Skip to main content
Pediatric Investigation logoLink to Pediatric Investigation
. 2021 Mar 22;5(1):33–37. doi: 10.1002/ped4.12250

Global and communicative development skills in preschool children with cleft lip and palate

Mayalle Rocha Bonfim Jurado 1, Maria Gabriela Cavalheiro 2, Camila de Castro Corrêa 3,4, Melina Evangelista Whitaker 2, Simone Rocha de Vasconcelos Hage 1, Dionísia Aparecida Cusin Lamônica 1, Luciana Paula Maximino 1,2,
PMCID: PMC7984020  PMID: 33778425

ABSTRACT

Importance

Cleft lip and palate (CLP) is globally among the most common childhood malformations. This disorder impacts childhood development, including speech and language, and affects children worldwide.

Objective

To analyze child development skills (adaptive fine motor, gross motor, personal‐social, and language) in preschool children with isolated CLP compared with children without this malformation.

Methods

The participants included an experimental group of 27 children with isolated CLP and a comparison group of 27 children without CLP aged between 48 and 59 months. The groups were evaluated using two instruments: the Denver Developmental Screening Test II (DDST‐II) and the Avaliação do Desenvolvimento da Linguagem (ADL‐Language Development Assessment). Data were analyzed by descriptive and inductive analyses, using the Student’s t‐test and the Mann‐Whitney test, at a significance level of P ˂ 0.05.

Results

All children in the comparison group performed within normal standards for their age range in the DDST‐II and the ADL. The worst performance in the experimental group was observed in language skills, followed, in declining order, by adaptive fine motor, personal‐social, and gross motor as measured by the DDST‐II. Children with isolated CLP also performed poorly in receptive, expressive, and global language in the ADL. No statistically significant differences were observed in the experimental group’s scores for the ADL and the DDST‐II.

Interpretation

Developmental skill levels were below expectations for children of this age with isolated CLP.

Keywords: Cleft lip and palate, Language, Child, Preschool

1. INTRODUCTION

Cleft lip and palate (CLP) is often related to communication disorders. 1 Speech impediments are related to the anatomical involvement of the structures of oral production, velopharyngeal dysfunction, and dentofacial deformity, such as alterations in articulatory production and speech hypernasality. 2 , 3

Research suggests CLP is a risk factor for child development, with possible damage including that to social, emotional, 4 motor, and cognitive skills. 5 It affects quality of life and is associated with language disorders, even in cases of CLP without comorbidities or genetic syndromes. 3 , 6 , 7 , 8 , 9

The alterations in speech development of children with CLP are associated with damage to middle‐ear functioning and is the primary cause of impaired cognitive development. This is because of insufficient stimulus or may be a consequence of speech disorders. 10 , 11 , 12

Studies have demonstrated the significance of language disorders, separate from other risk factors, in other with CLP. The evidence suggests a delay in learning first words, 13 , 14 , 15 a below‐average achievement in receptive and expressive skills, 9 , 13 and changes in vocabulary 15 , 16 , 17 and syntax. 18 However, other studies have indicated that language development is not different in children with or without CLP, with each population experiencing a delay in language acquisition. 19

This study compared the child development skills (adaptive fine motor, gross motor, personal‐social, and language) of preschool children with isolated CLP with those of children without malformation. We hypothesized that preschool children with CLP experience damage to their communicative and global performances. The characterization of these skills is necessary since research findings of language and development in children with clefts are controversial.

2. METHODS

2.1. Ethical approval

The Institutional Review Board approved this study at the university under report n.1.959.530 (CAAE: 63863917.9.0000.5417) and the hospital where the study was conducted under report n.1.994.369 (CAAE: 63863917.9.3001.5441). Data were collected in the Palatal Prosthesis sector. The written consent form was obtained from all participants.

2.2. Study population

The participants were divided into two groups: (1) the experimental group (EG), composed of 27 children with non‐syndromic repaired complete unilateral CLP, assisted at the Hospital for Rehabilitation of Craniofacial Anomalies of the University of São Paulo (HRAC‐USP), Bauru, São Paulo, Brazil, and (2) the comparison group (CG) comprising 27 children from the community who spontaneously responded to the request for participants.

The inclusion criteria for the CG were: 48 to 59 months old, of either sex, and no history of CLP or atypical development. Individuals in the EG had been submitted to primary palate repair, according to international protocols, before 2 years of age. 20 Individuals in the hospitalization routine for surgical reasons or medical complications were excluded from the EG. Children with a history of middle ear disease or diagnosis of genetic syndromes or other malformations were excluded from both EG and CG.

2.3. Procedures

Both groups were analyzed using two different instruments, which were administered directly to the child and collected by a single evaluator.

2.3.1. Denver Developmental Screening Test II (DDST‐II) 21

As described by Frankenburg et al, 21 screening has been used worldwide and standardized in more than a dozen countries (Brazilian Portuguese included). Health professionals apply it to children from birth to 6 years of age. It consists of 125 items, distributed in four areas of development: Personal‐social (PS), Language (LGG), Adaptive fine motor (AFM) and Gross motor (GM), measuring the risks of delaying neuropsychomotor development. Its application consists of direct skills testing, behavioral observation, and information reported by caregivers.

The results were interpreted according to the DDST‐II guideline criteria (pass or fail in the screening) and analyzed to assess whether each developmental skill the child displayed was in line with expected skill levels for their age

2.3.2. Avaliação do Desenvolvimento da Linguagem (ADL‐Language Development Assessment) 22

This scale was standardized for Brazilian Portuguese speaking children by Menezes, 22 who identified changes in the learning and development of oral language and the different etiologies of specific language changes in children aged 1 to 6 years and 11 months. It was used to evaluate receptive and expressive oral language (in tasks related to language domains: phonology, morphology, syntax, semantics, and pragmatics). The sample age range tasks were grouped in concepts such as quantity, quality (adjectives), spatial and temporal relationship, and sequence. Compensatory articulations (CA) were not considered, as suggested in the instrument guidelines. 22

For our analysis, 1 point was assigned to each correct response and 0 to each error or missing response. The results were classified according to the standard score: 115–85 corresponded to the normal standard; 84–77 to mild disorder; 76–70 to moderate disorder; and equal to or below 69 to severe disorder.

2.4. Data analysis

The categorical variables were described in number and percentages, and the continuous variables were expressed as mean ± standard deviation. Group comparisons used a significance level of P < 0.05. The Fisher test was used to assess categorical variables, and the Mann‐Whitney test was used for continuous variables. The Student’s t‐test was used for comparing means and the Spearman test for data correlations.

3. RESULTS

This study included a convenience sample of 54 children who were regularly registered at a specialized referral hospital. The EG group comprised 15 boys (56%) and 12 girls (44%), with a mean age of 53.0 ± 3.5 months. The CG group comprised 9 boys (33%) and 18 girls (67%), with a mean age of 53.0 ± 4.0 months and matched socioeconomic and educational levels.

All children in the CG performed within normal standards for their age range according to the DDST‐II, taking into account the pass‐fail criterion (the child shows no “delay” and, some children presented only one error, which for the test means “attention to development”). In the EG, 17 children (63%) failed and were classified as having delayed development; 10 children (37%) passed, highlighting the difference in general performances between the EG and CG (P = 0.0001). In the EG, language represented the most disorders (48%, 13 children), followed by adaptive fine motor skills (29%, 8 children), personal‐social skills (25%, 7 children), and gross motor skills (11%, 3 children). Performance was lower for all four domains in the EG. The mean performance of the EG in all skills measured by the DDST‐II was below that expected for the age range (Table 1), with a statistically significant difference between groups in language, adaptive fine motor, and personal‐social skills.

TABLE 1.

Comparison of skills assessed by the DDST‐II between the experimental group and comparison group

Items Experimental group Comparison group Mean difference (95% CI) P
PS 50.33 ± 7.15 53.78 ± 4.01 −3.45 (−6.61, −0.28) 0.03
AFM 50.89 ± 5.28 53.78 ± 4.01 −2.89 (−5.45, −0.33) 0.03
LGG 50.89 ± 3.89 53.78 ± 4.01 −2.89 (−5.03, −0.75) 0.01
GM 52.19 ± 4.13 53.78 ± 4.01 −1.59 (−3.82, 0.63) 0.16

DDST‐II, Denver Developmental Screening Test II; PS, personal‐social; LGG, language; AFM, adaptive fine motor; GM, gross motor; CI, confidence interval.

The CG children’s performance in the ADL was 100% normal. In the EG, however, 15% of the children performed below expectations, with the difference appearing between the means of groups (EG 104.26 ± 15.08 vs. CG 114.56 ± 5.21, P = 0.001). Table 2 shows the outcomes of the ADL test. Again, the EG’s performance was lower for receptive language, expressive language, and global score compared with the CG.

TABLE 2.

Comparison of skills assessed by ADL between the experimental group and the comparison group

Items Experimental group Comparison group Mean difference (95% CI) P
Receptive 101.33 ± 13.26 109.41 ± 4.81 −8.08 (−13.52, −2.63) 0.004
Expressive 106.44 ± 15.93 116.44 ± 6.85 −10.00 (−16.70, −3.30) 0.004
Global 104.26 ± 15.08 114.56 ± 5.21 −10.30 (−5.03, −0.75) 0.001

ADL, Language Development Assessment; CI, confidence interval.

No statistically significant differences were observed in the EG’s scores between the DDST‐II and the ADL (Table 3). In addition, 13 of the 17 children who failed the DDST‐II were classified within the normal standards according to the ADL, suggesting that the DDST‐II instrument was more sensitive to detect changes in the study group than the ADL.

TABLE 3.

Comparison of performance of the experimental group in ADL and DDST‐II

Related test Category/Classification DDST‐II P
Pass Fail
ADL classification Normality 10 13 0.33
Mild disorder 0 1
Moderate disorder 0 2
Severe disorder 0 1

ADL, Language Development Assessment; DDST‐II, Denver Developmental Screening Test II.

4. DISCUSSION

Owing to frequent speech disorders in children with CLP caused by velopharyngeal dysfunction, few studies focus on the performance of linguistic skills in this population. The focus of this study is on the possible hearing and stimulus risks 10 , 11 , 12 that may affect receptive and expressive language, which can worsen speech disorders caused by the anatomical and functional conditions in these children. 2 , 3

The present study’s hypothesis was supported: children with CLP aged 48 to 59 months (4 years of age) evidenced damage in their communicative and global performances compared with children without CLP. These findings are consistent with research that suggests CLP’s presence is a factor that causes a delay in overall child development, 4 , 5 including speech and language disorders. 3 , 9 , 13 , 14 , 15 , 16 , 17 , 18

Our sample of children with CLP showed delayed development: 63% failed in the DDST‐II, performed poorly in the skills analyzed (personal‐social: P = 0.03; adaptive fine motor: P = 0.03; language: P = 0.01), and displayed a significant language deficit. These findings are consistent with prior research, which reports that the failure rate of 5‐year‐old children with CLP in the DDST‐II is indicative of poor language performance. 23 Another study using the Kent Infant Developmental Scale and the Minnesota Child Development Inventory instruments on 36‐month‐old children also indicated poor performance in language and adaptive fine motor skills. 24

In the ADL, which assessed receptive and expressive language and the overall score, 15% of the EG showed more alterations compared with the CG. This corroborates studies that have reported damage to receptive and expressive language skills in this population. 9 , 25

A comparison of the children’s performances revealed a more significant incidence of language disorders (48%) when evaluated by the DDST‐II, compared with the ADL (15%). The complexity of language requires the application of various tools to investigate all expressive and receptive language levels, to ensure that the combination of these results can provide the diagnostic conclusion for language as early as possible. 10 , 26

Although the findings are evidence of a possible language disorder in children with CLP, some studies have reported similar findings in children with or without CLP. 20 This demonstrates the importance of conducting more research on children with CLP. The aim should be to establish language evaluation protocols and to take into account all linguistic aspects that will contribute to the rehabilitation of these children.

There are still a few studies that used the DDST‐II, which is an instrument for developmental screening in children. 23 , 24 , 25 For the present study, this instrument met our goal of identifying children at risk. However, future studies should consider using other global development instruments to compare global aspects and specific language development to better understand children with CLP.

Parents and caregivers of children with CLP are often concerned about the child’s communication skills, but have difficulty expressing their concerns about speech/language aspects. 27 It is therefore important to raise awareness of the wider effects of CLP. 28 Early interventions are performed by speech therapists trained to identify and treat speech and language disorders. A multidisciplinary approach is important because the age at which children receive primary palate surgical repair (approximately 12 months) is associated with better speech outcomes. 20 , 29 , 30 This can positively influence language development.

In conclusion, child development skills were found to be lower than expected in children with CLP, with a significant deficit in receptive and expressive language.

CONFLICT OF INTEREST

None.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

The authors thank the scholarship granted by CAPES in the conduction of this study, by granting a scholarship.

Jurado MRB, Cavalheiro MG, de Castro Corrêa C, Whitaker ME, de Vasconcelos Hage SR, Lamônica DAC, et al. Global and communicative development skills in preschool children with cleft lip and palate. Pediatr Investig. 2021;5:33‐37. 10.1002/ped4.12250

REFERENCES

  • 1. De Bodt M, Van Lierde K. Cleft palate speech and velopharyngeal dysfunction: the approach of the speech therapist. B‐ENT. 2006;2 Suppl 4:63‐70. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Hardin‐Jones MA, Chapman KL, Wright J, Halter KA, Schulte J, Dean JA, et al. The impact of early palatal obturation on consonant development in babies with unrepaired cleft palate. Cleft Palate Craniofac J. 2002;39:157‐163. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Chapman KL. The relationship between early reading skills and speech and language performance in young children with cleft lip and palate. Cleft Palate Craniofac J. 2011;48:301‐311. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Schuster M, Maier A, Haderlein T, Nkenke E, Wohlleben U, Rosanowski F, et al. Evaluation of speech intelligibility for children with cleft lip and palate by means of automatic speech recognition. Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol. 2006;70:1741‐1747. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Kuehn DP, Moller KT. Speech and language issues in the cleft palate population: The state of the art. Cleft Palate Craniofac J. 2000;37:348. [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Silva MARD, Balderrama IF, Wobeto AP, Werneck RI, Azevedo‐Alanis LR. The impact of nonsyndromic cleft lip with or without cleft palate on oral health‐related quality of life. J Appl Oral Sci. 2018;26:e20170145. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Hutters B, Bau A, Brøndsted K. A longitudinal group study of speech development in Danish children born with and without cleft lip and palate. Int J Lang Commun Disord. 2001;36:447‐470. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Brunnegård K, Lohmander A. A cross‐sectional study of speech in 10‐year‐old children with cleft palate: Results and issues of rater reliability. Cleft Palate Craniofac J. 2007;44:33‐44. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Cavalheiro MG, Lamônica DAC, de Vasconsellos Hage SR, Maximino LP. Child development skills and language in toddlers with cleft lip and palate. Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol. 2019;116:18‐21. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10. Ghayoumi Anaraki Z, Faham M, Derakhshandeh F, Hashemi Hosseinabad H, Haresabadi F. Language parameters of 4‐ to 7‐year‐old Persian‐speaking children with cleft lip and palate. Folia Phoniatr Logop. 2016;68:119‐123. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Cerom JL, Macedo Cde C, Feniman MR. Can peripheral hearing justify the speech disorders in children with operated cleft palate? Int Arch Otorhinolaryngol. 2014;18:27‐35. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Shaffer AD, Ford MD, Choi SS, Jabbour N. The impact of tympanostomy tubes on speech and language development in children with cleft palate. Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg. 2017;157:504‐514. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Prathanee B, Pumnum T, Seepuaham C, Jaiyoung P. Five‐year speech and language outcomes in children with cleft lip‐palate. J Cranio‐maxillofac Surg. 2016;44:1553‐1560. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14. Scherer NJ, Oravkinova Z, McBee MT. Longitudinal comparison of early speech and language milestones in children with cleft palate: A comparison of US and Slovak children. Clin Linguist Phon. 2013;27:404‐418. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Lu Z, Ma L, Luo Y, Fletcher P. The effects of unrepaired cleft palate on early language development in Chinese infants. Cleft Palate Craniofac J. 2010;47:400‐404. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16. Hardin‐Jones M, Chapman KL. Early lexical characteristics of toddlers with cleft lip and palate. Cleft Palate Craniofac J. 2014;51:622‐631. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17. Scherer NJ, Boyce S, Martin G. Pre‐linguistic children with cleft palate: Growth of gesture, vocalization, and word use. Int J Speech Lang Pathol. 2013;15:586‐592. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Young SE, Purcell AA, Ballard KJ. Expressive language skills in Chinese Singaporean preschoolers with nonsyndromic cleft lip and/or palate. Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol. 2010;74:456‐464. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19. Boyce JO, Kilpatrick N, Reilly S, Da Costa A, Morgan AT. Receptive and expressive language characteristics of school‐aged children with non‐syndromic cleft lip and/or palate. Int J Lang Commun Disord. 2018;53:959‐968. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20. Williams WN, Seagle MB, Pegoraro‐Krook MI, Souza TV, Garla L, Silva ML, et al. Prospective clinical trial comparing outcome measures between Furlow and von Langenbeck Palatoplasties for UCLP. Ann Plast Surg. 2011;66:154‐163. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Frankenburg WK, Dodds JB, Archer P, Shapiro H, Bresnick B. Denver II Training Manual. Aurora, CO: Denver Developmental Materials, Inc; 1992. [Google Scholar]
  • 22. Menezes, MLM . ADL: avaliação do desenvolvimento da linguagem. Rio de Janeiro, 2004. (in Portuguese)
  • 23. Rongbudsri S, Patjanasoontorn N, Pradabwong S, Chowchuen B. Development outcomes of Thai children with cleft lip/palate at 5‐years‐old. J Med Assoc Thai. 2012;95 Suppl 11:S88‐92. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24. Neiman GS, Savage HE. Development of infants and toddlers with clefts from birth to three years of age. Cleft Palate Craniofac J. 1997;34:218‐225. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25. Morgan AR, Bellucci CC, Coppersmith J, Linde SB, Curtis A, Albert M, et al. Language development in children with cleft palate with or without cleft lip adopted from non–English‐speaking countries. Am J Speech Lang Pathol. 2017;26:342‐354. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26. Lindau TA, Lucchesi FDM, Rossi NF, Giacheti CM. Systematic and formal language assessment tools for preschoolers in Brazil: A literature review. Revista CEFAC. 2015;17:656‐662. (in Portuguese) [Google Scholar]
  • 27. Subramaniyan B, Nagarajan R, Vaidyanathan R, Rajashekhar B, Sathiyasekaran BWC. Caregivers’ perception of speech and language status and related needs in children with cleft lip and palate. Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol. 2018;108:22‐25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28. Corrêa Cde C, Freire T, Zabeu JS, Martins A, Ferreira R, Francisconi PA, et al. Teleducation about cleft lip and palate: An interdisciplinary approach in the promotion of health. Int Arch Otorhinolaryngol. 2015;19:106‐111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29. Bertier CE, Trindade IEK, Silva Filho OG. Primary lip and palate surgeries. In: Trindade IEK, Silva Filho OG, eds. Cleft Lip and Palate: An Interdisciplinary Approach. São Paulo: Santos; 2007:73‐86. (in Portuguese) [Google Scholar]
  • 30. Menegueti KI, Mangilli LD, Alonso N, Andrade CRF. Speech profile of patients undergoing primary palatoplasty. Codas. 2017;29:e20160146. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Pediatric Investigation are provided here courtesy of Chinese Medical Association

RESOURCES