino-5-phosphonopentanoic acid (D-APV) intraventricular injection

Because D-APV poorly penetrates the blood–brain barrier (BBB) when administered systemically, we injected it directly into the lateral ventricle of targeted cortex. EGFP positive pups were given D-APV (5 μg/μl, Millipore-sigma, 165304–5 MG) twice-daily at an injection volume of 0.8–1 μl/injection. See section Intraventricular injection for injection details. See the following for the criteria of selecting effective dose of D-APV. Based on literature, one-time infusion of 5 μg D-APV into the basolateral amygdala of adult rat (~250 g) caused memory deficit persistent for at least 4 weeks (Milton et al., 2008). Blocking NMDARs by intracerebral infusion of 0.5 μl of 500 μM D-APV into P7 mouse pup reduced somatic calcium transients in pyramidal cells evoked by lateral olfactory tract stimulation, and caused memory deficits both in short-term (3 hr) and long-term (24 hr) odor preference memory (Mukherjee and Yuan, 2016). 0.5 μl of 500 μM D-APV is equal to 0.05 μg D-APV (molecular weight of D-APV: 197.13). To optimize the dose for chronic administration, intraventricular D-APV injection was given to two groups of pups (each group had four pups with mixed genders) from P4 (~4 g) to P9 (~8 g) with the concentrations of 5 μg/μl and 10 μg/μl twice-daily (0.8–1 μl/injection), respectively. Saline was given to four pups of the same litter as control. The body weight of all pups was measured before intraventricular injection. There were no body weight differences between saline-treated and D-APV-treated mice. However, D-APV-treated pups in both 5 μg/μl-treated and 10 μg/μl-treated groups developed unilateral muscle contractions in limbs on the opposite side of the injection within 10 min after injection, similar to the abnormal movements seen in MK-801-treated mice but only restricting in one side of body. No pups died during the 6 days of injections. As 5 μg/μl-treated and 10 μg/μl-treated had no dose-dependent effect on the abnormal behavior and based on dosage of D-APV used in literature, we chose 5 μg/μl D-APV treatment for our experiment.

Slice preparation and imaging

Mice were perfused with saline followed by 4% paraformaldehyde in phosphate buffered saline (PBS), pH 7.4. Brains were removed from mice and post-fixed in 4% paraformaldehyde overnight before being placed in 30% sucrose solution. The brains were then cut into 35 µm sections with cryostat (Leica VT1200S). Sections were imaged by Zeiss Axioscan Z.1 (Zeiss, Thornwood, NY, USA) with a 20× objective.

Immunostaining

Mouse pups were perfused with saline followed by 4% paraformaldehyde in PBS, pH 7.4. Brains were removed from mice and post-fixed in 4% paraformaldehyde overnight before being placed in 30% sucrose solution. The brains were then cut into 12 µm sections with cryostat (Leica VT1200S). Non-specific binding was blocked by adding 5% normal goat/donkey serum during pre-incubation and incubations in 1× PBS containing 0.05% TritonX-100. The primary antibodies were applied overnight at 4°. Secondary antibodies were applied for 1–2 hr at 4° and nuclei were stained with DAPI. Slides were mounted with Prolong Gold Anti-fade Mountant (P36930, Invitrogen).

Western blotting

Mouse brain tissue of somatosensory cortex from Emx1cre/+; Grin1fl/fl mice or littermate controls at P8 were collected for western blotting. Five mouse samples for each group. The protocol we used as described before (Yabut et al., 2015).

Primers for RT-PCR

Primers for EphB2: Forward Primer-ATTATTTGCCCCAAAGTGGACTC; Reverse Primer-GCAGCGGGGTATTCTCCTTC.

RT-PCR

Mouse brain tissue of somatosensory cortex from Emx1cre/+; Grin1fl/fl mice or littermate controls at P8 were collected for RT-PCR. Six mouse samples for each group. The protocol we used as described before (Yabut et al., 2015).

Quantification and statistical analysis

Callosal axon distribution analysis

Using the segmented line tool in ImageJ, a line was drawn with a width of 200 pixels from medial S1 to lateral S1/S2 border along cortical layer II/III. Fluorescence distribution was measured along the line by using ‘Plot Profile’ under ‘Analyze’ in ImageJ and produced data sets with distance points along the line (X) and fluorescence intensity (Y). The data was exported to Excel. In Excel, fluorescence intensity values (Y) were normalized by dividing by the max Y fluorescence value for that group. Finally, results were analyzed by using XY statistics in Prism version 5.0 (GraphPad Software).

Callosal axon density analysis

Sections were imaged using a Zeiss Axioscan Z.1 (Zeiss, Thornwood, NY, USA) with 20× objective over whole brain section. Each image was made up by the compression of three slices in 4 μm Z-stack. For each brain, only one section was chosen for data quantification. The callosal axon density (fluorescence density) in S1 was quantitatively analyzed by ImageJ software. First, each picture was converted to an 8-bit image, and then Brightness/Contrast (Figure 2—figure supplement 3A) and Threshold Ranges (Figure 2—figure supplement 3B) were set. Threshold Range was set to eliminate background fluorescence from affecting fluorescent density. Second, the cortical S1 region in the target side was identified according to Dapi staining as previously described (Zhou et al., 2013) and a box was drawn to encompass only the S1 (box I in Figure 2—figure supplement 3B). Third, fluorescence density was quantified in the S1 by counting the number of pixels within the threshold range and dividing by total number of pixels in the area. This is done by selecting ‘Area Fraction’ and ‘Limit to Threshold’ in ImageJ → Analyze → Set Measurement. Finally, the axon density in S1 was normalized by the average fluorescence density of midline for each image (box II in Figure 2—figure supplement 3B). The average fluorescence density of midline was calculated by measuring the fluorescence density of six non-overlapping points around the midline and averaging the values. A fixed sized box was used for all measurements of midline fluorescence density. Since a fixed sized box was used (total number of pixels is fixed for all), ‘Area’ instead of ‘Area Fraction’ was used for analysis. Results were analyzed by using two-tailed t-test in Prism version 5.0 (GraphPad Software). Please note that control groups and experimental groups followed exactly the same settings of measurements.

Acknowledgements

We thank Prof. Roger Nicoll and Jillian Iafrati for providing Grin2afl/fl and Grin2bfl/fl mice. We thank Wucheng Tao in Prof. Roger Nicoll’s lab for suggestions on the experimental design of MK-801 in vivo injection. We thank Drs. Ariele L Greenfield, Christopher M Bartley, and Michael R Wilson for critical comments and editing of this manuscript. Work in the Pleasure lab was supported by R56 MH119435, R01MH122471, UCSF Weill Institute for Neurosciences Innovation Award, and UCSF Marcus Program in Precision Medicine Innovation Transformative Integrated Research Initiative. JOB was supported by R01DE023337 from NIH/NIDCR.

Funding Statement

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Contributor Information

Samuel J Pleasure, Email: samuel.pleasure@ucsf.edu.

Carol A Mason, Columbia University, United States.

Jonathan A Cooper, Fred Hutchinson Cancer Research Center, United States.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grants:

  • National Institute of Mental Health R01MH119435 to Jing Zhou, Yong Lin, Trung Huynh, Hirofumi Noguchi, Samuel J Pleasure.

  • National Institute of Mental Health R01MH122471 to Jing Zhou, Yong Lin, Trung Huynh, Hirofumi Noguchi, Samuel J Pleasure.

  • National Institute of Dental and Craniofacial Research R01DE023337 to Jeffrey O Bush.

  • University of California, San Francisco UCSF Weill Institute for Neurosciences Innovation Award to Jing Zhou, Yong Lin, Trung Huynh, Hirofumi Noguchi, Samuel J Pleasure.

  • University of California, San Francisco UCSF Marcus Program in Precision Medicine Innovation Transformative Integrated Research Initiative to Jing Zhou, Yong Lin, Trung Huynh, Hirofumi Noguchi, Samuel J Pleasure.

Additional information

Competing interests

No competing interests declared.

Author contributions

Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Validation, Investigation, Visualization, Methodology, Writing - original draft, Writing - review and editing.

Data curation, Formal analysis, Writing - review and editing.

Data curation, Investigation, Writing - review and editing.

Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Writing - review and editing.

Resources, Methodology, Writing - review and editing.

Conceptualization, Resources, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Writing - original draft, Project administration, Writing - review and editing.

Ethics

Animal experimentation: This study was performed in strict accordance with the recommendations in the Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals of the National Institutes of Health. All of the animals were handled according to approved institutional animal care and use committee (IACUC) protocols (AN176415) of the University of California San Francisco.

Additional files

Transparent reporting form

Data availability

All data generated or analyses during this study are included in the manuscript.

References

  1. Adelsberger H, Garaschuk O, Konnerth A. Cortical calcium waves in resting newborn mice. Nature Neuroscience. 2005;8:988–990. doi: 10.1038/nn1502. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Allène C, Cattani A, Ackman JB, Bonifazi P, Aniksztejn L, Ben-Ari Y, Cossart R. Sequential generation of two distinct synapse-driven network patterns in developing neocortex. Journal of Neuroscience. 2008;28:12851–12863. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3733-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Bush JO, Soriano P. Ephrin-B1 regulates axon guidance by reverse signaling through a PDZ-dependent mechanism. Genes & Development. 2009;23:1586–1599. doi: 10.1101/gad.1807209. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Chen BS, Roche KW. Regulation of NMDA receptors by phosphorylation. Neuropharmacology. 2007;53:362–368. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2007.05.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Collingridge GL, Isaac JT, Wang YT. Receptor trafficking and synaptic plasticity. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2004;5:952–962. doi: 10.1038/nrn1556. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Colonnese MT, Constantine-Paton M. Developmental period for N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) receptor-dependent synapse elimination correlated with visuotopic map refinement. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 2006;494:738–751. doi: 10.1002/cne.20841. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Dalva MB, Takasu MA, Lin MZ, Shamah SM, Hu L, Gale NW, Greenberg ME. EphB receptors interact with NMDA receptors and regulate excitatory synapse formation. Cell. 2000;103:945–956. doi: 10.1016/S0092-8674(00)00197-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Davis S, Gale NW, Aldrich TH, Maisonpierre PC, Lhotak V, Pawson T, Goldfarb M, Yancopoulos GD. Ligands for EPH-related receptor tyrosine kinases that require membrane attachment or clustering for activity. Science. 1994;266:816–819. doi: 10.1126/science.7973638. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. De León Reyes NS, Mederos S, Varela I, Weiss LA, Perea G, Galazo MJ, Nieto M. Transient callosal projections of L4 neurons are eliminated for the acquisition of local connectivity. Nature Communications. 2019;10:4549. doi: 10.1038/s41467-019-12495-w. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Dingledine R, Borges K, Bowie D, Traynelis SF. The glutamate receptor ion channels. Pharmacological Reviews. 1999;51:7–61. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Dore K, Aow J, Malinow R. The emergence of NMDA receptor metabotropic function: insights from imaging. Frontiers in Synaptic Neuroscience. 2016;8:20. doi: 10.3389/fnsyn.2016.00020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Dupuis JP, Ladépêche L, Seth H, Bard L, Varela J, Mikasova L, Bouchet D, Rogemond V, Honnorat J, Hanse E, Groc L. Surface dynamics of GluN2B-NMDA receptors controls plasticity of maturing glutamate synapses. The EMBO Journal. 2014;33:842–861. doi: 10.1002/embj.201386356. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Facchinetti F, Ciani E, Dall'Olio R, Virgili M, Contestabile A, Fonnum F. Structural, neurochemical and behavioural consequences of neonatal blockade of NMDA receptor through chronic treatment with CGP 39551 or MK-801. Developmental Brain Research. 1993;74:219–224. doi: 10.1016/0165-3806(93)90007-W. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Fenlon LR, Suárez R, Richards LJ. The anatomy, organisation and development of contralateral callosal projections of the mouse somatosensory cortex. Brain and Neuroscience Advances. 2017;1:239821281769488. doi: 10.1177/2398212817694888. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Florance NR, Davis RL, Lam C, Szperka C, Zhou L, Ahmad S, Campen CJ, Moss H, Peter N, Gleichman AJ, Glaser CA, Lynch DR, Rosenfeld MR, Dalmau J. Anti-N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor (NMDAR) encephalitis in children and adolescents. Annals of Neurology. 2009;66:11–18. doi: 10.1002/ana.21756. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Forrest D, Yuzaki M, Soares HD, Ng L, Luk DC, Sheng M, Stewart CL, Morgan JI, Connor JA, Curran T. Targeted disruption of NMDA receptor 1 gene abolishes NMDA response and results in neonatal death. Neuron. 1994;13:325–338. doi: 10.1016/0896-6273(94)90350-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Foster AC, Gill R, Woodruff GN. Neuroprotective effects of MK-801 in vivo: selectivity and evidence for delayed degeneration mediated by NMDA receptor activation. The Journal of Neuroscience. 1988;8:4745–4754. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.08-12-04745.1988. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Golshani P, Gonçalves JT, Khoshkhoo S, Mostany R, Smirnakis S, Portera-Cailliau C. Internally mediated developmental desynchronization of neocortical network activity. Journal of Neuroscience. 2009;29:10890–10899. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2012-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Gray JA, Shi Y, Usui H, During MJ, Sakimura K, Nicoll RA. Distinct modes of AMPA receptor suppression at developing synapses by GluN2A and GluN2B: single-cell NMDA receptor subunit deletion in vivo. Neuron. 2011;71:1085–1101. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.08.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Griesbach GS, Amsel A. Immediate and long-term effects of neonatal MK-801 treatment on nonspatial learning. PNAS. 1998;95:11435–11439. doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.19.11435. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Hahm JO, Cramer KS, Sur M. Pattern formation by retinal afferents in the ferret lateral geniculate nucleus: developmental segregation and the role of N-methyl-D-aspartate receptors. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 1999;411:327–345. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1096-9861(19990823)411:2&#x0003c;327::AID-CNE12&#x0003e;3.0.CO;2-#. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Hand RA, Khalid S, Tam E, Kolodkin AL. Axon dynamics during neocortical laminar innervation. Cell Reports. 2015;12:172–182. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2015.06.026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Henkemeyer M, Itkis OS, Ngo M, Hickmott PW, Ethell IM. Multiple EphB receptor tyrosine kinases shape dendritic spines in the Hippocampus. Journal of Cell Biology. 2003;163:1313–1326. doi: 10.1083/jcb.200306033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Henson MA, Tucker CJ, Zhao M, Dudek SM. Long-term depression-associated signaling is required for an in vitro model of NMDA receptor-dependent synapse pruning. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory. 2017;138:39–53. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2016.10.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Huang Y, Song NN, Lan W, Zhang Q, Zhang L, Zhang L, Hu L, Chen JY, Zhao CJ, Li L, Xu L, Ding YQ. Sensory input is required for callosal axon targeting in the somatosensory cortex. Molecular Brain. 2013;6:53. doi: 10.1186/1756-6606-6-53. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Huettner JE, Bean BP. Block of N-methyl-D-aspartate-activated current by the anticonvulsant MK-801: selective binding to open channels. PNAS. 1988;85:1307–1311. doi: 10.1073/pnas.85.4.1307. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Innocenti GM. The target of exuberant projections in development. Cerebral Cortex. 2020;30:3820–3826. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhz344. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Innocenti GM, Price DJ. Exuberance in the development of cortical networks. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2005;6:955–965. doi: 10.1038/nrn1790. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Iwasato T, Datwani A, Wolf AM, Nishiyama H, Taguchi Y, Tonegawa S, Knöpfel T, Erzurumlu RS, Itohara S. Cortex-restricted disruption of NMDAR1 impairs neuronal patterns in the barrel cortex. Nature. 2000;406:726–731. doi: 10.1038/35021059. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Kania A, Klein R. Mechanisms of ephrin–Eph signalling in development, physiology and disease. Nature Reviews Molecular Cell Biology. 2016;17:240–256. doi: 10.1038/nrm.2015.16. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Káradóttir R, Cavelier P, Bergersen LH, Attwell D. NMDA receptors are expressed in oligodendrocytes and activated in ischaemia. Nature. 2005;438:1162–1166. doi: 10.1038/nature04302. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Kessels HW, Nabavi S, Malinow R. Metabotropic NMDA receptor function is required for β-amyloid-induced synaptic depression. PNAS. 2013;110:4033–4038. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1219605110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Khazipov R, Sirota A, Leinekugel X, Holmes GL, Ben-Ari Y, Buzsáki G. Early motor activity drives spindle bursts in the developing somatosensory cortex. Nature. 2004;432:758–761. doi: 10.1038/nature03132. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Khazipov R, Luhmann HJ. Early patterns of electrical activity in the developing cerebral cortex of humans and rodents. Trends in Neurosciences. 2006;29:414–418. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2006.05.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Korotkova T, Fuchs EC, Ponomarenko A, von Engelhardt J, Monyer H. NMDA receptor ablation on parvalbumin-positive interneurons impairs hippocampal synchrony, spatial representations, and working memory. Neuron. 2010;68:557–569. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.09.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Kutsuwada T, Kashiwabuchi N, Mori H, Sakimura K, Kushiya E, Araki K, Meguro H, Masaki H, Kumanishi T, Arakawa M. Molecular diversity of the NMDA receptor channel. Nature. 1992;358:36–41. doi: 10.1038/358036a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Lau CG, Zukin RS. NMDA receptor trafficking in synaptic plasticity and neuropsychiatric disorders. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2007;8:413–426. doi: 10.1038/nrn2153. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Liu XB, Murray KD, Jones EG. Switching of NMDA receptor 2A and 2B subunits at thalamic and cortical synapses during early postnatal development. Journal of Neuroscience. 2004;24:8885–8895. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2476-04.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Lo FS, Akkentli F, Tsytsarev V, Erzurumlu RS. Functional significance of cortical NMDA receptors in somatosensory information processing. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2013;110:2627–2636. doi: 10.1152/jn.00052.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Loftis JM, Janowsky A. The N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor subunit NR2B: localization, functional properties, regulation, and clinical implications. Pharmacology & Therapeutics. 2003;97:55–85. doi: 10.1016/S0163-7258(02)00302-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Luo L, O'Leary DD. Axon retraction and degeneration in development and disease. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 2005;28:127–156. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.28.061604.135632. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Madisen L, Zwingman TA, Sunkin SM, Oh SW, Zariwala HA, Gu H, Ng LL, Palmiter RD, Hawrylycz MJ, Jones AR, Lein ES, Zeng H. A robust and high-throughput cre reporting and characterization system for the whole mouse brain. Nature Neuroscience. 2010;13:133–140. doi: 10.1038/nn.2467. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Milton AL, Lee JL, Butler VJ, Gardner R, Everitt BJ. Intra-amygdala and systemic antagonism of NMDA receptors prevents the reconsolidation of drug-associated memory and impairs subsequently both novel and previously acquired drug-seeking behaviors. Journal of Neuroscience. 2008;28:8230–8237. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1723-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Mitrovic N, Mohajeri H, Schachner M. Effects of NMDA receptor blockade in the developing rat somatosensory cortex on the expression of the glia-derived extracellular matrix glycoprotein tenascin-C. European Journal of Neuroscience. 1996;8:1793–1802. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.1996.tb01323.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Mizuno H, Hirano T, Tagawa Y. Evidence for activity-dependent cortical wiring: formation of interhemispheric connections in neonatal mouse visual cortex requires projection neuron activity. Journal of Neuroscience. 2007;27:6760–6770. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1215-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Monyer H, Burnashev N, Laurie DJ, Sakmann B, Seeburg PH. Developmental and regional expression in the rat brain and functional properties of four NMDA receptors. Neuron. 1994;12:529–540. doi: 10.1016/0896-6273(94)90210-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Morris RG. Synaptic plasticity and learning: selective impairment of learning rats and blockade of long-term potentiation in vivo by the N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor antagonist AP5. The Journal of Neuroscience. 1989;9:3040–3057. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.09-09-03040.1989. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Mukherjee B, Yuan Q. NMDA receptors in mouse anterior piriform cortex initialize early odor preference learning and L-type calcium channels engage for long-term memory. Scientific Reports. 2016;6:35256. doi: 10.1038/srep35256. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Nabavi S, Kessels HW, Alfonso S, Aow J, Fox R, Malinow R. Metabotropic NMDA receptor function is required for NMDA receptor-dependent long-term depression. PNAS. 2013;110:4027–4032. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1219454110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Niethamer TK, Bush JO. Getting direction(s): The Eph/ephrin signaling system in cell positioning. Developmental Biology. 2019;447:42–57. doi: 10.1016/j.ydbio.2018.01.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Nilsson M, Carlsson A, Carlsson ML. Glycine and D-serine decrease MK-801-induced hyperactivity in mice. Journal of Neural Transmission. 1997;104:1195–1205. doi: 10.1007/BF01294720. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Nolt MJ, Lin Y, Hruska M, Murphy J, Sheffler-Colins SI, Kayser MS, Passer J, Bennett MV, Zukin RS, Dalva MB. EphB controls NMDA receptor function and synaptic targeting in a subunit-specific manner. Journal of Neuroscience. 2011;31:5353–5364. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0282-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Orioli D, Henkemeyer M, Lemke G, Klein R, Pawson T. Sek4 and nuk receptors cooperate in guidance of commissural axons and in palate formation. The EMBO Journal. 1996;15:6035–6049. doi: 10.1002/j.1460-2075.1996.tb00992.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Palmer A, Klein R. Multiple roles of ephrins in Morphogenesis, neuronal networking, and brain function. Genes & Development. 2003;17:1429–1450. doi: 10.1101/gad.1093703. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Paoletti P, Bellone C, Zhou Q. NMDA receptor subunit diversity: impact on receptor properties, synaptic plasticity and disease. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2013;14:383–400. doi: 10.1038/nrn3504. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Paul LK. Developmental malformation of the corpus callosum: a review of typical callosal development and examples of developmental disorders with callosal involvement. Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders. 2011;3:3–27. doi: 10.1007/s11689-010-9059-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Personius KE, Slusher BS, Udin SB. Neuromuscular NMDA receptors modulate developmental synapse elimination. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2016;36:8783–8789. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1181-16.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Rabacchi S, Bailly Y, Delhaye-Bouchaud N, Mariani J. Involvement of the N-methyl D-aspartate (NMDA) receptor in synapse elimination during cerebellar development. Science. 1992;256:1823–1825. doi: 10.1126/science.1352066. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Rajan I, Witte S, Cline HT. NMDA receptor activity stabilizes presynaptic retinotectal axons and postsynaptic optic tectal cell dendrites in vivo. Journal of Neurobiology. 1999;38:357–368. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-4695(19990215)38:3&#x0003c;357::AID-NEU5&#x0003e;3.0.CO;2-#. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Ribeiro Gomes AR, Olivier E, Killackey HP, Giroud P, Berland M, Knoblauch K, Dehay C, Kennedy H. Refinement of the primate corticospinal pathway during prenatal development. Cerebral Cortex. 2020;30:656–671. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhz116. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Robichaux MA, Chenaux G, Ho HY, Soskis MJ, Greenberg ME, Henkemeyer M, Cowan CW. EphB1 and EphB2 intracellular domains regulate the formation of the corpus callosum and anterior commissure. Developmental Neurobiology. 2016;76:405–420. doi: 10.1002/dneu.22323. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Rodríguez-Tornos FM, Briz CG, Weiss LA, Sebastián-Serrano A, Ares S, Navarrete M, Frangeul L, Galazo M, Jabaudon D, Esteban JA, Nieto M. Cux1 enables interhemispheric connections of layer II/III neurons by regulating Kv1-Dependent firing. Neuron. 2016;89:494–506. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.12.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Ruthazer ES, Akerman CJ, Cline HT. Control of axon branch dynamics by correlated activity in vivo. Science. 2003;301:66–70. doi: 10.1126/science.1082545. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Sans NA, Montcouquiol ME, Raymond J. Postnatal developmental changes in AMPA and NMDA receptors in the rat vestibular nuclei. Developmental Brain Research. 2000;123:41–52. doi: 10.1016/S0165-3806(00)00082-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Sheffler-Collins SI, Dalva MB. EphBs: an integral link between synaptic function and synaptopathies. Trends in Neurosciences. 2012;35:293–304. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2012.03.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Sheng M, Cummings J, Roldan LA, Jan YN, Jan LY. Changing subunit composition of heteromeric NMDA receptors during development of rat cortex. Nature. 1994;368:144–147. doi: 10.1038/368144a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Suárez R, Fenlon LR, Marek R, Avitan L, Sah P, Goodhill GJ, Richards LJ. Balanced interhemispheric cortical activity is required for correct targeting of the corpus callosum. Neuron. 2014;82:1289–1298. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.04.040. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Takasu MA, Dalva MB, Zigmond RE, Greenberg ME. Modulation of NMDA receptor-dependent calcium influx and gene expression through EphB receptors. Science. 2002;295:491–495. doi: 10.1126/science.1065983. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Traynelis SF, Wollmuth LP, McBain CJ, Menniti FS, Vance KM, Ogden KK, Hansen KB, Yuan H, Myers SJ, Dingledine R. Glutamate receptor ion channels: structure, regulation, and function. Pharmacological Reviews. 2010;62:405–496. doi: 10.1124/pr.109.002451. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Uttl L, Petrasek T, Sengul H, Svojanovska M, Lobellova V, Vales K, Radostova D, Tsenov G, Kubova H, Mikulecka A, Svoboda J, Stuchlik A. Chronic MK-801 application in adolescence and early adulthood: a spatial working memory deficit in adult Long-Evans rats but no changes in the hippocampal NMDA receptor subunits. Frontiers in Pharmacology. 2018;9:42. doi: 10.3389/fphar.2018.00042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Vearing CJ, Lackmann M. "Eph receptor signalling; dimerisation just isn't enough". Growth Factors. 2005;23:67–76. doi: 10.1080/08977190500055869. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Vissel B, Krupp JJ, Heinemann SF, Westbrook GL. A use-dependent tyrosine dephosphorylation of NMDA receptors is independent of ion flux. Nature Neuroscience. 2001;4:587–596. doi: 10.1038/88404. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Vogt D, Wu P-R, Sorrells SF, Arnold C, Alvarez-Buylla A, Rubenstein JLR. Viral-mediated labeling and transplantation of medial ganglionic eminence (MGE) Cells for in vivo studies. Journal of Visualized Experiments. 2015;98:52740. doi: 10.3791/52740. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Wang CL, Zhang L, Zhou Y, Zhou J, Yang XJ, Duan SM, Xiong ZQ, Ding YQ. Activity-dependent development of callosal projections in the somatosensory cortex. Journal of Neuroscience. 2007;27:11334–11342. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3380-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Weilinger NL, Lohman AW, Rakai BD, Ma EM, Bialecki J, Maslieieva V, Rilea T, Bandet MV, Ikuta NT, Scott L, Colicos MA, Teskey GC, Winship IR, Thompson RJ. Metabotropic NMDA receptor signaling couples src family kinases to pannexin-1 during excitotoxicity. Nature Neuroscience. 2016;19:432–442. doi: 10.1038/nn.4236. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Wu J, Zou H, Strong JA, Yu J, Zhou X, Xie Q, Zhao G, Jin M, Yu L. Bimodal effects of MK-801 on locomotion and stereotypy in C57BL/6 mice. Psychopharmacology. 2005;177:256–263. doi: 10.1007/s00213-004-1944-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Xu NJ, Henkemeyer M. Ephrin-B3 reverse signaling through Grb4 and cytoskeletal regulators mediates axon pruning. Nature Neuroscience. 2009;12:268–276. doi: 10.1038/nn.2254. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Yabut OR, Fernandez G, Huynh T, Yoon K, Pleasure SJ. Suppressor of fused is critical for maintenance of neuronal progenitor identity during corticogenesis. Cell Reports. 2015;12:2021–2034. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2015.08.031. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Yang JW, Hanganu-Opatz IL, Sun JJ, Luhmann HJ. Three patterns of oscillatory activity differentially synchronize developing neocortical networks in vivo. Journal of Neuroscience. 2009;29:9011–9025. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5646-08.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Zhang ZW, Peterson M, Liu H. Essential role of postsynaptic NMDA receptors in developmental refinement of excitatory synapses. PNAS. 2013;110:1095–1100. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1212971110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Zhou J, Wen Y, She L, Sui YN, Liu L, Richards LJ, Poo MM. Axon position within the corpus callosum determines contralateral cortical projection. PNAS. 2013;110:E2714–E2723. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1310233110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Zuo DY, Zhang YH, Cao Y, Wu CF, Tanaka M, Wu YL. Effect of acute and chronic MK-801 administration on extracellular glutamate and ascorbic acid release in the prefrontal cortex of freely moving mice on line with open-field behavior. Life Sciences. 2006;78:2172–2178. doi: 10.1016/j.lfs.2005.09.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision letter

Editor: Carol A Mason1

In the interests of transparency, eLife publishes the most substantive revision requests and the accompanying author responses.

Acceptance summary:

Your paper highlights a novel mechanism for the development of callosal projections from primary somatosensory cortex (S1), through demonstrating that the normal termination pattern of callosal projections are disrupted in cortex-specific NMDAR mutants. Rather than projecting selectively to the primary/secondary somatosensory cortex (S1/S2) border, axons are uniformly distributed throughout S1, with this pattern intensifying until a premature death. You also suggest that GluN2B-containing NMDA receptors mediate this phenotype during callosal innervation of somatosensory cortex and is independent of ion channel function. Finally, you propose that NMDAR functions with the ephrinB/EphB system of guidance molecules.

Decision letter after peer review:

Thank you for submitting your article "NMDA receptors control cortical axonal projections via EPHRIN-B/EPHB signaling" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by three peer reviewers, and the evaluation has been overseen by a Reviewing Editor and Jonathan Cooper as the Senior Editor. The reviewers have opted to remain anonymous.

The reviewers have discussed the reviews with one another and the Reviewing Editor has drafted this decision to help you prepare a revised submission.

We would like to draw your attention to changes in our revision policy that we have made in response to COVID-19 (https://elifesciences.org/articles/57162). Specifically, when editors judge that a submitted work as a whole belongs in eLife but that some conclusions require a modest amount of additional new data, as they do with your paper, we are asking that the manuscript be revised to either limit claims to those supported by data in hand, or to explicitly state that the relevant conclusions require additional supporting data.

Our expectation is that the authors will eventually carry out the additional experiments and report on how they affect the relevant conclusions either in a preprint on bioRxiv or medRxiv, or if appropriate, as a Research Advance in eLife, either of which would be linked to the original paper.

Summary:

Your study points to a novel mechanism for the development of callosal projections from primary somatosensory cortex (S1), namely, that NR2B-containing NMDA receptors are required to mediate the normal termination pattern of callosal projections. You also show that NR2B-containing NMDAR during callosal innervation of somatosensory cortex is independent of ion channel function in these receptors. An additional, novel slant is your proposal that NMDARs act as accessory axon guidance molecules via Eph/ephrin repulsion. You pose a model where these well-studied axon guidance molecules interact directly with the NMDAR, to coordinate the formation of axonal projections, again, indicating non-ionotrophic functions of the NMDAR.

From the appended reviews, you will see that the reviewers thought that the claims in the manuscript for the NR2B-containing NMDAR function during callosal innervation are well supported. However, all three reviewers questioned the link between the ephrin/Eph system and NMDAR function, and considered the analyses related to this link preliminary. They thought that this aspect should be strengthened, either through additional experiments (in vitro tests of repellent activity) or by toning down the discussion and potentially removing the link to ephrinB/EphB signaling from the title and Abstract. While it may not be possible to do the additional suggested experiments since lab shutdown and restarting, the revisions on data presentation and textually in the discussion are feasible and could be done in the coming months.

Revisions for this paper:

For the glutamate receptor data:

1) In the GluN2A and GluN2B floxed mice: at the ages studied, it is unclear how much GluN2A is expressed.

2) With MK801 and APV application to block the NMDAR, how effective are these drugs at blocking the NMDAR and what is the area of effect of the drugs? Are other activity-dependent events blocked in these animals (e.g., LGN lamination, whisker barrelette formation in PrV)?

The drugs blocking NMDAR activity lack sufficient controls for the conclusions stated in the manuscript: Loss of the innervation pattern of innervation is variable and the S1/S2 column indicates a possible dependency on activity, leading to other possible interpretations. Changes in the overall cortical activity may affect the stabilization of callosal projections, and therefore acute pharmacological blocking of NMDAR may not mimic the KO or antibody treatments. Early and permanent loss of NMDAR activity can trigger changes in the intrinsic excitability of the neurons that could override the phenotypes of increased innervation. This could be discussed textually

For the Eph/ephrin data:

3) You argue EphB2 protein levels are reduced in the cortex of conditional NR1 floxed mice and disruption of ephrin-B/EphB signaling in this system leads to similar termination defects as deleting NR2B. This is an apt argument but the reviewers felt that this was only a first step in the required analyses.

Reviewer #3:

4) Figure 8. By Western blot analysis, the reduction seems 30% compared to controls. This is not dramatic and instead suggests other mechanisms at work, since most heterozygous mutants (with target protein reductions at 50% or more) do not show phenotypes.

5) Partial reduction of EphB2 protein raises the possibility that further reductions of EphB2 signaling may enhance the phenotype. Instead, blocking NMDAR plus EphB2 functions (by injecting NR1Ab and ephrin-B1-Fc) did not enhance the phenotype of the single manipulations. One would expect additive effects.

6) Figure 8, only the timepoint P6 is shown. Rev 5 also points out that between P6 and P10 there is more variability in contralateral innervation correlating with the anterior-posterior axis. You should show ephrin B electroporations analyzed at P14-P30, or remove and/or tone down the text.

Reviewer #4:

7) EphB-ephrin-B signaling and NMDAR function likely has to do with EphB-ephrin-B repulsive cues or that EphBs interact and modulate NMDAR function, and regulate excitatory synapse development in forebrain. It seems at least equally likely that in the latter case, EphB modulation of NMDAR may mediate the events described in this manuscript, on synapse development and maintenance, not necessarily on repulsion/guidance.

8) EphB-NMDAR interaction results in phosphorylation of GluN2B, increased synaptic localization of the NMDAR, and increased NMDAR dependent Ca influx. Loss of EphB-ephrin-B signaling might cause a reduction in NMDAR Ca influx, but not a complete block of ionotropic signaling. To demonstrate that the findings in Figures 7 and 8 are linked, the authors should inject NMDAR blockers (after conducting the control experiments in point 2), in the ephrin-B1 knockout mice. The anti-NMDAR antibodies simply remove the receptor from the synapse. This does not address whether the effects are independent of the ionotropic modulation of the channel and should be discussed.

Revisions expected in follow-up work:

1) In implicating repellent activity that would restrict commissural axonal projections to the S1 cortex, the evidence for NMDAR via Eph B2 function as repellents for elongating callosal axons is lacking. in vitro experiments demonstrating repellent activity of EphB downstream of NMDAR would be necessary to make this claim. Given the time required to develop and execute such an assay, and the pandemic delays, you could take as long as needed to do this experiment, if you choose to do so.

2) Reviewers 4 and 5 point out that these pathways could be involved in refinement and pruning of axons: Reviewer 5 raises the possibility that in the mutants, axons are stabilized, to result in extra numbers of callosal neurons. NMDAR could thus be responsible for repulsion during refinement. Reviewer 4's comments on EphBs interacting with excitatory synapse development could play into this explanation. More high-resolution analysis of the impact of your manipulations on axonal branching would address the other reviewers' questions regarding whether the changes see are due defects in branching and/or refinement. This aspect as in the in vitro experiments require additional experiments which could understandably not be possible at this time.

[Editors' note: further revisions were suggested prior to acceptance, as described below.]

Thank you for submitting your article "NMDA receptors control development of somatosensory callosal axonal projections" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by three peer reviewers, and the evaluation has been overseen by a Reviewing Editor and Jonathan Cooper as the Senior Editor. The reviewers have opted to remain anonymous.

The reviewers have discussed the reviews with one another and the Reviewing Editor has drafted this decision to help you prepare a revised submission.

We would like to draw your attention to changes in our revision policy that we have made in response to COVID-19 (https://elifesciences.org/articles/57162). Specifically, we are asking editors to accept without delay manuscripts, like yours, that they judge can stand as eLife papers without additional data, even if they feel that they would make the manuscript stronger. Thus the revisions requested below only address clarity and presentation.

Summary:

Your revision highlights a novel mechanism for the development of callosal projections from primary somatosensory cortex (S1), through demonstrating that the normal termination pattern of callosal projections are disrupted in cortex-specific NMDAR mutants. Rather than projecting selectively to the primary/secondary somatosensory cortex (S1/S2) border, axons are uniformly distributed throughout S1, with this pattern intensifying until a premature death. You also suggest that GluN2B-containing NMDA receptors mediate this phenotype during callosal innervation of somatosensory cortex and is independent of ion channel function. Finally, you propose that NMDAR functions with the ephrinB/EphB system of guidance molecules.

Due to constraints put in place by COVID, in many cases you have had to reduce the strength of your claims rather than conduct new experiments, and you have satisfactorily downplayed the link between the ephrin/Eph system and NMDAR function. While you were not able to directly show that the NMDAR blockers used were effective at blocking NMDAR channel function, the demonstration of a potential link between activity-independent and activity-dependent signaling systems to impact axon development is considered exciting and novel.

The previous review stated that you could look to performing follow-up work in the future on: (a). in vitro experiments demonstrating repellent activity of EphB downstream of NMDAR to fortify the link between EphB and NMDAR expression levels. (b). experiments to show the effects of inhibiting Eph/ephrin signaling at later time points, especially on examining refinement and pruning of axons. We hope you will be able to pursue these interesting studies when conditions improve.

Revisions:

The rebuttal letter was difficult to follow. It was not clear where in the manuscript some of the changes were made. Figures were not labelled and main figures were mixed with supplementary figures. However, the following points appear not to have been addressed adequately:

1) In response to the criticism of overstating the 30% reduction in EphB2 protein expression in the Grin1 CKOs, the authors have provided images showing EphB2 immunofluorescence in Emx1Cre;Grin1fl/fl;tdTomato mice (Figure 8—figure supplement 2). While the pictures in this new figure suggest loss of EphB2 IF in Tom+;Grin1 cKO cells, but not in Tom+;control cells, the results should be quantified. Presenting images without quantification is not convincing. Please quantify and add the results to the paper.

2) Please address the following points that were raised in the previous critique and appear not to have been answered:

a) show images of all controls and experimental groups of Figure 3 in Supplementary data.

b) Provide better evidence for NR1 protein downregulation in the conditional NR1 knockouts (Figure 1—figure supplement 1).

eLife. 2021 Mar 4;10:e59612. doi: 10.7554/eLife.59612.sa2

Author response


Revisions for this paper:

For the glutamate receptor data:

1) In the GluN2A and GluN2B floxed mice: at the ages studied, it is unclear how much GluN2A is expressed.

We have performed western blot of S1 at P8 and found GluN2A is expressed at P8. We have added “To address previous studies that found that GluN2A was expressed at lower levels during development we checked the expression of GluN2A in S1 at P8, and found that it was dramatically diminished in Emx1cre/+; Grin2Afl/fl mice (Figure 6—figure supplement 2A, 2B). While the expression of GluN2B was the same in control and Emx1cre/+; Grin2Afl/fl mice (Figure 6—figure supplement 2A, 2C), and similarly GluN2A expression was unchanged in Emx1cre/+; Grin2Bfl/fl mice (Data not shown)” to the text when mentioned about “GluN2B, but not GluN2A, is required for callosal axon targeting”.

2) With MK801 and APV application to block the NMDAR, how effective are these drugs at blocking the NMDAR and what is the area of effect of the drugs? Are other activity-dependent events blocked in these animals (e.g., LGN lamination, whisker barrelette formation in PrV)?

We agree with these points raised by the reviewer. These were the exactly concerns we had when we began the experiments. We consulted local electrophysiology labs who suggested we use (+)-MK-801 hydrogen maleate and D(-)-2-amino-5-phosphonopentanoic acid (D-APV) which are most the effective isoforms for in vivo studies. We also carefully examined the previous literature to determine the dosage range for chronic treatment studies and tested on our own to find the optimal dosage (see the details in Materials and methods). Based on the literature, weight loss and abnormal behavior have been reported in neonatal mice after MK-801 administration (Facchinetti et al., 1993; Griesbach and Amsel, 1998; Wu et al., 2005) which can be used as a sign of effective MK-801 administration. We also found weight loss and abnormal behaviors after systemic i.p. MK-801 administration in mouse pups. For local D-APV injection, we did not see weight loss for pups but did see abnormal behaviors after injection.

We searched the original literature to address the question “Are other activity-dependent events blocked in these animals (e.g., LGN lamination, whisker barrelette formation in PrV)?” For the LGN, we calculated that the dose used for ferret pups was 0.4mg/kg MK-801 daily. Our dose was 1mg/kg MK-801 daily which is significantly higher than the published dose. It further suggested our dose is effective in mouse pups. So we add this sentence of “Previous studies showed that blocking NMDAR with a daily dose of 0.4mg/kg MK-801 in ferret pups between P14 and P21 disrupted axonal pattern formation by retinal afferents in the lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN) (Hahm et al., 1999)” in the text when mentioned about “NMDAR regulates callosal circuit development independent of NMDAR channel activity”. For PrV (Mitrovic et al., 1996), they used silicone carriers to locally apply MK-801 and D-APV, it was not clear how much MK-801 and APPV were delivered to the targeted tissue.

Of course, the most direct way to test whether our MK-801 or D-APV in vivo administration blocked or reduced Ca2+ influx of NMDAR, is doing calcium imaging or electrical recording on brain slides after acute and chronic MK-801 or D-APV administration. However, during the pandemic of COVID-19, it is difficult to extend our studies to perform this experiment currently, however, we believe the literature is fairly clear that our treatments are surely affecting NMDAR function profoundly. We do plan to pursue experiments like this in the future.

The drugs blocking NMDAR activity lack sufficient controls for the conclusions stated in the manuscript: Loss of the innervation pattern of innervation is variable and the S1/S2 column indicates a possible dependency on activity, leading to other possible interpretations. Changes in the overall cortical activity may affect the stabilization of callosal projections, and therefore acute pharmacological blocking of NMDAR may not mimic the KO or antibody treatments. Early and permanent loss of NMDAR activity can trigger changes in the intrinsic excitability of the neurons that could override the phenotypes of increased innervation. This could be discussed textually

We agree and appreciate the valuable advice. In the newly edited manuscript, we disused these possibilities in our Discussion “Previous studies have observed the role of NMDAR in axon elimination (Colonnese and Constantine-Paton, 2006; Henson et al., 2017; Personius et al., 2016; Rabacchi et al., 1992; Rajan et al., 1999; Zhang et al., 2013). in vivo time-lapse images of retinal axons from albino Xenopus tadpoles show that correlated neural activity influences axon branch dynamics of retinal axons in the optic tectum (Ruthazer et al., 2003). The axon branches whose activity is not correlated with that of their neighbors are selectively eliminated. However, the selective elimination can be abolished by blocking the Ca2+ influx of NMDAR by its antagonist MK-801. When we blocked the Ca2+ influx of NMDAR by MK-801 or D-APV, even though the overall callosal innervation pattern of somatosensory cortex was not affected, there were some subtle, local laminar-specific innervation differences at the S1/S2 border, suggesting that the refinement of axon arborization may rely on the Ca2+ influx-mediated activity changes through NMDAR” when mentioned about “The refinement of callosal innervation pattern”.

For the Eph/ephrin data:

3) You argue EphB2 protein levels are reduced in the cortex of conditional NR1 floxed mice and disruption of ephrin-B/EphB signaling in this system leads to similar termination defects as deleting NR2B. This is an apt argument but the reviewers felt that this was only a first step in the required analyses.

We have toned down the linkage between NMDAR and EPHRINB/EPHB in the Abstract, the Results and the Discussion. We also removed the linkage between NMDAR and EPHRINB/EPHB in the title. We will examine how and whether NMDAR regulates EPHRIN-B/EPHB signaling directly and the detailed molecular mechanism underlying their collaboration to regulate callosal innervation patterns in the primary somatosensory cortex in a following project.

Reviewer #3:

4) Figure 8. By Western blot analysis, the reduction seems 30% compared to controls. This is not dramatic and instead suggests other mechanisms at work, since most heterozygous mutants (with target protein reductions at 50% or more) do not show phenotypes.

5) Partial reduction of EphB2 protein raises the possibility that further reductions of EphB2 signaling may enhance the phenotype. Instead, blocking NMDAR plus EphB2 functions (by injecting NR1Ab and ephrin-B1-Fc) did not enhance the phenotype of the single manipulations. One would expect additive effects.

Questions 4 and 5 are interlinked, so we are addressing them together. We understand the reviewer’s concern that a 30% reduction of EPHB2 in S1 of Emx1cre/+; Grin1fl/fl mice by western blot was not dramatic. However, implicit in our studies was our assumption that the loss of EPHB2 would be selective for neurons that lost NMDAR and that this was a subset of cortical neurons. Also, we assumed that EPHB2 expressed in other compartments than the PSD might not be decreased. Thus, we believe that a 30% decrease is actually quite dramatic if the loss is limited to the PSD. We appreciate the reviewer asking us to more directly address this! Emx1cre/+; Grin1fl/fl mice only knocked out GluN1 in Emx1cre/+ positive cells which were only a subpopulation cells in the cortex (excitatory neurons with Cre penetrance). We found that there are still many cells expressiong GluN1 in Emx1cre/+ unaffected cell populations, such as interneurons (Figure 1—figure supplement 1). Also, to more specifically examine the expression of EPHB2 on Emx1cre/+; Grin1fl/fl positive cells, we crossed Emx1cre/+; Grin1fl/fl mice with Cre reporter mice-Rosa26<fs-tdTomatofl/fl mice to generate Emx1cre/+; Grin1fl/fl; Rosa26<fs-tdTomatofl/flmice. We found the expression of EPHB2 was completely gone from somatic post-synaptic densities on Emx1cre/+; Grin1fl/fl; Rosa26<fs-tdTomatofl/flpositive cells. Thus, we believe that a loss of 30% of total EPHB2 protein in the cortex is actually quite dramatic. Importantly, this can’t be compared to EPHB2+/- mice where presumably any lost protein would be distributed throughout all domains and in all cells where the protein is expressed.

We also add the following paragraph into the newly edited text “As discussed above, in Emx1cre/+; Grin1fl/fl mice, GluN1 was selectively deleted in excitatory neurons. Based on previous studies, the expression of GluN1 in interneurons (Korotkova et al., 2010), oligodendrocytes and oligodendrocyte precursor cells (Karadottir et al., 2005) in the cortex should not be affected in Emx1cre/+; Grin1fl/flmice. To examine the expression of EPHB2 specifically in GluN1 KO cells, we crossed Emx1cre/+; Grin1fl/flmice with Cre-reporter mice – Rosa26<fs-tdTomatofl/flmice that exhibit tdTomato expression after Cre-mediated recombination. Supporting our hypothesis that loss of GluN1 leads to loss of EPHB2, the punctate staining of EPHB2 on cell membranes was completely lost in red (recombined) cells of Emx1cre/+; Grin1fl/fl; Rosa26<fs-tdTomatofl/fl brain sections but not red cells of Emx1cre/+; Grin1wt/wt;Rosa26<fs-tdTomatofl/fl brain sections (Figure 8—figure supplement 2). Taken together, we demonstrated that the loss of NMDAR caused the loss of EPHB2 selectively on cells that lack GluN1 after excision, thus explaining the 30% reduction in EPHB2 protein expression. Given the known physical association between NMDAR and EPHB2, these data suggest reciprocity in this stabilizing interaction and dendritic localization” when mentioned about “EPHB2 protein expression is decreased in GluN1 KO mice”.

6) Figure 8, only the timepoint P6 is shown. Rev 5 also points out that between P6 and P10 there is more variability in contralateral innervation correlating with the anterior-posterior axis. You should show ephrin B electroporations analyzed at P14-P30, or remove and/or tone down the text.

We have toned down the discussion of the interaction between NMDAR and EPHRINB/EPHB.

Reviewer #4:

7) EphB-ephrin-B signaling and NMDAR function likely has to do with EphB-ephrin-B repulsive cues or that EphBs interact and modulate NMDAR function, and regulate excitatory synapse development in forebrain. It seems at least equally likely that in the latter case, EphB modulation of NMDAR may mediate the events described in this manuscript, on synapse development and maintenance, not necessarily on repulsion/guidance.

We agree and thank the reviewer for these suggestions. We have discussed these alternative possibilities in the newly edited manuscript. We have added the following paragraph into our Discussion “Regarding previous roles of EPHRIN-B/EPHB signaling in pruning, this pathway was previous shown to be involved in infrapyramidal bundle pruning in dentate gyrus (Xu and Henkemeyer, 2009) and it is know that activation of EPHB by EPHRIN-B potentiates Ca2+ influx of NMDA receptor (Takasu et al., 2002). These lines of evidence suggest that NMDAR and EPHRIN-B/EPHB signaling may cooperate in the refinement of the callosal innervation pattern and perhaps in other circuits. Further studies will examine how and whether NMDAR regulates EPHRIN-B/EPHB signaling directly and the detailed molecular mechanism underlying their collaboration to regulate callosal innervation patterns in the primary somatosensory cortex” when discussed “the refinement of callosal innervation pattern”.

8) EphB-NMDAR interaction results in phosphorylation of GluN2B, increased synaptic localization of the NMDAR, and increased NMDAR dependent Ca influx. Loss of EphB-ephrin-B signaling might cause a reduction in NMDAR Ca influx, but not a complete block of ionotropic signaling. To demonstrate that the findings in Figures 7 and 8 are linked, the authors should inject NMDAR blockers (after conducting the control experiments in point 2), in the ephrin-B1 knockout mice. The anti-NMDAR antibodies simply remove the receptor from the synapse. This does not address whether the effects are independent of the ionotropic modulation of the channel and should be discussed.

We agree and think this is a reasonable point. Our toned down discussion of the interaction hopefully will address this comment. We will perform this very sensible experiment when addressing the detailed molecular mechanism underlying the collaboration of NMDAR and EPHRINB/EPHB signaling in regulating callosal innervation patterns in the following project.

Revisions expected in follow-up work:

1) In implicating repellent activity that would restrict commissural axonal projections to the S1 cortex, the evidence for NMDAR via Eph B2 function as repellents for elongating callosal axons is lacking. in vitro experiments demonstrating repellent activity of EphB downstream of NMDAR would be necessary to make this claim. Given the time required to develop and execute such an assay, and the pandemic delays, you could take as long as needed to do this experiment, if you choose to do so.

We will address it in our follow-up project.

2) Reviewers 4 and 5 point out that these pathways could be involved in refinement and pruning of axons: Reviewer 5 raises the possibility that in the mutants, axons are stabilized, to result in extra numbers of callosal neurons. NMDAR could thus be responsible for repulsion during refinement. Reviewer 4's comments on EphBs interacting with excitatory synapse development could play into this explanation. More high-resolution analysis of the impact of your manipulations on axonal branching would address the other reviewers' questions regarding whether the changes see are due defects in branching and/or refinement. This aspect as in the in vitro experiments require additional experiments which could understandably not be possible at this time.

Thank you for your kindly considerations. We appreciate it. We have discussed all the possibilities raised by reviewers 4 and 5 in our last part of Discussion -“the refinement of callosal innervation pattern”. As for the high-resolution of individual axon branch raised by reviewer 3, we do appreciate the advice and have applied sparse labelling of axon branch in another project when addressing the effect of patient-derived anti-NMDAR autoantibodies on callosal innervation pattern in S1. We are also planning to sparsely label neurons in vivo when addressing the detailed molecular mechanism underlying the collaboration of NMDAR and EPHRINB/EPHB signaling in regulating callosal innervation patterns in the follow-up project. We do hope we can contribute more to the field in these follow up studies.

[Editors' note: further revisions were suggested prior to acceptance, as described below.]

Revisions:

The rebuttal letter was difficult to follow. It was not clear where in the manuscript some of the changes were made. Figures were not labelled and main figures were mixed with supplementary figures. However, the following points appear not to have been addressed adequately:

1) In response to the criticism of overstating the 30% reduction in EphB2 protein expression in the Grin1 CKOs, the authors have provided images showing EphB2 immunofluorescence in Emx1Cre;Grin1fl/fl;tdTomato mice (Figure 8—figure supplement 2). While the pictures in this new figure suggest loss of EphB2 IF in Tom+;Grin1 cKO cells, but not in Tom+;control cells, the results should be quantified. Presenting images without quantification is not convincing. Please quantify and add the results to the paper.

We have quantified the fluorescence density of EPHB2 immunostaining for each cell. See the Figure 8—figure supplement 2.

2) Please address the following points that were raised in the previous critique and appear not to have been answered:

a) show images of all controls and experimental groups of Figure 3 in Supplementary data.

The images of the rest of controls and experimental groups for double in utero electroporation have been added it into manuscript as Figure 3—figure supplement 1.

b) Provide better evidence for NR1 protein downregulation in the conditional NR1 knockouts (Figure 1—figure supplement 1).

In addition to the immunostaining data, we performed western blot of S1 at P8. Compared with control, the protein levels of GluN1 were greatly reduced in Emx1cre/+; Grin1fl/fl mice, see Figure 1—figure supplement 1.

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    Transparent reporting form

    Data Availability Statement

    All data generated or analyses during this study are included in the manuscript.


    Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

    RESOURCES