Skip to main content
Wiley Open Access Collection logoLink to Wiley Open Access Collection
. 2020 May 22;91(1):e12355. doi: 10.1111/bjep.12355

How executive functions contribute to reading comprehension

Suzan Nouwens 1,, Margriet A Groen 2,3, Tijs Kleemans 2, Ludo Verhoeven 2
PMCID: PMC7983997  PMID: 32441782

Abstract

Background

Executive functions have been proposed to account for individual variation in reading comprehension beyond the contributions of decoding skills and language skills. However, insight into the direct and indirect effects of multiple executive functions on fifth‐grade reading comprehension, while accounting for decoding and language skills, is limited.

Aim

The present study investigated the direct and indirect effects of fourth‐grade executive functions (i.e., working memory, inhibition, and planning) on fifth‐grade reading comprehension, after accounting for decoding and language skills.

Sample

The sample included 113 fourth‐grade children (including 65 boys and 48 girls; Age M = 9.89; SD = .44 years).

Methods

The participants were tested on their executive functions (working memory, inhibition and planning), and their decoding skills, language skills (vocabulary and syntax knowledge) and reading comprehension, one year later.

Results

Using structural equation modelling, the results indicated direct effects of working memory and planning on reading comprehension, as well as indirect effects of working memory and inhibition via decoding (χ2 = 2.46).

Conclusions

The results of the present study highlight the importance of executive functions for reading comprehension after taking variance in decoding and language skills into account: Both working memory and planning uniquely contributed to reading comprehension. In addition, working memory and inhibition also supported decoding. As a practical implication, educational professionals should not only consider the decoding and language skills children bring into the classroom, but their executive functions as well.

Keywords: executive functions, primary education, reading comprehension development, simple view of reading, structural equation modelling

Background

Becoming proficient in reading comprehension is an important goal of primary education. Reading comprehension, however, is a complex process, which requires several higher cognitive skills. As proposed by the ‘Simple View of Reading’ (Hoover & Gough, 1990) and consistently supported by other studies, reading comprehension is largely predicted by decoding and listening comprehension (i.e., the linguistic processes involved in the comprehension of oral language, which is commonly assessed with language skills such as vocabulary and syntax knowledge). In addition, it has been found that executive functions significantly account for individual variation in reading comprehension beyond the contributions of decoding skills and language skills (e.g., Cutting, Materek, Colé, Levine, & Mahone, 2009; Kieffer, Vukovic, & Berry, 2013; Sesma, Mahone, Levine, Eason, & Cutting, 2009). Executive functions can be seen as a multi‐componential construct including, among others, working memory, inhibition, cognitive flexibility (also referred to as shifting or switching), and planning (Lehto, Juujärvi, Kooistra, & Pulkkinen, 2003; Miyake et al., 2000). The significant relation between executive functions and reading comprehension has been demonstrated repeatedly (for a review, see Butterfuss & Kendeou, 2018; Follmer, 2018; Jacob & Parkinson, 2015). However, indirect effects of executive functions on reading comprehension via decoding and language skills have hardly been examined (Follmer & Sperling, 2019; Georgiou & Das, 2018; Ober, Brooks, Plass, & Homer, 2019), and studies including children in the upper primary grades (grades 4–6; children between the ages of 10–12 years) are even scarcer (Kieffer et al., 2013). Moreover, although several longitudinal studies regarding the relations between executive functions and early literacy have been conducted (e.g., Blair & Razza, 2007; De Franchis, Usai, Viterbori, & Traverso, 2017; Monette, Bigras, & Guay, 2011; Röthlisberger, Neuenschwander, Cimeli, & Roebers, 2013), studies applying a longitudinal approach in the upper primary grades are limited. In the present study, it was therefore investigated to what extent multiple executive functions in Grade 4 predicted reading comprehension in Grade 5. Specifically, we will be regarded both direct effects of executive functions on reading comprehension and indirect effects via decoding and language skills.

The role of decoding and language skills in reading comprehension

It is generally accepted that text comprehension depends on the ability to decode words (Lyon, 1995; Torgesen, 2000), as it allows the reader to draw word representations from the text (Perfetti, 1992; Stanovich, 2000). Another major component in establishing reading comprehension is the ability to construct semantic relations among successive words, phrases, and sentences to form a coherent and meaningful representation of the discourse (Kintsch, 1998). These components are captured in the Simple View of Reading (Hoover & Gough, 1990), which suggests that reading comprehension is the product of decoding and listening comprehension (Hoover & Gough, 1990). Listening comprehension comprises the processes involved in the comprehension of oral language, which include language skills such as vocabulary and grammar knowledge, and higher‐level comprehension skills such as the ability to draw inferences within and between sentences and to integrate information across sentences and ideas in a text (Perfetti, Landi, & Oakhill, 2005; Verhoeven & Perfetti, 2008). Studies have demonstrated that decoding and language skills predict individual variation in children’s reading comprehension both in the lower grades (Muter, Hulme, Snowling, & Stevenson, 2004) and in the upper grades of primary school (Goff, Pratt, & Ong, 2005; de Jong & van der Leij, 2002; Nation & Snowling, 2004; Oakhill, Cain, & Bryant, 2003; Verhoeven & van Leeuwe, 2008). The relative contribution of decoding and language skills to reading comprehension changes, however, with grade level (Gough & Tunmer, 1986; Hoover & Gough, 1990). Decoding is found to be particularly important for reading comprehension in beginning readers, (Bast & Reitsma, 1998; Catts, Hogan, & Adlof, 2005; Verhoeven & van Leeuwe, 2008), whereas when decoding becomes automatic (typically around Grade 4), more resources become available to process the meaning of a text (Perfetti, 1998; Samuels & Flor, 1997; Tunmer & Hoover, 1992), and the contribution of language skills to reading comprehension increases (Bast & Reitsma, 1998; Catts et al., 2005; Verhoeven & van Leeuwe, 2008).

The role of executive functions in reading comprehension

Evidence for the role of executive functions in reading comprehension can be derived from the Reading Systems Framework (Perfetti & Stafura, 2014), in which decoding and language comprehension take place within a cognitive system that affects reading comprehension. To be more specific, given that the cognitive system has a limited processing capacity, the processes involved in reading comprehension need to compete for the resources available in the cognitive system (see also Just & Carpenter, 1992; Kintsch, 1998). Effortful reading comprehension processes require more resources than automatic reading comprehension processes – that is, processes that occur without the need for attention (Bargh & Uleman, 1989). Automaticity of decoding would, therefore, allow for an increase of resources for other cognitive demands involved in reading comprehension (e.g., Cunningham, Stanovich, & Wilson, 1990; Perfetti, 1985). Given this, executive functioning takes a more prominent role in the upper primary grades, when the focus of primary education shifts from learning to read to reading to learn (Chall, 1983). Children are presented with larger passages and more complex sentences, which increases the demand on both language skills (Bast & Reitsma, 1998; Catts et al., 2005; Verhoeven & van Leeuwe, 2008) and executive functioning (e.g., Cunningham et al., 1990; Perfetti, 1985). Indeed, it has been demonstrated that, in addition to decoding and language skills, executive functions explain significant variance in reading comprehension in the upper grades of primary school (e.g., Cain, Bryant, & Oakhill, 2004; Christopher et al., 2012; Cutting et al., 2009; Kieffer et al., 2013; Locascio, Mahone, Eason, & Cutting, 2010; Sesma et al., 2009). Moreover, several longitudinal studies evidenced that the contribution of executive functions to reading comprehension increases in the upper primary grades when decoding skills are more developed (Nouwens, Groen, Kleemans, & Verhoeven, 2018; Seigneuric & Ehrlich, 2005).

Direct contributions of executive functions to reading comprehension

Executive functions reflect a family of top‐down mental processes that control and coordinate lower‐level cognitive abilities to reach goals as efficiently as possible. Hence, executive functions are proposed to facilitate reading comprehension by coordinating and controlling specific reading processes, such as integrating information, retrieving information from the mental lexicon, using strategies and simultaneously engaging in these multiple reading processes. Previous research has identified three core executive functions in adults (Miyake et al., 2000) and children (Lehto et al., 2003) including updating and monitoring of working memory processes (which is closely linked to the notion of working memory as reflected by Baddeley’s model; Jonides & Smith, 1997; Lehto, 1996), inhibition, and cognitive flexibility (also referred to as switching and/or shifting). From these core executive functions, higher‐order executive functions are built such as reasoning, problem solving, and planning (Collins & Koechlin, 2012; Lunt et al., 2012). Three executive functions that have been reported to significantly contribute to reading comprehension are as follows: working memory (e.g., Carretti, Borella, Cornoldi, & De Beni, 2009; Daneman & Merikle, 1996; Seigneuric & Ehrlich, 2005), inhibition (Arrington, Kulesz, Francis, Fletcher, & Barnes, 2014; Kieffer et al., 2013), and planning (Cutting et al., 2009; Sesma et al., 2009).

Working memory

One of the most dominant theoretical accounts in the literature concerning executive functions and reading comprehension is the working memory model of Baddeley and Hitch (1974; see also Baddeley, 2000). It proposes that working memory is a mechanism that facilitates the ability to store information while simultaneously carrying out processing operations (Daneman & Carpenter, 1980). The relationship between working memory and reading comprehension is well established (for reviews, see Butterfuss & Kendeou, 2018; Carretti et al., 2009; Daneman & Carpenter, 1980; Daneman & Merikle, 1996; Follmer, 2018). Working memory is considered to be drawn upon when integrating stored representations with incoming information (Baddeley & Hitch, 1974; Daneman & Carpenter, 1980; Masson & Miller, 1983). More specifically, when children are reading, they have to integrate the situation described by the text with information that has been read previously, and/or with prior knowledge. Previous research has consistently indicated significant direct contributions of working memory to reading comprehension in adults and children in both cross‐sectional (for meta‐analyses, see Carretti et al., 2009; Daneman & Merikle, 1996; Follmer, 2018) and longitudinal designs (Seigneuric & Ehrlich, 2005), after controlling for decoding and language skills (e.g., Cain et al., 2004; Kieffer et al., 2013; Oakhill et al., 2003; Sesma et al., 2009).

Inhibition

Inhibition is assumed to be needed for reading comprehension to form coherent text representations. During reading, a child may come across text passages that contain various sources of irrelevant information or ambiguity in the overall context that needs to be inhibited to form an accurate representation of the text (Cain, 2006; Carretti et al., 2009; De Beni & Palladino, 2000). Friedman and Miyake (2004) have distinguished three inhibitory‐related functions: (1) prepotent response inhibition, which reflects the inhibition of dominant and automatic responses activated by the presented stimulus; (2) response to distracter inhibition, which reflects the ability to be focus on relevant items by ignoring simultaneously presented irrelevant items; and (3) resistance to proactive interference, which reflects the ability to decrease the activation of no longer relevant items, and thus to resist memory intrusions (intrusion errors). Borella and de Ribaupierre (2014) investigated the role of these three types on inhibition in reading comprehension in 10‐ to 12‐year‐old children. They found that only resistance to distractor interference contributed significantly to individual differences in reading comprehension. De Beni and Palladino (2000), however, revealed that resistance to proactive interference in 8‐year‐olds, measured by intrusion errors, predicted their reading comprehension skills one year later. Both response to distracter inhibition and resistance to proactive interference have been found to be entwined with working memory (Friedman & Miyake, 2004). Therefore, the current study focused on prepotent response inhibition only, as the aim was to investigate the unique contribution of inhibition to reading comprehension, separate from working memory. Although some studies found prepotent response inhibition, and not working memory, to contribute significantly to reading comprehension while accounting for word reading and language comprehension (Kieffer et al., 2013), other studies found the opposite effect while accounting for word reading (e.g., Christopher et al., 2012). In a recent meta‐analysis, it was shown that the relation between prepotent response inhibition and reading comprehension was significant, but weak (Follmer, 2018).

Planning

Finally, planning can be seen as the ability to decide which actions are necessary to efficiently reach and complete a goal (Cartwright, 2009), and has been shown to significantly account for variation in reading comprehension, after controlling for decoding, language skills, and working memory in 9‐ to 15‐year‐olds (Cutting et al., 2009; Sesma et al., 2009). Similarly, Georgiou and Das (2018) demonstrated a contribution of planning to reading comprehension in young adults (M = 21.82 year). It has been proposed that good planners regularly monitor whether their text representations are correct and if not, may even change strategies, to achieve a correct understanding of the text (Cartwright, 2015).

Direct contributions of executive functions to decoding and language skills

In parallel to their relevance to reading comprehension, executive functions may also be needed for decoding and language skills. Concerning decoding, working memory is proposed to facilitate the mapping of graphemes to corresponding phonemes, while retaining the retrieved phonemes and syllables in storage so that words can be recognized (Just & Carpenter, 1992). The results concerning the relation between working memory and decoding are somewhat inconsistent. This might be because working memory can be measured by various tasks that tap into different underlying domains. When a more verbal‐oriented working memory measure was used, significant relations between working memory and decoding have been consistently reported (Arrington et al., 2014; Christopher et al., 2012; Gottardo, Stanovich, & Siegel, 1996; Miyake, Keenan, Pennington, & DeFries, 2012), but this was not the case for working memory measures that tapped into the visual spatial domain (Kieffer et al., 2013).

During the process of decoding, incorrect representations need to be suppressed, which is controlled by inhibition mechanisms (Altemeier, Abbott, & Berninger, 2008; Arrington et al., 2014). There is some evidence that prepotent response inhibition contributes significantly to decoding skills (Altemeier et al., 2008; Arrington et al., 2014; Kibby, Lee, & Dyer, 2014). However, others have reported the contribution of prepotent response inhibition to decoding not to be significant (Kieffer et al., 2013). This contradicting finding may be caused by differences in the assessment of decoding. Planning does not appear to contribute to individual differences in decoding. It has been hypothesized that higher‐order executive functions (such as planning) are only involved in complex tasks and not in simple tasks such decoding (Sesma et al., 2009).

Concerning language skills, it is generally assumed that working memory is needed to integrate information across individual word meanings (vocabulary knowledge) and word functions (syntax knowledge) in sentences while holding those representations in memory (Daneman & Carpenter, 1980; Masson & Miller, 1983). Previous research has demonstrated that individual differences in language skills can be explained by both verbal working memory (Adams & Gathercole, 1995) and non‐verbal working memory (Kaushanskaya, Park, Gangopadhyay, Davidson, & Weismer, 2017). Moreover, verbal working memory has been shown to account for listening comprehension (Florit, Roch, Altoè, & Levorato, 2009; Florit, Roch, & Levorato, 2011). However, listening comprehension taps cognitive factors in addition to language skills, complicating the interpretation.

Though positive significant relations between working memory and language skills have been reported (Adams & Gathercole, 1995; Florit et al., 2011; Kaushanskaya et al., 2017), relationships between other executive functions and language skills in typically developing children in the upper primary grades have rarely been investigated (Kaushanskaya et al., 2017). Kaushanskaya et al. (2017) investigated the contributions of inhibition, updating, and task shifting – all assessed with non‐verbal measures – to lexical–semantic and syntactic abilities in children between the ages of 8–11 years. Non‐verbal inhibition (but not shifting or updating) was found to predict children’s syntactic abilities. It has been proposed that inhibition may facilitate the suppression of ambiguous word representations and in turn aid in forming accurate integrations to establish a coherent representation of the text (Cain, 2006; Carretti et al., 2009; De Beni & Palladino, 2000).

Indirect and direct contribution of executive functions to reading comprehension

To our knowledge, only a few studies have examined both direct and indirect effects of executive functions on reading comprehension via decoding or language skills. These include studies with adults (Follmer & Sperling, 2019; Georgiou & Das, 2018), young adolescents (Ober et al., 2019), and children (Kieffer et al., 2013). However, the results across these studies vary, which may be explained by several factors.

When regarding working memory, studies showed either no effect on decoding, language skills, or reading comprehension (Georgiou & Das, 2018; Kieffer et al., 2013) or, in the study by Ober et al. (2019), no effects on decoding, but a significant contribution to reading comprehension. These varying findings may be explained by the way these studies assessed different constructs. For example, Georgiou and Das (2018) allowed participants to go back and forth between the text and questions, which may have affected working memory load. Moreover, differences in the extent to which working memory tasks (i.e., digit span and listening span, Georgiou & Das, 2018; visual matrix subtest, Kieffer et al., 2013; operation span, Ober et al., 2019) tapped into verbal processing may have influenced the relation between working memory on the one hand, and decoding, language skills, and reading comprehension on the other hand. Additionally, inhibition was found to explain variance solely in word fluency (Georgiou & Das, 2018), solely in reading comprehension (and no indirect associations of inhibition via word reading or language skills; Kieffer et al., 2013) or in both decoding and reading comprehension (Ober et al., 2019). Ober et al. (2019) proposed that the relation between executive functions, decoding, language skills, and reading comprehension may differ between adults and children (Georgiou & Das, 2018), as executive functions have been shown to develop well into adulthood (De Luca & Leventer, 2008), which may explain diverging results. Moreover, varying results were found for the indirect relations between executive functions, vocabulary, and reading comprehension (Follmer & Sperling, 2019; Georgiou & Das, 2018). However, these studies are hard to compare, as Follmer and Sperling (2019) used a latent variable to reflect executive functions (including inhibition, shifting, and updating) and Georgiou and Das (2018) regarded the unique contribution of separate executive functions to reading comprehension.

Together, these variations in research design make comparisons across studies difficult. Inherently, more research is needed to obtain more insight into the relations between executive functions and reading comprehension. Besides, although these studies provide insight into the direct and indirect contributions of executive functions to reading comprehension, only a few of these studies included variables representing both decoding and language skills, as mediating variables (Georgiou & Das, 2018; Kieffer et al., 2013). Moreover, to our knowledge, only Kieffer et al. (2013) have investigated these relations in children.

The present study

From the research conducted so far, it is clear that decoding skills, language skills, and executive functions directly contribute to reading comprehension. Additionally, there is some evidence that executive functions affect decoding and language skills. However, the possible indirect effects of executive functions via decoding and language skills on reading comprehension skills are less well known. Furthermore, only a few attempts have been made to study the role of executive functions on reading comprehension throughout the upper primary grades using a longitudinal approach (Nouwens et al., 2018; Seigneuric & Ehrlich, 2005), which makes it difficult to draw conclusions regarding the predictive role of executive functions in reading comprehension. The research question of the present study was, thus, to what extent fourth‐grade executive functions (i.e., working memory, inhibition, and planning) directly and indirectly (through decoding and language skills) predict fifth‐grade reading comprehension. We expected that both decoding skills and language skills would contribute to variation in reading comprehension (hypothesis 1). Furthermore, we hypothesized that executive functions would contribute directly to reading comprehension (hypothesis 2), and, finally, we expected that executive functions contributed indirectly to reading comprehension through decoding skills and language skills (hypothesis 3; an overview of included relations is depicted in Figure 1).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Proposed research questions represented in a structural equation model.

Method

Participants

A total of 122 Dutch fourth‐grade children were recruited from four primary schools in the Netherlands. We decided to include fourth graders as in this grade children’s decoding becomes automated, and as a consequence, more cognitive resources become available for higher‐level comprehension processes (e.g., Cunningham et al., 1990; Perfetti, 1985). These four schools had a total of six fourth‐grade classes.

In Grade 4, we assessed children’s non‐verbal cognitive ability by using the Raven’s Coloured Progressive Matrices (Raven, Raven, & Court, 2003). Five children scored below the 25% percentile on this standardized measure and were therefore excluded from the present study. After Grade 4, two children dropped out of the study due to having moved (n = 1) or repeating Grade 4 (n = 1). Furthermore, in Grade 5, another two children were excluded because they failed to answer over 10% of the questions on the reading comprehension test. Therefore, in the present study, the final sample consisted of 113 children, including 65 boys and 48 girls (Time 1, Grade 4: Age M = 9.89; SD = .44 years; Time 2, Grade 5: Age M = 10.07; SD = .42 years). Decoding criteria for inclusion were not applied. Scores on a variety of tasks were not obtained for seven additional children, due to illness or technical malfunctions. Therefore, these children were not included in further analyses, leading to a total of 106 children. The percentage of non‐native speakers of Dutch included in the analyses was less than 3%, which falls below the average minority representation (15%) in Dutch primary schools (Tesser, Merens, & Van Praag, 1999).

Thirty‐six schools situated in the Netherlands were informed over the research by personalized letters and subsequently contacted by telephone. Of the 30 schools that were reached, four schools (i.e., 13%) – all in regions with an average to slightly higher than average social–economic status (Sociaal Cultureel Planbureau, 2017) – showed interest in participating. Recruitment of participants included informing children and their parents or guardians about the research and obtaining passive consent from parents or caregivers. For each school, children were informed about the research in class, and an announcement was placed in the school newsletter. Subsequently, parents and caregivers were given a brochure with information concerning the research (e.g., ‘what will be investigated and why’, ‘what will happen to the results’, ‘what does participation entail’, ‘consent’ etcetera). For questions, parents and caregivers were directed to the school principal. Informed parental consent was obtained for all children. Within these four schools (including six classes), 100% of approached participants agreed to be in the study.

Materials

Executive functions measured at Time 1 (Grade 4)

Working memory

Working memory was assessed by means of the backward digit span (Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children‐III‐NL, 2004) and a Dutch translation of Gaulin and Campbell’s (1994) Competing Language Processing Task.

The backward digit span required participants to store orally presented digits and to reproduce these in reverse order. The backward digit span contained seven blocks, including two sequences per block. The number of digits in a sequence increased over blocks, starting with two and ending with eight digits. When participants were able to successfully recall at least one sequence per block, they moved up to the next block, with a longer sequence of digits. The test ended when the participant incorrectly recalled both trials within a block. The number of correctly recalled trials reflected working memory. The maximum possible score was 14. The internal consistency reliability for this test is reported to be .78 (Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children‐III‐NL, 2004).

Gaulin and Campbell’s (1994) Competing Language Processing Task requires participants to recall sentence‐final words after judging if the orally presented sentences are semantically correct or incorrect. It is therefore very similar to Daneman and Carpenter (1980)’s listening span task, but contains shorter and simpler sentences and is, therefore, more suitable for use with children. The task contained five blocks of two sets. The number of sentences increased gradually for each pair of sets, starting with two sentences per set and increasing to six sentences per set. The percentage of correct judgements (whether sentences were semantically correct or incorrect) was over 90% for all participants. The total number of correctly recalled words was taken as an indication of working memory. The maximum possible score was 42. The split‐half reliability of the task (calculated by dividing the equal‐sized sets) was .68 after Spearman–Brown correction for test length.

Inhibition

Inhibition was assessed with the Colour–Word Interference Test (Delis, Kaplan, & Kramer, 2001). Participants were presented with the words ‘red’, ‘green’, and ‘blue’ and were asked to name the incongruent ink colour (red, green, or blue) of the printed words out loud, as quickly and accurately as possible. For example, the ink colour of the word ‘red’ was blue (correct answer: blue). Naming the ink colour of the word requires suppression of the overlearned response (reading the word) to execute the less automatized response (naming the ink colour) and is an effortful process. Inhibition was reflected by the time (in s) needed to complete the task. In case the participant was not able to finish the task within 180 s, the task was ended. Hence, the maximum possible score was 180, where a higher score reflected a poorer performance. During the task, errors (naming the wrong colour) were also measured. As very few errors and very little variation in errors were observed, errors were not taken into account in the computation of the final score. The reliability of this task is reported to range between .70 and .79 (Delis et al., 2001).

Planning

Planning was measured with the tower task (Delis et al., 2001). Participants were required to build nine increasingly difficult towers from a prearranged initial state to a goal state (a tower) that was presented in a picture. Participants were limited by a time frame. Towers were built by moving a set of three to five discs varying in size across three pegs. The instructions were to use as few moves as possible while adhering to specific rules regarding the movement of the discs. The trials ended when participants completed the tower or were discontinued after the time limit had been reached. The total number of moves used to reach the goal states of all towers was used to reflect planning skills. A higher score, therefore, reflected a poor performance. The minimum possible score to complete all towers was 85. Internal consistency reliability for this task is reported as .84 (Delis et al., 2001).

Reading measures at Time 2 (Grade 5)

Reading comprehension

Reading comprehension was assessed with a standardized Dutch task, Diatekst (H. I. Hacquebord, personal communication, school year 2011–2012). The task consisted of six textbook texts with an average difficulty level suitable for Grade 5. For each text, participants were instructed to read the text before answering the 10–12 accompanying multiple‐choice questions. Questions covered micro‐, meso‐, and macrostructures of the text. Questions on micro‐level covered word knowledge and syntactical constructions. Meso‐level questions reflected relations between sentences of the text. Macro‐level questions reflected global text comprehension. The texts remained available for reading during the entire test. The total score reflected the total number of correct answers, with a maximum of 67. The reliability analyses showed Cronbach’s alpha of .89.

Decoding

Decoding was assessed by means of the Klepel (Brus & Voeten, 1999), a standardized Dutch test where participants were asked to read a list of pseudowords as quickly and accurately as possible. The list contained 116 items, increasing in length and difficulty. The total number of pseudowords read correctly within two minutes of reflected decoding. The parallel‐forms correlation for Grade 5 is reported to be .92.

Language skills

Language skills assessed were receptive vocabulary and productive syntactic complexity.

Vocabulary was measured using the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test‐III‐NL (PPVT‐III‐NL; Dunn & Dunn, 2005). Participants were presented with four pictures on a computer screen, while they heard a word. Children were requested to indicate which picture best reflected the presented word. Words were presented in blocks of 12 items. The task was discontinued when participants made nine mistakes or more within one block. The maximum possible score was 204. The test–retest reliability is reported to range between .89 and .97 (Dunn & Dunn, 2005).

Syntactic complexity was measured using the Beach Story of the Expression, Reception, and Recall of Narrative Instrument (ERRNI; Bishop, 2004). The participants were presented with 15 pictures that were linked together sequentially to form a story. After careful examination of all the pictures, participants were asked to tell the story that the pictures displayed. We transcribed the recorded audio files and subsequently calculated the mean length of T‐Units in words (minimal terminable syntactic units; for guidelines see Hunt, 1966), as an indicator of the child's syntactic complexity, using Child Language Analysis – a programme for analysing transcripts (MacWhinney, 2000).

Procedure

This study includes the data of a comprehensive longitudinal study into the role of executive functions in children’s reading comprehension development in the upper primary grades (i.e., grades 4‐6). Other aspects of the study are reported in Nouwens et al. (2018); Nouwens, Groen, and Verhoeven (2016a); Nouwens, Groen, and Verhoeven (2016b) and therefore include the same sample of children as the current study. While these studies also regarded the relation between executive functions and reading comprehension – while taking decoding and vocabulary into account – these studies differ from the current study on several points. Specifically, although it was demonstrated that working memory and cognitive flexibility (and not inhibition and planning) significantly explained variance in reading comprehension, indirect effects were not assessed and findings were based on cross‐sectional data (data from Grade 5; Nouwens et al., 2018; Nouwens et al., 2016a). On the contrary, a third study did apply a longitudinal approach (data from grades 4, 5, and 6), but this focused on the relation between memory retrieval processes and reading comprehension. It therefore did not include standardized measures of working memory, inhibition, cognitive flexibility, and planning.

Reading comprehension and non‐verbal cognitive ability were administered in class by one researcher. The remaining tasks were administered individually and were divided over two sessions in a fixed order (session 1: vocabulary knowledge, decoding, inhibition; session 2: working memory, planning, and syntax knowledge) to prevent cognitive overload.

The assessments were administered by that same researcher and two assistants who each underwent a training programme consisting of (1) observing two assessment sessions carried out by the researcher, (2) performing two ‘mock’ assessment sessions carried out by the assistant, which included a comparison of scores (between the researcher and assistant) and feedback, and (3) two assessments carried out independently by the assistant and observed by the researcher, including intervention and feedback when necessary. Reliability checks were not conducted, as there appeared to be little to no variation in the scores obtained when testing was carried out by the researcher or the assistants.

Data‐analyses

Before performing the correlation and structural equation model analyses, we conducted several steps to transform the data. First of all, as a higher score on inhibition and planning reflected a poorer performance, these scores were transposed such that a higher score reflected a better performance. Next, all raw scores were converted into z‐scores (M = 0; SD = 1). We then computed composite scores for language skills (PPVT‐III‐NL and ERRNI) and working memory (backward digit span and Competing Language Processing Task), by calculating the average z‐scores.

Structural equation modelling, using LISREL software (Jöreskog & Sörbom, 2003), was undertaken to test the contribution of (1) decoding and language skills to reading comprehension (Model 1); (2) the contribution of executive functions (i.e., working memory, inhibition, and planning) to individual differences in reading comprehension (Model 2); and (3) the contribution of executive functioning to individual differences in decoding, language skills, and reading comprehension (Model 3). To evaluate data fit, the p‐value associated with the chi‐square distribution should exceed .05 (Barrett, 2007). Furthermore, the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) should not exceed .06, and the value of the standardized root mean square residual (SRMR) should be smaller than .08. Finally, both the comparative fit index (CFI) and the non‐normed fit index (NNFI) should exceed .95 (Hu & Bentler, 1999).

The direct effects were calculated by the analyses in LISREL. The indirect effects of X predicting Z via Y were calculated by multiplying the direct effects. For example, the indirect effect of working memory on reading comprehension was calculated by multiplying the path between working memory and decoding and the path between decoding and reading comprehension. The total effects were calculated by adding the indirect effects to the direct effects.

Results

Descriptive statistics are in raw scores and displayed in Table 1. All variables met the criteria for normal distribution as skewness and kurtosis were <1.5 and> −1.5 (cf. Voeten & Van den Bercken, 2003).

Table 1.

Descriptive statistics (N = 106) of executive functions (at Time 1), and decoding skills, language skills, and reading comprehension (at Time 2). Table 1 includes mean scores and standard deviations (SD), minimum (Min.) and maximum (Max.) scores, and kurtosis and skewness values

  M SD Min. Max. Skewness Kurtosis
Time 1
Working memory
Backward digit span 4.491 1.587 0 8 −0.004 −0.301
Competing Language Processing Task 25.085 3.765 18 36 0.577 −0.033
Inhibition 86.698 20.295 51 153 0.988 1.367
Planning 170.793 37.527 98 307 0.543 0.592
Time 2
Decoding skills 62.613 18.142 22 102 0.088 −0.363
Language skills
PPVT‐III‐NL 124.594 13.617 96 155 −0.427 −0.186
ERRNI 7.926 0.951 4.67 10.63 −0.367 1.449
Reading comprehension 50.81 9.915 25 66 −0.881 0.083

All scores reflect raw scores. The score for working memory, decoding, vocabulary (PPVT‐III‐NL), and reading comprehension reflects the number of correct answers. The score for planning reflects the number of moves made to complete the task; the score for inhibition reflects the time needed (in s) to complete the task. The score for syntactic complexity reflects the mean length of T‐Units in words.

The Pearson correlations are displayed in Table 2. The results showed that both decoding and language skills correlated significantly with reading comprehension. Furthermore, reading comprehension was significantly correlated with the executive functions working memory and planning, but the correlation with inhibition was not significant. Finally, decoding was positively correlated with inhibition and working memory. We found no correlations between working memory, inhibition, and planning. Although this appears somewhat remarkable at first sight, weak‐to‐moderate correlations among executive tasks have been observed by others as well (Kieffer et al., 2013; Miyake et al., 2000). These findings appear to reflect the proposed independence of underlying executive functions (Miyake & Shah, 1999).

Table 2.

Correlations (N = 106) among executive functions (at Time 1), and decoding, language skills, and reading comprehension (at Time 2)

  1 2 3 4 5 6
Time 1
1 Working memory          
2. Inhibition .141        
3. Planning .144 .147        
Time 2
4. Decoding skills .320** .402** .049    
5. Language skills .096 –.033 .130 .139  
6. Reading comprehension .396** –.055 .214* .344** .336**
*

p < .05

**

p < .01

***

p < .001.

The results of the structural equation modelling analysis are displayed in Figure 2. The values represent standardized coefficients. Model 1 regarded the relative contribution of decoding and language skills in Grade 4 to reading comprehension in Grade 5. The analyses yielded a saturated model indicating that both decoding and language skills contributed significantly to reading comprehension. Furthermore, Model 2 regarded the relative contributions of working memory, inhibition, and planning to reading comprehension. The analyses yielded a saturated model indicating that both working memory and planning, but not inhibition, predicted reading comprehension one year later. Finally, Model 3 again showed an excellent model fit (χ2 = 2.46, df = 7, p = 0.930, RMSEA < 0.001, SRMR = 0.031, CFI = 0.992, NNFI = 1.140). Both working memory and planning had a direct, significant effect on reading comprehension. In Model 3, the direct relation between inhibition and reading comprehension was also significant (i.e., in contrast to the findings in Model 2). Furthermore, working memory and inhibition also had indirect, significant contributions to reading comprehension via decoding skills. None of the executive functions contributed to language skills.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Structural equation models investigating the relations between executive functions, decoding, language skills, and reading comprehension, in line with our research questions. Note. The values in figure 2 represent standardized coefficients.

Discussion

The present study investigated how executive functions (i.e., working memory, inhibition, and planning) contributed to decoding skills, language skills, and reading comprehension. Consistent with the previous literature (Goff et al., 2005; de Jong & van der Leij, 2002; Nation & Snowling, 2004; Oakhill et al., 2003; Verhoeven & van Leeuwe, 2008) and confirming our first hypothesis (Model 1), the current study demonstrated the importance of decoding and language skills in reading comprehension, supporting the Simple View of Reading (Hoover & Gough, 1990). With respect to the second hypothesis (Model 2), the contribution of executive functions to reading comprehension, the results indicated that both working memory and planning as measured in Grade 4, accounted for variance in reading comprehension one year later. This is in line with previous research that showed both working memory (Daneman & Merikle, 1996; Seigneuric & Ehrlich, 2005) and planning (Cutting et al., 2009; Sesma et al., 2009) to have direct, significant relations with reading comprehension. Moreover, the current study demonstrated that a domain‐general, higher‐order executive function (i.e., planning) contributes to reading comprehension, after controlling for working memory. Inhibition, on the contrary, did not directly predict reading comprehension skills in Model 2, but did have a significant effect in Model 3. With respect to the third hypothesis and the final model (Model 3, see Figure 2 and Table 3), the results showed that, in addition to direct effects of executive functions on reading comprehension, executive functions had significant indirect effects on reading comprehension, via decoding skills but not via language skills.

Table 3.

Direct and indirect effect of the structural equation models, which reflects the relations between executive functions, decoding, language skills, and reading comprehension, in line with our research questions

Relationships Direct Indirect Total
Working memory → decoding skills .27 .27
Inhibition → decoding skills .37 .37
Planning → decoding skills –.04 –.04
Working memory → language skills –.09 –.09
Inhibition → language skills –.06 –.06
Planning → language skills .13 –.13
Decoding skills → reading comprehension .31 .31
Language skills → reading comprehension .24  
Working memory → reading comprehension .29 .10 .39
Inhibition → reading comprehension –.23 .10 –.13
Planning → reading comprehension .16 .02 .18

Direct and indirect contributions of working memory to reading comprehension

The current study demonstrated significant contributions of working memory to decoding skills in addition to its contribution to reading comprehension. Significant effects for working memory on decoding skills have been reported in other studies as well (Arrington et al., 2014; Christopher et al., 2012; Gottardo et al., 1996; Miyake et al., 2012). Working memory is proposed to be needed for decoding to efficiently integrate graphemes with corresponding phonemes (Just & Carpenter, 1992). When comparing our findings to those of previous studies that have regarded both direct and indirect effects of executive functions to reading comprehension via decoding and language skills (Georgiou & Das, 2018; Kieffer et al., 2013), there are several differences that need to be discussed. First of all, unlike these other studies, we found a significant direct relation between working memory and reading comprehension, and an indirect relation via decoding. The absence of significant relations in other studies is remarkable given the extensive evidence of the relationship between working memory and reading comprehension (Butterfuss & Kendeou, 2018; Carretti et al., 2009; Daneman & Carpenter, 1980; Daneman & Merikle, 1996; Follmer, 2018). Georgiou and Das (2018) propose that this may be explained by the nature of their comprehension task. Kieffer et al. (2013) pointed out that they used a visual working memory task and not a verbal working memory task. It has been reported repeatedly that tasks measuring working memory in the language domain (i.e., domain‐specific tasks) are better predictors of children’s reading comprehension than tasks measuring working memory in a domain other than language (Shah & Miyake, 1996). In short, these studies show inconsistent results; however, the studies regarding the direct and indirect effects of working memory to reading comprehension in the upper primary grades, via decoding and language skills, are limited. Future work should address this issue, using working memory tasks that vary in the language processes that are involved.

Direct and indirect contributions of inhibition to reading comprehension

With respect to inhibition, the results showed that the direct relation between inhibition and reading comprehension was significant in Model 3. In contrast, the direct relation between inhibition and reading comprehension was not significant in Model 2. These latter findings support the results of Christopher et al. (2012), who also found no contribution of prepotent response inhibition to reading comprehension in children aged 8–16 years old. Similar results have been reported in adolescents (Arrington et al., 2014).

Based on the correlation analysis and Model 2, it appears that the results of Model 3 are due to a statistical artefact (possibly caused by the strong relationship between inhibition and decoding skills). Moreover, all other results in Model 3 are in line with the correlation analysis, and models 1 and 2, suggesting that the statistical artefact is limited to the relation between inhibition and reading comprehension. Another explanation for these results might be the lack of correlations between the executive functions measures. Although this finding is not uncommon in itself (Kieffer et al., 2013; Miyake et al., 2000), it may have influenced the structural equation model analyses.

Our findings concerning prepotent response inhibition and decoding skills are concurrent with other studies (Altemeier et al., 2008; Arrington et al., 2014). It has been suggested that inhibition mechanisms facilitate decoding skills as it facilitates suppressing incorrect mental representations, evoked by the words in the text, such as homophones and homonyms (Altemeier et al., 2008; Arrington et al., 2014). It would be interesting to further explore the relationship between inhibition and decoding by including both word and non‐word reading tasks. According to the dual‐route hypothesis, these tasks vary in the extent in which different types of mental representations (phonological, orthographic, syntactic, and semantic representations) are required for processing (e.g., Coltheart, 2000). Inhibition may play a different role in these processes. Similarly, it would be interesting to investigate whether the relation between inhibition and decoding is influenced by factors such as word frequency and text difficulty. Moreover, if inhibition skills depend on a child’s quality of phonological, orthographic, syntactic, and semantic representations, this may also affect the relation between inhibition and decoding.

No significant results were found regarding the contributions of executive functions to language skills. This is in contrast to what has been reported in pre‐schoolers (Florit et al., 2009, 2011) and in 4‐ to 5‐year‐old children (cf. Adams & Gathercole, 1995 ). One explanation is that retrieval and integration of linguistic information may be more effortful for younger children than for older children. Another explanation may be the way language skills are assessed. While in the current study language skills were assessed with receptive vocabulary and productive syntactic complexity, Florit et al. (2009), Florit et al. (2011) used a listening comprehension task, which taps into cognitive factors in addition to language skills, which may explain why they found a contribution of executive functions.

Again, regarding studies that included direct and indirect effects of executive functions to reading comprehension, we see that our results are in line with Georgiou and Das (2018), but in contrast to Kieffer et al. (2013). Kieffer et al. (2013) found that prepotent response inhibition accounted for individual differences in reading comprehension in 9‐ to 10‐year‐old children, but not in word reading. The diverging findings of Kieffer et al. (2013) may again be caused by the type of task: The current study used a colour–word interference task – that requires participants to suppress language processing – to reflect inhibition. Kieffer et al. (2013) used an inhibition task where participants had to suppress number processing. The differences in the way that these inhibitions tap into language processing may have affected their relations with (pseudo)word reading. Moreover, as mentioned previously, the presence of a significant relation between prepotent response inhibition may be caused by their sample’s below‐average performance on reading comprehension. Lastly, others have suggested that, rather than prepotent response inhibition, resistance to proactive interference (the ability to suppress or remove outdated information to help maintain relevant stimuli in working memory; Friedman & Miyake, 2004) is relevant for reading comprehension (Arrington et al., 2014). Future work should clarify the specific nature of the contribution of inhibition to reading comprehension, as different types of inhibition appear to contribute to different aspects of reading comprehension.

Direct and indirect contributions of planning to reading comprehension

Finally, the results of the present study indicate that planning only contributed directly (and not the indirectly) to reading comprehension. These results seem to fit with previous cross‐sectional studies (Cutting et al., 2009; Sesma et al., 2009), but our results are the first to provide a longitudinal indication for the role of planning in reading comprehension skills. Planning can be seen as a higher‐level executive function, which is involved in metacognitive processes such as monitoring of one’s own understanding and reasoning (Diamond, 2013; Vellutino, Scanlon, & Lyon, 2000). This may explain why planning is important for reading for comprehension, but not for decoding and language skills. However, the role of planning in reading comprehension development is still somewhat inconclusive, and this relation should be clarified in future studies by using different types of planning tasks and regarding multiple components of reading (e.g., decoding, word reading) and reading comprehension (e.g., sentence comprehension, micro‐ and macro‐level reading comprehension, inference making).

Limitations and suggestions for future research

Some limitations should be acknowledged at this point. To begin with, it should be noted that there is no clear consensus about how executive functioning and working memory are being conceptualized. While some authors have proposed working memory to be a distinct and separate executive function (Lehto et al., 2003; Miyake et al., 2000), others have proposed that multiple executive functions are involved in working memory (Baddeley, 1996; Baddeley, Della Sala, Roberts, Robbins, & Weiskrantz, 1996). Moreover, it is generally agreed that there are three core executive functions including updating and monitoring of working memory processes, inhibition, and cognitive flexibility. In the current study, we did not address the contribution of cognitive flexibility – the ability to shift between multiple operations and mental states (Anderson, 2002; Diamond, 2013) – to reading comprehension. Cognitive flexibility is needed for reading comprehension to adapt existing text representations to new text representations (Diamond, 2013) or to adjust reading strategies based on the reading goal and task difficulty (Ramsel & Grabe, 1983). Indeed, recent studies have shown that cognitive flexibility significantly explained variance in reading comprehension, even after accounting for working memory and/or inhibition in children (Kieffer et al., 2013; Nouwens et al., 2016a), young adolescents (Latzman, Elkovitch, Young, & Clark, 2010), and adults (Georgiou & Das, 2018). Moreover, it is thought to aid in simultaneous processing of phonological and semantic information for reading comprehension or to retrieve multiple mental representations from the mental lexicon or long‐term memory (Cartwright, 2009), which indicates that cognitive flexibility may be involved in decoding and language skills. Indeed, there is some evidence that cognitive flexibility contributes significantly to decoding skills in children (e.g., Cartwright, 2012) and adolescents (Ober et al., 2019), and to language skills in children (Kieffer et al., 2013). However, there is also some evidence that contradicts a relation between cognitive flexibility and language skills (Georgiou & Dass, 2018). Future work should investigate the unique contribution of cognitive flexibility – alongside other executive functions – to decoding, language skills, and reading comprehension in children.

Similarly, other cognitive processes that are related to executive functioning, such as attention (Diamond, 2013), have been positively related to reading comprehension (e.g., Arrington et al., 2014). Therefore, it would be interesting to include these processes in future research. Moreover, previous studies have demonstrated indications for a reciprocal relationship between executive functions and reading comprehension (for a review, see Follmer, 2018). It would be interesting to examine these bidirectional associations between executive functions and reading comprehension, as this may be greatly beneficial for fundamental insights and practical implications.

Lastly, studies applying a longitudinal approach could provide more insight into the developmental trajectory of these relations. Although the current study included data from time‐points one year apart, an autoregressive effect for reading comprehension was not included. Hence, our findings are only partly longitudinal. Additionally, the results await replication in follow‐up studies following a longitudinal design with larger samples of children, as this may strengthen future results.

Conclusion and practical implications

To conclude, the present study on reading comprehension highlighted the importance of executive functions in addition to decoding and language skills, in the upper grades of primary school. These findings are in line with other research showing that the automaticity of decoding allows for an increase of resources for other cognitive demands involved in reading comprehension (e.g., Cunningham et al., 1990; Perfetti, 1985), and fit well with the proposal of a limited cognitive system, as suggested by the Reading Systems Framework (Perfetti & Stafura, 2014). As a practical implication, when it comes to assessment and intervention of reading comprehension skills, educational professionals should not only consider decoding and language skills children bring into the classroom but their executive functions as well. More specifically, teachers should be aware that children first need to form a solid basis in decoding in order to free up cognitive resources needed for executive functioning (Perfetti, 1998; Samuels & Flor, 1997; Tunmer & Hoover, 1992). Furthermore, planning is a higher‐order executive function that is built on the three executive functions working memory, inhibition, and cognitive flexibility (Lehto et al., 2003; Miyake et al., 2000). This entails that deficiencies in working memory may result in planning problems. This may be important to realize as great emphasis is placed on planning in the reading comprehension curriculum by teaching reading strategies, such as reading titles, monitoring comprehension, and summarizing.

Conflicts of interest

All authors declare no conflict of interest.

Author contributions

Suzan Nouwens (Conceptualization; Formal analysis; Methodology; Writing – original draft) Margriet A. Groen (Supervision; Writing – review & editing) Tijs Kleemans (Writing – review & editing) Ludo Verhoeven (Conceptualization; Supervision; Writing – review & editing).

Data availability statement

Data available on request from the authors.

References

  1. Adams, A. M. , & Gathercole, S. E. (1995). Phonological working memory and speech production in preschool children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 38, 403–414. 10.1044/jshr.3802.403 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Altemeier, L. E. , Abbott, R. D. , & Berninger, V. W. (2008). Executive functions for reading and writing in typical literacy development and dyslexia. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 30, 588–606. 10.1080/13803390701562818 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Anderson, P. (2002). Assessment and development of executive function (EF) during childhood. Child Neuropsychology, 8, 71–82. 10.1076/chin.8.2.71.8724 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Arrington, C. N. , Kulesz, P. A. , Francis, D. J. , Fletcher, J. M. , & Barnes, M. A. (2014). The contribution of attentional control and working memory to reading comprehension and decoding. Scientific Studies of Reading, 18, 325–346. 10.1080/10888438.2014.902461 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Baddeley, A. D. (1996). Exploring the Central Executive. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49(1), 5–28. 10.1080/713755608 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  6. Baddeley, A. D. (2000). The episodic buffer: A new component of working memory? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 417–423. 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01538-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Baddeley, A. D. , Della Sala, S. , Roberts, A. C. , Robbins, T. W. , & Weiskrantz, L. (1996). Working memory and executive control. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 351, 1397–1403. 10.1098/rstb.1996.0123 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Baddeley, A. D. , & Hitch, G. J. (1974). Working memory. The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 8, 47–89. [Google Scholar]
  9. Bargh, J. , & Uleman, J. S. (1989). Unintended thought. New York, NY: Guilford Publication. [Google Scholar]
  10. Barrett, P. (2007). Structural equation modeling: adjudging model fit. Personality and Individual Differences, 42, 815–824. 10.1016/j.paid.2006.09.018 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  11. Bast, J. , & Reitsma, P. (1998). Analyzing the development of individual differences in terms of Matthew effects in reading: Results from a Dutch longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 34, 1373–1399. 10.1111/j.1745-3992.1984.tb00768.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Bishop, D. (2004). Expression, reception and recall of narrative instrument. London, UK: Harcourt Assessment. [Google Scholar]
  13. Blair, C. , & Razza, R. P. (2007). Relating effortful control, executive function, and false belief understanding to emerging math and literacy ability in kindergarten. Child Development, 78, 647–663. 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01019.x(17381795) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Borella, E. , & de Ribaupierre, A. (2014). The role of working memory, inhibition, and processing speed in text comprehension in children. Learning and Individual Differences, 34, 86–92. 10.1016/j.lindif.2014.05.001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  15. Brus, T. , & Voeten, M. (1999). Eén‐minuut‐test. Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Harcourt Test Publishers. [Google Scholar]
  16. Butterfuss, R. , & Kendeou, P. (2018). The role of executive functions in reading comprehension. Educational Psychology Review, 30, 801–826. 10.1007/s10648-017-9422-6 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  17. Cain, K. (2006). Individual differences in children's memory and reading comprehension: An investigation of semantic and inhibitory deficits. Memory, 14, 553–569. 10.1080/09658210600624481 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Cain, K. E. , Bryant, P. E. , & Oakhill, J. (2004). Children's reading comprehension ability: Concurrent prediction by working memory, verbal ability, and component skills. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96(1), 31–42. 10.1037/0022-0663.96.1.31 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  19. Carretti, B. , Borella, E. , Cornoldi, C. , & De Beni, R. (2009). Role of working memory in explaining the performance of individuals with specific reading comprehension difficulties: A meta‐analysis. Learning and Individual Differences, 19, 246–251. 10.1016/j.lindif.2008.10.002 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  20. Cartwright, K. B. (2009). The role of cognitive flexibility in reading comprehension: Past, present, and future. New York, NY: Routledge. [Google Scholar]
  21. Cartwright, K. B. (2012). Insights from cognitive neuroscience: The importance of executive function for early reading development and education. Early Education & Development, 23(1), 24–36. 10.1080/10409289.2011.615025 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  22. Cartwright, K. B. (2015). Executive skills and reading comprehension. New York, NY: Guilford. [Google Scholar]
  23. Catts, H. W. , Hogan, T. P. , & Adolf, S. M. (2005). Developmental changes in reading and reading disabilities. In Catts H. W. & Kamhi A. G. (Eds.), The connections between language and reading disabilities (pp. 25–40). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. [Google Scholar]
  24. Chall, J. S. (1983). Stages of reading development. New York: Harcourt Brace. [Google Scholar]
  25. Christopher, M. E. , Miyaka, A. , Keenan, J. M. , Pennington, B. , De Fries, J. C. , Wadsworth, S. J. , … Olson, R. K. (2012). Predicting word reading and reading comprehension with executive function and speed measures across development: A latent variable analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 141, 470–488. 10.1037/a0027375 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Collins, A. , & Koechlin, E. (2012). Reasoning, learning, and creativity: Frontal lobe function and human decision making. PLoS Biology, 10(3), e1001293. 10.1371/journal.pbio.1001293 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Coltheart, M. (2000). Dual routes from print to speech and dual routes from print to meaning: Some theoretical issues. In Kennedy A., Radach R., Pynte J. & Heller D. (Eds.), Reading as a perceptual process. Oxford: Elsevier. [Google Scholar]
  28. Cunningham, A. E. , Stanovich, K. E. , & Wilson, M. R. (1990). Cognitive variation in adult college students differing in reading ability. In Carr T. H. & Levy B. A. (Eds.), Reading and its development (pp. 129–159). San Diego, CA: Academic. [Google Scholar]
  29. Cutting, L. E. , Materek, A. , Colé, C. A. S. , Levine, T. M. , & Mahone, E. M. (2009). Effects of fluency, oral language, and executive function on reading comprehension performance. Annals of Dyslexia, 59, 34–54. 10.1080/09297040802220029 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Daneman, M. , & Carpenter, P. A. (1980). Individual‐differences in working memory and reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 19, 450–466. 10.1016/S00225371(80)90312-6 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  31. Daneman, M. , & Merikle, P. M. (1996). Working memory and language comprehension: A meta‐analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 3, 422–433. 10.3758/BF03214546 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. De Beni, R. , & Palladino, P. (2000). Intrusion errors in working memory tasks: Are they related to reading comprehension ability? Learning and Individual Differences, 12, 131–143. 10.1016/S1041-6080(01)00033-4 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  33. De Franchis, V. , Usai, M. C. , Viterbori, P. , & Traverso, L. (2017). Preschool executive functioning and literacy achievement in Grades 1 and 3 of primary school: A longitudinal study. Learning and Individual Differences, 54, 184–195. 10.1016/j.lindif.2017.01.026 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  34. de Jong, P. , & van der Leij, A. (2002). Effects of phonological abilities and linguistic comprehension on the development of reading. Scientific Studies of Reading, 6, 51–77. 10.1207/S1532799XSSR0601_03 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  35. De Luca, C. R. , & Leventer, R. J. (2008). Developmental trajectories of executive functions across the lifespan. In Anderson V., Jacobs R., & Anderson P. J. (Eds.), Neuropsychology, neurology, and, (Eds.), Neuropsychology, neurology, and cognition. Executive functions and the frontal lobes: A lifespan perspective (pp. 23–56). Philadelphia, PA: Taylor & Francis. [Google Scholar]
  36. Delis, D. C. , Kaplan, E. , & Kramer, J. H. (2001). Delis‐Kaplan executive function system (D‐KEFS). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation. [Google Scholar]
  37. Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64(1), 135–168. 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Dunn, L. M. , & Dunn, M. D. (2005). Peabody Picture Vocabulary — III‐NL. Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Pearson. [Google Scholar]
  39. Florit, E. , Roch, M. , Altoè, G. , & Levorato, M. C. (2009). Listening comprehension in preschoolers: The role of memory. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 27, 935–951. 10.1348/026151008x397189 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Florit, E. , Roch, M. , & Levorato, M. C. (2011). Listening text comprehension of explicit and implicit information in preschoolers: The role of verbal and inferential skills. Discourse Processes, 48, 119–138. 10.1080/0163853X.2010.494244 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  41. Follmer, D. J. (2018). Executive function and reading comprehension: A meta‐analytic review. Educational Psychologist, 53(1), 42–60. 10.1080/00461520.2017.1309295 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  42. Follmer, D. J. , & Sperling, R. A. (2019). A latent variable analysis of the contribution of executive function to adult readers’ comprehension of science text: The roles of vocabulary ability and level of comprehension. Reading and Writing, 32(2), 377–403. 10.1007/s11145-018-9872-3 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  43. Friedman, N. P. , & Miyake, A. (2004). The relations among inhibition and interference control functions: A latent‐ variable analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 133, 101–135. 10.1037/0096-3445.133.1.101 http://dx.doi.org/ [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Gaulin, C. , & Campbell, T. (1994). Procedure for assessing verbal working memory in normal school‐age children: Some preliminary data. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 79, 55–64. 10.2466/pms.1994.79.1.55 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Georgiou, G. K. , & Das, J. P. (2018). Direct and indirect effecs of executive function on reading comprehension in young adults. Journal of Research in Reading, 41(2), 243–258. 10.1111/1467-9817.12091 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  46. Goff, D. , Pratt, C. , & Ong, B. (2005). The relations between children’s reading comprehension, working memory, language skills and components of reading decoding in a normal sample. Reading and Writing: an Interdisciplinary Journal, 18, 583–616. 10.1007/s11145-004-7109-0 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  47. Gottardo, A. , Stanovich, K. E. , & Siegel, L. S. (1996). The relationships between phonological sensitivity, syntactic processing, and verbal working memory in the reading performance of third‐grade children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 63, 563–582. 10.1006/jecp.1996.0062 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Gough, P. B. , & Tunmer, W. E. (1986). Decoding, reading, and reading disability. Remedial and Special Education, 7(1), 6–10. 10.1177/074193258600700104 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  49. Hoover, W. A. , & Gough, P. B. (1990). The simple view of reading. Reading and Writing. an Interdisciplinary Journal, 2, 127–160. 10.1007/BF00401799 http://dx.doi.org/ [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  50. Hu, L. T. , & Bentler, P. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6, 1–55. 10.1080/10705519909540118 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  51. Hunt, K. W. (1966). Sentence structures used by superior students in grades four and twelve, and by superior adults. CRP 5–0313. Tallahassee, FL: Florida State University. [Google Scholar]
  52. Jacob, R. , & Parkinson, J. (2015). The potential for school‐based interventions that target executive function to improve academic achievement: A review. Review of Educational Research, 85, 512–552. 10.3102/0034654314561338 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  53. Jonides, J. , & Smith, E. E. (1997). Working memory: A review from neuroimaging. Cognitive Psychology, 33, 5–42. 10.1006/cogp.1997.0658 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Jöreskog, K. G. , & Sörbom, D. (2003). LISREL 8.84 for Windows. Lincolnwood, IL: Scientific Software International Inc. [Google Scholar]
  55. Just, M. A. , & Carpenter, P. A. (1992). A capacity theory of comprehension: Individual differences in working memory. Psychological Review, 99(1), 122–149. 10.1037/0033-295X.99.1.122 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Kaushanskaya, M. , Park, J. S. , Gangopadhyay, I. , Davidson, M. M. , & Weismer, S. E. (2017). The relationship between executive functions and language abilities in children: A latent variables approach. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 60, 912–992. 10.1044/2016_JSLHR-L-15-0310 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Kibby, M. Y. , Lee, S. E. , & Dyer, S. M. (2014). Reading comprehension is predicated by more than phonological processing. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1–7. 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00960 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Kieffer, M. J. , Vukovic, R. K. , & Berry, D. (2013). Direct and indirect roles of executive functioning in reading comprehension for students in urban fourth grade classrooms. Reading Research Quarterly, 48, 333–348. 10.1002/rrq.54 http://dx.doi/ [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  59. Kintsch, W. (1998). Comprehension: A paradigm for cognition. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  60. Latzman, R. D. , Elkovitch, N. , Young, J. , & Clark, L. A. (2010). The contribution of executive functioning to academic achievement among male adolescents. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 32, 455–462. 10.1080/13803390903164363 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Lehto, J. E. (1996). Are executive function tests dependent upon working memory capacity? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49A, 29–50. 10.1080/027249896392793 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  62. Lehto, J. E. , Juujärvi, P. , Kooistra, L. , & Pulkkinen, L. (2003). Dimensions of executive functioning: Evidence from children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 21, 59–80. 10.1348/026151003321164627 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  63. Locascio, G. , Mahone, E. M. , Eason, S. E. , & Cutting, L. E. (2010). Executive dysfunction among children with reading comprehension deficits. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 3, 441–454. 10.1177/0022219409355476 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Lunt, L. , Bramham, J. , Morris, R. G. , Bullock, P. R. , Selway, R. P. , Xenitidis, K. , & David, A. S. (2012). Prefrontal cortex dysfunction and “jumping to conclusions”: Bias or deficit? Journal of Neuropsychology, 6, 65–78. 10.1111/j.1748-6653.2011.02005.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Lyon, G. R. (1995). Toward a definition of dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 45(1), 1–27. 10.1007/BF02648210 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. MacWhinney, B. (2000). The CHILDES project: Tools for analyzing talk. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. [Google Scholar]
  67. Masson, M. E. J. , & Miller, J. A. (1983). Working memory and individual differences in comprehension and memory of text. Journal of Educational Psychology, 2, 314–318. 10.1037/0022-0663.75.2.314 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  68. Miyake, A. , Friedman, N. P. , Emerson, M. J. , Witzki, A. H. , Howerter, A. , & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49–100. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Miyake, A. , Keenan, J. M. , Pennington, B. , & DeFries, J. C. (2012). Predicting word reading and comprehension with executive function and speed measures across development: A latent variable analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 141, 470–488. 10.1037/a0027375 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Miyake, A. , & Shah, P. (1999). Models of working memory: Mechanisms of active maintenance and executive control. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  71. Monette, S. , Bigras, M. , & Guay, M. C. (2011). The role of the executive functions in school achievement at the end of Grade 1. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 109, 158–173. 10.1016/j.jecp.2011.01.00821349537 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Muter, V. , Hulme, C. , Snowling, M. J. , & Stevenson, J. (2004). Phonemes, rimes and language skills as foundations of early reading development: Evidence from a longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 40, 663–681. 10.1037/0012-1649.40.5.665 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Nation, K. , & Snowling, M. J. (2004). Beyond phonological skills: Broader language skills contribute to the development of reading. Journal of Research in Reading, 27(4), 342–356. 10.1111/j.1467-9817.2004.00238.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  74. Nouwens, S. , Groen, M. A. , Kleemans, T. , & Verhoeven, L. (2018). The role of semantic retrieval in children’s reading comprehension development in the upper primary grades. Journal of Research in Reading, 41, 597–614. 10.1111/1467-9817.12128 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  75. Nouwens, S. , Groen, M. A. , & Verhoeven, L. (2016a). How storage and executive functions contribute to children’s reading comprehension. Learning and Individual Differences, 47, 96–102. 10.1016/j.lindif.2015.12.008 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  76. Nouwens, S. , Groen, M. A. , & Verhoeven, L. (2016b). How working memory relates to children’s reading comprehension: The importance of domain‐specificity in storage and processing. Reading and Writing, 30(1), 105–120. 10.1007/s11145-016-9665- [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Oakhill, J. , Cain, K. , & Bryant, P. E. (2003). The dissociation of word reading and text comprehension: Evidence from component skills. Language and Cognitive Processes, 18, 443–468. 10.1080/01690960344000008 http://dx.doi.org/ [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  78. Ober, T. M. , Brooks, P. J. , Plass, J. L. , & Homer, B. D. (2019). Distinguishing direct and indirect effects of executive functions on reading comprehension in adolescents. Reading Psychology, 40(6), 551–681. 10.1080/02702711.2019.1635239 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  79. Perfetti, C. A. (1985). Reading ability. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  80. Perfetti, C. A. (1992). The representation problem in reading acquisition. In Gough P. B., Ehri L. C. & Treiman R. (Eds.), Reading acquisition (pp. 145–174). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. [Google Scholar]
  81. Perfetti, C. A. (1998). Two basic questions about reading and learning to read. In Reitsma P. & Verhoeven L. (Eds.), Problems and interventions in literacy development (pp. 15–48). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Publishers. [Google Scholar]
  82. Perfetti, C. A. , Landi, N. , & Oakhill, J. (2005). The acquisition of reading comprehension skill. In Snowling M. J. & Hulme C. (Eds.), The science of reading: A handbook (pp. 227–247). Oxford: Basil Blackwell. [Google Scholar]
  83. Perfetti, C. , & Stafura, J. (2014). Word knowledge in a theory of reading comprehension. Scientific Studies of Reading, 18(1), 22–37. 10.1080/10888438.2013.827687 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  84. Ramsel, D. , & Grabe, M. (1983). Attention allocation and performance in goal‐ directed reading: Age difference in reading flexibility. Journal of Reading Behavior, 15, 55–65. 10.1080/10862968309547489 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  85. Raven, J. , Raven, J. C. , & Court, J. H. (2003). Manual for Raven's progressive matrices and vocabulary scales. San Antonio, TX: Harc. [Google Scholar]
  86. Röthlisberger, M. , Neuenschwander, R. , Cimeli, P. , & Roebers, C. M. (2013). Executive functions in 5‐ to 8‐year‐olds: Developmental changes and relationship to academic achievement. Journal of Educational & Developmental Psychology, 3, 153–167. 10.5539/jedp.v3n2p153 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  87. Samuels, S. J. , & Flor, R. (1997). The importance of automaticity for developing expertise in reading. Reading and Writing Quarterly, 13, 107–122. 10.1080/1057356970130202 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  88. Seigneuric, A. , & Ehrlich, M. F. (2005). Contribution of working memory capacity to children’s reading comprehension: A longitudinal investigation. Reading and Writing: an Interdisciplinary Journal, 18, 617–656. 10.1007/s11145-005-2038-0 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  89. Sesma, H. W. , Mahone, E. M. , Levine, T. , Eason, S. H. , & Cutting, L. E. (2009). The contribution of executive skills to reading comprehension. Child Neuropsychology, 15(3), 232–246. 10.1080/09297040802220029 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Shah, P. , & Miyake, A. (1996). The separability of working memory resources for spatial thinking and language processing: An individual differences approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 125(1), 4–27. 10.1037/0096-3445.125.1.4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Sociaal Cultureel Planbureau (2017). Status scores. Retrieved from: https://www.volksgezondheidenzorg.info/onderwerp/sociaaleconomische‐status/regionaal‐internationaal/regionaal#bronverantwoording [Google Scholar]
  92. Stanovich, K. E. (2000). Progress in understanding reading: Scientific foundations and new frontiers. New York: Guilford Press. [Google Scholar]
  93. Tesser, P. T. M. , Merens, J. G. F. , & van Praag, C. S. (1999). Rapportage minderheden 1999: Positie in het onderwijs en op de arbeidsmarkt. In Idema J. (Ed.), Sociaal en Cultureel Planbureau. Den Haag: Elsevier bedrijfsinformatie. [Google Scholar]
  94. Torgesen, J. K. (2000). Individual differences in response to early interventions in reading: The lingering problem of treatment resisters. Learning Disabilities Research and Practice, 15, 55–64. 10.1207/SLDRP1501_6 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  95. Tunmer, W. E. , & Hoover, W. A. (1992). Cognitive and linguistic factors in learning to read. In Gough P. B., Ehri L. C. & Treiman R. (Eds.), Reading acquisition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. [Google Scholar]
  96. Vellutino, F. R. , Scanlon, D. M. , & Lyon, G. R. (2000). Differentiating between difficult to remediate and readily remediated poor readers: More evidence against the IQ Achievement discrepancy definition of reading disability. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33(3), 223–238. 10.1177/002221940003300302 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Verhoeven, L. , & Perfetti, C. (2008). Advances in text comprehension: Model, process and development. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 22, 293–301. 10.1002/acp.1417 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  98. Verhoeven, L. , & van Leeuwe, J. (2008). Predictors of text comprehension development. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 22, 407–423. 10.1002/acp.1414 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  99. Voeten, M. J. M. , & Van den Bercken, J. H. L. (2003). (Linear regression analysis) Lineaire regressie analyse. Groningen, The Netherlands: Stenfert‐Kroese. [Google Scholar]
  100. Wechsler, D. L. (2004). Wechsler intelligence scale for children (4th ed.). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation. [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Data Availability Statement

Data available on request from the authors.


Articles from The British Journal of Educational Psychology are provided here courtesy of Wiley

RESOURCES