An official website of the United States government
Here's how you know
Official websites use .gov
A
.gov website belongs to an official
government organization in the United States.
Secure .gov websites use HTTPS
A lock (
) or https:// means you've safely
connected to the .gov website. Share sensitive
information only on official, secure websites.
As a library, NLM provides access to scientific literature. Inclusion in an NLM database does not imply endorsement of, or agreement with,
the contents by NLM or the National Institutes of Health.
Learn more:
PMC Disclaimer
|
PMC Copyright Notice
letter . 2005 Jan;26(1):195–198.
Absence of Memory Dysfunction after Bilateral Mammillary Body and Mammillothalamic Tract Electrode Implantation: Preliminary Experience in Three Patients
The anatomic bases for memory disorders have been widely debated. Lesions of the medial dorsal (MD) and anterior nuclei (AN) of the thalami and lesions of the mammillary bodies (MB) are most commonly involved in amnesic syndromes in humans (1, 2).
In a monkey model, Mishkin (3) conceptualized the existence of a double limbic circuitry supporting the memory function (i.e., the medial limbic [hippocampal] and the basolateral limbic [amygdaloid] circuits). In this functional scheme, the AN and the MD thalamic nuclei act as “nodal” points of convergence where bilateral tissue damage can result in memory function impairment, similar to bilateral lesions of the mammillothalamic tract (MTT), which connects the MB to the AN.
In an article in the June/July issue of the AJNR, Yoneoka et al (4) reported on the onset of a Korsakoff syndrome in a patient with acute ischemic damage of the left MTT assessed by positive diffusion-weighted imaging who had previously suffered from a contralateral right MTT mirror infarction. The authors suggested that bilateral MTT dysfunction may be sufficient to cause severe amnesic state, which matched Mishkin’s hypothesis. Berger highlighted this in an editorial of the same issue of the AJNR, stating that the article added one more site of the hippocampal-limbic system—namely, the MTT—from which bilateral lesion may result in amnesia (5).
The neurosurgical team at our institution has initiated a clinical trial aimed at treating patients with chronic refractory epilepsy by bilateral stereotactic implantation of stimulation electrodes (DBS lead 3389, Medtronic, Minneapolis, MN) within the MB through the longest part as possible of the proximal segment of the MTT (6). We therefore performed a preliminary in vivo anatomic study and derived biometric measurements of the MTT by 3D processing of unenhanced 3D T1-weighted spoiled gradients data in a normative cohort of nine healthy volunteers without structural abnormalities of the brain (7).
In light of the functional directionality of the MTT projections from the MB to the AN, we clearly delineated 1) a proximal segment, named the “mammillary” segment, which has posterior-cranial orientation in the sagittal plane and lateral-cranial one in the coronal plane, and 2) a distal “thalamic” segment, which has frontal and caudal-cranial orientation (Fig 1A, B). An empirical method using the 3D capabilities of the Advantage Windows, release 4.0, software (GEMS, Milwaukee, WI) running on an off-line Ultra 60 Creator 3D station (Sun Microsystems, Santa Clara, CA) was designed to define the cortical entry point combining targeting of the MB epicenter together with so-called catheterization of the longest possible part of the mammillary segment of the MTT. We obtained the entry point by superimposing the straight line connecting the MB to cortical surface on the longitudinal axis of the mammillary segment of the MTT. It was located in the posterior third of the middle frontal gyrus in the coronal plane (Fig 1C) and in the central sulcus or in the precentral gyrus when drawing the line in the sagittal plane (Fig 1D). The first option was chosen to avoid primary functional areas, and the value of this empirical approach was assessed postoperatively by coregistrating the pre- and postoperative images in the similar juxta/supra-MB section location in all the three patients being recruited up to now (Fig 2). A passage through the most proximal segment of the MTT—at least partially—was demonstrated for the six electrodes, which have four stimulation contacts, the distal one (numbered 0) being located within the MB, and the three proximal ones (numbered 1–3) within the MTT.
None of the three patients experienced any memory deficit, neither immediately after surgical implantation, nor during global or elective stimulations (left side versus right side versus both sides; 0, 1, 2, or 3 only, versus any combination of the four stimulation contacts), which were performed under close neuropsychological monitoring. Additional comprehensive cognitive tests were repeatedly performed, all of which failed to reveal any early or delayed mental decline after implantation.
These data demonstrate the absence of significant memory dysfunction induced by bilateral MB/MTT implantation and electrical stimulation. They agree with the recent experimental work by Vann and Aggleton (8) on a rat model, and with Harding et al (9) on alcoholics with and those without Korsakoff psychosis, who demonstrated that AN degeneration or lesions by far more critically impaired memory than MB or MTT lesions.
The disconnection of the AN from its MB projections through the MTT (the so-called Delay and Brion connection) does not impair the direct fornical route from the hippocampus to the AN. This alternate pathway may explain why MB or MTT lesions, even if bilateral, are not as disruptive as AN lesions. Korsakoff syndrome in the patient reported by Yoneoka et al (4) could have been triggered by bilateral ischemic damage not limited to the MTTs. The newly infarcted left area and the old right lesion seem to extend far beyond the MTTs to involve the anterior thalamic nuclei (Figs 1A–D and 2A, B, p. 965; and Figs 4A, B, p. 967 [AJNR; Vol 25 No 6]). In accordance with the theoretical statement by Vann and Aggleton in their recent review on the topic (10), we think the presence of concurrent anterior thalamic disease to have outweighed the MTT damage in that patient.
References
1.Victor M, Adams RD, Collins GH. The Wernicke-Korsakoff syndrome: a clinical and pathological study of 245 patients, 82 with post-mortem examinations.
Contemp Neurol Ser
1971;7:1–206 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
2.Shear PK, Sullivan EV, Lane B, Pfefferbaum A. Mammillary body and cerebellar shrinkage in chronic alcoholics with and without amnesia.
Alcohol Clin Exp Res
1996;20:1489–1495 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
3.Mishkin M. Memory in monkeys severely impaired by combined but not by separate removal of amygdala and hippocampus.
Nature
1978;273:297–298 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
4.Yoneoka Y, Takeda N, Inoue A, et al. Acute Korsakoff syndrome following mammillothalamic tract infarction.
AJNR Am J Neuroradiol
2004;25:964–968 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
5.Berger JR. Memory and the mammillothalamic tract: editorial.
AJNR Am J Neuroradiol
2004;25:906–907 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
6.Raftopoulos C, van Rijckevorsel K, Abu Serieh B, et al. Chronic electrical stimulation of the mammillary bodies and mammillothalamic tracts in chronic refractory epilepsy [Abstract]. 1st meeting of the Benelux Neuromodulation Society Chapter of the International Neuromodulation Society, November 21–22, 2003, Leuven, Belgium. Neuromodulation
2004;7:148 [Google Scholar]
7.Abu Serieh B, Duprez T, van Rijckevorsel K, Raftopoulos C. Anatomical study of mammillothalamic tract in humans using 3D T1-weighted SPGR MR imaging. Abstract, 16th Congress of the European Society for Stereotactic and Functional Neurosurgery. June 23–26, 2004, Vienna, Austria. Acta Neurochir (Wien) 146:878 ,2004 [Google Scholar]
8.Vann SD, Aggleton JP. Evidence of spatial encoding deficits in rats with lesions of the mammillary bodies or mammillothalamic tracts.
J Neurosci
2003;23:3506–3514 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
9.Harding A, Halliday G, Caine D, Kril J. Degeneration of anterior thalamic nuclei differentiates alcoholics with amnesia.
Brain
2000;123:141–154 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
10.Vann SD, Aggleton JP. The mammillary bodies: two memory systems in one?
Nat Rev/Neurosci
2004;5:35–44 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
We thank Duprez et al for their attention and valuable comments concerning our article “Acute Korsakoff Syndrome Following Mammillothalamic Tract Infarction” (1) and also for presentation of their advanced work in neuroradiology and neuromodulation (2, 3). Their excellent depictions of the mammillothalamic tract (MTT) and implanted neuromodulation electrodes with MR imaging allow us the opportunity to further discuss amnesia-initiating lesion(s) in our case.
Korsakoff syndrome is defined as a disproportionate impairment in memory, relative to other aspects of cognitive function, resulting from a nutritional (thiamine) depletion (4). In our case, however, the patient developed acute amnesia with features of Korsakoff syndrome that were not attributable to malnutrition but were the result of a left MTT infarction. MR imaging studies revealed acute ischemic damage of the left MTT assessed by diffusion-weighted imaging and a previous contralateral right MTT infarct on T2-weighted and fluid-attenuated inversion recovery images, which caused prolonged global amnesia manifesting as Korsakoff syndrome. Thus, we reported this rare case as an acute “Korsakoff syndrome” following MTT infarction. At clinical follow-up, he continued to suffer from the severe amnesic syndrome, 8 months after the onset of symptoms.
As Duprez et al suggested, we cannot completely rule out the anterior thalamic nucleus disease, at least on the left side, because the anterior thalamic nucleus (AN) lies in a similar vascular territory to the ipsilateral MTT (5–7). T2-weighted images (Figs 1B, 2B, p. 965 [AJNR; Vol 25 No 6]) may suggest the possibility of the left AN ischemic damage (1). It is unclear whether the right AN was affected or not on MR imaging (Figs 1, 2, p. 965; Fig 3, p. 966; and Fig 4, p. 967 [AJNR; Vol 25 No 6]) (1). We think, however, the MTT infarcts spared at least some of the left AN and most of the right AN because MR images examined 8 weeks after onset showed no significant changes of the ependymal contour around the foramen of Monro and there were unchanged AN volumes even in the left side (Fig 4, p. 967 [AJNR; Vol 25 No 6]) (1).
Concerning the crucial lesion(s) in our case of amnesic syndrome reported as acute “Korsakoff syndrome,” there are two main candidates: 1) bilateral MTTs or 2) bilateral MTTs and AN(s). Clarke et al (8) reported a case of amnesia after unilateral left anterior thalamic infarction in which unilateral left thalamic infarction caused pure amnesia with slight frontal type dysfunction and thalamic aphasia. In this amnesic case of unilateral small ischemic lesion of the left thalamus, the patient’s memory improved partially within 8 months. If perchance the left AN was affected by the left MTT infarction, there could be some improvement with resolution of the ischemic lesion during 8 months of follow-up. Although a small ischemic lesion caused prolonged global amnesia without aphasia in our case, the patient’s amnesic symptoms hardly improved during the follow-up. MR images examined 8 weeks after onset demonstrated a right MTT lesion and a left MTT lesion, possibly involving some of the AN. For these reasons, we concluded that both MTT lesions cut off the hippocampal input through the mammillary bodies (MBs), into the AN, thereby producing a severe amnesic state.
T2-weighted images presented by Duprez et al show the remarkable accuracy of stereotactic procedures of deep-brain stimulation (DBS) as related to the MB/MTTs. There exist four general hypotheses to explain the therapeutic mechanism(s) of DBS: (1) stimulation-induced alterations in the activation of voltage-gated currents that block neuronal output near the stimulating electrode (depolarization blockade); (2) indirect inhibition of neuronal output by means of activation of axon terminals that make synaptic connections with neurons near the stimulating electrode (synaptic inhibition); (3) synaptic transmission failure of the efferent output of stimulated neurons as a result of transmitter depletion (synaptic depression); (4) stimulation-induced disruption of pathologic network activity (9). In view of these four DBS mechanisms, we have found it difficult to understand the reason(s) why their three patients never show any neuropsychiatric deficits when their DBS (2) affected MB/MTTs bilaterally.
We consider separately the following three propositions: (1) concurrent AN disease outweighed both MTT damage in our case, (2) no MB/MTT, but an AN lesion is crucial for alcoholic Korsakoff amnesia, and (3) bilateral MB/MTT functionally lessening modulation never causes neuropsychiatic deficits including amnesia: In other words, bilateral MTT dysfunction can spare memory. We have discussed the first proposition above. Concerning the second point, bilateral MTT lesions can cause amnesia closely resembling Korsakoff syndrome. For the third point, the evidence that an AN degeneration or lesion more critically impairs memory than MB/MTT lesions in Korsakoff psychosis (10) does not mean that bilateral MTT lesions do not affect memory. Further experiences of MB/MTT-DBS will clarify the role of the MB/MTT(s) in memory. On the assumption that the MB/MTTs play an important part in memory, it is difficult to explain why bilateral MB/MTT-DBSs never cause any neuropsychiatric deficits. Perhaps MB/MTT-alternative pathway(s) for memory exists in their three patients with chronic refractory epilepsy.
In any event, we are attentive to further studies of bilateral MB/MTT-DBS lead implantation by Duprez’s colleagues (2). Their future work will advance the frontiers of amnesia research as more experience is obtained.
References
1.Yoneoka Y, Takeda N, Inoue A, et al. Acute Korsakoff syndrome following mammillothalamic tract infarction.
AJNR Am J Neuroradiol
2004;25:964–968 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
2.Raftopoulos C, van Rijckevorsel K, Abu Serieh B, et al. Chronic electrical stimulation of the mammillary bodies and mammillothalamic tracts in chronic refractory epilepsy. Abstract, 1st meeting of the Benelux Neuromodulation Society Chapter of the International Neuromodulation Society, November 21–22, 2003, Leuven, Belgium. Neuromodulation
2004;7:148 [Google Scholar]
3.Abu Serieh B, Duprez T, van Rijckevorsel K, Raftopoulos C. Anatomical study of mammillothalamic tract in humans using 3D T1-weighted SPGR MR imaging. Abstract, 16th Congress of the European Society for Stereotactic and Functional Neurosurgery. June 23–26, 2004, Vienna, Austria. Acta Neurochir (Wien) 146:878 ,2004 [Google Scholar]
5.Percheron G. Arteries of the human thalamus. I. Artery and polar thalamic territory of the posterior communicating artery [in French].
Rev Neurol (Paris)
1976;132:297–307 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
6.Pedroza A, Dujovny M, Artero JC, et al. Microanatomy of the posterior communicating artery.
Neurosurgery
1987;20:228–235 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
7.Pedroza A, Dujovny M, Cabezudo-Artero J, et al. Microanatomy of the premamillary artery.
Acta Neurochir (Wien)
1987;86:50–55 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
8.Clarke S, Assal G, Bogousslavsky J, et al. Pure amnesia after unilateral left polar thalamic infarct: topographic and sequential neuropsychological and metabolic (PET) correlations.
J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry
1994;57:27–34 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
9.Kopell BH, Greenberg B, Rezai AR. Deep brain stimulation for psychiatric disorders.
J Clin Neurophysiol
2004;21:51–67 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
10.Harding A, Halliday G, Caine D, Kril J. Degeneration of anterior thalamic nuclei differentiates alcoholics with amnesia.
Brain
2000;123:141–154 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]