Abstract
Background
Early detection of anxiety symptoms in older people is capital as it may be linked to increased physical/functional disabilities, onset and progression of neurodegenerative disorders, and poor cognitive functioning. Nonetheless, there is a paucity of psychometrically validated anxiety measures in the elderly.
Aims
This study aimed at assessing the psychometric properties of the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory-Form Y (STAI-Y) and providing the first normative data for the Italian elderly population.
Methods
The sample included 361 individuals aged 65–94. All subjects underwent the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE), the Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS), and the state (S-Anxiety) and trait anxiety (T-anxiety) scales of the STAI-Y.
Results
The S-Anxiety and T-Anxiety scales showed reliable internal consistency and, overall, good item characteristics. Divergent validity was “apparently” threatened, with S-Anxiety scale correlating with MMSE and GDS, and T-Anxiety scale only with GDS. The principal component analysis revealed a three-factor solution for both scales, i.e., presence and absence of state (or trait) anxiety, and performance anxiety. Since no effect of sociodemographic variables was found, unadjusted cutoffs were provided.
Conclusions
Although some questions on the psychometric properties of the STAI-Y remain unanswered, this normative study can help clinicians and researchers to monitor anxiety levels in the Italian elderly population.
Supplementary Information
The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1007/s40520-021-01815-0.
Keywords: State-trait anxiety inventory, Elderly, Psychometric properties, Normative study, Italian population
Introduction
In the present paper, we focus on a frequent symptom among the elderly: anxiety. Anxiety disorders affect from 3 to 15% of the elderly [1]; sub-threshold anxiety symptoms are even more frequent, with prevalence ranging from 15 to 52% [2]. There are several reasons why clinicians and researchers need to pay close attention to anxiety symptoms in the elderly.
First, anxiety disorders, as well as sub-threshold anxiety symptoms, are associated with a physical disability, higher risk of adverse outcomes after myocardial infarction [3, 4], functional impairment, poorer quality of life, sleep disorders, and increased mortality [5].
Second, anxiety appears to be a risk factor for the development of age-related neurocognitive disorders. Higher levels of anxiety were found to be associated with greater cognitive decline over time [6], with cognitive deficits in Parkinson’s disease [7], with increased risk of developing dementia [8] and of converting from mild cognitive impairment to dementia [9].
Last but not least, anxiety negatively affects cognitive performance [10]. According to the attentional control theory (ACT) [11], anxiety enhances the influence of the stimulus-driven attentional system over the more efficient goal-driven system; as a result, the susceptibility to threat-related information increases, hence hindering task performance. Some studies showed that, in community-dwelling elderly, sub-threshold anxiety affected executive functions [12] and episodic memory [1]. Other studies, however, found no relationship between anxiety and cognitive performance [13].
Still, it is relevant to stress that any effect of anxiety on cognitive performance is highly relevant for both clinical- and research-related assessment involving older adults [14]. Indeed, anxiety may confound testing results and the physiological age-related cognitive decline may be exacerbated in anxious individuals.
The above overview suggests how important is detecting anxiety in the elderly. Self-report measures, such as the Form-Y of the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI-Y) [15], the Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI) [16], and the Taylor Manifest Anxiety Scale (TMAS) [17] are some of the most commonly used tools for quantifying general anxiety, the majority of which have been, however, validated in younger populations. Among these, the STAI-Y may be a good candidate to explore anxiety in older adults. STAI-Y norms and psychometric data for older people are currently available only in the French population [18, 19]. No normative data exist for the Italian population. The present paper is an attempt to fill this gap.
The employment of the STAI-Y for measuring anxiety in the elderly may be convenient for different reasons. It showed good psychometric properties in samples of both younger [15] and older adults [20, 21]. In addition, since the STAI-Y is less oriented towards somatic components of anxiety (e.g., pain, difficulty in breathing, sleep disorders) than other widely used scales such as the BAI and TMAS, the risk of overlap between medical and psychological determinants of anxiety is lower [22]. Moreover, several versions of the STAI-Y are available in different languages; this makes it useful for investigating anxiety across cultures and, after collecting normative data from elderly cohorts, also across the life span. Finally, as the STAI-Y is the only anxiety scale allowing for a clear separation between state and trait anxiety [15, 20, 22], it is particularly suitable for detecting transitory rises of anxiety, which may bias clinical and experimental outcomes in assessment contexts involving elderly.
One important weakness of the STAI-Y concerns its limited construct validity, as indexed by the weak-to-moderate correlation (ranging from 0.16 to 0.57) with measures of worry [23]. Even so, the STAI-Y is the best-known and most widely used self-report questionnaire to assess anxiety in research and clinical practice [24].
The aims of the present study are therefore to assess the psychometric properties of the STAI-Y and provide normative data for the Italian elderly population. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first normative Italian study addressing this issue.
Methods
Subjects and procedure
A total of 398 community-dwelling Italian older adults (226 females) from the Campania region participated in this study as volunteers. All subjects performed the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) [25], the Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS) [26], and the Italian version of the STAI-Y [27].
The STAI-Y is a self-report questionnaire consisting of two 20-item scales providing separate measures of state and trait anxiety (S-Anxiety and T-Anxiety, respectively). S-Anxiety is a transitory response to an event perceived as adverse, characterized by feelings of tension, apprehension, nervousness, and worry. T-Anxiety is, instead, a more stable predisposition to perceive stressful situations as dangerous or threatening [15]. Consequently, the S-Anxiety scale includes twenty items evaluating how the subject feels “right now, at this moment”, while the T-Anxiety scale includes twenty items evaluating how the subject feels “generally”. On a 4-point Likert scale (1–4), a score equal to 4 indicates the presence of a higher level of anxiety for ten S-Anxiety (3, 4, 6, 7, 9, 12, 13, 14, 17, 18) and eleven T-Anxiety items (2, 4, 5, 8, 9, 11, 12, 15, 17, 18, 20). As for the remaining items, the scoring weights are reversed. The total score for both scales ranges from 20 to 80, with higher scores indicating more severe anxiety [15].
Inclusion criteria were as follows: age equal or greater than 65 years old, at least 3 years of formal education, adjusted MMSE score higher than the normative cutoff value, and completion of the whole testing protocol. Exclusion criteria were as follows: neurocognitive or psychiatric disorders (e.g., mild cognitive impairment, dementia, Parkinson’s disease, schizophrenia), history of epilepsy, transient ischemic attack, stroke, or head injury, evidence of serious health condition, history of alcohol or substance abuse, and intake of psychotropic drugs. To prevent a “hyper-normality bias”, any subject with well-pharmacologically compensated diseases (e.g., hypertension, diabetes) was not excluded [28].
According to the exclusion criteria, data from 37 subjects were removed. The final normative sample included 361 individuals. The distribution of sociodemographic variables is reported in Table 1. Descriptive statistics on demographic and clinical variables are summarized in Table 2.
Table 1.
Education (years) | Sex | Age (years) | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
65–69 | 70–74 | 75–79 | 80–84 | 85–89 | 90–94 | Total | ||
3–5 | Male | 29 | 12 | 15 | 13 | 4 | 1 | 74 |
Female | 36 | 40 | 16 | 20 | 12 | 4 | 128 | |
6–8 | Male | 32 | 9 | 6 | 1 | 2 | – | 50 |
Female | 29 | 7 | 3 | – | – | – | 39 | |
9–13 | Male | 15 | 8 | 2 | 1 | – | – | 26 |
Female | 12 | 1 | 2 | – | 1 | – | 16 | |
> 13 | Male | 11 | 1 | – | – | – | – | 12 |
Female | 9 | 3 | 2 | 1 | 1 | – | 16 | |
Total | 173 | 81 | 46 | 36 | 20 | 5 | 361 |
Table 2.
Demographic and clinical variables | Subjects included (n = 361) | Subjects excluded (n = 37) | p value (χ2/U) |
---|---|---|---|
Sex (female/male) | 199/162 | 27/10 | < 0.05 |
Age (years), mean (SD) | 71.75 (7.06) | 70.97 (5.47) | 0.93 |
Education (years), mean (SD) | 7.01 (4.06) | 6.92 (3.39) | 0.68 |
MMSE raw score, mean (SD) | 28.37 (1.55) | 25.89 (3.85) | < 0.01 |
GDS, mean (SD) | 5.23 (5.03) | 6.89 (5.70) | < 0.05 |
S-Anxiety scale, mean (SD) | 37.08 (11.46) | 41.43 (13.72) | 0.06 |
T-Anxiety scale, mean (SD) | 37.19 (11.50) | 40.22 (15.71) | 0.45 |
MMSE Mini-Mental State Examination, GDS Geriatric Depression Scale, U Mann–Whitney U Test
Statistical analyses
To detect potential effects of the sociodemographic variables on the S-Anxiety and T-Anxiety scales, we performed two separate simultaneous multiple linear regression analyses assuming the total S-Anxiety (and T-Anxiety) score as dependent variable and sex, age, and education as predictors. If a significant contribution to the scale’s variance was found for at least one of the variables under examination, the correction procedure proposed by Spinnler and Tognoni [29] would have been applied (see also Garofalo et al. [28]).
Item characteristics, reliability, and scaling assumptions were tested using the corrected Pearson’s item-total correlation. An item-total correlation > 30 was deemed acceptable [30].
As made for regression analysis, we performed two separate principal component analyses (PCAs) with Varimax orthogonal rotation to approximate the factorial solutions for both the S-Anxiety and T-Anxiety scales. The number of factors to be extracted was determined following the Mineigen criterion (eigenvalues > 1 [31]) or inspecting the scree plot [32]. Furthermore, Spearman’s correlation analysis (rrho) was used to quantify the association between items, or factors, and total score, and to test divergent validity. The Cohen’s conventions were used to interpret the effect size (weak, rrho < 0.30; moderate, rrho = 0.30–0.50; strong, rrho > 0.50). Internal consistency was computed using Cronbach’s alpha coefficient.
Since both scales were constructed so that higher scores reflect more severe anxiety, cutoffs were fixed at the non-parametric outer tolerance limit on the 95th centile, with 95% confidence interval [28]. Finally, we quantified the “uncertainty” associated with our normative data by applying Crawford and Garthwaite’s procedure [33]. Statistical analyses were performed by IBM SPSS Statistics (v. 26), STATA (v. 15), and QUAND.exe programs.
Results
We ran a preliminary analysis to test the univariate normality of S-Anxiety and T-Anxiety total scores. Skewness and kurtosis indexes were examined. For each scale, the skewness and kurtosis values were in the acceptable range (– 1.0 and + 1.0), thus suggesting the absence of appreciable deviations from normality [34]. As shown in Table 2, the mean S-Anxiety score was 37.08 (SD = 11.46, range = 20–76; n = 361) and the mean T-Anxiety score was 37.19 (SD = 11.50, range = 22–84; n = 361). No difference was found between men and women on the age variable (χ2(5) = 7.739, p = 0.17). Conversely, a significant difference on the education variable was observed (χ2(3) = 15.114, p < 0.001, φ = 0.20), i.e., a larger number of lowly educated women.
About the regression analysis, we found no effect of sex, age, or education on S-Anxiety score (R2 = 0.01, F(3, 357) = 1.311, p = 0.27; BSex = 0.49, SE = 1.22; p = 0.69; BAge = 0.02, SE = 0.09, p = 0.86; BEducation = – 0.27, SE = 0.16, p = 0.09). Similarly, T-Anxiety score was unaffected by sociodemographic variables (R2 = 0.002, F(3, 357) = 0.269, p = 0.85; BSex = – 0.72, SE = 1.23; p = 0.56; BAge = – 0.06, SE = 0.09, p = 0.53; BEducation = – 0.02, SE = 0.16, p = 0.88). Consequently, no adjustment of raw scores was required.
Most of the items of the S-Anxiety scale showed a significant moderate (items 1, 7, 16–19; rrho range = 0.38–0.50, ps < 0.001) to strong correlation (items 2–6, 8–12, 14, 15, 20; rrho range = 0.51–0.69, ps < 0.001) with the total score, and a more than acceptable level of discrimination (item-total correlations, range = 0.38–0.70). Only item 13 was weakly correlated to the total score (rrho = 0.19, p < 0.001) and had a poor level of discrimination (item-total correlation = 0.16). As for the T-Anxiety scale, all items showed a significant moderate (items 1, 4, 9, 10, 12, 16–20; rrho range = 0.35–0.43, ps < 0.001) to strong correlation (items 2, 3, 5–8, 11, 13–15; rrho range = 0.51–0.64, all ps < 0.001) with the total score, and a good level of discrimination (item-total correlation, range = 0.37–0.69), with the exception of item 20 (item-total correlation = 0.23) (see Online Resource 1). Both questionnaires demonstrated reliable internal consistency as shown by a Cronbach’s α of 0.90.
Divergent validity was measured by correlating S-Anxiety and T-Anxiety scores with MMSE and GDS. The S-Anxiety scale showed a small linear association with MMSE (rrho = – 0.13, p < 0.05) and GDS (rrho = 0.23, p < 0.001). Conversely, the T-Anxiety scale showed no correlation with MMSE (rrho = – 0.09, p = 0.07), and a moderate correlation with GDS (rrho = 0.48, p < 0.001).
Using the Mineigen criterion (eigenvalues > 1), the PCA suggested a three-factor solution for the S-Anxiety scale, and a four-factor solution for the T-Anxiety scale (with one factor including only one item, i.e., item 20). From inspection of the scree plots, a three-factor solution was confirmed for the S-Anxiety scale; conversely, for the T-Anxiety scale, a three-factor model provided the best fit. The three-factor models presented a clear and parsimonious structure, with components showing strong loadings (all > 0.50) and a minimal number of overlapping items between factors (i.e., item 19 of the T-Anxiety scale loading ≥ 0.50 on both factor 1 and 2). Table 3 shows the factorial structures of both scales.
Table 3.
Items | Factor 1 | Factor 2 | Factor 3 |
---|---|---|---|
S-Anxiety scale | |||
2. I feel secure | 0.802 | 0.100 | 0.150 |
20. I feel pleasant | 0.763 | 0.117 | 0.318 |
19. I feel steady | 0.755 | 0.048 | 0.050 |
5. I feel at ease | 0.740 | 0.107 | 0.335 |
11. I feel self-confident | 0.728 | 0.165 | 0.262 |
8. I feel satisfied | 0.679 | 0.088 | 0.292 |
16. I feel content | 0.625 | 0.115 | 0.318 |
1. I feel calm | 0.618 | 0.011 | 0.271 |
9. I feel frightened | – 0.067 | 0.866 | 0.113 |
10. I feel comfortable | 0.121 | 0.847 | – 0.085 |
14. I feel indecisive | 0.089 | 0.828 | 0.233 |
15. I am relaxed | 0.305 | 0.825 | – 0.102 |
12. I feel nervous | 0.098 | 0.820 | 0.155 |
18. I feel confused | 0.295 | 0.196 | 0.647 |
13. I am jittery | 0.012 | – 0.232 | 0.611 |
7. I am presently worrying over possible misfortunes | 0.337 | 0.135 | 0.603 |
17. I am worried | 0.277 | – 0.004 | 0.593 |
3. I am tense | 0.471 | 0.125 | 0.588 |
4. I feel strained | 0.386 | 0.222 | 0.569 |
6. I feel upset | 0.415 | 0.226 | 0.561 |
Variance explained (%) | 38.03 | 15.87 | 6.31 |
Correlation (rrho) with total score | 0.46* | 0.73* | 0.33* |
Cronbach’s α | 0.90 | 0.90 | 0.80 |
T-Anxiety scale | |||
18. I take disappointments so keenly that I can’t put them out of my mind | 0.713 | 0.074 | 0.157 |
12. I lack self-confidence | 0.701 | 0.075 | 0.038 |
4. I wish I could be as happy as others seem to be | 0.697 | – 0.067 | 0.157 |
9. I worry too much over something that really doesn’t matter | 0.686 | – 0.094 | 0.216 |
10. I am happy | 0.657 | 0.352 | – 0.071 |
16. I am content | 0.619 | 0.441 | – 0.113 |
17. Some unimportant thought runs through my mind and bothers me | 0.618 | 0.105 | 0.150 |
1. I feel pleasant | 0.598 | 0.507 | – 0.118 |
19. I am a steady persona | 0.513 | 0.502 | – 0.087 |
14. I make decisions easily | 0.132 | 0.777 | 0.311 |
6. I feel rested | 0.069 | 0.711 | 0.285 |
13. I feel secure | 0.086 | 0.681 | 0.429 |
3. I feel satisfied with myself | 0.161 | 0.638 | 0.438 |
7. I am “calm, cool, and collected” | – 0.002 | 0.614 | 0.376 |
15. I feel inadequate | 0.104 | 0.126 | 0.762 |
5. I feel like a failure | – 0.020 | 0.341 | 0.751 |
8. I feel that difficulties are piling up so that I cannot overcome them | 0.266 | 0.354 | 0.679 |
2. I feel nervous and restless | 0.219 | 0.300 | 0.678 |
11. I have disturbing thoughts | 0.247 | 0.374 | 0.677 |
20. I get in a state of tension or turmoil as I think over my recent concerns and interests | – 0.081 | – 0.001 | 0.566 |
Variance explained (%) | 35.48 | 15.15 | 7.26 |
Correlation (rrho) with total score | 0.45* | 0.58* | 0.52* |
Cronbach’s α | 0.84 | 0.86 | 0.84 |
Major loading > 0.50 for each item are displayed in bold
*p < .001
aItem 19 (T-Anxiety) loaded on two components with a similar extent and was not assigned to any specific factor
The cutoffs were fixed at the non-parametric outer tolerance limits on the 95th centile (95% confidence interval). As for the S-Anxiety scale, the cutoff was 67.13 and should guarantee higher specificity. A lower cutoff score, e.g., the external tolerance limit on the 90th centile, could ensure higher sensitivity (90th centile cutoff = 61). As for the T-Anxiety scale, the cutoffs calculated on the 90th and 95th centiles were 54.19 and 66.25, respectively.
The QUAND.exe program (Quantifying the Uncertainty Attached to Normative Data) was used to test the robustness/fallibility of our normative data [33]. It requires the entry of the mean and SD of the normative sample, the sample size, and the minimum and maximum test scores. The software converts each raw score into the relative z-score (95% confidence limits). We performed two separate analyses for each scale. The analysis’ output is available in Online Resource 2. From a statistical standpoint, confidence intervals should become wider as the difference between the raw score and the normative sample mean increases. As a consequence, wider intervals for scores close to the mean suggest the presence of measurement errors [33]. The results of Crawford and Garthwaite’s test supported the robustness of our normative data since confidence intervals were tighter for scores close to the mean and became, instead, wider for scores above the normative cutoff.
Discussion
The present paper aimed at assessing the psychometric properties of the STAI-Y and providing the first normative data for Italian elderly people.
In line with previous studies [18, 19, 21], both the S-Anxiety and T-Anxiety scales demonstrated strong internal consistency. Indeed, almost all items showed a good discriminant power apart from item 13 of the S-Anxiety scale and item 20 of the T-Anxiety scale. These two items, therefore, seem not very appropriate for screening anxiety in the Italian elderly.
Both the S-anxiety and T-Anxiety scales showed a significant positive linear association with GDS. The well-known partial overlap between symptoms of anxiety and depression (e.g., sleep disturbance, fatigue, difficulty concentrating) [35] likely accounts for this result. Symptoms of anxiety and depression are frequently comorbid both in young [36] and elderly populations [37]. Whether anxiety and depression are discrete disorders or belong to the same nosographic category is still a matter of intense debate [38].
The S-Anxiety scale, moreover, correlated weakly, and negatively, with MMSE, whereas the T-Anxiety scale did not. Note that the neuropsychological setting is a potentially state-anxiety-inducing context per se, which can generate evaluation apprehension: since the subject is conscious of being under assessment, she/he tries to appear as “normal” as possible in response to the current canons of social desirability. Therefore, the pre-test and post-test evaluation of the S-Anxiety could help clinicians, as well as researchers, to control this confounded variable for increasing reliability of diagnosis and experimental results.
The PCA identified a clear and parsimonious three-factor structure for both the S-Anxiety and T-Anxiety scales. About the former, Factor 1 included items reflecting the absence of state anxiety (e.g., self-confidence, satisfaction, psychological well-being), Factor 2 included items likely referring to performance anxiety, i.e., fear of performing poorly a specific task (e.g., nervousness, tension, hesitation), and Factor 3 included items reflecting the presence of state anxiety (e.g., agitation, confusion, concern, worry). About the latter, Factor 1 included items reflecting the presence of trait anxiety (e.g., unhappiness, worry, disappointment, rumination), Factor 2 included items reflecting the absence of trait anxiety (e.g., self-confidence, safety), and Factor 3 included items likely referring to performance anxiety (e.g., poor resilience, tension, worry, intrusive thoughts, sense of inadequacy). Our finding of a present-absent-anxiety dichotomy is in line with results reported in previous investigations [20, 39]. This is, however, the first study on older subjects in which an additional factor, related to performance anxiety, was found (see Table 3).
As for the normative data, we found no effect of sex, age, or education either on S-Anxiety or on T-Anxiety scores. Therefore, we provided unadjusted cutoffs for both scales, one promoting higher specificity, and the other higher sensitivity. The choice of the reference cutoff should be guided by the aims of the assessment. For instance, experimental settings typically pose anxiety as an exclusion criterion; therefore, the cutoff fixed at the 95th centile should be preferred as it guarantees higher specificity, and hence fewer false positives. In the clinical settings, where decreasing the risk of false negatives is imperative, the cutoff fixed at the 90th centile should be preferred as it guarantees higher sensitivity.
With respect to age, in earlier studies, T-Anxiety scores were found to increase with age [40], while S-Anxiety scores were found to decrease with age [19]. However, there are studies in which age did not affect either T-Anxiety [18, 20, 21, 41] or S-Anxiety scores [20, 41]. These sparse and mixed findings make unclear whether anxiety shows stable or variable patterns in late life, and whether changes across old ages—if any—are towards higher or lower anxiety. One might theoretically expect that as risk factors for negative affect are highly prevalent among the elderly, anxiety should increase while aging proceeds. Furthermore, aging is associated with abnormal structural connectivity between the ventral prefrontal cortex and amygdala [42]. This may decrease the frontal regulatory control over the amygdala [43], thus determining more anxiety symptoms in the elderly. Nevertheless, if one focuses on the broader literature linking emotional functioning and aging, an age-related upward trajectory should be expected, with emotional maturation enhancing as a function of age [44].
As concerns the effect of education on the STAI-Y, results of earlier studies showed inconsistencies. Some studies reported higher T-Anxiety [20, 45] and S-Anxiety [19, 20] in less educated elderly, while other studies found equivalent T-Anxiety across educational levels [18]. Anyhow, education is known to enable people to preserve mental resources, that, in turn, may boost higher resilience in regard to strain or stresses, hence protecting against anxiety [46].
Finally, even the effect of sex on the STAI-Y appears controversial. In earlier studies, S-Anxiety [19] and T-Anxiety scores [18, 21] were found to be higher in old women as compared to men; yet, old males and females were found to have equivalent scores on both the T-Anxiety and S-Anxiety scales [20]. However, it should be noted that women of all ages seem to be more at risk of developing anxiety as compared to men [47].
To better evaluate the contribution of our results, it is important to highlight the weaknesses of this study. Our sample has a limited size, and it includes only subjects residing in the Campania region. This might be considered a threat to the external/population validity of the study. However, our sample is representative of the Italian elderly population, as portrayed by the Italian National Institute of Statistics [48]. Furthermore, it includes a trifling number of individuals over eighty years of age; this may have hampered the possibility to disclose a non-linear relationship [45] between age and anxiety within the upper limit of the age distribution. In the face of these limitations, our data still represent a point where to start from to improve measurement and monitoring of anxiety in the Italian elderly population, thereby ensuring accurate and timely treatments of late-life anxiety. Any development in that direction will certainly improve both personal and public health, even more so in view of the psychological repercussions of the COVID-19 outbreak [49].
Supplementary Information
Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.
Funding
No funding was received for conducting this study.
Compliance with ethical standards
Conflict of interest
On behalf of all authors, the corresponding author states that there is no conflict of interest.
Ethics approval
The study was approved by the Suor Orsola Benincasa University Ethics Committee and performed in accordance with the ethical standards as laid down in the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki and its later amendments.
Informed consent
Written informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.
Footnotes
Publisher's Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
References
- 1.Yochim BP, Mueller AE, Segal DL. Late life anxiety is associated with decreased memory and executive functioning in community dwelling older adults. J Anxiety Disord. 2013;27:567–575. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2012.10.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Bryant C, Jackson H, Ames D. The prevalence of anxiety in older adults: methodological issues and a review of the literature. J Affect Disord. 2008;109:233–250. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2007.11.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Ilardi A, Caruso G, Ilardi CR, et al. Chest pain management: use of troponins in internal medicine wards. Diagnosis (Berl) 2020 doi: 10.1515/dx-2020-0133. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Roest AM, Zuidersma M, De Jonge P. Myocardial infarction and generalised anxiety disorder: 10-year follow-up. Br J Psychiatry. 2012;200:324–329. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.111.103549. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Wolitzky-Taylor KB, Castriotta N, Lenze EJ, et al. Anxiety disorders in older adults: a comprehensive review. Depress Anxiety. 2010;27:190–211. doi: 10.1002/da.20653. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Pietrzak RH, Maruff P, Woodward M, et al. Mild worry symptoms predict decline in learning and memory in healthy older adults: a 2-year prospective cohort study. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2012;20:266–275. doi: 10.1097/JGP.0b013e3182107e24. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Maggi G, D’Iorio A, Di Meglio D, et al. The role of the motor subtypes on the relationship between anxiety and cognitive dysfunctions in Parkinson’s disease. J Neural Transm (Vienna) 2020;127:893–898. doi: 10.1007/s00702-020-02179-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Santabárbara J, Villagrasa B, López-Antón R, et al. Clinically relevant anxiety and risk of Alzheimer’s disease in an elderly community sample: 4.5 years of follow-up. J Affect Disord. 2019;250:16–20. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2019.02.050. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Rosenberg PB, Mielke MM, Appleby BS, et al. The association of neuropsychiatric symptoms in MCI with incident dementia and Alzheimer disease. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2013;21:685–695. doi: 10.1016/j.jagp.2013.01.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Eysenck MW. Anxiety: the cognitive perspective. Hove: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1992. [Google Scholar]
- 11.Eysenck MW, Derakshan N. New perspectives in attentional control theory. Pers Individ Differ. 2011;50:955–960. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2010.08.019. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Beaudreau SA, O’Hara R. The association of anxiety and depressive symptoms with cognitive performance in community-dwelling older adults. Psychol Aging. 2009;24:507–512. doi: 10.1037/a0016035. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.de Vito A, Calamia M, Greening S, et al. The association of anxiety, depression, and worry symptoms on cognitive performance in older adults. Aging Neuropsychol Cogn B Aging Neuropsychol Cogn. 2019;26:161–173. doi: 10.1080/13825585.2017.1416057. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Tales A, Basoudan N. Anxiety in old age and dementia—implications for clinical and research practice. Neuropsychiatry. 2016;6:142–148. doi: 10.4172/Neuropsychiatry.1000133. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Spielberger CD, Gorsuch RL, Lushene RE, et al. State-Trait Anxiety Inventory for adults: manual, instrument and scoring guide. Palo Alto: Mind Garden; 1983. [Google Scholar]
- 16.Beck AT, Epstein N, Brown G. An inventory for measuring clinical anxiety: psychometric properties. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1988;56:893–897. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.56.6.893. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Taylor JA. A personality scale of manifest anxiety. J Abnorm Psychol. 1953;48:285–290. doi: 10.1037/h0056264. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Bergua V, Meillon C, Potvin O, et al. The STAI-Y trait scale: psychometric properties and normative data from a large population-based study of elderly people. Int Psychogeriatr. 2012;24:1163–1171. doi: 10.1017/S1041610212000300. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Potvin O, Bergua V, Meillon C. Norms and associated factors of the STAI-Y State anxiety inventory in older adults: results from the PAQUID study. Int Psychogeriatrics. 2011;23:869–879. doi: 10.1017/S1041610210002358. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Bouchard S, Ivers H, Gauthier JG. Psychometric properties of the French version of the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (form Y) adapted for older adults. Can J Aging. 1998;17:440–453. doi: 10.1017/S0714980800012708. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Fuentes K, Cox B. Assessment of anxiety in older adults: a community-based survey and comparison with younger adults. Behav Res Ther. 2000;38:297–309. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7967(99)00067-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Therrien Z, Hunsley J. Assessment of anxiety in older adults: a systematic review of commonly used measures. Aging Ment Health. 2012;16:1–16. doi: 10.1080/13607863.2011.602960. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Dennis RE, Boddington SJ, Funnell NJ. Self-report measures of anxiety: are they suitable for older adults? Aging Ment Health. 2007;11:668–677. doi: 10.1080/13607860701529916. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Balsamo M, Cataldi F, Carlucci L, et al. Assessment of anxiety in older adults: a review of self-report measures. Clin Interv Aging. 2018;13:573–593. doi: 10.2147/CIA.S114100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. “Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res. 1975;12:189–198. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Yesavage JA, Brink TL, Rose TL, et al. Development and validation of a geriatric depression screening scale: a preliminary report. J Psychiatr Res. 1982;17:37–49. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(82)90033-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Pedrabissi L, Santinello M. Inventario per l’ansia di «Stato» e di «Tratto»: nuova versione italiana dello STAI Forma Y: Manuale. Firenze: Organizzazioni Speciali; 1989. [Google Scholar]
- 28.Garofalo E, Iavarone A, Chieffi S, et al. Italian version of the Starkstein Apathy Scale (SAS-I) and a shortened version (SAS-6) to assess “pure apathy” symptoms: normative study on 392 individuals. Neurol Sci. 2020 doi: 10.1007/s10072-020-04631-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Spinnler H, Tognoni G. Standardizzazione e Taratura Italiana di Test Neuropsicologici. Ital J Neurol Sci. 1987;1:8–120. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.De Vaus D. Surveys in social research. London: Routledge; 2004. [Google Scholar]
- 31.Kaiser HF. The application of electronic computers to factor analysis. Educ Psychol Meas. 1960;20:141–151. doi: 10.1177/001316446002000116. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Cattell RB, Vogelmann S. A comprehensive trial of the scree and kg criteria for determining the number of factors. Multivar Behav Res. 1977;12:289–325. doi: 10.1207/s15327906mbr1203_2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Crawford JR, Garthwaite PH. On the “optimal” size for normative samples in neuropsychology: capturing the uncertainty when normative data are used to quantify the standing of a neuropsychological test score. Child Neuropsychol. 2008;14:99–117. doi: 10.1080/09297040801894709. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Marcoulides GA, Hershberger SL. Multivariate statistical methods: a first course. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1997. [Google Scholar]
- 35.Zbozinek TD, Rose RD, Wolitzky-Taylor KB, et al. Diagnostic overlap of generalized anxiety disorder and major depressive disorder in a primary care sample. Depress Anxiety. 2012;29:1065–1071. doi: 10.1002/da.22026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Jenkins PE, Ducker I, Gooding R, et al. Anxiety and depression in a sample of UK college students: a study of prevalence, comorbidity, and quality of life. J Am Coll Health. 2020 doi: 10.1080/07448481.2019.1709474. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Cassidy EL, Lauderdale S, Sheikh JI. Mixed anxiety and depression in older adults: clinical characteristics and management. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol. 2005;18:83–88. doi: 10.1177/0891988705276060. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Kalin NH. The critical relationship between anxiety and depression. Am J Psychiatry. 2020;177:365–367. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2020.20030305. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Vagg PR, Spielberger CD, O’Hearn TP. Is the state-trait anxiety inventory multidimensional? Pers Individ Differ. 1980;1:207–214. doi: 10.1016/0191-8869(80)90052-5. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Himmelfarb S, Murrell SA. The prevalence and correlates of anxiety symptoms in older adults. J Psychol. 1984;116:159–167. doi: 10.1080/00223980.1984.9923632. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Kvaal K, Laake K, Engedal K. Psychometric properties of the state part of the Spielberger State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) in geriatric patients. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2001;16:980–986. doi: 10.1002/gps.458. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Davis SW, Dennis NA, Buchler NG. Assessing the effects of age on long white matter tracts using diffusion tensor tractography. Neuroimage. 2009;46:530–541. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.01.068. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Bishop SJ. Neural mechanisms underlying selective attention to threat. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2008;1129:141–152. doi: 10.1196/annals.1417.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Carstensen LL. Motivation for social contact across the life span: a theory of socioemotional selectivity. In: Jacobs JE, editor. Nebraska symposium on motivation. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press; 1993. pp. 209–254. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Teachman BA. Aging and negative affect: the rise and fall and rise of anxiety and depression symptoms. Psychol Aging. 2006;21:201–207. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.21.1.201. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Mirowsky J, Ross CE. Education, personal control, lifestyle and health: a human capital hypothesis. Res Aging. 1998;20:415–449. doi: 10.1177/0164027598204003. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Boehlen FH, Herzog W, Schellberg D, et al. Gender-specific predictors of generalized anxiety disorder symptoms in older adults: results of a large population-based study. J Affect Disord. 2020;262:174–181. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2019.10.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.ISTAT (2011) General Population and Housing Census. Italian National Institute of Statistics. http://dati-censimentopopolazione.istat.it/Index.aspx. Accessed 10 Oct 2020
- 49.Sepúlveda-Loyola W, Rodríguez-Sánchez I, Pérez-Rodríguez P, et al. Impact of social isolation due to COVID-19 on health in older people: mental and physical effects and recommendations. J Nutr Health Aging. 2020 doi: 10.1007/s12603-020-1469-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.