Abstract
BACKGROUND AND PURPOSE: A technique of segmenting total gray matter (GM) and total white matter (WM) in human brain is now available. We investigated the effects of age and sex on total fractional GM (%GM) and total fractional WM (%WM) volumes by using volumetric MR imaging in healthy adults.
METHODS: Fifty-four healthy volunteers (22 men, 32 women) aged 20–86 years underwent dual-echo fast spin-echo MR imaging. Total GM, total WM, and intracranial space volumes were segmented by using MR image-based computerized semiautomated software. Volumes were normalized as a percentage of intracranial volume (%GM and %WM) to adjust for variations in head size. Age and sex effects were then assessed.
RESULTS: Both %GM and %WM in the intracranial space were significantly less in older subjects (≥50 years) than in younger subjects (<50 years) (P < .0001 and P = .02, respectively). Consistently, %GM decreased linearly with age, beginning in the youngest subjects. %WM decreased in a quadratic fashion, with a greater rate beginning only in adult midlife. Although larger GM volumes were observed in men before adjustments for cranium size, no significant differences in %GM or %WM were observed between the sexes.
CONCLUSION: GM volume loss appears to be a constant, linear function of age throughout adult life, whereas WM volume loss seems to be delayed until middle adult life. Both appear to be independent of sex. Quantitative analysis of %GM and %WM volumes can improve our understanding of brain atrophy due to normal aging; this knowledge may be valuable in distinguishing atrophy of disease patterns from characteristics of the normal aging process.
The quantitative assessment of brain atrophy is becoming an important consideration in monitoring the clinical outcome and treatment effects in many diseases, such as Alzheimer disease, multiple sclerosis, schizophrenia, alcoholism, and AIDS-related dementia. The reason is because recent considerable advances in MR imaging and computer technology have allowed the study of brain morphometrics in vivo, which could provide an accurate, reproducible, and quantitative measure for assessing brain atrophy. Age-associated changes in brain tissue measurements in healthy adults have also been the subject of great interest in recent years, because the determination of normal age-specific values in brain have a role in the evaluation of both clinical-pathologic conditions and normal aging processes. Many investigators examine the age effects on the basis of specific brain regions, such as the corpus callosum (1), hippocampus (2), frontal and temporal lobes (3, 4), and cerebellum (5). The quantitative information from the analyses has shown that age-related brain tissue loss may vary greatly among different brain regions (6) and between the hemispheres (7). These volume measurements in brain tissue appear to vary with sex as well (8, 9); this observation indicates that the use of fractional measures to correct for population- and sex-related differences in head size is essential.
However, because brain parenchyma is generally composed of gray matter (GM) and white matter (WM), the quantitative analysis of brain atrophy underlying separate GM and WM may have implications for our understanding and monitoring of the aging process in the brain. Previous groups have examined age-related changes in GM and WM volume by using either a regional analysis of a specific brain lobe or by using a few sections of the brain. Several methods have been used with variable results (10–18). The brain must be assessed as a whole to provide a reliable indication of how total GM and WM volumes change with age on the basis of a normalized and standard measure. Furthermore, because aging seems likely to affect the entire brain during the aging process (6), global volume measures represent the cumulative effect of all physiologic changes that occur in the entire brain. Compared with other measures, these may be more accurate and important to correlate with the clinical outcome. GM and WM differ both anatomically and functionally, and they have different patterns in brain development (19); thus, findings in GM and WM are not necessarily coincident with each other in terms of the timing and extent of tissue loss in brain aging. The purpose of this study was to investigate our hypothesis that GM and WM volumes have different patterns of change that may contribute in different ways to brain atrophy in the normal aging process. To our knowledge, information specifically related to when and how the normalized brain tissue volumes change during aging, which is critical to our understanding of general brain atrophy in elderly people, has not been well addressed (20).
This present study was designed to investigate the effects of age and sex by using fractional measures of total GM and WM volume based on a thin-section (3-mm) standard MR imaging protocol performed in adults with no evidence of neurologic disease.
Methods
Subjects
Fifty-four healthy volunteers (32 women, 22 men; age range, 20–86 years; mean age, 46.8 years ± 19.3) were examined by using our imaging protocols. The mean age of the female subjects (48.3 years ± 19.1) was not significantly different (P = .82) from the mean age of the male subjects (44.7 years ± 19.9). An experienced neurologist (D.L.K.) examined the subjects by using a structured clinical interview to exclude any present or past neuropsychiatric illnesses and abuse of alcohol or illicit drugs. Additionally, a negative history of psychiatric disease in first-degree relatives was required. Physical examination showed normal findings in all participants, and no patient reported a history of any serious medical condition. The numbers of subjects in each decade were as follows: 13 in the 20–29-year-old group, 10 in the 30–39-year-old group, nine in the 40–49-year-old group, six in the 50–59-year-old group, seven in the 60–69-year-old group, five in the 70–79-year-old group, and four in the 80–89-year-old group. Informed consent was obtained from all volunteers, and the local ethics committee approved the protocol.
MR Imaging
All subjects underwent imaging with a 1.5-T MR unit (Signa; GE Medical Systems, Milwaukee, WI) with a quadrature transmit-receive head coil. Whole-brain axial dual-echo fast spin-echo (FSE) images were acquired with a TR/TE1 and TE2/NEX of 2500/18 and 90/1, a 3-mm section thickness, a 22-cm field of view, and a 192 × 256 matrix. The echo train length was eight, and the pixel size was 0.86 mm. More than 50 sections were obtained in each subject to cover the whole brain. To minimize variations in the volume calculations in the whole-brain parenchyma among subjects, we included only certain sections from the MR image sets: those starting from the section just before the image that showed the cerebellum at the bottom and ending with the last section that showed brain at the top.
Image Processing and Analysis
The first (proton density-weighted) and second (T2-weighted) echoes of the FSE sequence in each study were transferred directly to a Sparc workstation (Sun Microsystems, Mountain View, CA) via the picture archiving and communications system in our department. GM, WM, and CSF were segmented with the 3DVIEWNIX software system (21) by analyzing both proton density- and T2-weighted images. The process began with segmentation of the intracranial brain (GM, WM, and CSF) by using the theory of fuzzy connectedness (22). All segmented brain volume images (more than 50 sections in each subject’s brain) were individually reviewed, and an experienced neuroradiologist (Y.G.) excluded any residual extracranial components, if needed. GM, WM, and CSF were each identified as individual three-dimensional fuzzy connected objects (Fig 1) according to their “affinity,” “fuzzy adjacency,” and “hanging togetherness” (22). This technique created a volume image by using thin-section dual-echo proton density- and T2-weighted images for GM, WM, and CSF from all sections that covered the whole brain. In repeated studies, the coefficient of variation was shown to be 2.1% for the GM volume, 1.9% for the WM volume, and 1.5% for the CSF volume (23). To normalize for head size variability, the %GM volume and %WM volume were computed as the percentage of the intracranial volume; that is, %GM = (GM/intracranial volume) × 100%.
Statistical Methods
Descriptive statistics, including the mean and standard deviations of the MR imaging volumetric measures, were calculated and compared in different age and sex groups. Age- and sex-related effects were evaluated with t tests by using a Satterthwaite correction for unequal variances. Relationships between age and %GM, as well as age and %WM, were assessed with Pearson product-moment correlations. To determine whether the rate of change in each variable was the same at all ages, least-squares regression was implemented to assess the utility of a prediction model that was a quadratic function of age. If the quadratic term is significant, the rate of change first accelerates and then decelerates over time, or vice versa.
Results
Effects of Age on MR Imaging Volumes
To assess the effects of normal aging on MR imaging volume data, we assigned subjects into two age groups: 20–49 years (n = 32; mean, 33.0 years ± 8.9) and 50–86 years (n = 22; mean, 67.0 years ± 10.4). The mean, median, and standard deviation for volume measures in each age group are shown in Table 1. No significant difference was observed in the total intracranial volumes of these two age groups; this finding indicated that they were comparable with respect to head size. We noted a 91.7-mL, or 4.9%, difference in absolute GM and %GM between the older group and the younger group (P ≤ .001), as well as a 54.6 mL, or 2.6%, difference in absolute WM and %WM between the two groups (P ≤ .02). No significant difference in the GM/WM ratio was found between the older and younger subject groups.
TABLE 1:
Variables | 20–49 y |
50–86 y |
P Values | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mean | SD | Mean | SD | ||
Intracranial (mL) | 1367.3 | 147.4 | 1314.8 | 152.6 | .22 |
GM (mL) | 717.8 | 96.8 | 626.1 | 84.3 | .001 |
WM (mL) | 499.9 | 73.7 | 445.3 | 56.0 | .003 |
% GM | 52.5 | 4.1 | 47.6 | 3.4 | <.0001 |
% WM | 36.6 | 4.0 | 34.0 | 3.9 | .02 |
GM/WM | 1.5 | 0.3 | 1.4 | 0.2 | .54 |
The Pearson product-moment correlation was used to identify age-predictive effects on MR imaging volume measurements, as shown in Table 2. Regardless of sex, the %GM was significantly associated with age (r = −0.57, P < .0001), as was %WM (r = −0.35, P = .009), although the correlation between age and %WM was not as strong as that between age and %GM.
TABLE 2:
Variables | r | P |
---|---|---|
Intracranial (mL) | −0.19 | .17 |
GM (mL) | −0.48 | .0004 |
WM (mL) | −0.42 | .001 |
% GM | −0.57 | .0002 |
% WM | −0.35 | .009 |
GM/WM | −0.08 | .55 |
Least-squares regression was implemented to assess the utility of a model in predicting %WM and %GM as quadratic functions of age. The pattern of changes differed for the two tissues (Fig 2A and B). Specifically, %GM displayed a decline beginning with the youngest subjects, and this change was constant and linear across the span of early to late adulthood. We found that the rate of decline in %GM was not significantly different (P = .45) among younger subjects (20–49 years) compared with older subjects (aged 50 years and older). However, %WM demonstrated a quadratic pattern of change, increasing slightly in subjects until the age of approximately 40 years, and it decreased thereafter. The rate of change in %WM was significantly different in subjects younger than 50 years compared with those aged 50 years or older (P = .038). Once %WM began to decline, its rate of decrease was faster (−0.2%) than that of %GM (−0.09%). The rate of change was not significantly different for either total intracranial volume or the GM/WM ratio (Fig 2C) between the two age groups.
Effects of Sex on MR Imaging Volumes
Sex-related differences in MR imaging volume measurements are shown in Table 3. Significant differences were observed in total absolute intracranial volume and absolute GM volume (P ≤ .003) between female subjects (1291.5 mL ± 134.9 and 645.9 mL ± 89.6, respectively) and male subjects (1425.1 mL ± 138.3 and 730.6 mL ± 99.3, respectively); differences in absolute WM volume were not significant. However, no significant difference was noted in either %GM or %WM between female and male subjects after the values were normalized for head size, although a trend toward slightly higher %WM in female subjects (P = .085) was present. To determine whether the sexes differed in terms of the rate of change in %GM and %WM with age, results of a separate analysis of variance was plotted (Fig 3). Although %GM decreased with age slightly faster in male subjects than in female subjects, the difference in the rate of change was not statistically significant for either %GM or %WM.
TABLE 3:
Variables | Female Subjects |
Male Subjects |
P Values | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mean | SD | Mean | SD | ||
Intracranial (mL) | 1291.0 | 135.0 | 1425.0 | 138.0 | .001 |
GM (mL) | 645.9 | 89.6 | 730.6 | 99.3 | .003 |
WM (mL) | 469.8 | 73.3 | 489.1 | 69.6 | .333 |
% GM | 50.0 | 4.1 | 51.3 | 5.0 | .302 |
% WM | 36.4 | 4.0 | 34.4 | 4.1 | .085 |
GM/WM | 1.4 | 0.2 | 1.5 | 0.3 | .078 |
Discussion
In this study, we measured both absolute and fractional %GM and %WM volumes in healthy adults aged 20–86 years and evaluated the data by age and sex. The results showed the patterns of age-associated change in GM and WM volumes at a global level. The decline in %GM appears to occur by a relatively young age (age 20 years in our study), and the decrease is constant and linear. The %WM, in contrast, shows a quadratic pattern of change, slightly increasing until an age of approximately 40 years, then decreasing quickly thereafter. These combined changes might explain why cerebral volume remains relatively stable up to the age of 40–50 years (24), when brain atrophy starts, although GM loss occurs much earlier. The effects of sex were not significant in terms of %GM and %WM, although female subjects tended to have slightly higher %WMs, compared with male subjects. The rates of change in %GM and %WM were not statistically different between male and female subjects.
In the present study, we observed no difference in the total intracranial volume between the younger and older groups. This finding indicates that age-dependent changes in cranium size do not occur in healthy adults and that a comparison of the absolute (and fractional) segmented volumes in each tissue subtype in the two age groups is reasonable. In contrast, we found a significant difference in cranial sizes and GM volumes between the male subjects and female subjects at every age. Apparently, absolute volumes should not be used; instead, fractional controlling volume measures should be used to assess the effects of sex. The difference between male subjects and female subjects was not statistically significant after we controlled for head size by using the %GM.
We found a 4.9% difference in %GM between the younger group and the older groups; this observation indicated that a significant age-related GM loss occurs with advancing age. Our finding of a linear decline in %GM (Fig 2A) that started from the youngest subject age (age 20 years) indicates that the GM degeneration process could begin in the early stage of adult life. This idea was also proved in another study by Pfefferbaum et al (24), which was based on seven sections of the brain. Similarly, a recent study by Courchesne et al (20), which involved volumetric MR imaging, revealed that the absolute GM volume increased from early childhood (19–33 months) to later childhood (6–9 years) and decreased thereafter. They limited their study to an analysis of the absolute tissue volumes rather than the controlled measures for head size. Furthermore, they focused on brain development and offered few data on the normal aging process. However, the phenomenon of an early decline in GM volume in the healthy aging process is still not well known; this decline may be due to the neuronal and synaptic pruning in the human cortex reorganization period (25) after the neural maturation (ie, neurulation, proliferation, migration). This early regression event may be a normal process that may not reflect degeneration, but rather, the elimination of redundant neurons and synaptic connections in early adult life. Our study demonstrated that this decrease in GM volume is linear (as reflected by a global normalized measure) throughout adult life, with no difference in the rates of GM decline. This result is consistent with that of a longitudinal study (26) in which certain regions of brain GM were measured. However, findings from postmortem studies in middle and late adult life (27–29) have suggested that the GM loss (shrinkage) might be correlated with a decrease in the size of large neurons rather than a notable decrease in the number of neurons.
Although age-related brain tissue loss has often been described as GM loss or cortical atrophy (30), WM loss was also found in normal aging (13, 14). We found that the %WM volume change that occurred with advancing age in adults was not linear but quadratic. It showed a slight increase between the ages of 20 and 40 years, and then it decreased until later life (Fig 2B). This finding agrees with the results from other volumetric studies (20, 24, 31) that suggested the WM maturation may continue into the midlife after a large increase from early childhood to adolescence. However, the measures of WM volume in those studies were based on regional or non-normalized analyses. The incidence of similar findings with the uses of different methods suggests that, relative to the early decrease in GM, WM volume loss occurs relatively late in adulthood in healthy individuals. Compared with these other studies (20, 24, 31), our study was more focused on adult brains and involved a normalized global measure that was designed to quantify the total tissue loss and avoid the difficulty in regional brain segmentation. However, because of this pattern of change in WM in the present study, the rates of change in %WM (Fig 2B) were significantly different between young subjects (who had a slow increase) and older subjects (who had a fast decrease).
Once the %WM decreases in late life, the rate of decrease seemed to be consistent and fast in comparison with that of %GM. This finding was also suggested in other studies (13, 15, 24) in which age-related atrophy was greater in WM than in GM, although the time when this volume loss started was not investigated. All of these results imply that both GM and WM volume changes contribute to brain atrophy. WM loss occurs later in life and may be more apparent, whereas GM loss is constant and starts from late childhood. Although the biologic mechanism of brain maturation in WM expansion to midlife is not fully understood, it is likely associated with continued myelination and axonal growth (25). However, in the elderly, the periventricular hyperintensities in WM have been shown to be unrelated to the total WM volume loss (32). Additional data from diffusion-weighted and magnetization transfer (MT) imaging studies (33, 34) reveal that fractional anisotropy and the MT ratio (measures of the integrity of the microstructure) in WM significantly decrease with age. This decrease suggests that WM volume loss may result from these microscopic structural changes, which include myelin and axonal destruction (17, 35, 36), dilatation of perivascular spaces (37, 38), and gliosis (39, 40).
Regarding sex-related differences, despite no difference in the rate of %WM change across ages in male subjects and female subjects, female subjects generally had a higher %WM at every age (Fig 3B); however, this tendency was not statistically significant (P = .085). Further, a consensus exists regarding a significant sex-related difference in intracranial space and GM volume (8, 41), as determined with absolute size measurements. No sex difference in %GM was observed once GM volume was adjusted for head size. This observation indicates that differences in brain sizes between men and women are primarily attributable to the differences of total GM volume. We also observed a trend toward greater %GM loss in male subjects (Fig 3A) although the difference was not statistically significant, and it was not as large as that of other groups, which showed that the loss of brain tissue in men was greater than that of women (9, 42–45). However, our results were based on the total intracranial %GM and %WM volumes, which may have diluted the sex effects, whereas others’ results were based on absolute values in the whole brain or subregional areas of different lobes.
In our study, the GM/WM ratio changed only slightly within the age range of 20–86 years. No significant effects of either age or sex on the GM/WM ratio were found. This ratio shows that at no period was the volume of WM greater than that of GM. As shown in Figure 2C, the ratio of GM to WM slowly decreased until the age of approximately 50 years and increased slightly thereafter. In the present study, given the combined changes in GM and WM with age, one could expect that no substantial, detectable brain atrophy occurs until midlife, although a constant change occurs in both brain tissues during the adult life span. However, the time when these changes in GM and WM become clinically apparent, and the mechanisms by which they occur in late life are still not entirely resolved. If the GM loss starts earlier and the WM loss starts late but with a faster rate in normal aging (as we found in this study), the cognitive decline in abnormal aging (ie, Alzheimer disease) is probably not exclusively due to GM loss or cortical atrophy. WM loss attributable to axonal loss could also be involved (46). Furthermore, the findings of this volumetric study of GM and WM suggest that the normal aging process may start with WM loss, which begins at midlife and becomes more profound in later life, as compared with the GM loss.
Several limitations should be noted in the present study. First, this was a cross-sectional study without a direct comparison within subjects. Ideally, a longitudinal study over several decades should be performed. Second, we excluded subjects with past or present neurologic symptoms but not subjects with some periventricular WM hyperintensities on MR images, a feature common in older adults (47). Thus, this approach may have made the comparison of images in younger subjects and those in older subjects more difficult. However, Guttmann et al (13) demonstrated that no correlation existed between high-signal-intensity foci in WM on MR images and brain volume in healthy adults. Further, these hyperintense spots in the periventricular regions do not distinguish patients with Alzheimer disease from healthy elderly adults; thus, these spots are more likely to be associated with the normal aging process rather than with notable diseased patterns (48). Third, although a quantitative estimation of brain tissue loss in aging can be performed in healthy, living subjects to avoid the fixation artifacts of postmortem study, the accuracy of computer-generated segmentation of young and older brains may not be the same. This issue needs to be further evaluated.
Conclusion
This quantitative volumetric study demonstrated possible normal patterns of global GM and WM volume across adult life, from 20–86 years, in healthy individuals. The volume changes in %GM and %WM indicate that GM loss occurs in a linear pattern starting from early adult age, whereas WM change occurs in a quadratic pattern, with a volume increase in early adulthood and with an accelerated rate of decrease beginning in middle adulthood. These changes illustrate both processes of brain maturation and aging that represent different zones of the continuum during the adult life, and they may be taken as benchmarks in assessing pathologic conditions when they reach a critical threshold of impaired function in each decade. Differences in the brain sizes between male and female subjects may be primarily attributable to the differences of total volume of GM.
Footnotes
Supported in part by grant NS29029 from the National Institutes of Health.
References
- 1.Biegon A, Eberling JL, Richardson BC, et al. Human corpus callosum in aging and Alzheimer’s disease: a magnetic resonance imaging study. Neurobiol Aging 1994;15:393–397 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Giedd JN, Vaituzis AC, Hamburger SD, et al., Quantitative MRI of the temporal lobe, amygdala, and hippocampus in normal human development: ages 4–18 years. J Comp Neuro 1996;366:223–230 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Daigneault S, Braun CMJ, Whitaker HA. Early effects of normal aging on preservative and nonpreservative prefrontal measures. Dev Neuropsychol 1992;8:99–114 [Google Scholar]
- 4.Convit A, de Leon MJ, Hoptman MJ, Tarshish C, De Santi S, Rusinek H. Age-related changes in brain: I. Magnetic resonance imaging measures of temporal lobe volumes in normal subjects. Psychiatr Q 1995;66:343–355 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Luft AR, Skalej M, Schulz JB, et al. Patterns of age-related shrinkage in cerebellum and brainstem observed in vivo using three-dimensional MRI volumetry. Cerebr Cortex 1999;9:712–721 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Coffey CE, Wilkinson WE, Parashos IA, et al. Quantitative cerebral anatomy of the aging human brain: a cross-sectional study using magnetic resonance imaging. Neurology 1992;42:527–536 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Miller AK, Alston RL, Corsellis JA. Variation with age in the volumes of grey and white matter in the cerebral hemispheres of man: measurements with an image analyzer. Neuropathol Appl Neurobiol 1980;6:119–132 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Cowell PE, Turetsky BI, Gur RC, Grossman RI, Shtasel DL, Gur RE. Sex differences in aging of the human frontal and temporal lobes. J Neurosci 1994;14:4748–4755 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Xu J, Kobayashi S, Yamaguchi S, Iijima K, Okada K, Yamashita K. Gender effects on age-related changes in brain structure. Am J Neuroradiol 2000;21:112–118 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Jernigan TL, Press GA, Hesselink JR. Methods for measuring brain morphologic features on magnetic resonance images: validation and normal aging. Arch Neurol 1990;47:27–32 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Raz N, Gunning FM, Head D, et al. Selective aging of the human cerebral cortex observed in vivo: differential vulnerability of the prefrontal gray matter. Cereb Cortex 1997;7:268–282 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Pruessner JC, Collins DL, Pruessner M, Evans AC. Age and gender predict volume decline in the anterior and posterior hippocampus in early adulthood. J Neurosci 2001;21:194–200 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Guttmann CR, Jolesz F, Kikinis R, Killiary RJ, Moss MB, Sandor T, Albert MS. White matter changes with normal aging. Neurology 1998;50:972–978 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Meier-Ruge W, Ulrich J, Bruhlmann M, Meier E. Age-related white matter atrophy in the human brain. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1992;26:260–269 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Double KL, Kalliday GM, Kril JJ, et al., Topography of brain atrophy during normal aging and Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol Aging 1996;17:513–521 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.DeCarli CD, Murphy G, et al. Lack of age-related differences in temporal lobe volume of very healthy adults. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol 1994;15:689–696 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Salat DH, Kaye JA, Janowsky JS. Prefrontal gray and white matter volumes in healthy aging and Alzheimer disease. Arch Neurol 1999;56:338–344 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Sullivan EV, Marsh L, Mathalon DH, Lim KO, Pfefferbaum A. Age-related decline in MRI volumes of temporal lobe gray matter but not hippocampus. Neurobiol Aging 1995;16:591–606 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Giedd JN, Snell JW, Lange N, et al. Quantitative magnetic resonance imaging of human brain development: ages 4–18. Cereb Cortex 1996;6:551–560 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Courchesne E, Chisum HJ, Townsend J, et al. Normal brain development and aging: quantitative analysis at in vivo MR imaging in healthy volunteers. Radiology 2000;216:672–682 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Udupa JK, Oduhner D, Samaraskera S, et al. 3DVIEWNIX: an open, transportable, multidimensional, multimodality, multiparametric imaging software system. Proc SPIE 1994;2164:58–73 [Google Scholar]
- 22.Udupa JK, Wei L, Samarasekera S, Miki Y, van Buchem Grossman RI. Multiple sclerosis lesion quantification using fuzzy-connectedness principles. IEEE Trans Med Imaging 1997;16:598–609 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Ge Y, Grossman RI, Udupa JK, Babb JS, Nyul LG, Kolson DL. Brain atrophy in relapsing-remitting multiple sclerosis: a fractional volumetric analysis of gray matter and white matter. Radiology 2001;220:606–610 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Pfefferbaum A, Mathalon DH, Sullivan EV, Rawles JM, Zipusky RB, Lim KO. A quantitative magnetic resonance imaging study of changes in brain morphology from infancy to late adulthood. Arch Neurol 1994;51:874–887 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Webb SJ, Monk CS, Nelson CA. Mechanisms of postnatal neurobiological development: implications for human development. Dev Neuropsychol 2001;19:147–171 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Mueller EA, Moore MM, Kerr DC, et al. Brain volume preserved in healthy elderly through the eleventh decade. Neurology 1998;51:1555–1562 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Haug H. Are neurons of the human cerebral cortex really lost during aging? A morphometric evaluation. In: Traber J, Gispen WH, eds. Senile Dementia of the Alzheimer Type Berlin,Germay: Springer-Verlag;1985. :150–163
- 28.Terry RD, Deteresa R, Hansen LA. Neocortical cell counts in normal human adult aging. Ann Neurol 1987;21:530–539 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Peters A, Morrison JH, Rosene DL, Hyman BT. Feature article: are neurons lost from the primate cerebral cortex during normal aging? Cereb Cortex 1998;8:295–300 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Kitagaki H, Hirono N, Ishii K, Mori E. Corticobasal degeneration: evaluation of cortical atrophy by means of hemispheric surface display generated with MR images. Radiology 2000;216:31–38 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Bartzokis G, Beckson M, Lu PH, Nuechterlein KH, Edwards N, Mintz J. Age-related changes in frontal and temporal lobe volumes in men: a magnetic resonance imaging study. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2001;58:461–465 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Braffman BH, Zimmerman RA, Trojanowski JQ, Gonatas NK, Hickey WF, Schlaepfer WW. Brain MR: pathologic correlation with gross and histopathology, II: hyperintense foci in the elderly. Am J Neuroradiol 1988;9:629–636 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Scheltens P, Barkhof F, Leyes D, Wolters EC, Ravid R, Kamphorst W. Histopathologic correlates of white matter changes on MRI in Alzheimer’s disease and normal aging. Neurology 1995;45:883–888 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Virta A, Barnett A, Pierpaoli A. Visualizing and characterizing white matter fiber structure and architecture in the human pyramidal tract using diffusion tensor MRI. Magn Reson Imaging 1999;17:1121–1133 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.van Swieten JC, van Den Hout JHW, van Ketel BA, Hydra A, Wokke JHJ, van Gijn J. Periventricular lesions in the white matter on magnetic resonance imaing in the elderly. Brain 1991;114:761–774 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Sze G, DeArmond S. Brant-Zawadski M, Davis RL, Norman D, Newton TH. Foci of MRI signal (pseudo lesions) anterior to the frontal horns: histologic correlations of a normal finding. Am J Neuroradiol 1986;7:381–387 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Fazekas F, Kleinert R, Offenbacher H, et al. Pathologic correlates of incidental MRI white matter signal hyperintensities. Neurology 1993;43:1683–1689 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Awad IA, Johnson PC, Spetzler RF, Hodak JA. Incidental subcortical lesions identified on magnetic resonance imaging in the elderly, II: postmortem pathological correlations. Stroke 1986;17:1090–1097 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Fazekas F, Kleinert R, Offenbacher H, et al. The morphologic correlate of incidental white matter hyperintensities on MR images. Am J Neuroradiol 1991;12:915–921 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Grafton ST, Sumi SM, Stimac GK, Alvord EC. Jr., Shaw CM, Nochilin D. Comparison of postmortem magnetic resonance imaging and neuropathologic findings in the cerebral white matter. Arch Neurol 1991;48:293–298 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Hofman PA, Kemerink GJ, Jolles J, Wilmink JT. Quantitative analysis of magnetization transfer images of the brain: effect of closed head injury, age and sex on white matter. Magn Reson Med 1999;42:803–806 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Narayana PA, Borthakur A. Effect of radio frequency inhomogeneity correction on the reproducibility of intra-cranial volumes using MR image data. Magn Reson Med 1995;33:396–400 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Passe TJ, Rajagopalan P, Tupler LA, Byrum CE, MacFall JR, Krishnan KR. Age and sex effects on brain morphology. Prog Neuropsychopharmacology Biol Psychiatry 1997;21:1231–1237 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Oguro H, Okada K, Yamaguchi S, Kobayashi S. Sex differences in morphology of the brain stem and cerebellum with normal ageing. Neuroradiology 1998;40:788–792 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Murphy DG, Decarli C, Mclntosh AR, et al. Sex differences in human brain morphometry and metabolism: an in vivo quantitative magnetic resonance imaging and positron emission tomography study on the effect of aging. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1996;53:585–594 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Schuff N, Ezekiel F, Gamst AC, et al. Region and tissue differences of metabolites in normally aged brain using multislice 1H magnetic resonance spectroscopic imaging. Magn Reson Med 2001;45:899–907 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Autti T, Raininko R, Vanhanen SL, Kallio M, Santavuori P. MRI of the normal brain from early childhood to middle age, II: age dependence of signal intensity changes on T2-weighted images. Neuroradiology 1994;36:649–651 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Leys D, Soetaert G, Petit H, Fauquette A, Pruvo JP, Steinling M. Periventricular and white matter magnetic resonance imaging hyperintensities do not differ between Alzheimer’s disease and normal aging. Arch of Neurol 1990;47:524–527 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]