Skip to main content
AJNR: American Journal of Neuroradiology logoLink to AJNR: American Journal of Neuroradiology
. 2004 Jun;25(6):977–984.

Age-Related, Regional, Hemispheric, and Medial-Lateral Differences in Myelin Integrity in Vivo in the Normal Adult Brain

Carol L Armstrong a,c, Elfrides Traipe c, Jill V Hunter d, John C Haselgrove c, George E Ledakis c, Emily M Tallent c, David Shera b,c, Mark A van Buchem e
PMCID: PMC7975677  PMID: 15205135

Abstract

BACKGROUND AND PURPOSE: Clinical validation of magnetization transfer (MT) imaging is important for investigating clinical disease and organization of normal brain function. We determined whether an in vivo quantitative measure sensitive to white matter is distributed in functionally important ways.

METHODS: Axial 1.5-T MR images with and those without MT were obtained. MT ratios (MTRs) were computed for 33 regions of interest (ROIs) in 27 healthy adults (aged 18–69 years) without evidence of cognitive or radiographic abnormalities. Three tests of reliability yielded coefficients above 0.97. MTRs for the whole brain, groups of structures, and individual ROIs were calculated. Low standard errors confirmed the consistency of the technique.

RESULTS: Age, education, sex, and hand dominance were not correlated with whole-brain MTR (mean = 37.35, SD = 1.25), but age was associated with the cerebellum and some lobes at a trend level. MTRs were as follows, in descending order: corpus callosum, cingulate, white matter, brain stem, subcortical nuclei, and cerebellum. MTRs were selectively higher in the prefrontal lobe versus the posterior frontal lobe and in the lateral temporal lobe versus medial temporal lobe. MTR was higher in the left hemisphere than in the right hemisphere for the whole brain, frontal and temporal lobes, and lenticular nuclei.

CONCLUSION: MT imaging showed selective age, medial-lateral, and hemispheric differences, giving evidence of normal aging effects on the white matter in the absence of T2- weighted hyperintensities. These differences support neurocognitive theories of the organization of brain function. MT imaging appears to be a robust technique for use in cognitive neuroscience.


There is burgeoning interest in understanding the white matter (WM) of the brain, how to measure it quantitatively, and its functional importance (16). Studies have shown that WM is primarily responsible for aging-related brain loss based on the greater volume reduction of myelinated nerve fibers than of nerve cells (710). In this study, we sought to examine the functional implications of regional differences in the distribution of myelin in the brain by using magnetization transfer (MT) imaging. Previous studies of healthy cohorts analyzed the MT ratio (MTR) histograms of the WM for the whole brain by using semiautomated segmentation methods (1114) or hand-placed, small, circular, or regular regions of interest (ROIs) (1518). We sought to measure in greater detail the regions of the brain most vulnerable to ischemic and demyelinating damage, for the original purpose of comparing the findings with those of patients who received brain radiation therapy. We included a greater number of ROIs than previous studies did and sampled structures more completely to examine the clinical validity of MTR for its sensitivity to WM and cognition.

MT imaging is a technique that is sensitive to the exchange of magnetization between a pool of free protons and a pool of protons that are bound to macromolecules. The amount of this exchange can be quantified by calculation of the MTR (19), defined as a value of percentage. Exogenous cellular components are the major contributors to the MT effect in brain tissue, and therefore the concentration and integrity of myelin is reflected in MTR values (16, 20, 21). The MTR is the quantitative measure of the amount of MT that occurs between the bound and the mobile water molecules in a given ROI. Normal WM, because of its dense structure of primarily lipids, has a higher MTR than GM, which is about 10% of WM structures (12) and not different from unmyelinated WM in the pediatric brain (17).

Several studies of regional variations in MTR in healthy subjects have been done. Higher MTR is hierarchically distributed with highest values in the corpus callosum; the central and subcortical U fibers of the frontal, temporal, parietal, and occipital lobes; the pons; and the subcortical structures that contain myelinated WM fibers (15, 18). The subcortical nuclei (ie, basal ganglia and thalami) have reliably lower MTR. In the study by Mehta et al (15), the thalamus had the highest value of the gray matter (GM) structures (cortical and subcortical) because of the presence of myelinated WM fibers related to its cortical connectivity. To our knowledge, lateralization of MTR has been examined only once previously (18), and although not significant, values were consistently higher in left than in right WM regions. No hemispheric lateralization of MTR was found in relation to hand preference.

Although the greatest regional differences in WM reflect lesion burden, normal aging effects in MTR are greater in children than in adults, and they have been related to myelination and, more specifically, to the concentration of galactocerebrocides (17). MT can measure the increasing heterogeneity of the maturing brain (22), and greatest regional differences are found in the tracts projecting to the primary cortical areas (16). Age-related differences in adults are more controversial. Mehta et al (15) reported no age-related differences in the MTR of any brain structure among adult age-defined groups, although the data show increasing values in their group of adults aged 21–78 years whose MR images did not show hyperintensities. In contrast, other studies have reported a decrease in MTR with age in older adults (1113, 18).

In studies that examined MTR in relation to cognitive function (13, 14, 23), the MTR was sensitive to cognitive burden (14) or more sensitive than cognitive measures to normal aging effects (13). MTR was sensitive to early neurodegenerative changes in the temporal lobe, as it was indistinguishable in older adults with mild cognitive impairment and in patients with Alzheimer disease, and values for the whole brain and frontal lobe were generally between those of the Alzheimer and healthy control groups (14). If MTR is sensitive to relatively small differences in cognition, differences in myelin integrity might be discernable in brain structures that are instrumental for rapid sequential processing, such as language and movement.

In this study, we reexamined demographic variables, hemispheric differences if any, and possible differences between the mesial and lateral regions of the WM of the cerebral lobes. We expected any differences found to be consistent with the known neurocognitive functions of the implicated brain regions. We hypothesized that regions of the left cerebral hemisphere that are dominantly involved in language processing would have a higher MTR because of their reliance on the rapid temporal variations of language sensory signals (24) and also because of the sequencing demands of processing language at other levels (morphologic, sentence level). Histologic evidence of this notion (1, 25) is that the increased length of the left posterior superior temporal gyrus, associated with dominant linguistic functions, is due to thicker myelin rather than to increased glial proliferation, neural density, or axonal diameter. Finally, we compared central areas of subcortical WM with lateral areas and hypothesized that MTR is higher in lateral areas because of relatively greater volume of WM from cortical-cortical fiber associations than the lesser cortical-subcortical connectivity of the limbic areas of the central or mesial portions of the lobes.

Methods

Subjects.—

Adults were screened by using a neurodiagnostic interview with health history, neuropsychological, and neuroradiologic examinations to serve as healthy control subjects in studies of patients with brain tumors. Relatives of patients and known members of the community were recruited. To prospectively examine the dependence of the MTR on demographic variables and its validity for studying WM based on regional variations, we used highly selective inclusion criteria to identify healthy subjects. Exclusion criteria included the following: 1) structural abnormalities on T2-weighted or other MR images, 2) history of neurologic comorbidity, 3) psychiatric comorbidity, 4) learning disability, 5) history of head injury, 6) cardiovascular disease, 7) pulmonary disease, 8) use of medications 9) diabetes, 10) history of cancer, 11) history of substance abuse, 12) autoimmune disorder, and 13) renal failure or any other disorder that could cause cerebral ischemia. One subject was excluded because of poor memory performance on examination and a history of mild head injury in his youth, although his MR images were normal. Another subject had a benign pineal cyst but was not excluded. This process resulted in a group of 27 healthy subjects aged 18–69 years (mean, 41.07 years; SD, 15.80; median, 41 years). The age distribution by decade was as follows 18–19 years, n = 2; 22–29 years, n = 7; 31–38 years n = 4; 41–49 years, n = 6; 52–57 years, n = 3; and 61–69 years, n = 5. Education ranged from 10 to 20 years (mean, 16.33 years; SD, 2.96), 44% were male and 56% were female, and 89% were right-hand mixed dominant versus 11% left-hand mixed dominant (as determined by interview).

Image Acquisition—

All MR studies were performed by using a 1.5-T whole-body system (Vision; Siemens Islin, NJ) with a standard quadrature head coil. For the magnetization studies, two image sets were acquired. These were identical except for the the standard magnetization pulse. The first set of images was completed with the magnetization pulse on, and the second set was acquired with the pulse turned off. Parameters were as follows: gradient-echo sequence (fast low-angle shot [FLASH]), TR/TE = 1200/10; flip angle = 30°; 31 sections 3-mm thick; section gap, 1.5 mm; and matrix, 128 × 256). The field of view was adjusted to give the best resolution for the particular patient and was in the range of 220–230 mm. The MT pulse was the standard MT pulse, which consisted of a 1.5-m/s pulse applied 1.5 kHz off-resonance.

Regions of Interest.—

ROIs were chosen on the basis of vulnerability to ischemic injury and demyelination, as related to the parent study of the damaging effects of radiation therapy on the brain (26, 27). The boundaries for cerebral structures were defined by Brodmann areas (BAs) by using Damasio and Damasio’s templates (28) with cytoarchitectonic markings corresponding to the horizontal plane (parallel to the inferior orbitomeatal line). This was useful to differentiate the limbic areas of the temporal and parietal lobes that defined the mesial and lateral portions of these lobes and also the motor and prefrontal areas of the frontal lobe that defined the posterior and prefrontal lobes, respectively. The selected ROIs were the WM of the left and right hemispheres and subcortical structures, as follows: 1) Prefrontal, anterior to frontal horns of lateral ventricles (BAs 8–12, 25, 32, 46, 47); 2) posterior frontal, the remainder of frontal lobes and centrum semiovale (BAs 4, 6, 44, 45); 3) anterior cingulate cortex (BAs 24, 33); 4) posterior cingulate cortex (BAs 23, 31); 5) medial temporal lobe, mesial to the temporal horn of the lateral ventricle, including hippocampus (BAs 27, 28, 34, 38); 6), lateral temporal lobe, lateral to the temporal horn of the lateral ventricle (BAs 20–22, 35–37, 41, 42); 7) medial parietal lobe, medial to lateral ventricles (mesial areas of BA in lateral parietal lobe and 26, 29, 30); 8) lateral parietal lobe, lateral to the lateral ventricles (BAs 1–3, 5, 7, 39, 40); 9) occipital lobe (BAs 17–19); 10) lentiform nucleus (globus pallidus and putamen); 11) head of the caudate; 12) thalamus; 13) internal capsule; 14) genus of the corpus callosum (not hemispheric); 15) body of the corpus callosum (not hemispheric); 16) splenium of the corpus callosum (not hemispheric); 17) cerebellum; and 18) brain stem. CSF was also measured.

A whole-brain MTR was computed by using the means of all ROIs. The ROIs were weighted by region size. For analyses, ROIs were grouped into WM structures (frontal, temporal, parietal, and occipital regions); anterior and posterior portions of the cingulate; segments of the corpus callosum; subcortical nuclei and related structures (basal ganglia, thalamus, and internal capsule); cerebellum; and brain stem. Bonferroni corrections were made for all multiple comparisons. Results were significant when P < .05 was divided by the number of regions in a comparison.

Image Postprocessing.—

Images were transferred to a workstation (Sparc 1300; Sun Microsystem Santa Clara, CA), where processing was performed by using programs written with programming language IDL (Research Systems Inc, Boulder CO). Both a neuroradiologist (E.T.) and a research fellow (G.L.) trained by a licensed neuroradiologist (J.H.) observed and outlined the images. Outlining the ROIs was done on the non-MT, T2-weighted images. The T2-weighted (spin-echo) images were used to position the ROIs on the data matrix, and the ROIs were then automatically translated to the MT images for computation of MTR by using an in-house software routine. We included only the WM, avoiding sulci and leaving a surrounding rim of WM/GM gradation to minimize the partial volume effect from GM or CSF (Fig 1). While prior groups used regular ROI shapes, we outlined the form by keeping to WM boundaries or the boundary of the subcortical GM nucleus.

Fig 1.

Fig 1.

ROI outlining method.

Inter-rater reliability was calculated as an intraclass correlation coefficient of 0.999, revealing no significant difference between the observers in this study. Because the outlining was dependent on subjective observation, we also examined the potential differences in the observer’s perception of the boundary of WM. We examined inter-rater reliability by postprocessing the data twice, by one observer using a more liberal approach to defining an ROI (including smaller margins around ROIs and therefore a greater amount of WM area sampled from the ROI), and another observer using a more conservative approach (including a larger rim around WM at the expense of total area sampled) to avoid partial-volume effects. This comparison was made for 132 ROIs; that is, all ROIs for two cases. Results again did not differ (intraclass correlation coefficient = 0.999). A third reliability study was conducted to determine if equivalent values could be calculated by using the MT sequence in the same individual on two dates. One subject was re-imaged a few weeks after an initial session, and the intraclass correlation coefficient of 0.977 was considered acceptable.

MTRs were calculated as percentages on a pixel-by-pixel basis from the intensities of the images with (Im) and those without (Io) the MT pulse by using this equation: MTR = 100 (Io –Im)/Io. Although any of the three images (Io, Im, MTR) could have been used to define ROIs from which the program calculates the mean, SD, and area of the MTR, both technical processors consistently used the Io image.

Mean MTR and its SD were calculated for an ROI on each imaged section where the ROI was visible, for up to 28 sections of 3-mm thickness. Ultimately, the MTR means and SDs used for analyses represented all calculated pixel values for an ROI or those weighted by area for individual and combined ROIs.

Analysis.—

We computed association of mean signal intensity and area of each ROI with age, stratified age, education, hand dominance, and sex by means of either correlation coefficients or t tests, as appropriate. The ROIs were grouped into six sets of related regions and a multivariate analysis of variance (MANCOVA) was run to compare the sets. Lastly, we checked for medial-lateral, anterior-posterior, and hemispheric effects between ROIs by use of paired t tests. Bonferroni adjustment of the test level was incorporated to avoid excessive false-positive results.

Results

Associations of Demographic Variables with MTR for Brain Regions.—

The whole-brain MTR was normally distributed, with a mean of 37.35 (SD = 1.25) and a range of 35.3–40.23. Age was not significantly correlated with the MTR mean for the whole brain (r = 0.06, P = .78). To further examine age effects, subjects were stratified into a younger group (age 18–38 years; n = 13; five male, eight female) and an older group (age 41–69 years; n = 14; six male, eight female). Student t tests demonstrated no differences in either the mean MTR for all ROIs (younger mean = 39.31, SD = 4.67; older mean = 40.90, SD = 5.12; not significant) or the mean for the WM ROIs (younger mean = 39.98, SD = 3.71; older mean = 40.90, SD = 3.16; not significant). However, the two groups were different in the mean WM ROI area, as observed by a single neuroradiologist (E.T.) who was blinded to the subject’s age (younger mean = 57.99 mm2, SD = 20.94; older mean = 39.19 mm2, SD = 14.02; P = .005). In addition, age was not significantly associated with the major groupings of WM and subcortical ROIs, with the exception that older age was significantly associated with lower MTR in the cerebellum (r = −0.48, P = .01). However, trends for positive correlation of age and MTR were found in the cingulate (r = 0.42, P = .03) and cerebral WM (r = 0.37, P = .06). Among the cerebral WM regions, trends for positive correlations with age were found for the left (r = 0.41, P = .04) and right (r = 0.40, P = .04) occipital lobes and for the right temporal lobe (r = 0.36, P = .06).

Education was unrelated to whole-brain and other regional groups of ROIs, except for a moderate correlation of higher education with higher MTR in the cerebellum (r = 0.49, P = .01). Hand dominance showed no association with whole brain, cortical, corpus callosal, subcortical, cerebellar, or brain stem MTR values, although there were only three of 27 lefthand- or mixed-dominant individuals. We also examined whether hand dominance was related to higher contralateral MTRs. No effect of hand dominance was found: The right-hand–dominant group had nonsignificantly higher left than right whole -pain and cortical MTRs, as did the small non–right-hand–dominant group. No significant sex differences were found in MTR for whole brain or for groupings of ROIs.

Hierarchy of MTR among Anatomic Structures.—

The brain was segmented as described earlier and grouped into six sets of related structures: 1) segments of the WM of the cerebral lobes combined (frontal, temporal, parietal, occipital), 2) segments of the corpus callosum combined (genus, body, splenium), 3) WM of the anterior and posterior cingulate gyrus combined, 4) subcortical nuclei and structures combined (lentiform nuclei, heads of the caudates, thalami, and internal capsules), 5) cerebellum, and 6) brain stem. MANOVA confirmed that these groups varied significantly in MTR value, with F (4, 104) = 181.02 and P < .0001. Mean MTRs ± SDs were as follows, from highest to lowest: corpus callosum, 39.99 ± 1.51; cingulate gyri, 39.87 ± 1.89; WM, 38.61 ± 1.47; brain stem, 37.62 ±1.36; subcortical nuclei and GM, 36.58 ± 1.26; and cerebellum, 31.93 ± 1.66 . MTRs for the WM of the cerebral lobes were also hierarchically distributed, with F (3, 78) = 8.08 and P < .0001. Values were as follows: frontal, 38.93 ± 1.51; parietal, 38.87 ± 2.09; occipital 38.67 ± 1.65; and temporal, 38.06 ± 1.47. The subcortical structures varied significantly, with F (3, 78) = 334.75 and P < .0001. Values from highest to lowest were in the internal capsules, thalami, and caudate, to the lentiform nuclei (Table 1).

TABLE 1:

Mean magnetization transfer ratios and analysis of variance for substructures of cortical and subcortical regions

Region Mean SE Significance (F)
Mesial temporal 37.35 0.10 P = .0005
Lateral temporal 38.21 0.10
Prefrontal 39.63 0.16 P = .0005
Medial frontal 38.36 0.16
Mesial parietal 38.97 0.14 P = .61
Lateral parietal 38.87 0.14
Genu 40.98 0.22 P = .0005
Body 39.52 0.22
Splenium 39.62 0.22
Anterior cingulate 39.46 0.26 P = .11
Posterior cingulate 40.09 0.27
Lentiform nucleus 35.11 0.13 P = .0005
Head of caudate 35.22 0.13
Thalamus 38.07 0.13
Internal capsule 39.80 0.13

Medial-Lateral and Anterior-Posterior Differences in MTR.—

Medial or central versus lateral areas of three of the cerebral hemispheric WM regions were compared (Table 1). MTRs were significantly higher in the prefrontal versus posterior part of the frontal lobes, with F (1, 26) = 30.37 and P < .0001, and in the lateral versus medial part of the temporal lobes, with F (1, 26) = 35.08 and P < .0001. However, no difference was found in the lateral versus medial part of the parietal lobes; F (1, 26) = 0.271 and P < .61. No significant difference in MTR was found in the anterior versus posterior cingulate; F (1, 26) = 2.74 and P < .11. In the corpus callosum, values were significantly higher in the genu, with F (2, 52) = 13.99 and P < .0001, than in the body and splenium which did not differ from each other (Table 1).

Hemispheric Differences.—

The entire left hemisphere mean MTR was significantly greater than that of the right hemisphere, with paired t (26) = 5.41 and P < .0001, although the left hemispheric augmentation of MTR was significant only for the cerebral WM, with paired t (26) = 5.60 and P < .0001 (Table 2). Left-hemispheric MTR augmentation was found in the frontal and temporal lobes primarily, a trend was found for the parietal lobes, and the occipital lobes showed no hemispheric effect (Table 2). Higher MTR for the posterior frontal and lateral temporal areas accounted for most of this left-hemispheric effect: posterior frontal paired t (26) = 4.52 and P < .0001, prefrontal, lateral temporal paired t (26) = 8.24 and P < .0001, and mesial temporal not significant (Table 2). The intraindividual left-hemispheric superiority in MTR for the posterior frontal region was found in 78% of cases, for the prefrontal region in 48% of cases, for the lateral temporal region in 96% of cases, and for the mesial temporal region in 56% of cases. Subcortical structures together showed a similar trend, with paired t (26) = 2.3 and P < .03. In the subcortical ROIs (internal capsules, thalami, heads of the caudate, lenticular nuclei), the lentiform nucleus showed a significant left-hemispheric effect (Table 2), which was found in 78% of the cases: paired t (26) = 2.86 and P < .008.

TABLE 2:

Hemispheric differences in magnetization transfer ratio of regions as measured by paired t tests

Region Left Mean SD Right Mean SD Paired t P Significance*
Hemisphere mean 37.55 1.25 37.25 1.31 5.41 .0005 *
Cortical regions 38.77 1.44 38.45 1.52 5.57 .0005 *
Cingulate 39.79 2.04 39.92 1.79 1.08 .287
Subcortical 36.65 1.29 36.52 1.25 2.30 .030
Brain stem 37.64 1.42 37.61 1.34 0.44 .666
Cerebellum 31.85 1.61 32.01 1.76 1.25 .222
Frontal 39.06 1.50 38.81 1.54 3.42 .002 *
 Prefrontal 39.90 1.60 40.05 1.67 1.35 .188
 Posterior frontal 38.86 1.59 38.54 1.63 4.52 .0005 *
Parietal 38.97 1.96 38.76 2.23 2.32 .029
 Lateral parietal 38.98 2.00 38.76 2.29 2.25 .033
 Mesial parietal 39.03 1.99 38.90 2.14 1.29 .207
Occipital 38.69 1.61 38.65 1.75 0.34 .733
Temporal 38.35 1.41 37.77 1.55 7.52 .0005 *
 Lateral temporal 38.54 1.42 37.87 1.59 8.24 .0005 *
 Mesial temporal 37.48 1.74 37.25 1.63 1.14 .267
Internal Capsule 39.95 1.50 39.67 1.58 1.94 .063
Thalamus 38.02 1.33 38.11 1.30 1.40 .174
Head of the caudate 35.32 1.30 35.11 1.33 1.25 .221
Lentiform nucleus 35.21 1.43 35.01 1.27 2.86 .008
*

Significance indicates those results that reach an accepted α of P < .0036 after applying a Bonferroni correction.

Discussion

We found selective differences in MTR of the WM of the brain and subcortical nuclei. This observation suggests organization of regions by WM density consistent with inherent adaptive brain functions, most notably those of motor functions, language, and executive functions. This study reproduced the hierarchical organization of myelin density found in prior studies that used MT imaging to examine normal brains and expanded on these prior findings with evidence of hemispheric effects and mesial versus lateral differences. We also revisited the question of how demographic variables might account for differences in myelination.

Demographic variables had no effect on the mean MTR of all regions, but specific ROIs showed borderline significant correlations with age. The lack of association of handedness and sex with MTR of the WM is consistent with prior imaging results (18), which are further supported by those of a diffusion tensor imaging study reporting a lack of sex effect in healthy aging men and women (29). Prior pathologic studies that directly examined myelin in adults reported no sex differences in the myelination of the hippocampal formation or WM beyond early adulthood (30) until late adulthood (8). Education was positively correlated only with cerebellar results (P < .01), although this finding is brought into question with Bonferroni correction. Without other studies for comparison, this finding remains in isolation.

The finding of nearly significant positive correlation of age with MTR in the occipital and right temporal lobes (with trends for other WM structures) and the equivalent MTRs for our younger and older subjects deviates from reports of negative correlations of age with WM (1113, 18). To our knowledge, only one study besides ours reported a positive (though nonsignificant) correlation of age and MTR (15), but the presence of WM hyperintensities appears to account for these differences in the age effect. In all of the studies showing a negative correlation of age and MTR, WM hyperintensities were not screened out. Age was strongly and negatively correlated with these hyperintensities in one study (13). In the studies finding some positive correlation, subjects with WM hyperintensities were prospectively excluded. We also found that the total gross volume of WM (multiple sections × area) of the older age group was 68% that of the younger group. While multiple mechanisms have been examined for age-related shrinkage of the WM such as loss of smaller diameter axons, structural age-related changes in both primates and humans evidence no loss of glial cells, relative reduction of the number of small diameter nerve fibers, and ubiquitous increases in myelin though myelin integrity degenerates (6).

Pathologic findings of aging effects on myelin suggest that a slow progression of MTR, which indicates increasing density of myelin, is expected in adulthood before old age, followed by a precipitate decline (8, 9). The positive correlations of age with MTR we found in some cerebral structures is consistent with the increases in WM reported in pathologic until the seventh or eighth decade. At that point, myelin breaks down rapidly (8), resulting in a change in conduction rates, loss of synchrony in cortical neuronal circuits, and cognitive impairment. Cognitive impairment in old age may be more related to a breakdown of myelin integrity or thinning of myelin in the normal aging brain (including more accelerated loss of WM chemistry) than to loss of cortical neurons or synapses (79, 31). Some morphographic studies show regional WM increases until the fifth decade and decreases thereafter (32). However, pathologic studies show increases in myelin until age 60 or 70 years as a result of accumulated myelin without axonal loss; this partly reflects damage to and repair of myelin causing increasing numbers of myelin laminae, ballooning of sheaths, redundancy of myelin, and splitting of myelin (9, 33). Furthermore, larger-diameter axons have thicker myelin sheaths (33), and small-diameter nerve fibers have a greater reduction with age (6). Thus, the stability of MTR across age groups and the positive association with age that we found in some cortical WM areas (occipital and right temporal lobes) may represent the accumulation of myelination described in studies of normal aging before the onset of the morphographic abnormalities or cognitive impairment (which were eliminated in our screening process). The adults selected for this study permitted an examination of aging effects that was not dominated by the rapid changes that occur after age 70 in both neuronal structure and chemistry or by the heightened risk of latent brain abnormalities in late aging.

Findings in the cerebellum demonstrated a negative association with age; this is consistent with results demonstrating early age-related sensitivity of the cerebellum beginning at age 50 years (34, 35). Although this age-related loss was associated with the GM in one study (35), our findings suggest that the cerebellar WM is also sensitive to age-related loss. The cerebellum, comprising micromodules that process in a parallel array, results in a network capable of execution and learning at exceptionally high speed (36). Our results may suggest that the cerebellum—with its different cytoarchitecture that is less reliant on serial neural transmission than on parallel processing—may tolerate more thinning of myelin before cognitive impairment results. The parallel network of neural transmission in the cerebellum may be sufficiently efficient to compensate for a reduction in the speed of short transmissions when myelin is lost. This could account for a negative aging effect on MTR without corresponding cognitive impairment. Alternately, whether the cerebellum is less prone to WM chemical disintegration than the cerebral areas is untested, as is the idea of whether the cerebellum is more dependent on neuronal loss in aging than is the cerebrum.

Hemispheric effects were found for the cortex, specifically the frontal and temporal lobes, and a trend was found for the lentiform nucleus. The frontal findings were predicted on the basis of the dominance of motor processing in the frontal lobe and were supported by the left hemispheric superiority only in the posterior, or motor, portion of the frontal lobe. The prediction was that the temporal lobe would also show left-hemispheric superiority in MTR due to the language specialization of the left temporal lobe, especially of the superior (and transverse superior), inferior temporal, and supramarginal temporal gyri (37). As expected, the hemispheric effect was due to the greater MTR in the left lateral rather than mesial and limbic portion of the temporal lobe.

The basal ganglia—lentiform nucleus (putamen and globus pallidus) and caudate—have been associated with fluent speech and, possibly, with language, although whether extensive disruption of cortical tracts are necessary for a language disturbance remains controversial (38). Speech problems due to defects in motivation have been excluded (38). However, the head of the caudate and the globus pallidus are closely related elements in Crosson’s proposed mechanism for release of preformulated language (39), and both have been frequently implicated in subcortical aphasia. Therefore, a left hemispheric specialization for the lentiform nucleus and not the head of the caudate is difficult to explain. One possible explanation is that the caudate appears to be bihemispherically involved in several functions of language and memory (4043), whereas the language-dominant left globus pallidus and putamen may be more specifically involved in subcortical aphasic disorders involving speech production (38). The putamen is involved in motor production circuits, and it may also contribute to the hemispheric effect in MTR for the lentiform nucleus due to left-hemisphere motor dominance. A left-hemispheric effect for the lentiform nucleus is also consistent with the robust left-hemispheric effect found in the left posterior frontal region, as speech production is dependent on the connectivity of the left basal ganglial–posterior frontal loop.

An aphasic syndrome sometimes occurs after lesions of the dominant thalamus, and thus, we might question why the thalamic MTR did not demonstrate a language-based hemispheric laterality effect. Both right and left thalamic aphasias are characterized by failure of semantic, attention, or visuoperceptual (rather than phonemic) contributions to reading and speech (4446). Therefore, the thalamus may not be as dependent on speeded sequential processing dominantly processed in the left hemisphere, and it may be more dependent on bihemispheric cortical processing. Indeed, intrathalamic organization shows multiple corticothalamic and thalamocortical connections associated with aphasias because of widespread connections with the frontal, temporal, and parietal lobes (45).

Our sample differed from that of prior regional studies of MTR in healthy adults because our subjects were screened by means of neurodiagnostic interview, MR imaging, and neurocognitive testing. In their study of regional differences, Mehta et al (15) relied on a sample with “single, isolated complaints” of cognitive function for whom MR imaging was requested to rule out correlative structural abnormalities and in whom no abnormalities were found. Our adult group did not extend to old age; thus, we eliminated the unequivocal “normal” aging abnormalities of that period and some degree of noise, but this limitation also precluded an examination of an expected decline in MTR after age 70 years. Furthermore, our small sample prevented a closer examination of age effects.

The lack of change in MTR with age in this group up to age 69 years with a corresponding loss of WM area is a paradoxical effect that could not be resolved in this study. To examine this paradox, a systematic study of age throughout the adult age span in super-healthy individuals is needed of MT and volumetric studies of WM.

Conclusion

In this study, a quantitative measure of the WM of the brain was distributed in functionally important ways. The relevance of myelin density to brain function may be an ontologic or an adaptive function or both, as suggested by published work (47). MT is a structural imaging technique with which we can understand the role of myelin. There was a lack of MTR change with age, yet smaller WM volume was associated with older age in our study sample as screened for WM hyperintensities on MR images. This suggests that the WM atrophy is salient even in very healthy individuals without cognitive impairment before old age and is therefore not due to thinning of myelin.

MT imaging may also be a technique to evaluate the role of myelin in studies of cognition. It is sensitive to cognitive impairment or clinical symptoms in disease models, including multiple sclerosis (23, 48, 49), schizophrenia (50), and dementia (14). Although not yet tested, MT imaging may also be relevant to studies of normal cognitive function because of its capacity to measure the small variations in myelin and axonal integrity in studies of healthy individuals prior to major changes from myelin loss due to old age.

Acknowledgments

We thank Christie Seiler, PsyD, for her thoughtful comments on this manuscript and Frank Lexa, MD, who helped to initiate these studies.

Footnotes

Funded by a grant from the National Cancer Institute (RO1 CA 65438) and a grant from the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (3P30 HD26979–04S2) to the University of Pennsylvania and the Children’s Hospital of Philadelphia Mental Retardation and Developmental Disorder Research Center (D.S.).

References

  • 1.Anderson B, Southern BS, Powers RE. Anatomic Asymmetries of the posterior superior temporal lobes: a postmortem study. Neuropsychiatry Neuropsychol Behav Neurol 1999;12:247–254 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Gunning-Dixon FM, Raz N. The cognitive correlates of white matter abnormalities in normal aging: a quantitative review. Neuropsychology 2000;14:224–232 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Lim KO, Choi SJ, Pomara N, Wolkin A, Rotrosen JP. Reduced frontal white matter integrity in cocaine dependence: a controlled diffusion tensor imaging study. Biol Psychiatry 2002;51:890–895 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Markowitsch HJ, Tulving E. Cognitive processes and cerebral cortical fundi. NeuroReport 1995;6:413–418. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.de Groot JC, de Leeuw FE, Breteler MMB. Cognitive correlates of cerebral white matter changes. J Neural Transm Suppl 1998;53:41–67 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Peters A. Structural changes in the normally aging cerebral cortex of primates. In: Azmitia EC, DeFelipe J, Jones EG, Rakic P, Ribak CE, eds. Progress in Brain Research. Vol. 136. Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Elsevier Science;2002 [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 7.Meier-Ruge W, Ulrich J, Brühlmann M, Meier E. Age-related white matter atrophy of the human membrane. In: Annals of New York Academy of Sciences: Physiopathological Processes of Aging—Towards a Multicausal Interpretation. Vol. 673. New York: New York Academy of Sciences;1992. :260–269 [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 8.Svennerholm L, Bostrom K, Junghjer B. Changes in weight and compositions of major membrain components of human brain during the span of adult human life of Swedes. Acta Neuropathol 1997;94:345–352 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Peters A, Rosene DL, Moss MB, et al. Neurobiological bases of age-related cognitive decline in the rhesus monkey. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol 1996;55:861–874 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Anderson JM, Hubbard BM, Coghill GR, Slidders W. The effect of advanced old age on the neurone content of the cerebral cortex. J Neurol Sci 1983;58:233–244 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Hofman PAM, Kemerink GJ, Jolles J, Wilmink JT. Quantitative analysis of magnetization transfer images of the brain: effect of closed head injury, age, and sex on white matter. Magn Reson Med 1999;42:803–806 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Ge Y, Grossman RI, Babb JS, Ragin ML, Mannon LJ, Kolson DL. Age-related total gray matter and white matter changes in normal adult brain, II: quantitative magnetization transfer ratio histogram analysis. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol 2002;23:1334–1341 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Tanabe J, Ezekiel F, Jagust W, Schuff N, Fein G. Volumetric method for evaluating magnetic transfer ratio of tissue categories: application to areas of white matter signal hyperintensity in the elderly. Radiology 1997;204:570–575 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.van der Flier WJ, van den Heuvel DMJ, Weverling-Rijnsburger AWE, et al. Magnetization transfer imaging in normal aging, mild cognitive impairment, and Alzheimer’s disease. Ann Neurol 2002;52:62–67 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Mehta RC, Pike GB, Enzmann DR. Magnetization transfer MR of the normal adult brain. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol 1995;16:2085–2091 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Rademacher J, Engelbrecht V, Burgel U, Freund JJ, Zilles K. Measuring in vivo myelinization of human white matter fiber tracts with magnetization transfer MR. NeuroImage 1999;9:393–406 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Engelbrecht V, Rassek M, Preiss S, Wald C, Modder U. Age-dependent changes in magnetization transfer contrast of white matter in the pediatric brain. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol 1998;19:1923–1929 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Silver NC, Barker GJ, MacManus DG, Tofts PS, Miller DH. Magnetization transfer ratio of normal brain white matter: a normative database spanning four decades of life. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1997;62:223–228 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Wolff SD, Balaban RS. Magnetization transfer contrast (MTC) and tissue water proton relaxation in vivo. Magn Reson Med 1989;10:135–144 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Dousset V, Grossman R, Ramer K, et al. Experimental allergic encephalomyelitis and multiple sclerosis: lesion characterization with magnetization transfer imaging. Radiology 1992;182:483–491 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Rocca MA, Falini A, Colombo B, Scotti G, Comi G, Filippi M. Adaptive functional changes in the cerebral cortex of patients with nondisabling multiple sclerosis correlate with the extent of brain structural damage. Ann Neurol 2002;51:330–339 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.van Buchem MA, Steens SCA, Vrooman HA, et al. Global estimation of myelination in the developing brain on the basis of magnetization transfer imaging: a preliminary study. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol 2001;22:762–766 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.van Buchem MA, Grossman RI, Armstrong C, et al. Correlation of volumetric magnetization transfer imaging with clinical data in MS. Neurology 1998;50:1609–1617 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Tallal P, Miller S, Fitch RH. Neurobiological basis of speech: a case for the pre-eminence of temporal processing. In: Annals of New York Academy of Sciences: Temporal Information Pressing in the Central Nervous System—Special Reference to Dyslexia and Dysphasia. Vol. 682. New York: New York Academy of Sciences;1993. :27–47 [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 25.Southern BD, Anderson B. Planum temporale asymmetry in women is determined by white matter volume. Neurology 1999;52:A489 [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Armstrong CL, Hunter JV, Ledakis GE, et al. Late cognitive and radiographic changes related to radiotherapy: initial prospective findings. Neurology 2002;59:40–48 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Armstrong CL, Ledakis GE. The significance of magnetization transfer imaging in measuring cognition and white matter damage. J Intl Neuropsychol Soc 2002;8:244 [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Damasio H, Damasio AR. Lesion Analysis in Neuropsychology. New York: Oxford University Press;1989
  • 29.Sullivan EV, Adalsteinsson E, Hedehus M, et al. Equivalent disruption of regional white matter microstructure in aging healthy men and women. NeuroReport 2001;12:99–104 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Benes FM, Turtle M, Khan Y, Farol P. Myelination of a key relay zone in the hippocampal formation occurs in the human brain during childhood, adolescence, and adulthood. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1994;51:477–484 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Fazekas F, Schmidt R, Kleinert R, Kapeller P, Roob G, Flooh E. The spectrum of age-associated brain abnormalities: their measurement and histopathological correlates. J Neural Transm Suppl 1998;53:31–39 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Bartzokis G, Beckson M, Lu PH, Nuechterlein KH, Edwards N, Mintz J. Age-related changes in frontal and temporal lobe volumes in men: a magnetic resonance imaging study. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2001;58:461–465 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Peters A, Sethares C, Killiany RJ. Effects of age on the thickness of myelin sheaths in monkey primary visual cortex. J Comp Neurol 2001;435:241–248 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Luft A, Skalej M, Schultz J, et al. Patterns of age-related shrinkage in cerebellum and brainstem observed in vivo using three-dimensional MRI volumetry. Cereb Cortex 1999;9:712–721 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Sullivan EV, Deshmukh A, Desmond J, Pfefferbaum A. Cerebellar volume decline in normal aging, alcoholism, and Korsakoff’s syndrome: relation to ataxia. Neuropsychology 2000;14:341–352 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Riva D, Giorgi D. The cerebellum contributes to higher functions during development: evidence from a series of children surgically treated for posterior fossa tumours. Brain 2000;123:1051–1061 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Cytowic RE. The neurological side of neuropsychology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press;1996
  • 38.Crosson B. The basal ganglia and subcortical white matter in language. Subcortical Structures in Language. New York: Guilford Press;1992. :42–79
  • 39.Crosson B. Theories of subcortical functions in language. In: Subcortical Structures in Language. New York: Guilford Press;1992. :111–146
  • 40.Watkins KE, Vargha-Khadem F, Ashburner J, et al. MRI analysis of an inherited speech and language disorder: structural brain abnormalities. Brain 2002;125:465–478 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Watkins KE, Paus T, Lerch JP, et al. Structural asymmetries in the human brain: a voxel-based statistical analysis of 142 MRI scans. Cereb Cortex 2001;11:868–877 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Moro A, Tettamanti M, Perani D, Donati C, Cappa SF, Fazio F. Syntax and the brain: disentangling grammar by selective anomalies. NeuroImage 2001;13:110–118 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Abdullaev YG, Bechtereva NP, Melnichuk KV. Neuronal activity of human caudate nucleus and prefrontal cortex in cognitive tasks. Behav Brain Res 1998. :97:159–177 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Saffran EM, Coslett HB. Pure alexia: the case of JG. In: Funnell E, ed. Case Studies in the Neuropsychology of Reading. East Sussex: Psychology Press;2000. :13–26
  • 45.Crosson B. The thalamus in language. In: Subcortical Structures in Language. New York: Guilford Press;1992. :80–110
  • 46.Henderson VS, Alexander MP, Naeser MA. Right thalamic injury, impaired visuospatial perception, and alexia. Neurology 1982;32:235–240 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Gaser C, Schlaug G. Brain structures differ between musicians and non-musicians. J Neurosci 2003;23:9240–9245 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Rovaris M, Filippi M, Minicucci L, et al. Cortical/subcortical disease burden and cognitive impairment in patients with multiple sclerosis. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol 2000;21:402–408 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Rovaris M, Filippi M, Falautano J, et al. Relation between MR abnormalities and patterns of cognitive impairment in multiple sclerosis. Neurology 1998;50:1601–1608 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Foong J, Symms M, Barker G, et al. Neuropathological abnormalities in schizophrenia: evidence from magnetization transfer imaging. Brain 2001;124:182–192 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from AJNR: American Journal of Neuroradiology are provided here courtesy of American Society of Neuroradiology

RESOURCES